You are on page 1of 8

Developmental Psychology Copyright 1991 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1991. Vol. 27, No. 3, 432-439 0012-1649/91/$3.00

Infant Cardiac Activity:


Developmental Changes and Relations With Attachment
Carroll E. Izard Stephen W Porges
University of Delaware University of Maryland

Robert E Simons, Q Maurice Haynes, Ben Cohen


Christopher Hyde, and Maria Parisi State University of New York, Buffalo
University of Delaware

In this study the stability over thefirst13 months of life of measures of infant cardiac activity (heart
period and heart-period variability), their relations with each other, and their relations with a
continuous-variable index of infant-mother attachment were investigated. The indexes of cardiac
activity changed in an orderly way with development (increasing heart-rate variability, decreasing
heart rate). There were moderate to high intercorrelations among the cardiac measures, particu-
larly those indexing heart-rate variability (i.e., vagal tone, heart-period variance, and heart-period
range). Regression analyses showed that the measures of heart-rate variability at 3,6, and 9 months
were significant predictors of the continuous-variable index of security. The higher the infants'
heart-rate variability, the higher were their attachment insecurity scores. Analyses of whether the
conventional secure/insecure classification was related to the early infant cardiac measures indi-
cated that measures of heart-rate variability were significantly higher in the insecure children.

Recently, researchers have investigated the relation between har, Waters, & Wall, 1978), may be a manifestation of individ-
attachment classification and measures of temperament ual differences in physiological reactivity.
(Belsky & Rovine, 1987; Bretherton, CConnell, &Tracey, 1980; For the most part, investigations of physiological reactivity
Miyake, Chen, & Campos, 1985). This research has demon- have involved base-level measures of autonomic activity, partic-
strated a relation between concurrent measures of tempera- ularly heart-rate patterns (i.e., heart rate, heart-rate variability,
ment and attachment and suggests that temperament, assessed and cardiac vagal tone). Such studies assume that baseline mea-
during early infancy, may be a reliable predictor of attachment sures of heart-rate patterns convey information regarding stable
classification. For example, Miyake et al. (1985) reported that neurophysiological mechanisms potentiating autonomic and
infant irritability measured at 3 months of age was predictive of behavioral reactivity. That is, it is assumed that the heart-rate
patterns of resistant behavior during reunion in the strange patterns reflect homeostatic mechanisms mediated by brain-
situation at 1 year. These findings suggest that temperament stem structures. It is also assumed that basal patterns of cardiac
may contribute significantly to the prediction of the quality of activity index the organism's potential to react to stimulation by
attachment. transitory inhibition of homeostatic processes and to self-regu-
Neurophysiological processes may underlie the construct of late by the reinstatement of homeostasis.
temperament and mediate aspects of the relation between tem- The specific central structures mediating both the auto-
perament and attachment. Contemporary theoretical perspec- nomic and behavioral dimensions of temperament have been
tives have conceptualized temperament as a psychobiological debated. For example, Kagan, Reznick, and Snidman (1987)
construct (e.g., Goldsmith & Campos, 1982; Rothbart & Derry- argued that heart-rate patterns reflect both sympathetic and
berry, 1981; Thomas & Chess, 1977). For example, Rothbart parasympathetic activity and that the relative contribution of
and Derryberry defined temperament in terms of constitu- each can be detected through spectral analysis. In contrast,
tional differences in physiological reactivity and self-regulation Porges (in press) maintained that with current technology,
of reactivity. Thus, children's approach and avoidant behaviors, heart-rate patterns can be effective only in providing an index
observed in the strange-situation procedure (Ainsworth, Ble- of parasympathetic tone through the vagus. This view is sup-
ported by Saul, Rea, Eckberg, Berger, and Cohen (1990), who
demonstrated that heart-rate oscillations do not provide an ac-
curate measure of mean cardiac sympathetic activity.
This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Despite the different theoretical underpinnings of the re-
Grant MH42O5OO3 to Carroll E. Izard and HD-22628 (National Insti-
search programs, both Kagan and Porges rely heavily on beat-
tute of Child Health and Human Development) to Stephen W Porges.
Special thanks to Sharon L. Antonio, Michelle Claypool, Scott Lem- to-beat variability in heart rate to mark individual differences
pert, John Brown, Marion C. Hyson, Gail Chisholm, Teri L. Brown, in temperament. Both would agree that heart-rate patterns are a
and Shannon Voirol for their assistance with this project. complicated and often idiosyncratic time series, with the beat-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to to-beat pattern continuously influenced by the changing neural
Carroll E. Izard, University of Delaware, Department of Psychology, influence from the brainstem to the heart.
220 Wolf Hall, Newark, Delaware 19716. There are two basic approaches to quantification of heart
432
INFANT CARDIAC ACTIVITY 433

