You are on page 1of 6

Personality and Individual Differences 131 (2018) 51–56

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Big Five personality relationships with general intelligence and specific T


Cattell-Horn-Carroll factors of intelligence

David C. Osmona, , Octavio Santosa, Dmitriy Kazakova, Michelle T. Kassela, Quintino R. Manob,
Ashten Mortha
a
University of Wisconsin – Milwaukee, Department of Psychology, United States
b
University of Cincinnati, Department of Psychology, United States

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Objective: Though relationships among Big Five personality traits and general intelligence have been found
IQ consistently, less is known of how personality traits relate to specific factors of intelligence. The present study
Personality examined relationships between Big Five personality traits on the NEO Personality Inventory-Revised (NEO-PI-R;
Big Five Costa & McCrae, 1992) and g-residualized scores of the seven factors of the Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) model on
IQ factors
the Woodcock-Johnson-III (WJ-III; Mather & Woodcock, 2001).
Method: College students referred to a learning disorders clinic were assessed with a comprehensive neu-
ropsychological battery that involved cognitive, achievement, and personality testing. The sample was culled for
failure on a sensitive effort measure (Word Memory Test; Green, 2003), leaving 140 participants.
Results: Openness (O) accounted for significant variance in all seven WJ-III IQ factors with the O-crystallized
intelligence (Gc) relationship being strongest and all other personality-IQ relationships being small. However,
using residualized scores to remove common intellectual variance showed that Openness related only to Gc while
Extraversion related to both processing speed and Gc.
Conclusions: The role that personality plays in the specific aspects of CHC factors of intellectual test performance
is discussed in the context of learning difficulties in college students.

Substantial relationships between the Big Five factors of personality otherwise be due to range restriction in both the personality and in-
and intelligence have been found (e.g., Ashton, Lee, Vernon, & Jang, telligence measures. Finally, the sample was culled for participants
2000; Bartels et al., 2012; Bates & Shieles, 2003; Holland, Dollinger, providing insufficient effort in testing in order to dampen experimental
Holland, & MacDonald, 1995; McRae & John, 1992; Moutafi, Furnham, error associated with mismeasurement of personality and intelligence.
& Crump, 2003; von Stumm, Chamorro-Premuzic, Quiroga, & Colom, It is still unclear whether personality traits relate just to overall
2009; Wolf & Ackerman, 2005). Although much of this work has fo- intelligence or to factors of intelligence, or to both overall and specific
cused on the relation between personality traits with broad constructs factors of intelligence. As noted by Reeve et al. (2006), various factors
of intelligence, relatively little is known of how personality traits relate of intelligence have shown different relationships with the Big Five
to more specific factors of intelligence. The purpose of the present study traits across studies (e.g., -N: Austin, Hofer, Deary, & Eber, 2000; +E:
was to refine our understanding of personality-IQ relationships by ex- Wolf & Ackerman, 2005; A: Krebs & Sturrup, 1982; –C: Furnham &
amining the Big Five traits of personality alongside the Cattell-Horn- Chamorro-Premuzic, 2005). Nevertheless, it might be argued that the
Carroll (CHC) seven-factor model. Furthermore, the present study ad- most consistent personality-IQ relationships holds for Openness
dressed various criticisms of the extant literature. Specifically, to (Moutafi et al., 2003; Zeidner & Matthews, 2000). Specifically, Ashton
identify the relationship of personality traits to factors of intelligence, et al. (2000) found Openness to correlate better with crystallized in-
those factors must be independent of g, general intelligence factor telligence (Gc) than fluid intelligence (Gf), which is consistent with
(Reeve, Meyer, & Bonaccio, 2006). Additionally, a sample referred for previous studies (e.g., Ackerman & Goff, 1994; Goff & Ackerman,
clinical evaluation of learning difficulties and psychiatric diagnoses was 1992). Similarly, a meta-analysis confirmed the relationship of Open-
used that provided a broader range of intellectual performance to avoid ness to Gc over other factors of intelligence (Ackerman & Heggestad,
attenuated correlations between personality and intelligence that might 1997, although see Reeve et al., 2006 and von Stumm et al., 2009 for a


