You are on page 1of 8

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/234823080

Neuropsychological Performance, IQ, Personality, and Grades in a Longitudinal


Grade-School Male Sample

Article in Individual Differences Research · December 2003

CITATIONS READS
11 2,485

5 authors, including:

Jordan B Peterson Robert O Pihl


University of Toronto McGill University
133 PUBLICATIONS 10,644 CITATIONS 334 PUBLICATIONS 17,169 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Properties of finite mixture modelling View project

MIREC Study (Maternal-Infant Research on Environmental Chemicals) View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Jordan B Peterson on 04 June 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Individual Differences Research, 2003, 1(3) 159 160 Individual Differences Research, 2003, 1(3)
ISSN: 1541-745X www.idr-journal.com IQ tests are robustly associated with academic achievement (Brody, 1997),
but may not comprehensively assess intellectual capacity (Gardner, 1993;
©2003 Individual Differences Research Group. All rights reserved. Sternberg, Wagner, Williams & Horvath, 1995), and reveal comparatively little
about specific brain function (Kandel & Freed, 1989; Lezak, 1985; Milner &
Neuropsychological Performance, IQ, Petrides, 1984; Moffitt & Silva, 1988; Stuss & Benson, 1986). Studies of the
cognitive performance of individuals who have sustained prefrontal cortical
Personality, and Grades in a Longitudinal damage in adulthood provide evidence for the limitations of standard IQ tests,
Grade-School Male Sample as well as detailed information about how aspects of cognitive function
overlooked by such tests might be assessed.
Prefrontally brain-damaged adults frequently score at their premorbid level
Jordan B. Peterson* for IQ (or at least obtain scores in the normal range, when their premorbid IQ
University of Toronto is not known). Damasio (1994), for example, described a prefrontally-damaged
Robert O. Pihl patient who had a pre- and post-surgical IQ 3 SD's above the mean, despite his
marked practical cognitive deterioration. Earlier observations in this line
McGill University
bolstered attempts to use prefrontal surgery to relieve psychiatric
Daniel M. Higgins symptomatology, since such surgery apparently did not produce general IQ
Harvard University decrements (Fleming, 1942; Grassi, 1950). However, later, more cautious
observers (Luria, Homskay & Blinkov, 1967) noted that the prefrontal cortex
Jean R. Séguin & Richard E. Tremblay was involved in goal-directed action, among other complex forms of behavior
Université de Montréal and thought. The contrast between the real-world deterioration of
prefrontally-damaged individuals and their generally positive objective
ABSTRACT - This study assessed the statistical relationship between intelligence test scores drove decades of research into more narrowly defined
neuropsychological performance, IQ and personality test results and school tests of "executive function" - the metaphoric term frequently used to describe
grades in a longitudinal sample of adolescent males. One-hundred and forty- the cognitive operations of the prefrontal cortex (Goldman-Rakic, 1996). These
eight boys completed six years of WISC-R short forms (Block Design and tests include, but are not limited to, the "executive function" tasks included in
Vocabulary) and provided six years of math and language grades and grade the present study.
failure data while in elementary school. In junior high school, the same boys Other theoretically specific forms of cognitive ability have also been assessed
completed an extensive neuropsychological test battery and the NEO-PI-R, a using neuropsychological tests, including verbal fluency (Lezak, 1983),
standard big five personality trait measure. Neuropsychological test scores were associated with the function of the left frontal and bilateral temporal cortices
more powerfully associated with grades than were IQ scores, despite their later (Frith, Friston, Liddle & Frackowiak, 1991; Parks, Loewenstein, Dodrill,
and single administration. In addition, hierarchical regression analysis Barker, Yoshii, Chang, Emran, Apicella, Sheramata & Duara, 1988; Randolph,
demonstrated that three of four neuropsychological test score factors (Verbal Braun, Goldberg & Chase, 1993); attention (Lezak, 1983) and resistance to
Learning, Executive Function, and Tactile Laterality improved the statistical distractibility (Sattler, 1988), using the Digit Span test from the Wechsler
association with six-year averaged failure-weighted grades over and above IQ Memory Scales (Wechsler, 1987); memory, both verbal, using the Paired
(averaged Vocabulary and Block Design). NEO-PI-R Agreeableness was Associates test from the Wechsler Memory Scales (Wechsler, 1987), and spatial,