rate variability: (a) the use of descriptive statistics (i.e,, mean, Method
standard deviation, etc.), and (b) the modeling of the heart-rate Subjects
pattern to extract a component determined by a known physio-
logical mechanism (e.g., respiratory sinus arrhythmia). Exam- The infants were from White, middle-class families. Their mothers
ples of the descriptive approach would include Kagan's use of were recruited from birth announcements in local newspapers to par-
heart-period mean and variability (Kagan et al., 1987) and the ticipate in a longitudinal project on emotional development. Infants
involved in this study were seen periodically from 2.5 to 63 months of
attempts of Yen, Forsythe, and Hon (1973) to partition heart- age. Heart-rate data from this study were obtained during the 3-, 4.5-,
rate variability into two distinct components that emphasize 6-, 9-, and 13-month visits, and the attachment classification was ob-
short-term (fast) and long-term (slow) variability. The short- tained during the 13-month visit. Eighty-eight mother-infant dyads
term variability is generally calculated as a standard deviation participated through the first year of the project, but because of the
or the mean absolute difference between sequential heart beats difficulty in obtaining psychophysiological data free from movement
and is assumed to be associated with vagal influences. Long- and recording artifact, the number of subjects represented at each age
term variability is generally calculated as the maximum heart were different.
rate minus minimum heart rate (i.e., range) and is assumed to
reflect the balance between sympathetic and vagal influences. Procedure
To a limited degree, short-term variability has been demon-
During the 3-, 4.5-, 6-, and 9-month visits, the session began by plac-
strated to be related to vagal influences, but long-term variabil- ing the recording electrodes on the infant and then escorting both
ity has not been empirically demonstrated to be related to spe- mother and infant into the experimental room for 4 min of heart-rate
cific sympathetic influences or to the vagus. recording. During these 4 min, the mother was asked to sit down and
Porges (1985), on the other hand, has chosen to model the hold the infant as quietly as possible. After the recording was com-
heart-rate pattern to extract an accurate measure of the ampli- pleted , the electrodes were detached and the mothers were prepared for
tude of respiratory sinus arrhythmia as an index of cardiac subsequent mother-infant interaction procedures in which infant emo-
tion-expression data were obtained (those data are not part of this
vagal tone (V). This method deals with the nonstationary quali-
report). During the 13-month visit, the heart-rate recording occurred
ties of the baseline heart-rate pattern (a violation of an assump- after the strange-situation procedure to preserve the integrity of the
tion necessary for spectral analyses and time-domain niters), standardized strange-situation assessment.
the effect of the duration of data sampled, and the interactive The strange situation. When infants were 13 months of age, the
effects of sympathetic, parasympathetic, and endocrine sys- Ainsworth strange-situation procedure (Ainsworth et al., 1978) was
tems on the heart. This method has been validated in numerous used to provide a measure of quality of attachment. This standardized
studies across different laboratories (Billman & Dujardin, in procedure is composed of eight 3-minute episodes designed to assess
press; Dellinger, Taylor, & Porges, 1987; Jansen & Dellinger, the quality of the child's attachment to the mother. The "strangers"
1988) and has been demonstrated to be associated with the involved in this procedure were all White, female undergraduate re-