Corresponding author at: University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee, Department of Psychology, 2441 E. Hartford Ave., Milwaukee, WI 53211, United States.
E-mail address: neuropsy@uwm.edu (D.C. Osmon).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.04.019
Received 14 December 2017; Received in revised form 6 April 2018; Accepted 11 April 2018
0191-8869/ © 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
D.C. Osmon et al. Personality and Individual Differences 131 (2018) 51–56

divergent opinion). Despite the weight of evidence favoring the Open- 1. Methods
ness-Gc relationship, criticisms of existing evidence need to be ad-
dressed. First, a systematic analysis using a CHC model of intelligence is 1.1. Participants
lacking in present literature, and it can therefore not be ruled out that
additional Big Five personality traits may differentially relate to the Participants initially included a total of 240 individuals referred to
various factors of intelligence. Such a model (crystallized intelligence the university Learning Disability Clinic for academic concerns. Sixty-
[Gc], long-term retrieval [Glr], visual-spatial thinking [Gv], auditory two of those participants were excluded for failing the Word Memory
processing [Ga], short-term memory [Gsm], fluid reasoning [Gf], and Test (any of the IR, DR, CNS, MC, PA scales (Green, 2003); see the
processing speed [Gs]) represents the synthesis of a comprehensive following paragraph for details) leaving 178 participants of which 145
account of intelligence and, when measured in a single battery, can had an available primary diagnosis/no diagnosis: ADHD (38), Psy-
provide a more comprehensive study of the personality-intelligence chiatric (28), Cognitive Disorder-NOS (10), Learning Disorder (LD)-
relationship. Second, consistent with Reeve et al. (2006) specific factors NOS (8), Math LD (8), No Warranted Diagnosis (17), Reading LD (35),
of intelligence that are not substantially correlated with g must be ex- Writing LD (1). Thirty-six percent of the sample had co-morbid diag-
amined for their association to personality variables. Only in this way noses and 28 of the participants were found to have only psychiatric
can personality be related to specific intellectual variance apart from diagnoses but were included to fairly represent a sample referred for
overall intelligence. academic problems. The sample had an average age of 26.71
Another confounding variable that may be clouding the in- (SD = 9.66) and were college students who were given a multitude of
telligence-personality relationship is insufficient effort in test perfor- psychoeducational measures for clinical purposes, which included the
mance. Poor effort is a nuisance variable that confounds assessment Woodcock-Johnson cognitive and achievement tests (WJ-III), stand-
literature results (Green, 2007). Thus, ensuring adequate effort in the alone specific reading measures (e.g., Test of Word Reading Efficiency),
participant sample can lessen experimental error, yielding a better ex- neuropsychological measures (e.g., memory, attention, executive
amination of the personality relationship with intelligence. For ex- function tests), broad-based personality (i.e., NEO-PI-R) and college
ample, college student samples show a 5–12% failure rate on sensitive adjustment measures (e.g., Achievement Motivation Profile, Career
measures of effort (Ross et al., 2015; Santos, Kazakov, Reamer, Park, & Decisions Inventory, SACQ, etc.). Because this battery required on
Osmon, 2014; Silk-Eglit et al., 2014). Likewise, poor participant effort average 11.5 h to complete, participants were tested over two different
and/or feigning/malingering have been found to explain sizeable sessions on consecutive days. Participants who were fluent in English
amounts of variance in neuropsychological performance, especially and who completed the majority of measures including the complete
when incentive to falsify performance is present (e.g., to obtain At- WJ-III cognitive and NEO-PI-R were included in the analyses. Five
tention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) medication; participants had incomplete data on the WJ-III cognitive and/or NEO-
Constantinou, Bauer, Ashendorf, Fisher, & McCaffrey, 2005; Green, PI-R, thus all analyses were conducted on the remaining 140 partici-