significantly and positively related to grades, over and above both IQ and using the Spatial Memory task developed by Smith & Milner (1981; 1989); and
neuropsychological function. somaesthetic ability, using the Dichaptic Lateralization task, which is
theoretically associated with the functions of the right hemisphere, and assesses
the ability to perceive nonlinguistically presented spatial/tactual stimuli
Key Words: Academic performance, neuropsychology, IQ, personality, Big Five
(Witelson, 1974; 1976).
Finally, there is some evidence that personality may also be a valid predictor
*Address correspondence to Jordan B. Peterson; Department of Psychology; of performance or achievement, both academic (Wentzel, 1993) and industrial
University of Toronto; 100 St. George Street, Toronto; Ontario, Canada; M5S (Hunter & Schmidt, 1996; Barrick & Mount, 1996). This is particularly true in
3G3; jbpeterson@psych.utoronto.ca (email). the academic case when the dependent variable is in-class performance, rather
than written work (Rothstein, Paunonen, Rush & King, 1999). Furthermore, it
Individual Differences Research, 2003, 1(3) 161 162 Individual Differences Research, 2003, 1(3)
appears that inclusion of personality trait measures can improve the predictive IQ Estimates
validity of batteries assessing planning and organizational skill (McHenry, All available boys were administered the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
Hough, Toquam, Hanson & Ashworth, 1990). Personality research has Children - Revised (WISC-R) Block Design (Wechsler, 1974) subtest regularly
converged on a big-five trait model, assessing the orthogonal dimensions of from ages 7-12 and again at age 15, and the WISC-R Vocabulary subtest
positive emotionality, negative emotionality, conscientious achievement, (Wechsler, 1974) regularly from ages 7-13 and again at age 15. Scaled subtest
agreeable amiability, and openness or aesthetic/intellectual interest (McCrae & scores were computed within, and then averaged across available years. The
John, 1992; Goldberg, 1992). boys averaged 2.78 Block Design observations (SD= 1.73), with a range of 1-6,
Some trait features, such as higher levels of conscientious achievement and and 4.18 Vocabulary observations (SD= 2.15), with a range of 1-6. Mean raw
lower levels of negative emotionality, appear universally associated with higher score for Block Design = 10.61 (SD= 2.65); for Vocabulary, 8.05 (SD= 2.18).
levels of performance (Hunter & Schmidt, 1996; Barrick & Mount, 1996). The This corresponds to a scaled score of ~ 105 for Block Design and ~ 85 for
desirability of other trait features appears more situation-specific. Individuals Vocabulary.
working in sales, for example, should be characterized by higher levels of The mean correlation between Block Design measures was r = .66 (SD= .16).
positive emotionality or extraversion (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Salgado, 1997). The mean correlation between the averaged Block Design measure and any
Prosociality, which appears to be an attribute somewhat equivalent to given year was r = .86 (SD= .06), while it was r = .85, p < .0001 for the measure
agreeableness, appears associated with better grades (Wentzel, 1993), with taken at age 15. The alpha reliability for Block Design (N = 33, because of
antisociality predicting the reverse outcome. Enhanced agreeableness is missing data across years) was .92. The mean correlation between Vocabulary
similarly associated with increased interpersonal skill, and improved work team measures was r = .63 (SD= .13). The mean correlation between the averaged
performance (Neuman & Wright, 1999), but may not be associated with high Vocabulary measure and any given year was r = .82 (SD= .13), while it was r
levels of creative achievement (King, McKee & Broyles, 1996), and is not = .75, p < .0001 for the measure taken at age 15. The alpha reliability for
necessarilydesirable among high autonomy managers (Barrick & Mount, 1993). Vocabulary (N = 33, because of missing data across years) was .90.
Openness, finally, appears associated with flexibility in thinking and creativity
(King, McKee and Broyles, 1996). Grade Estimates
We therefore attempted to determine if neuropsychological test and Grades for all available boys were obtained for every year from age 10 to 14.
personality test scores would be associated with school achievement, in a The boys attended a variety of schools, which graded according to varying
longitudinally-followed population of adolescent males. We hypothesized (1) criteria. For the purposes of comparison, grades were converted to a five-point
that academic achievement would be as strongly associated with scale, with 1= fail/poor, 2= fair, 3= good, 4= very good, and 5= excellent.