dimensions of temperament: reactivity, self-regulation, and ex- search assistants. They had been specially trained in using standard-
pressivity (Field, Healy, Goldstein, Perry, & Bendell, 1988; Fox, ized instructions.
1989; Porter, Porges, & Marshall, 1988). The play area for the strange situation was a 2.7 X 2.7 m carpeted
section of a room, with (standardized) toys in the middle of the play
Although temperament dimensions have recently been area. This area was separated from the rest of the room by one Plexi-
linked to both neurophysiological mechanisms and attachment glas barrier 65 cm high (with a door for mother's entrance and exit) and
behaviors, the relations between physiological response pat- two wooden barriers, each 125 cm high. A window-sized two-way
terns and attachment has not been directly investigated. There- mirror in one wall enabled videotaping of the session. Mother sat in a
fore, this study was designed to examine the stability of four chair toward the left side of the play area, while the stranger entered
indexes of infants' cardiac activity (heart period, X heart-pe- and sat on the right side.
riod variance, and heart-period range) and to explore relations The strange-situation procedure was videotaped by two Panasonic
among individual differences in these cardiac activity patterns, WV-34R black/white cameras; one recorded a wide-angle view of the
assessed longitudinally (at 3,4,5,6, and 9 months), and attach- entire play area and the other recorded close-ups of the child's face. A
digital counter, measuring time in tenths of a second, facilitated video
ment behavior assessed in the Ainsworth strange situation
synchronization as well as precise coding of other behaviors. A micro-
(Ainsworth et al., 1978) at 13 months. phone attached to the ceiling above the play area recorded vocaliza-
Of special interest in our study is the developmental course of tions from child, mother, and stranger.
the relations between heart-rate patterns and attachment. Attachment classification. The child's behavior during the strange
Stifter, Fox, and Porges (1989), for example, have reported that situation was coded using the standardized method developed by
an early (5 months) measure of V was more strongly related to Ainsworth et al. (1978). A judgment was then made based on the num-
expressivity than a later measure of V (10 months). In a similar ber and intensity of certain behaviors (such as proximity- and contact-
manner, as mentioned earlier, Miyake et al. (1985) found that seeking behavior, contact-maintaining behavior, resistant behavior,
infant irritability measured at 3 months was predictive of resis- avoidant behavior, search behavior, and distance interactions) as to
whether the child demonstrated a secure (B) or insecure attachment (A
tant reunion patterns in the strange situation at 1 year. Given
or C) to the mother.1 A secure child (B) responds positively to the
the strong association of V with both irritability/reactivity (Co-
hen, 1988; Healy, 1989; Porges, in press) and expressivity (Stifter
et al., 1989), our study is particularly concerned with early in- 1
Our primary coder was trained by Dante Cicchetti during a sum-
fancy (3- to 4.5-month) measures of cardiac function and subse- mer at Harvard University. Our other coders were trained by the pri-
quent level of attachment security, although relations between mary coder, and they also obtained reliability with precoded tapes
later (6-, 9-, and 13-month) heart-rate measurements and attach- supplied by Everett Waters of State University of New York at Stoney-
ment will also be assessed. brook.
434 IZARD, PORGES, SIMONS, HAYNES, HYDE, PARISI, COHEN

