You are on page 1of 4

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/317178520

Self-Assessment Manikin

Chapter · January 2017


DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_77-1

CITATIONS READS

9 13,717

2 authors:

Teah-Marie Bynion Matthew T Feldner


University of Arkansas Canopy Growth Corporation
7 PUBLICATIONS 20 CITATIONS 157 PUBLICATIONS 5,119 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

An Evaluation of the Validity of a Script-Driven Imagery Procedure View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Teah-Marie Bynion on 20 September 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


S

Self-Assessment Manikin inside or outside the organism, specifically


changes that challenge states and systems neces-
Teah-Marie Bynion and Matthew T. Feldner sary for survival” (p. 25; Keil and Miskovic
Department of Psychological Science, University 2015). Emotions can be measured across at least
of Arkansas, Fayetteville, AR, USA three different modes of response: physiological,
cognitive, and behavioral (Lang 1985). Within
this broad approach to defining emotion, there
Definition are at least two major perspectives adopted for
more specifically understanding emotions. The
The Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM) is a picture- first approach can be referred to as a “discrete
oriented questionnaire developed to measure an emotions” perspective. Within this perspective,
emotional response (Bradley and Lang 1994). The emotions are generally thought to be specific,
questionnaire is designed to measure three fea- cross-cultural, innate, systemic responses
tures of an emotional response that have been (Ekman 1992). For example, fear can be thought
identified as central to emotion in research of as a discrete emotion that across cultures is
conducted by Lang and colleagues (Lang et al. characterized by elevated physiological arousal,
1993). Specifically, there are single-item scales perceived threat, and escape behavior (Barlow
that measure valence/pleasure of the response 2002). In contrast to the discrete emotions per-
(from positive to negative), perceived arousal spective, the dimensional perspective suggests
(from high to low levels), and perceptions of that emotions are responses to environmental
dominance/control (from low to high levels). stimuli that vary along dimensions of key features
or characteristics. Within the dimensional per-
spective on emotion, theory and empirical work
Introduction has converged to suggest at least three core fea-
tures to an emotional response (i.e., valence/plea-
Several theorists have explained human emotions sure, arousal, and dominance/control; Osgood
as responses to environmental challenges that et al. 1957). These dimensions are theorized to
increase the individual’s likelihood of survival. respond somewhat independently resulting in
While there is ongoing debate regarding what dimensions that can differentially respond across
the defining features of an emotion are, emotions time to emotion eliciting events (Mehrabian and
can be broadly conceptualized as “adaptive action Russell 1974).
tendencies that occur in response to changes

# Springer International Publishing AG 2017


V. Zeigler-Hill, T.K. Shackelford (eds.), Encyclopedia of Personality and Individual Differences,
DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-28099-8_77-1
2 Self-Assessment Manikin