neuropsychological test performance as with IQ, particularly in the case of tests Grades were calculated for French and Math classes, averaged within, and then
ofexecutive function and verbal learning, (2) that neuropsychological test scores across available years. Finally, grades were failure-weighted, such that every
would account for variance in academic achievement over and above that of IQ, individual who was not in an age-appropriate class at age 14 (N= 69) had their
and (3) that personality variables, particularly agreeableness (following grade estimate multiplied by .5.
Wentzel, 1993) as well as conscientiousness and reversed neuroticism The boys averaged 3.90 French grade measures (SD= 1.44), with a range of
(following Hunter & Schmidt, 1996 and Barrick & Mount, 1996) would be 1-5, and 3.88 Math grade measures (SD= 1.43). Mean raw score for French =
positivelyrelated to academic achievement, and would account for variance over 2.65 (SD= .70); for Math, 2.85 (SD= 2.65). The mean correlation between
and above that of IQ and neuropsychological performance. French measures was r = .44 (SD= .12). The alpha reliability for the French
average (N = 83, because of missing data across years) was .82. The mean
Method correlation between the failure weighted averaged grade measure and any given
Participants year of French was r = .59 (SD= .12), while it was r = .60, p < .0001 for the
Participants were drawn from an ongoing longitudinal study of the entire measure taken at age 14. The mean correlation between Math measures was r
1984 male kindergarten population of the 53 most impoverished Montreal = .46 (SD= .06). The alpha reliability for the Math average (N = 80, because of
Catholic School districts (original N=1034) (Tremblay, Pihl, Vitaro & Dobkin, missing data across years), was also .82. The mean correlation between the
1994). At age 14, a lab sample of 177 boys was selected for participation in failure weighted averaged grade measure and any given year of Math was r =
summertime laboratory studies, according to criteria detailed in Séguin, Pihl, .59 (SD= .10), while it was r = .52, p < .0001 for the measure taken at age 14.
Harden, Tremblay and Boulerice (1995). Complete longitudinal data for IQ,
neuropsychological function, and grades was available for 148 of these boys. Neuropsychological Performance
Personality data was also available for 126 of these 148. Over the ages of 13 and 14, all boys were administered a neuropsychological
Individual Differences Research, 2003, 1(3) 163 164 Individual Differences Research, 2003, 1(3)
test battery, detailed in Séguin et al. (1995), composed of the Dichaptic Table 2
Lateralization (DL) (Witelson, 1976), Digit Span (DS) and Paired Associates Zero Order Correlations between Incidental Spatial
(PASS) (Wechsler, 1987), Nonspatial Conditional Association (NSP) (Petrides, Learning Factor and Subtests
1990), Self-Ordered Pointing (SOP) (Milner, Petrides & Smith, 1985), Spatial
Memory (SM) (Smith & Milner, 1981), Strategic Problem Solving (SPS) ISLF SMI SMT
(Becker, Butters, Rivoira, & Miliotis, 1986), Subjective Ordering or Number SMI .75
Randomization (NR) (Wiegersma, van der Scheer & Human, 1990; Petrides, SMT -.67 -.28
Alivisatos, Meyer & Evans, 1993), and Verbal Learning [semantic (SFL) and SMR -.62 -.28 .26
letter (LFL) (Lezak, 1983)] tests.
Séguin et al. (1995) factor analyzed (principal components analysis; oblique Notes: ISLF = Incidental Spatial Learning Factor, SMI = Spatial Memory Immediate
rotation) the results of this battery, deriving four factors with eigenvalues >1, Recall, SMT = Spatial Memory Time to Completion, SMR = Spatial Memory Recall
accounting for 58% of the variance: Verbal Learning, composed of the SFL, (Delayed). All p's < .001.
LFL, PASS and DS tests (17.0% of the variance, Alpha = .76); Incidental
Spatial Learning, composed of three SM subscores, (time for completion, Table 3
immediate and delayed recall; 11.5% of the variance, Alpha = .46); Tactile Zero Order Correlations between Tactile Lateralization Factor and Subtests
Lateralization, composed of two DL subscores (correct answers for right and left
hands; 11.8% of variance, Alpha = .64); and Executive Function, composed of TLF DLL
the NSP, NR, SOP, and SPS tests (15.0% of the variance, Alpha = .61). DLR .84
Correlations between these four factors and their subtests are presented in DLL .81 .47
Tables 1-4. The mean correlation for VLF and its subcomponents was .71, for
ISLF and its subcomponents .71, for TLF and its subcomponents .52, and for EF Notes: TLF = Tactile Lateralization Factor, DLR = Dichaptic Lateralization Right, DLL
and its subcomponents .60. = Dichaptic Lateralization Left. All p's < .0001.