mother, seeking and maintaining contact with her, as well as receiving Results
comfort at reunion. Type B children exhibit little or no resistant behav-
ior and are able to use their mothers as a 'secure base' from which to Cardiac Activity: Developmental Trends
explore the environment. Insecure/avoidant children (A) show little or
Table 1 contains data for the four cardiac measures (X heart-
no tendency to seek proximity or contact with mothers, even during
reunion episodes. Type A children may ignore mothers or exhibit more period [HP] variance, HP range, and mean HP) recorded dur-
salient avoidant behaviors such as turning away, looking away, or with- ing thefivelaboratory visits (at 3,4.5,6,9, and 13 months). The
drawing from mothers. Type A children typically demonstrate few first column of means and standard deviations is for all subjects
search behaviors and little or no distress at separation. Insecure/resis- with cardiac data at each age. The second column of means and
tant children (C) tend to display angry resistant behaviors toward their standard deviations is for the 25 subjects whose cardiac data
mothers. Type C children demonstrate limited exploration of their were intact for all five visits. As the table indicates, the data
surroundings, and they are difficult to soothe after separation. Sixteen from the subset of 25 subjects with complete data are quite
infants were classified independently by two coders. They agreed on 14 similar to the data set for the entire sample.
of 16 (88%) of the cases. To assess statistically the changes in cardiac activity that oc-
Heart-rate recording and quantification. To obtain the heart-rate cur with development and not dilute the potential influence of
measures, an electrocardiogram was recorded by placing three Med stable individual differences on the developmental trends, the
Associates 4 mm Ag-AgCl disposable electrodes on the infant's chest
data for each cardiac index from the 25 subjects with complete
(left lowerribcage, right lower rib cage, and under the left clavicle). The
EKG was amplified with a Grass Model 7D Polygraph via a 7P4 cardiac data were subjected to a repeated-measures multivar-
preamplifier. Polygraphic output was continuously monitored iate analysis of variance (MANOVA) to explore changes in car-
throughout the 4 min to detect movement artifact. The EKG was diac activity as a function of age. The effect of age was signifi-
stored on analog tape on a Vetter Model C-4 FM tape recorder for cant for all four cardiac measures. V:F(4,96) = 12.86,p<.001;
subsequent off-line analysis on a PDP11/23 laboratory computer. The HP variance: F(4, 96) = 8.88, p < .001; HP range: F{4, 96) =
computer (a) detected the peak of the R-wave for each cardiac cycle and 11.33, p < .001; and mean HP: F(4, 96) = 22.25, p < .001, all
(b) timed the interval (heart period) between successive heart beats to increased from the 3-month to the 13-month visit. Age-related
the nearest msec. The data file of successive heart periods for each increases in the first three indexes reflect developmental in-
subject was manually edited for artifact and analyzed for mean heart creases in heart-rate variability; the increase in mean HP indi-
period, Y heart-period variance, and heart-period range using soft- cates an age-related slowing of heart rate. For the first three
ware based on the Porges algorithms (Delta-Biometrics, Bethesda).
measures, there was no significant change between 3 and 4.5
The procedures for estimating V (Porges, 1985) consist of the follow- months, yet the data recorded at each subsequent age were sig-
ing steps: (a) The heart periods were converted into time-based data by nificantly different from the average of all previous ages (reverse
sampling during successive 250 msec intervals; (b) the time-based data
Helmert contrasts; Finn, 1974, pp. 228ff). The effect of age on
were detrended with a 21-point moving cubic polynomial to remove
the influence of non-stationarities in the trend and slow periodicities mean HP was associated with contrasts revealing no significant
on the amplitude of respiratory sinus arrhythmia; (c) the detrended differences between the 3- and 4.5-month visits, followed by age
data were processed by a digital band-pass filter to remove sources of differences indicating significant heart-rate slowing through
variance outside the frequency band characteristic of spontaneous the 13-month visit.
breathing for the infant (.24 to 1.04 Hz or approximately 15 to 60
breaths/min); and (d) the natural logarithm of the band-passed vari-