2007; Green, Rohling, Lees-Haley, & Allen, 2001; Williamson et al., pants.
2004). Furthermore, poor participant effort has been found to operate Sixty-two (26%) of the 240 total participants were removed from
even when no discernable incentive to appear deficient has been found. final analyses due to inadequate effort defined by performance cutoff
For example, An, Zakzanis, and Joordens (2012) found that almost half scores below threshold for any of the five symptom validity scales of the
of college participants in a study of neuropsychological test perfor- Word Memory Test (IR, DR, CNS, MC, PA (Green, 2003)). Fig. 1 shows
mance failed a common performance validity measure. While this the entire distribution of participants on the WJ-III GIA compared to the
finding is probably not a good estimate of college student effort in distribution with participants removed for poor effort. Both distribu-
general (see Ross et al., 2015; Santos et al., 2014; Silk-Eglit et al., tions were fitted adequately by a normal distribution with delta Akai-
2014), it does suggest that under some circumstances effort problems ke's Information Criterion-corrected (AICc) values within < 2 of the
can grossly affect experimental findings. Therefore, well-designed as- best fitting distributions (Johnson Su and Gamma, respectively). Like-
sessment studies must ensure internal validity by at least excluding wise, distributions for the two NEO-PI-R variables were also modeled by
participants for failure on a sensitive measure of performance validity, multiple distributions. Openness fit a normal distribution both before
such as Green's Word Memory Test (WMT) or Medical Symptom Va- and after culling participants. Conscientiousness fit a Normal 2 Mixture
lidity Test (MSVT). distribution according to delta AICc values. Nevertheless, the distribu-
The present study used a sample of participants that excluded for tion was normal looking except for a slight overrepresentation at the
insufficient effort in order to minimize experimental error in evaluating low end of the distribution. Additionally, removing participants for
the relationship between personality and overall intelligence and its poor effort nearly eliminated outliers in the GIA distribution, as evident
various components. Because there may be a differential relationship in the box plot. Since culling participants improved the distribution of
between each personality trait and the different factors of intelligence, the retained sample, further analyses were completed with the retained
the Woodcock-Johnson-III (WJ-III; Mather & Woodcock, 2001) instru- sample. Additionally, parametric statistics appear to be warranted for
ment was used to assess seven CHC factors of intelligence as well as a these distributions, even for Conscientiousness because the statistics are
full-scale score (GIA). While the WJ-III provides an aggregate in- robust in near-normal distributions.
telligence score (GIA) as well as specific intelligence scores (Gc, Gf, Glr,
Gsm, Ga, Gv, Gs; see Materials below for description), each of these 1.2. Materials
specific scores correlate substantially with the aggregate score and,
therefore, do not purely represent specific intelligence factors. In order The NEO Personality Inventory-Revised (NEO-PI-R; Costa &
to mitigate the concern about the overestimation of personality re- McCrae, 1992) is a 240-item questionnaire that measures the Big Five
lationships with specific factors of intelligence (see Reeve et al., 2006, personality traits: Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to Experience,
for a discussion), residualized scores for the CHC factor scores were Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness, each of which is comprised of six
obtained by removing common intelligence variance (GIA) and then facets. NEO-PI-R measures used in the study included composites (mean
were subjected to additional regression analyses for comparison to non- of 50; standard deviation of 10) of the Big Five variables. Neuroticism
residualized CHC factor scores. (N) is the predisposition to experience psychological stress as mani-
fested by anxiety, anger, depression or other negative affect. Ex-
troversion (E) includes sociability, liveliness, and cheerfulness. Open-
ness (O) comprises aesthetic sensitivity, intellectual curiosity, need for
variety, and non-dogmatic attitudes. Agreeableness (A) consists of trust,