Personality
Table 4
The revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R; Costa & McCrae, 1992)
Zero Order Correlations between Executive Function Factor and Subtests
was also administered at age 16, to a reduced set of 126 individuals. The
NEO-PI-R consists of 5 general factors (Extraversion, Neuroticism,
Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, and Openness to Experience). Each subscale EFF NSP NR SOP
is composed of 48 items, coded on a five-point scale. Each general factor may NSP -.81
be further subdivided into 6 facets of 8 items each. For the current analysis, the NR .65 -.38
NEO-PI-R was utilized as five subscale scores. Alpha reliability of these SOP -.58 .37 -.17
subscale scores averages ~ .90 (Costa & McCrae, 1992). SPS .56 -.34 .22 -.20

Notes: EFF = Executive Function Factor, NSP = Non-Spatial Conditioned Association (number of trials,
Table 1 higher is worse), NR = Number Randomization (number of trials, easy version, higher is better), SOP
Zero Order Correlations between Verbal Learning Factor and Subtests = Self-Ordered Pointing, total number of errors, higher is worse), SPS = Strategic Problem Solving
(number of successful tests, higher is better). All p's < .05.
VLF SF LF PA
SF .78
LF .78 .54 Results
PA .62 .45 .36 Correlational Analysis: IQ, Neuropsychological Test Factor Scores and
DS .66 .41 .45 .36 Failure-Weighted Grades
Notes: VLF = Verbal Learning Factor, SF = Semantic Fluency, LF = Letter Fluency, PA The zero-order correlations for the cognitive variables (IQ and
= Paired Associates, DS = Digit Span. All p's < .0001. neuropsychological test factor scores) and failure-weighted grades are presented
in Table 5. The zero-order correlations between personality and failure-weighted
grades were non-significant.
Individual Differences Research, 2003, 1(3) 165 166 Individual Differences Research, 2003, 1(3)
Correlational Analysis: IQ, Individual Neuropsychological Tests, and Regression Analyses: IQ, Neuropsychological Test Factor Scores, Personality
Failure-Weighted Grades and Failure-Weighted Grades
Table 6 contains zero-order correlations for the cognitive tests (IQ and Two stepwise linear regressions were first conducted, to assess the separate
individual neuropsychological test scores) and failure-weighted grades. relationships between IQ and grades and neuropsychological function and
grades. Entry F was set at .05, and removal at .10, for the stepping procedure.
Table 5 1. IQ and Grades: IQ and grades were strongly related, R²= .34, r= .58,
Zero Order Correlations between Cognitive Variables (IQ and F(2,145)= 37.37, p < .0001, with significant contributions from averaged
Neuropsychological Factor Scores) and Failure-Weighted Grades Vocabulary (β = .49, t= 6.97, p< .0001) and averaged Block Design (β = .19, t=
2.68, p< .008).
VLF ISLF TFF Vocab BD FW Grade 2. Neuropsychological Function and Grades: The neuropsychological test
EFF .28 .15 .19 .37 .31 .43 factor scores and grades were strongly related as well, R²= .46, r= .68,
VLF -.00 .12 .51 .33 .60 F(3,144)= 40.94, p< .0001, with significant contributions from the Verbal
ISLF .02 .05 .10 .09 Learning (β = .51, t= 7.91, p< .0001), Executive Function (β = .26, t= 4.01, p<
TFF .17 .14 .28 .0001), and Tactile Laterality (β = .17, t= 2.68, p< .008) factor scores.
Vocab .31 .55 Incidental Spatial Learning stepped out (β in= .05, t= .85, p< .40). The
BD .35 relationship between regression-derived neuropsychological performance
z-scores is presented graphically in Figure 1. It is of some interest to note that
Notes: Exec F= Executive Function factor score, Verb L= Verbal Learning factor score, this relationship appears quadratic, as indicated, rather than linear. This may
TcLT= Tactile Laterality factor score, M Voc= Averaged Vocabulary score, M BD= be valid, or may be a consequence of an apparent floor effect with regards to
Averaged Block Design score, FW Grade= Failure Weighted Grades. All p's < .05,
grades. In any case, analysis of the quadratic relationship revealed R² = .49, r
except ns, underlined.
= .7, p < .0001.
Table 6
Significant Zero Order Correlations between Individual
Figure 1
Relationship between Regression-Derived Neuropsychological
Neuropsychological Scores and Failure-Weighted Grades
Performance Z-Scores and Failure-Weighted Grades
FW Grade
Non Spatial Conditional Association -.36
Number Randomization .13 5
Self-Ordered Pointing (Concrete) -.25
Self-Ordered Pointing (Abstract) -.39
4
Strategic Problem Solving .46
Semantic Fluency .47
Letter Fluency .51 3
Paired Associate .40
Digit Span .50
Dichaptic Lateralization Right .35 2

Notes: FW Grade = Failure-Weighted Grade, Non-Spatial Conditioned Association

Weighted
(number of trials, higher is worse), Number Randomization (number of trials, easy 1

Grades
Failure
version, higher is better), Self-Ordered Pointing (Concrete and Abstract) (number of
errors, higher is worse), Strategic Problem Solving (number of successful tests, higher 0
is better), Paired Associate (number correct, higher is better), Digit Span (number
correct, higher is better), Dichaptic Lateralization Right (number correct, higher is -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3
better). Dichaptic Lateralization Left did not correlate significantly with FW Grade. All
Regression-Derived Neuropsychological Z-Scores
p's < .01.