ance was calculated and used as the measure of cardiac vagal tone or V Stability of the Cardiac Indexes
Heart-period variance was calculated as the natural logarithm of the Table 2 shows the intra-index stability (the correlation of each
variance of all heart periods within each 30-sec epoch. Heart-period
cardiac index with itself) for each combination of age points. As
range was calculated as the maximum heart period minus the mini-
mum heart period within each 30-sec epoch. the data indicate, early measures of V (3 and 4.5 months) were
related to all subsequent measures of V Five of the cross-age
To increase the statistical stability of V and the other cardiac mea-
correlations for V were significant, whereas only two were signif-
sures (heart period, heart-period range, and the natural logarithm of
heart-period variance), values were calculated for sequential 30-sec icant for HP variance, one for mean HP, and none for HP range.
epochs, and the mean of these epochs within the 4-min baseline condi-
tion were used in the statistical analyses. Heart-period variance and Relations Among Cardiac Indexes
range measures are greatly influenced by the duration of data being
analyzed, and when calculated over longer epochs, they are influenced Table 3 shows the inter-index correlations among the cardiac
by slower heart-rate trends. Thus, the calculation of variance or range indexes for all subjects who had complete cardiac data at 3 and
over 30 sec is usually different than over a 4-min period. The partition- 4.5 months of age and the corresponding matrix for ages 6 and 9
ing of the 4-min data collection period into 30-sec epochs functions to months. As the table indicates, there were high inter-index
filter some of the slow heart-rate activity that may contribute to vari- correlations among the three indexes of heart-rate variability (Y
ance and range. Therefore, our measures of range and heart-period
HP variance, and HP range) within each age level.
variance may not be generalizable to other reports in which these mea-
sures were calculated over longer intervals. Moreover, because the
slower heart-rate activity is not clearly vagal and reflects interactions Cardiac Activity and Attachment Security
with the sympathetic nervous system, its removal reduces the non-
vagal influences on these measures. Thus, one would expect vagal Of the 54 children with heart-rate measures at 3 and 4.5
influences to be the primary contribution to all our measures of heart- months (early infancy) and an attachment classification at 13
rate variability. Correlations of the heart-period variance and range months, 40 children were securely attached (B) and 14 children
measures with the vagal tone index will enable the evaluation of the were insecurely attached: 8 insecure/avoidant (A) and 6 inse-
vagal contribution to these measures. cure/resistant (C). To obtain a continuous index of the level of
INFANT CARDIAC ACTIVITY 435
Table 1 with a significant F change (p < .021) was V at 3 months. With
Means and Standard Deviations on All Subjects at Each Age the probability for eliminating a variable set at. 16, HP at 4.5
and on 25 Subjects with Cardiac Data at All Ages months was retained in thefinalequation, p< .15. Both V and
HP were positively correlated with attachment insecurity
N = 25 scores.
Cardiac Age A similar regression analysis for the cardiac indexes at 6 and
measure (in months) N M SO M SD
9 months yielded R = .51, F(3,32) = 3.69, p< .022. HP variance
Vagal tone 3.0 86 2.53 .80 2.51 .72 at 6 and 9 months and heart-periodrangeat 6 months produced
4.5 83 2.76 .81 2.67 .50 significant F changes. HP variance and range were positively
6.0 58 2.96 .93 3.07 .99
9.0 54 3.49 .81 3.51 .85 correlated with level of attachment insecurity.
13.0 47 3.61 .76 3.62 .73 A stepwise regression with backward selection was used to
HP variance 3.0 86 5.19 .65 5.24 .63 test the concomitant relation between the four 13-month in-
4.5 83 5.22 .72 5.21 .46 dexes of cardiac activity and the 13-month attachment security
6.0 58 5.56 .91 5.74 1.00 (discriminant function) scores yielded R = .30, F(i, 44) = 4.39,
9.0 54 5.87 .54 5.90 .53
13.0 47 5.98 .60 5.98 .52 p < .042. HP variance produced the only significant F change.
HP range 3.0 86 62.29 18.38 64.96 18.25 MANOVAs were used to examine the relations of the conven-
4.5 83 65.02 21.23 64.00 15.79 tional qualitative categories of secure and insecure attachment
6.0 58 72.23 20.68 73.93 19.08 (independent variable) to the cardiac measures (dependent vari-
9.0 54 84.47 19.11 84.78 19.05
13.0 47 87.00 18.95 88.22 17.68 able). In the MANOVA for the 3 and 4.5 month data, the multi-