52
D.C. Osmon et al. Personality and Individual Differences 131 (2018) 51–56

Fig. 1. WJ-III GIA and NEO-PI-R Openness and Conscientiousness distributions highlighting the participants removed from the sample for failing at least one of the
five symptom validity scales on the Word Memory Test (IR, DR, CNS, MC, PA; excluded participants are displayed in dark color on top row). Bottom row shows the
distributions with only the participants retained in the sample (N = 140), noting that normal distributions in all three cases are adequate fits indistinguishable (AIC-
c < 2) from the best fitting distributions.

altruism, and sympathy. Conscientiousness (C) encompasses a dis- Each participant was tested within the Learning Disability Clinic across
ciplined striving after goals and a strict adherence to principles. Com- two different sessions on two consecutive days totaling about 11 to
posites from these five personality traits were used in the study. 12 h. The participants and their data were treated in accordance with
The Woodcock-Johnson III (WJ-III; Mather & Woodcock, 2001) Institutional Review Board procedures.
Tests of Cognitive Abilities comprise a wide age-range and compre-
hensive system for measuring general and specific intellectual abilities.
1.4. Analyses
WJ-III measures used in the study included standard scores (mean of
100; standard deviation of 15) for the General Ability Index (GAI:
All statistics were completed using JMP Pro 12 (SAS, 2015). Dis-
overall intelligence quotient), Gc (crystallized intelligence composite),
tributional analyses were run to ensure appropriateness of distribu-
Gf (fluid intelligence composite), Glr (long-term retrieval composite),
tional statistics and to determine whether excluding participants on the
Gsm (short-term memory composite), Gv (visual processing composite),
basis of effort measure failure was necessary. Akaike's Information
Ga (auditory processing composite), and Gs (processing speed compo-
Criterion-corrected (AICc) values were used to examine the best fitting
site).
distribution and box plots were employed to detect outliers since these
values can have serious consequences in correlational analyses.
1.3. Procedure Forward regression analyses were conducted to examine how well
the NEO-PI-R predicted the GIA and 7 CHC intelligence factors both
All of the testing was done within the university Learning Disability separately and in combination. A total of 8 hierarchical regressions
Clinic. Prior to the evaluation, informed consent was obtained from were run using the 5 NEO-PI-R variables to predict separately the GIA
each participant subsequent to outlining the purpose, procedure, con- and the 7 intellectual factors, with Openness forced into the equation
fidentiality, possible risks and benefits of the evaluation, and informing first because of its clear high correlation with all the intellectual factors
participants that they could withdraw from the evaluation at any time. relative to the other 4 Big Five variables. The minimum Bayesian

53
D.C. Osmon et al. Personality and Individual Differences 131 (2018) 51–56

Information Criterion (BIC) was used to select the best model fit be- equation and the other four personality traits loading according to
cause of its greater penalty of models with the greater number of probability value (p < .05). Moreover, Conscientiousness, and Agree-
parameters compared to the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC). ableness scores predicted small amounts of variance in addition to that
In order to evaluate the concern that common variance among predicted by Openness in the GIA and Gs scores, respectively (values
factors of intelligence leads to overestimation of the role of personality range from F[1137] = 5–7, p < .03), while Neuroticism predicted
(Reeve et al., 2006), another seven hierarchical regressions were run on additional variance beyond Openness in Gsm (values range from F
the residual scores of each of the seven specific factor IQ scores (Gc, Gf, [1137] = 4, p < .05). Multiple correlations (see Table 1) were positive
Glr, Gsm, Ga, Gv, Gs). Residual specific IQ scores were obtained by indicating that higher intellectual ability was associated with greater
regressing each factor on overall intelligence (GIA). This procedure levels of Openness and Neuroticism, while negative correlations in-
removed common IQ variance, yielding residual scores that reflected dicated that higher intellectual function was associated with lower
specific IQ factor scores. Those residual scores were then predicted by Agreeableness for Gs, and lower Conscientious for GIA. No other NEO-
each of the Big Five personality variables, forcing Openness first into PI-R variables predicted significant variance in any of the IQ variables.
the equation. Table 1 also shows the percent variance accounted for when corrected
All 16 regression analyses used K-fold cross validation procedures to for attenuation due to unreliability of measures (R2~) and range re-
correct for overfitting a model of personality-IQ relationships. K-fold striction (R2’) using values of reliability from Schrank, McGrew, and
cross validation uses subsets of data to validate the model (five subsets Woodcock (2001) and McCrea, Kurtz, Yamagata, and Terracciano
were used in the current analyses), yielding a K-fold R2 value that is the (2011).
average of the K-fold R2 values of the subsets and represents a better Using residualized specific IQ factor scores yielded different results,
estimate of the predictive value of the model. Both the R2adj and the K- reducing the relationship between personality and specific IQ scores.
fold R2 are reported to give a sense of the over-fitting of the non-vali- Openness and extraversion each predicted about 3% of the variance in
dated model. Gc, respectively (F[2134] = 4.87, p < .01, R2adj = 0.05; K-
foldR2 = 0.005) with Openness showing a positive relationship to Gc
2. Results while Extraversion showed a negative relationship to Gc. The only other
significant result was for Gs (F[2139] = 4.95, p < .01). Here, only
2.1. IQ-NEO relationships Extraversion contributed to the prediction (R2adj = 0.05; K-
foldR2 = 0.04), showing a positive relationship to Gs.
Variance was predicted in all of the WJ-III GIA and 7 CHC factors by
the NEO-PI-R Openness score (see Table 1; values range from F 3. Discussion
[1137] = 7–48, p < .001) according to hierarchical regression with
order determined by Openness being forced first into the regression The current results are consistent with the bulk of studies that have