History and Structure Applications

Self-reports are commonly used to measure the The SAM is an imagery-based measure that there-
three dimensions of an emotional response. Prior fore can be thought of as language-free. Thus, use
to the SAM, the Semantic Differential Scale of the SAM is not circumscribed to any one cul-
(SDS; Mehrabian and Russell 1974) was a widely ture, and it can be easily understood and appro-
used measure for measuring valence, arousal, and priate for use in different countries (Bradley et al.
control. However, with this measure came signif- 1992). Similarly, the SAM can be administered
icant limitations, including a laborious data set effectively with both children and adults (Lang
that was cumbersome to analyze and was difficult 1985) as well as various clinical populations
to use with individuals who were non-English (Bradley and Lang 1994).
speakers (Bradley and Lang 1994). Another feature of the SAM that makes it
In order to address limitations of previous widely applicable is that it is brief. Due to its
dimensional measures of emotion, Lang (1980) brevity, it can be used to capture emotional
devised the SAM to assess an emotional response responses to a wide array of emotion elicitation
to an object or event. The SAM is a picture- methods. For example, it has been used before and
oriented instrument containing five images for after biological challenges (e.g., Feldner et al.
each of the three affective dimensions that the 2003) to measure how emotional state changes
participant rates on either a 9- or 21-point scale. in response to these procedures. As a second
While the SAM instrument was initially adminis- example, the SAM has been used to measure
tered via computer and uses a 21-point scale, a emotional state in numerous advertising studies
pencil-and-paper version also exists that has (Morris et al. 1994). The SAM is typically used to
respondents place an “X” either on or between measure emotional states (Meagher et al. 2001).
each of the five figures (resulting in a 9-point To the best of our knowledge, a version of the
scale). The meaning of each scale is described to SAM designed to measure emotion-related traits
respondents, and they are asked to place the “X” (e.g., frequent experiences of elevated arousal)
on a figure (or between figures) that best repre- has not been developed.
sents how they currently feel. Valence is depicted
from positive (a smiling figure), to neutral, to
negative (a frowning figure). Arousal is depicted Conclusion
ranging from high arousal (eyes wide open) to low
arousal (eyes closed). The arousal scale, using Overall, the SAM is a brief and nonverbal mea-
these same figures, also depicts the intensity of sure of emotional state that has been used across a
arousal with additional imagery over the abdomen variety of settings with various populations (e.g.,
area that ranges from high intensity (imagery gender, age, race; Bradley et al. 1992; Lang 1985).
representing an explosive-like burst) to low inten- The SAM is based on dimensional models of
sity (imagery representing a small pin prick). emotion and therefore measures key dimensions
Finally, dominance/control ranges from feeling of emotional responding.
controlled or submissive (a very small figure) to
feeling in control or dominant (a very large figure;
Morris 1995). Correlations between the original
References
SDS and SAM were obtained for valence (.94),
arousal (.94), and dominance (.66). Barlow, D. H. (2002). Anxiety and its disorders: The nature
and treatment of anxiety and panic (2nd ed.).
New York: Guilford.
Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion:
The self-assessment manikin and the semantic differ-
ential. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental
Psychiatry, 25(1), 49–59.
Self-Assessment Manikin 3

Bradley, M. M., Greenwald, M. K., Petry, M., & Lang, P. J. Lang, P. J., Greenwald, M. K., Bradley, M. M., & Hamm,
(1992). Remembering pictures: Pleasure and arousal in A. O. (1993). Looking at pictures: Evaluative, facial,
memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learn- visceral, and behavioral responses. Psychophysiology,
ing, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 379–390. 30(3), 261–273.
Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cog- Meagher, M. W., Arnau, R. C., & Rhudy, J. L. (2001). Pain
nition & Emotion, 6(3–4), 169–200. and emotion: Effects of affective picture modulation.
Feldner, M. T., Zvolensky, M. J., Eifert, G. H., & Spira, Psychosomatic Medicine, 63(1), 79–90.
A. P. (2003). Emotional avoidance: An experimental Mehrabian, A., & Russell, J. A. (1974). An approach to
test of individual differences and response suppression environmental psychology. Cambridge, MA: MIT.
using biological challenge. Behaviour Research and Morris, J. D. (1995). Observations: SAM: The Self-
Therapy, 41(4), 403–411. Assessment Manikin; an efficient cross-cultural mea-
Keil, A., & Miskovic, V. (2015). Human emotions: surement of emotional response. Journal of Advertising
A conceptual overview. In K. A. Babson & M. T. Research, 35(6), 63–68.
Feldner (Eds.), Sleep and affect: Assessment, theory, Morris, J. D., Bradley, M. M., & Karrh, J. A. (1994).
and clinical implications (pp. 23–44). San Diego: Assessing affective response to television advertising
Elsevier. using the Self-Assessment Manikin (SAM). Working
Lang, P. J. (1980). Behavioral treatment and bio-behavioral paper, University of Florida, College of Journalism
assessment: Computer applications. In J. B. Sidowski, and Communications.
J. H. Johnson, & T. A. Williams (Eds.), Technology in Osgood, C., Suci, G., & Tannenbaum, P. (1957). The
mental health care delivery systems (pp. 119–137). measurement of meaning. Urbana: University of
Norwood: Ablex. Illinois.
Lang, P. J. (1985). The cognitive psychophysiology of
emotion: Anxiety and the anxiety disorders. Hillsdale:
Lawrence Eribaum.

View publication stats

You might also like