Note: Linear R² = .46, Quadratic R² = .49, p< .0001, uncorrected for reliability.
Individual Differences Research, 2003, 1(3) 167 168 Individual Differences Research, 2003, 1(3)
The capacity of neuropsychological test performance to provide an & Mount, 1996), would be positively related to academic achievement, and
incremental improvement to the relationship between IQ and failure-weighted would account for variance over and above that of IQ and neuropsychological
grades was assessed next, using hierarchical regression analysis. Averaged performance.
Vocabulary and Averaged Block Design were entered into block 1, while the Neuropsychological test performance (particularly executive function and
four neuropsychological variables were allowed to step in block 2. The overall verbal learning) indeed appeared powerfully associated with grades - at least as
model was highly significant (R²= .51, r= .71, F(5,142)= 29.15, p < .0001), with potently as IQ, and perhaps more so. It is reasonable to draw attention to the
significant block 1 contributions from averaged Vocabulary (β = .24, t= 3.37, magnitude of this relationship: a correlation approaching .70, the magnitude
p< .001), but not averaged Block Design (β = .03, t= .99, p< .32). Verbal obtained in this study, is extremely high. Correlations of .50 or higher are
Learning (β = .38, t= 5.42, p< .0001, incremental r= .089), Executive Function characteristic of only 11% of psychological assessment studies, and 3% of social
(β = .19, t= 2.89, p< .005, incremental r= .026), and Tactile Laterality (β = .14, psychologystudies, on average (Hemphill, 2003). Frequently, assessments of the
t= 2.38, p< .019, incremental r= .014) stepped in significantly in block 2, relationship between general mental ability and various important life outcomes
adding a total incremental r= .13, with Incidental Spatial Learning stepping out, approach or exceed .50. Hunter & Schmidt (1998) reported a large
once again (β in= .04, t= .73, p< .47). A simple stepwise regression with both meta-analytically derived estimate of .51 for the relationship between general
IQ and all four neuropsychological factor scores produced R²= .50, r= .71, mental ability and job performance, for example. However, this estimate is
F(4,143)= 36.19, p< .0001, with Verbal Learning (β = .39, t= 5.71, p< .0001), corrected for restriction of range and reliability - correction that substantively
Averaged Vocabulary (β = .25, t= 3.51, p< .0006), Executive Function (β = .20, increases its magnitude. Assuming, in the present case, that the
t= 3.11, p< .002), Tactile Laterality (β = .15, t= 2.45, p< .02) stepping in and neuropsychological tasks are about as reliable as the grade estimates (.82) - an
averaged Block Design (β in= .06, t= .99, p< .32) and Incidental Spatial assumption that seems reasonable, given that the correlation between the factor
Learning stepping out (β in= .05, t= .80, p< .32). scores and the subcomponent task scores are about the same magnitude as the
The capacity of personality test scores to provide an incremental averaged grade score and grades for any given year - the true correlation could
improvement to the relationship between IQ/neuropsychological performance be as high as .68/.82 = .83. This is an effect size very rarely found in
and failure-weighted grades was then assessed, once again using hierarchical psychology.
regression analysis. The two IQ and four neuropsychological variables were Furthermore, neuropsychological test scores, particularly executive function
entered in block 1, and the five personality variables allowed to step in, in block and verbal learning, appeared to add validity incremental (r = 0.13) to that of
2. The overall model was highly significant R²= .54, r= .74, F(7,118)= 19.88, IQ, alone - no easy task, given that the relationship between IQ and academic
p < .0001, with significant block 1 contributions from Verbal Learning (β = .40, performance is already so powerful. It might be objected that the IQ assessment
t= 5.01, p< .0001), averaged Vocabulary (β = .25, t= 3.20, p< .002), Executive was limited to Vocabulary and Block Design, and that this limitation suppressed
Function (β = .17, t= 2.43, p< .016), and Tactile Laterality (β = .15, t= 2.31, p< the potential power of the IQ measures. However, IQ estimates derived from
.023), but not averaged Block Design (β = .05, t= .76, p< .44), or Incidental Block Design/Vocabulary subtests typically correlate at .91 with full-scale IQ
Spatial Learning (β = .10, t= .76, p< .44). Agreeableness stepped in scores (Sattler, 1988; Table C-10, p. 820). Furthermore, grade and IQ estimates
significantly in block 2 (β = .21, t= 3.36, p< .02), while Extraversion, were taken concurrently in the present study, while the neuropsychological