Table 1
Percent variance accounted for in WJ-III variables by NEO-PI and increases in variance accounted for when correlations are corrected for attenuation due to
unreliability of measures (rel-R2) and range restriction (rr-R2).
WJ-III Openness Conscientiousness Extraversion Agreeableness Neuroticism

rr-R2/rel-R2 rr-R2/rel-R2 rr-R2/rel-R2 rr-R2/rel-R2 rr-R2/rel-R2

GIA .26** −.02* – – –


0.37/0.30 0.02/0.03 –/– –/– –/–
Gc .28** 0.01 0.01 – –
0.41/0.34 –/– –/– –/– –/–
Gc-r .03* – −.03* – –
0.04/0.03 –/– −0.04/−0.04 –/– –/–
Glr .13** – – – –
0.18/0.17 –/– –/– –/– –/–
Glr-r – – – – –
–/– –/– –/– –/– –/–
Gv .07** – – – –
0.15/0.11 –/– –/– –/– –/–
Gv-r – – – – –
–/– –/– –/– –/– –/–
Ga .25** 0.01 – – –
0.54/0.33 –/– –/– –/– –/–
Ga-r 0.02 – – – –
0.02/0.03 –/– –/– –/– –/–
Gf .14** – – – –
0.22/0.22 –/– –/– –/– –/–
Gf-r – – – – 0.01
–/– –/– –/– –/– –/–
Gs .04* – – −.04* –
0.05/0.06 –/– –/– −5/−5 –/–
Gs-r 0.02 – .05** – –
0.02/0.02 –/– 0.05/0.06 –/– –/–
Gsm 0.14 – – – 0.03
0.15/0.15 –/– –/– –/– 0.04/0.04
Gsm-r – – – – –
–/– –/– –/– –/– –/–

Note. WJ-III = Woodcock Johnson-3rd edition; GIA = General intellectual ability; Gc = crystallized; Glr = long-term retrieval; Gv = visual; Ga = auditory;
Gf = fluid reasoning; Gs = processing speed; Gsm = short-term memory; xx-r = residual score version; Minus sign means an inverse relationship.* p < .05; **
p < .01; rr-R2 = correlation corrected for range restriction, rel-R2 = correlation corrected for attenuation due to unreliability; – = < 1%.