Mean HP 3.0 86 392.83 29.01 393.84 22.00 variate effect approached significance, Wilks's X = .757, F(S,
4.5 83 395.86 25.22 397.46 20.92 45) = 1.81, p < . 10. The univariate FsQ, 52) for V at 3 and 4.5
6.0 58 408.09 24.89 411.10 24.47 months, and HP variance at 3 months were significant, ps <
9.0 54 423.63 23.82 423.98 21.73 .036, .004, and .030, respectively. The insecurely attached group
13.0 47 446.76 34.93 449.91 39.53
had higher means on V and HP variance than did the securely
Note. HP = heart period. attached group. The results of the MANOVA for the cardiac
indexes at 6 and 9 months were nonsignificant. The MANOVA
for the 13-month cardiac indexes revealed a nonsignificant
attachment security, all the attachment classification data from multivariate effect. For HP variance there was a significant uni-
the parent project (N = 81; Bs = 59, As = 12, Cs = 10) were used variate effect, F(\, 44) = 4.51, p < .039. However, all the univar-
in deriving discriminant function scores for each infant. For iate analyses for the secure/insecure groups must be treated
this purpose, the insecure subgroups (A and C) were combined. with caution because the multivariate effects were not clearly
Following a scheme described by Richters, Waters, and Vaughn significant.
(1988), we used ratings on selected interactive behaviors (prox- MANOVAs were also run to evaluate possible differential
imity seeking, contact maintaining, contact resistance, and relations of attachment types A (insecure/avoidant) and C (inse-
proximity avoidance) and frequency of crying in Reunion Epi- cure ambivalent/resistant) to cardiac functioning.2 The results
sodes 5 and 8 as independent variables in a discriminant func- were generally similar to those just described for the secure/in-
tion analysis to distinguish secure and insecure infants (as clas- secure dichotomy, except in the case of the data at 3 and 4.5
sified by the coders). We obtained a single discriminant func- months, where there was a significant multivariate effect,
tion, \ = .393, F(9, 71) = 12.17, p < .0001, that correctly Wilks's A = 1.96, F{\6, 90) = 1.96, p < .025, and a significant
identified 95% (56 of 59) of the infants classified as secure and univariate effect for V at 4.5 months (p< .014). However, Tu-
86% (19 of 22) classified as insecure, for an overall success rate key range tests revealed no significant differences bejtween A
of 93%. The infants1 scores on the linear discriminant function and C infants on V or any of the other indexes of cardiac func-
were used as a continuous-variable index of attachment secu- tioning at any age. B infants could be differentiated from the A
rity. In the distribution of security scores, the higher the score and C infants, but no differences in cardiac functioning could
the higher the likelihood the infant was classified as insecure. be discerned between the latter two groups described behav-
Because of our special interest in very early measures of car- iorally as insecurely attached. (See Table 4.)
diac activity and our need to maximize statistical power, the
efficacy of the indexes of cardiac activity at 3 and 4.5 months Discussion
and those at 6 and 9 months in predicting attachment security
was tested in separate regression analyses. The four cardiac The results generally confirmed our expectations. There were
indexes at 3 months together with the four indexes at 4.5 developmental changes over the first year of life for all four
months were regressed on the continuous measure of attach-
ment security derived from the behavioral ratings in the Strange
2
Situation at 13 months. The relatively large set of independent Examination of the distributions of the cardiac measures for at-
variables (k = 8) dictated the use of stepwise regression with tachment classes revealed no outliers. There were considerable nono-
backward selection to minimize capitalization on chance (Ap- verlapping areas in the distributions for the secure (B) and insecure (A
pelbaum & McCall, 1983). Residual analyses showed that the and C) infants. For example, for Vat 4.5 months, 71% of Bs had values
less than 2.86, whereas only 14% of Cs and 33% of As had values this
assumptions underlying regression were met and there were no
low. Nevertheless, these multivariate analyses of variance must be in-
outliers. The results of the stepwise regression were as follows: terpreted with caution because of the small cell size when the insecure
R = .35, F(2,51) = 3.63, p<. 034. The only variable associated group was split.
436 IZARD, PORGES, SIMONS, HAYNES, HYDE, PARISI, COHEN