Neuroticism, Conscientiousness and Openness stepped out (all β in < .06, t < assessment occurred at the end of the period of time for which academic data
.86, p > .12). Very similar results were obtained when the analysis was repeated, were available. Finally, the IQ assessments occurred multiple times, increasing
using only the significant IQ and neuropsychological predictors from the their reliability, while the neuropsychological assessment only occurred once.
previous analysis (Vocabulary, Verbal Learning, Executive Function, and We have also produced recent evidence that neuropsychological test
Tactile Laterality).¹ performance (restricted, in this case, to tests of Executive Function and Verbal
Learning) also strongly predicts supervisor and self ratings of job performance,
Discussion among managers, administrators and line-workers of a medium sized
We hypothesized that academic achievement would be as strongly associated manufacturing company (Peterson, Higgins, Lee & Pihl, 2001; Higgins,
with neuropsychological test performance as it was with IQ, particularly in the Peterson, Lee & Pihl, submitted a) and are strongly positive associated with
case of tests of executive function and verbal learning. We also hypothesized grades at Harvard University (Peterson, Higgins, Lee & Pihl, 2001; Higgins,
that neuropsychological test scores would account for variance in academic Peterson, Lee & Pihl, submitted c) and the University of Toronto (Higgins,
achievement, over and above that of IQ. Finally, we hypothesized that Big-Five Peterson, Lee & Pihl, submitted b). Perhaps tests derived from the
personality variables, particularly agreeableness (Wentzel, 1993), as well as neuropsychological literature might be usefully applied to the twin problems of
conscientiousness and reversed neuroticism (Hunter & Schmidt, 1996; Barrick predicting and understanding academic and industrial performance, and help
Individual Differences Research, 2003, 1(3) 169 170 Individual Differences Research, 2003, 1(3)
us master the so far elusive task of adding incremental validity to IQ. .0001, with Aggression Category (beta = -.21, t= -3.41, p< .0008), Verbal Learning
Personality testing did not fare so well, at least on its own. One potential (beta= .41, t= 5.81, p< .0001), Averaged Vocabulary (beta= .25, t= 3.51, p< .0006),
reason for this is the lag between grade assessment and personality testing in the Executive Function (beta= .22, t= 3.18, p< .002) and Tactile Laterality (beta= .16, t=
present study. However, traits are supposed to remain constant over time periods 2.74, p< .02) stepping in, and averaged Block Design (beta in= .04, t= .75, p< .46) and
Incidental Spatial Learning stepping out (beta in= .05, t= .74, p< .46). The beta weights
of at least three to seven years, at least in theory (Costa & McCrae, 1992) (with
for executive function were slightly suppressed in the course of these new analyses, but
r's ranging from .51 to .82). Some evidence did emerge, supporting Wentzel's this was because aggression category and executive function were significantly related,
(1993) presuppositions: once variance in IQ and neuropsychological test as reported in Séguin et al. 1995.
performance had been accounted for, with regards to failure-weighted grades,
Agreeableness appeared positively related. Perhaps this is because children with
better interpersonal skills are looked upon favorably by teachers, and given the
benefit of the doubt; perhaps this is because agreeable children actually learn
References
Barrick, M.R. & Mount, M.K. (1991). The Big Five personality dimensions and job
more. performance: a meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44, 1-26.
The present study suffered from a number of methodological limitations, Barrick, M.R. & Mount, M.K. (1993). Autonomy as a moderator of the relationships
despite what it revealed. First, the relationship between neuropsychological between the Big Five personality dimensions and job performance. Journal of Applied
performance and grades was assessed retrospectively, eradicating any possibility Psychology, 78, 111-118.
of drawing a clear causal relationship. Second, assessment of IQ, while Barrick, M.R. & Mount, M.K. (1996). Effects of impression management and
reasonably complete, could have benefitted from inclusion of a highly g-loaded self-deception on the predictive validity of personality constructs. Journal of Applied
test like the Raven's Progressive Matrices (Raven, 1998), as well as additional Psychology, 81, 261-272.
measures of crystallized intelligence. In recent work at the University of Becker, J. T., Butters, N., Rivoira, P. & Miliotis, P. (1986). Asking the right questions:
Toronto, we have done precisely this in the context of a concurrent study, and Problem solving in male alcoholics with Korsakoff's syndrome. Alcoholism: Clinical