54
D.C. Osmon et al. Personality and Individual Differences 131 (2018) 51–56

found the strongest personality-IQ correlation between Openness and The current results do not support the past findings that greater
Gc (Ackerman & Heggestad, 1997; Ashton et al., 2000; Goff & intelligence is associated with positive personality traits because one
Ackerman, 1992; McCrae & Costa, 1985). They are also consistent with positive trait, specifically Extraversion, showed a negative relationship
Ackerman and Heggestad (1997) who found school performance and to Gc while another positive trait, specifically Openness, showed a
the knowledge aspect of Gc to be related to Conscientiousness and positive relationship to both GIA and Gc. Likewise, the negative per-
Openness (and especially its cognate: Typical Intellectual Engagement, sonality trait (Neuroticism) did not relate to any residualized specific
or amount of intellectual effort “typically” proffered in a work situa- intellectual factor or to GIA.
tion). The current results replicated the relationship between Openness Limitations of the present results relate largely to the particular
and various intellectual factors but did not show Conscientiousness to sample used and to issues associated with the Openness construct, as
be related to any of the CHC seven factors. Past work along with the noted in three following points. First, the present results are general-
current results reinforces the notion that Conscientiousness relates to izable only to college students seeking evaluation for learning difficulty,
academic and general life success irrespective of intellectual ability although meta-analysis results have provided preliminary support for
(Ackerman & Goff, 1994; Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2008; Noftle the O-Gc relationship being more widespread across the population at
& Robins, 2007; Paunonen & Ashton, 2001). Below is a brief discussion large and across a greater age range (Ackerman & Heggestad, 1997).
of results. The relatively large number of participants and the large effect sizes for
While the current results were generally consistent with past re- the relationship of Openness with GIA suggest that these results are
search, the fact that we did not find Neuroticism to be strongly asso- robust, as does the generally consistent findings with past literature.
ciated with any CHC factors (except the 3% variance explained in Gsm) However, the smaller relationships of personality to residualized in-
stands in contrast to prior results. For example, Ackerman and tellectual factors suggest that personality has a limited effect in specific
Heggestad (1997) noted a consistent finding that Neuroticism was ne- intellectual factors. Additionally, the O-Gc finding is likely to be un-
gatively correlated with intelligence while a positive relationship to reliable, although the E-Gs result may be more robust in cross valida-
intelligence was found in the present results. This discrepancy in the tion. Future studies with a larger sample size are desirable and using
direction of the Neuroticism correlation may be related to measurement residualized scores for intellectual factors will be important in testing
issues. That is, Neuroticism as measured by the NEO-PI-R may involve how robust the current findings are in cross-validation. Second, some
fewer psychopathological aspects and be more related to stress man- have criticized the Openness trait on theoretical grounds arguing that it
agement/tolerance. As a result, Neuroticism, as measured in a more is “the most debatable of the five dimensions” that lacks cohesiveness
psychopathological fashion, might carry the negative trait relationship and replicability with at least three different interpretations of “Cul-
with intelligence (à la Moutafi et al., 2003) while Neuroticism as ture…Openness…and Intellect” (Digman, 1990, pp. 433). Third, accu-
measured by the NEO-PI-R might carry the positive trait relationship to rate relationships between two constructs that are measured with dif-
intelligence. fering methodologies (i.e., questionnaire and ability tests) may be
Methodological issues have been noted to cloud personality-IQ re- called into question. For example, as noted by one anonymous re-
lationships (Reeve et al., 2006; von Stumm et al., 2009), especially viewer, self-estimates of intelligence may show stronger correlations
those that relate to confounding common and specific intellectual with some personality questionnaire-based constructs than actual
variances. These concerns were addressed in current results by using measured intelligence.
the WJ-III residual scores of the specific factors (Gc, Gf, Glr, Gsm, Ga, Despite these limitations, the present results appear to offer robust
Gv, Gs), derived by removing common intelligence variance (GIA). findings regarding relatively strong relationships between personality
Doing so supported the concerns of Reeve et al. (2006) that common and general intellectual power. It is unclear why the present results
intellectual variance both confounded and inflated the relationship yielded higher correlations than did previous studies (r ~ 0.4–0.5 vs.
between personality and specific factors of intelligence. The current r ~ 0.3–0.4 in other studies), although excluding participants for subpar
results indicate that personality scores (specifically Openness and effort, using an IQ measure built upon the theoretical foundation of
Conscientiousness) accounted for about 28% of the variance in overall CHC theory, and using a population of participants with impairments
intelligence. When general intellectual variance was removed by using that increase variance in cognitive functioning may help explain the
residualized intellectual factor scores, personality relationships were stronger results. Such strong relationships need greater consideration in
much reduced and smaller, accounting for only 2–3% of variance. clinical work with college students with learning difficulties because
Specifically, only residualized Openness and residualized Extraversion personality traits may be protective for commonly lower intellectual
accounted for significant variance in Gc and only residualized Extra- ability. There are also implications of the present results for intellectual
version related to Gs. No other residualized factor scores had relation- and neuropsychological assessment in general. The consistency of per-
ships to personality. sonality-IQ relationships in the literature and validation of this litera-
The present results clarify some inconsistencies in the literature by ture in the current results requires clinicians to consider the importance
suggesting that Openness is mainly related to general intellectual power of openness when interpreting the meaning of intellectual performance.
(g), and that some specific cognitive abilities are weakly related to In clinical psychology, there is a tendency to consider personality and
Extraversion, Conscientiousness, and Neuroticism. Specifically, being intelligence as independent constructs but that is incommensurate with
more open to experience does relate to the knowledge acquisition as- the 25–30% of shared variance in intellectual performance and the
pects of intelligence, consistent with the speculation that Openness openness and conscientiousness personality traits.
relates to “interests in knowledge” or Gc (e.g., Ackerman & Heggestad, Finally, the present findings warrant exploration of the underlying
1997; Bates & Shieles, 2003; Cattell, 1943; Chamorro-Premuzic & cause of the correlation of Openness with general intellectual power,
Furnham, 2006; Moutafi, Furnham, & Paltiel, 2005). Interestingly, the and the small but apparently robust relationship between Extraversion
current results also suggest that being less Extraverted is associated and mental speed. While it is difficult to envision a true experimental
with Gc independent of general intellectual power. Unfortunately, the paradigm for examining causative relationships between personality
Gc results may not be reliable given the low K-fold R2 value compared and intelligence, it is conceivable that “openness” induction procedures
to the R2adj value. This weak result was surprising given the balance of could be used to investigate intellectual performance. For example,
results in the literature suggesting the strength of the Openness-Gc re- viewing films of open versus closed individual relations preceding
lationship. Finally, the current results suggest that being more Extra- problem-solving tasks may be one way to attempt to induce openness
verted is associated with greater mental speed independent of general attitudes and assess subsequent changes in intellectual performance.
intellectual power. Notably, this result may be more reliable given the Additionally, more quasi-experimental paradigms of the sort used by
small drop off in K-fold R2 value compared to the R2adj value. Gregory, Nettelbeck, and Wilson (2010) would also be useful, although