T3
O
•c

* «• * #
* # # #

C3
E
OH
X

0.

p.

v c
< g

18
a 15 v

dJ OJ CU O H 4> V &« OH
00 can* T
•5 g q <3 t: e B 1** <*< '"S X q
ra-
ble

2 c c Si v > > 2 2 a c § I § 2g§ ; V


CL, O. OH ft, « U CU 0- DH OH « «
£ o o <* XXXXSS
>XXXXSS
INFANT CARDIAC ACTIVITY 437

measures of cardiac functioning. Of the four measures, V and


HP variance proved to be the most reliable and the best predic-
tors of attachment security.
X! The use of discriminant function analysis to derive a continu-
o ous-variable index of attachment security probably contributed
a to the detection of significant relations between this develop-
mental outcome measure and indexes of cardiac functioning.
Preliminary regression analyses with conventional attachment
r*1 —
u classes (either secure/insecure or A/B/C) as dependent variable
produced weaker results and failed to identify some of the car-
II diac indexes that proved significant in the analyses reported
m here. Regression analysis with a continuous rather than a cate-
gorical dependent variable is more robust because the underly-
ing assumptions are better satisfied. As indicated earlier, resid-
3 ual analyses with the continuous variable as criterion showed
that the assumptions underlying regression analysis were met.
o Furthermore, the continuous-variable index of security tends to
compensate for error in the clinical judgment that enters into
classification, particularly for marginal cases or in some of the
decisions between classifications Q and B4 or between A2
and B ( .
II
03
Developmental Changes in Cardiac Activity
The developmental changes in cardiac functioning were pre-
cisely as expected. All four measures of cardiac activity in-
creased with age (increasing X HP variance, and HP range re-
flecting increasing heart-rate variability, and the increasing HP
reflecting a slowing of heart rate). The age-related increases
manifested in the three indexes of heart-rate variability appear
o — — to be mediated, at least in part, by a maturational increase in
4 vagal tone (Porges & Fox, 1986). These developmental data,
therefore, are consistent with Porges's view that the vagal con-
II
u trol of the heart indexes individual differences in nervous sys-
£ tem capacity to mediate physiological and behavioral reactivity
II
(Porges, 1976, in press). The increase in heart-rate variability
parallels the normal developmental increase in the infant's ca-
pacity to self-regulate. As self-regulation approaches asymptote,
•3
heart-rate variability should become more stable and a more
reliable measure of individual differences.
Of all the cardiac measures, the vagal tone index appeared to
be the most stable (Table 2). However, the stability of the vagal
tone index or any other autonomic measures is dependent on
maturation. Stifter and Fox (1990) have reported that neonatal
5U
V is not related to subsequent developmental assessments, al-
though they reported significant relations between 5- and 14-
months V In the older child, Fox and Field (1989) have reported
test-retest correlations above .80 with preschool children re-
corded 6 months apart. In a similar manner, there are data
(S. W Porges, personal communication, July 1990) demonstrat-
ing significant relations in infants between 9 and 20 months
14 during baseline and during an attention-demanding task. For
the present data, assessed during baseline conditions, the aver-
X! S age of all 10 cross-age V correlations was .34, and the average for
Si the 7 cross-age correlations for V at 3 and 4.5 months was .39.
The other heart-rate variability measures were less stable than
V These findings are consistent with the literature.