the incremental validity of neuropsychological performance over IQ remained and Experimental Research, 10, 641-646.
Brody, N. (1997). Intelligence, schooling and society. American Psychologist, 52,
extant (Higgins et al., submitted b). A prospective longitudinal study would
1046-105.
constitute the best possible design, with neuropsychological performance Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO personality inventory and NEO
assessed prior to academic and other performance measures. We are currently five-factor inventory professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment
undertaking such a study in the industrial domain. Resources.
Damasio, A.R (1994). Descartes' error. New York: Putnam.
Acknowledgment Fleming, G.W.T.H. (1942). Some preliminary remarks on prefrontal leucotomy. Journal
The National Science and Engineering Research Council of Canada, the Canadian of Mental Science, 88, 282-284.
Institutes of Health Research, and the Fonds pour la Formation des Chercheurs et l'Aide Frith, C. D., Friston, K. J., Liddle, P. F. & Frackowiak, R. S. J. (1991). A PET study of
à la Recherche (FCAR) provided scholarships that made this collaborative work word finding. Neuropsychologia, 29, 1137-1148.
possible. The Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada, FCAR, and Gardner, H. (1993). Multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books.
the Conseil Québécois en Recherche Sociale funded the project and Research Unit on Goldberg, L.R. (1992). The development of markers for the big-five factor structure.
Child Psychosocial Maladjustment based at Université de Montréal. Psychological Assessment, 4, 26-42.
Goldman-Rakic, P.S. (1996). Regional and cellular fractionation of working memory.
Notes Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 93, 13473-1348.
¹The original selection criteria for the boys (Séguin et al., 1995) involved their Grassi, J.R. (1950). Abstract behavior following prefrontal lobotomy. Psychiatric
categorization according to low, moderate and high multi-year averaged levels of Quarterly, 24, 74-88.
teacher-rated aggression. The focus of the current paper was the relationship between Hemphill, J.F. (2003). Interpreting the magnitudes of correlation coefficients. American
IQ, neuropsychological function, personality and grades. However, the authors were Psychologist, 58, 78-8.
concerned that selection for aggression may have affected the generalizability of the Higgins, D.M., Peterson, J.B., and Pihl, R.O. (submitted a) Computerized
present results. All analysis were therefore re-run, exactly as presented, except that all neuropsychological and big-5 personality task scores and concurrent corporate
blocks were shifted up one (so block 1 became block 2, etc.) and aggression category was managerial/administrative and line-worker performance.
entered into block 1. The results were essentially unchanged as a consequence of this Higgins, D.M., Peterson, J.B., Lee, A. and Pihl, R.O. (submitted b). Prefrontal cognitive
procedure, although aggression category always entered into the equations significantly. ability, the g factor, and personality in predicting academic performance among
So, for example, the final simple stepwise regression with aggression category, IQ and university undergraduates.
all four neuropsychological factor scores produced R²= .54, r= .74, F(5,142)= 33.44, p< Higgins, D.M., Peterson, J.B., Lee, A. and Pihl, R.O. (submitted c). Prefrontal cognitive
Individual Differences Research, 2003, 1(3) 171 172 Individual Differences Research, 2003, 1(3)
ability and conscientiousness predict academic performance in high-achieving retrieval failures. Neuropsychology, 7, 82-88.
university undergraduates. Raven, J. C. (1998). Raven's Progressive Matrices. San Antonio, Texas: The
Higgins, D.M., Peterson, J.B., Pihl, R.O. & Lee, A.G. (2001). Computerized Psychological Corporation.
neuropsychological and big-5 personality performance and academic achievement Rothstein, M.G., Paunonen, S.V., Rush, J.C. & King, G.A. (1994). Personality and
at Harvard. Paper presented at the American Psychological Society, Toronto, Ontario. cognitive predictors of performance in graduate business school. Journal of
Hunter, J.E. & Schmidt, F. (1996). Intelligence and job performance: Economic and Educational Psychology, 86, 516-53.
social implications. Psychology, Public Policy and Law, 2, 447-472. Salgado, J.F. (1997). The five factor model of personality and job performance in the
Kandel, E. & Freed, D. (1989). Frontal-lobe dysfunction and antisocial behavior: A European Community. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 30-43.
review. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45, 404-413. Sattler, J.M. (1988). Assessment of children. San Diego: Author