55
D.C. Osmon et al. Personality and Individual Differences 131 (2018) 51–56

using less “Gc-saturated” participants may be a better methodology for Gregory, T., Nettelbeck, T., & Wilson, C. (2010). Openness to experience, intelligence, and
this purpose. successful ageing. Personality and Individual Differences, 48(8), 895–899.
Holland, D. C., Dollinger, S. J., Holland, C. J., & MacDonald, D. A. (1995). The re-
lationship between psychometric intelligence and the five-factor model of personality
References in a rehabilitation sample. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 79–88.
Krebs, D., & Sturrup, B. (1982). Role-taking ability and altruistic behaviour in elementary
school children. Journal of Moral Education, 11(2), 94.
Ackerman, P. L., & Goff, M. (1994). Typical intellectual engagement and personality:
Mather, N., & Woodcock, R. W. (2001). Examiner's manual: Woodcock-Johnson III tests of
Reply to Rocklin (1994). Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 150–153.
cognitive abilities. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
Ackerman, P. L., & Heggestad, E. D. (1997). Intelligence, personality, and interests:
McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1985). Updating Norman's “adequate taxonomy”:
Evidence for overlapping traits. Psychological Bulletin, 121(2), 219–245.
Intelligence and personality dimensions in natural languages and in questionnaires.
An, K. Y., Zakzanis, K. K., & Joordens, S. (2012). Conducting research with non-clinical
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 710–721.
healthy undergraduates: Does effort play a role in neuropsychological test perfor-
McCrea, R. R., Kurtz, J. E., Yamagata, S., & Terracciano, A. (2011). Internal consistency,
mance? Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 27(8), 849–857.
retest reliability, and their implications for personality scale validation. Personality
Ashton, M. C., Lee, K., Vernon, P. A., & Jang, K. L. (2000). Fluid intelligence, crystallized
and Social Psychology Review, 15, 28–50.
intelligence, and the openness/intellect factor. Journal of Research in Personality, 34,
McRae, R. R., & John, O. P. (1992). An introduction to the five-factor model and its
198–207.
applications. Journal of Personality, 60(2), 175–215.
Austin, E. J., Hofer, S. M., Deary, I. J., & Eber, H. W. (2000). Interactions between in-
Moutafi, J., Furnham, A., & Crump, J. (2003). Demographic and personality predictors of
telligence and personality: Results from two large samples. Personality and Individual
intelligence: A study using the NEO-personality inventory and the Myers–Briggs type
Differences, 29(3), 405–427.
indicator. European Journal of Personality, 17, 79–94.
Bartels, M., van Weegen, F. I., van Beijsterveldt, C. E. M., Carlier, M., Polderman, T. J. C.,
Moutafi, J., Furnham, A., & Paltiel, L. (2005). Can personality factors predict intelligence?
Hoekstra, R. A., & Boomsma, D. I. (2012). The five factor model of personality and
Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 1021–1033.
intelligence: A twin study on the relationship between the two constructs. Personality
Noftle, E. E., & Robins, R. W. (2007). Personality predictors of academic outcomes: Big
and Individual Differences, 53, 368–373.
Five correlates of GPA and SAT scores. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93,
Bates, T. C., & Shieles, A. (2003). Crystallized intelligence as a product of speed and drive
116–130.
for experience: The relationship of inspection time and openness to g and Gc.
Paunonen, S. V., & Ashton, M. C. (2001). Big Five predictors of academic achievement.
Intelligence, 31, 275–287.
Journal of Research in Personality, 35, 78–90.
Cattell, R. B. (1943). The description of personality: Basic traits resolved into clusters.
Reeve, C. L., Meyer, R. D., & Bonaccio, S. (2006). Intelligence–personality associations
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 38(4), 476–506.