King, L.A., McKee, W. & Broyles, S.J. (1996). Creativity and the five-factor model. Séguin, J.R., Pihl, R.O., Harden, P.W., Tremblay, R.E. & Boulerice, B. (1995).
Journal of Research in Personality, 30, 189-203. Cognitive and neuropsychological characteristics of physically aggressive boys.
Lezak, M. D. (1985). Neuropsychological assessment. In J. A. M. Frederiks (Ed.), Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 614-624.
Handbook of clinical neurology: Vol. 1. Clinical neuropsychology (pp. 515-530). Smith, M. L. & Milner, B. (1981). The role of right hippocampus in the recall of spatial
New York: Elsevier. location. Neuropsychologia, 19, 781-793.
Lezak, M. D. (1983). Neuropsychological assessment (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford Smith, M. L. & Milner, B. (1989). Right hippocampal impairment in the recall of spatial
University Press. location: Encoding deficit or rapid forgetting? Neuropsychologia, 27, 71-81.
Luria, A.R., Homskaya, E.D. & Blinkov, S.M. (1967). Impaired selectivity of mental Smith, M., and Milner, B. (1981). The role of right hippocampus in the recall of spatial
processes in association with a lesion of the frontal lobe. Neuropsychologia, 5, location. Neuropsychologia, 19, 781-793.
105-117. Sternberg, R.J., Wagner, R.K., Williams, W.M. & Horvath, J.A. (1995). Testing
McCrae, R.R. & John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor model and its common sense. American Psychologist, 50, 630-641.
applications. Journal of Personality, 60, 175-215. Stuss, D. T. & Benson, D. F. (1986). The frontal lobes. New York: Raven.
McHenry, J.J., Hough, L.M., Toquam, J.L., Hanson, M.A. & Ashworth, S. (1990). Tremblay, R.E., Pihl, R.O., Vitaro, F., Dobkin, P.L. (1994). Predicting early onset of
Project A validity results: The relationship between predictor and criterion domains. male antisocial behavior from preschool behavior. Archives of General Psychiatry,
Personnel Psychology, 43, 335-354. 51, 732-739.
Milner, B. & Petrides, M. (1984). Behavioural effects of frontal-lobe lesions in man. Wechsler, D. (1974). Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children - Revised.
Trends in Neurosciences, 7, 403-407. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Milner, B., Petrides, M. & Smith, L. (1985). Frontal lobes and the temporal organization Wechsler, D. (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale - Revised. New York: Psychological
of memory. Human Neurobiology, 4, 137-142. Corporation.
Moffitt,T. E. & Silva,P. A. (1988). Neuropsychological deficit and self-reported Wentzel, K.R. (1993). Does being good make the grade? Social behavior and academic
delinquency in an unselected birth cohort. Journal of the American Academy of Child competence in middle school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 357-364.
and Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 233-24. Wiegersma, S., van der Scheer, E., and Human, R. (1990). Subjective ordering,
Neuman, G.A. & Wright, J. (1999). Team effectiveness: Beyond skills and cognitive short-term memory, and the frontal lobes. Neuropsychologia, 28, 95-98.
ability. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 376-389. Witelson, S. F. (1976). Sex and the single hemisphere: Specialization of the right
Parks, R. W., Loewenstein, D. A., Dodrill, K. L., Barker,W. W., Yoshii, F., Chang, J. hemisphere for spatial processing. Science, 193, 425-427.
Y., Emran, A., Apicella, A., Sheramata, W. A. & Duara, R. (1988). Cerebral Witelson, S.F. (1974). Hemispheric specialization for linguistic and nonlinguistic tactual
metabolic effects of a verbal fluency test: A PET scan study. Journal of Clinical and perception using a dichotomous stimulation technique. Cortex, 10, 3-17.
Experimental Neuropsychology, 10, 565-575.
Peterson, J.B., Higgins, D.M., Lee, A.G. & Pihl, R.O. (2001). Computerized
neuropsychological and big-5 personality task scores and concurrent corporate Received 01/21/2003; Revision Received 05/12/2003; Accepted 05/12/2003
managerial/administrative performance. Paper presented at the American
Psychological Society, Toronto, Ontario.
Petrides, M. (1990). Nonspatial conditional learning impaired in patients with unilateral
frontal but not unilateral temporal lobe excisions. Neuropsychologia, 28, 137-149.
Petrides, M., Alivisatos, B., Meyer, E., & Evans, A. C. (1993). Functional activation of
the human frontal cortex during the performance of verbal working memory tasks.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 90,
878-882.
Randolph, C., Braun, A. R., Goldberg, T. E. & Chase, T. N. (1993). Semantic fluency
in Alzheimer's, Parkinson's, and Huntington's disease: Dissociation of storage and

View publication stats

You might also like