reconsidered: The importance of distinguishing between general and narrow di-
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2006). Intellectual competence and the in-
mensions of the intelligence. Intelligence, 34, 387–402. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
telligent personality: A third way in differential psychology. Review of General
intell.2005.11.001.
Psychology, 10, 251−267.
Ross, T. P., Poston, A. M., Rein, P. A., Salvatore, A. N., Wills, N. L., & York, T. M. (2015).
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2008). Personality, intelligence, and approaches
Performance invalidity base rates among healthy undergraduate research partici-
to learning as predictors of academic performance. Personality and Individual
pants. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 31, 97–104.
Differences, 44(7), 1596–1603.
Santos, O. A., Kazakov, D., Reamer, M. K., Park, S. E., & Osmon, D. C. (2014). Effort in
Constantinou, M., Bauer, L., Ashendorf, L., Fisher, J., & McCaffrey, R. J. (2005). Is poor
college undergraduates is sufficient on the Word Memory Test. Archives of Clinical
performance on recognition memory effort measures indicative of generalized poor
Neuropsychology, 29(7), 609–613.
performance on neuropsychological tasks? Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20(2),
SAS (2015). JMP 12 basic analysis. Cary, NC: SAS Institute, Inc.
191–198.
Schrank, F. A., McGrew, K. S., & Woodcock, R. W. (2001). Technical abstract (Woodcock-
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Professional manual: Revised NEO personality inventory
Johnson III Assessment Service Bulletin No. 2). Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
and NEO five-factor inventory. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
Silk-Eglit, G. M., Stenclik, J. H., Gavett, B. E., Adams, J. W., Lynch, J. K., & McCaffrey, R.
Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model. Annual
J. (2014). Base rate of performance invalidity among non-clinical undergraduate
Review of Psychology, 41, 417–440.
research participants. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 29(5), 415–421.
Furnham, A., & Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2005). Personality and intelligence: Gender, the
von Stumm, S., Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Quiroga, M. A., & Colom, R. (2009). Separating
big five, self-estimated and psychometric intelligence. International Journal of
narrow and general variances in intelligence-personality associations. Personality and
Selection and Assessment, 13(1), 11–24.
Individual Differences, 47, 336–341.
Goff, M., & Ackerman, P. L. (1992). Personality–intelligence relations: Assessment of
Williamson, D. J., Drane, D. L., Stroup, E. S., Miller, J. W., Holmes, M. D., & Wilensky, A.
typical intellectual engagement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 537–553.
J. (2004). Detecting cognitive differences between patients with epilepsy and patients
Green, P. (2003). Word memory test for Windows: User's manual. Edmonton, Alberta:
with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: Effort matters. Epilepsia, 45(Suppl. 7), 179.
Green's Publishing.
Wolf, M. B., & Ackerman, P. L. (2005). Extraversion and intelligence: A meta-analytic
Green, P. (2007). The pervasive influence of effort on neuropsychological tests. Physical
investigation. Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 531–542.
Medicine and Rehabilitation Clinics of North America, 18(1), 43–68.
Zeidner, M., & Matthews, G. (2000). Intelligence and personality. In R. Sternberg (Ed.).
Green, P., Rohling, M., Lees-Haley, P., & Allen, M. (2001). Effort has a greater effect on
Handbook of intelligence (pp. 581–610). New York: Cambridge University Press.
test scores than severe brain injury in compensation claimants. Brain Injury, 15(12),
1045–1060.

56

You might also like