Vagal Influences and Attachment Security


a u u B
31S The cardiac data demonstrate the following two points: (a)
CO u that all measures of heart-rate variability used in this study
438 IZARD, PORGES, SIMONS, HAYNES, HYDE, PARIS!, COHEN

reflect vagal mechanisms and (b) that greater vagal mediation result in an attenuation of V, suggesting that our estimate of V
of heart-rate activity is a characteristic of the insecure infant, forC infants was conservative. Our data showed astrong similar-
regardless of the method of indexing attachment security The ity in the level of the vagal tone index in the two insecure
methods used to quantify heart-period variance and range in groups.
this study produced values that were highly correlated with the Although it is only speculation, it is plausible that avoidant
validated vagal tone index. Regression analyses evaluating at- (A) children react physiologically but suppress negative emotion
tachment as a continuous variable and MANOY^s evaluating expressions as an early coping mechanism (Ainsworth et al.,
attachment by the conventional three-group classification 1978; Main & Weston, 1982; Izard, 1990). When nonhuman
scheme (i£., A, B, C) consistently demonstrated across the primate infants were separated from their mothers, some
various ages that infant measures of heart-rate variability are showed no overt behavioral reactions but had markedly in-
related to attachment. The data demonstrated that insecure creased cortisol levels, a physiological measure of stress
infants, regardless of whether they were classified as A or C or (Suomi, 1987). Observers might have concluded that these in-
identified by high scores on the continuous-variable index, had fants were not distressed, but physiological data indicated oth-
higher scores on heart-rate variability That V was the signifi- erwise. Avoidance behavior has been conceptualized as an
cant predictor of attachment security at age 3 months, and HP adaptive reaction of A children that allows them to maintain
variance the significant predictor at 6 and 9 months, suggest control over potentially disorganizing and distressing experi-
that there may be a shift from largely parasympathetic modula- ences (Main & Weston, 1982).
tion of HR variability in early infancy to a combination of Our findings relating to avoidant (A) infants indicate that
parasympathetic and sympathetic influences at later ages. they have a relatively high level of parasympathetic tone. This
Heart-rate level measured with HP was not consistently re- high parasympathetic tone may be related to autonomic reactiv-
lated to attachment security. The only measure of HP that con- ity and subjective experience, yet they express relatively little
tributed to the prediction of insecurity scores was that at 3 negative emotion under the stressful conditions of the strange
months when mean HP scores were higher (lower heart rates) situation. In this regard, avoidant (A) infants differ from both
for As than they were for Bs and Cs. The contribution was secure (B) and insecure/resistant (C) infants in the manner in
marginal (p< .15). which they regulate emotion or respond to autonomic activity
These results support the work of other researchers who have Recent studies with hospitalized patients suggest that some
found that infants' physiological and behavioral characteristics young children have a style of emotion regulation similar to
affect their attachment status (Belsky & Rovine, 1987; Brether- that of avoidant infants (Knight et al., 1979; Siegel, 1981). Never-
ton, O'Conneil, & Tracy; 1980; Miyake et al., 1985). Miyake et al. theless, all our speculation regarding A-C differences in the
found that infant irritability measured at 3 months of age was relations between autonomic function and affective expression
predictive of resistant reunion patterns in the strange situation must be regarded as highly tentative because of the small num-
at 1 year, a particularly important finding, given the strong ber in each of these classes.
association of heart-rate variability and irritability/reactivity The widely held view of temperament as a psychobiological
(e.g., Porges, in press). In addition, Belsky and Rovine (1987) construct reflects the belief that differences in physiological
have found temperament and possible autonomic activity dif- processes (i.e., sensory thresholds and reactivity) may be re-
ferences (tremulousness, amount of startle, and motor matu- flected in individual differences in behavioral reactivity. Our
rity) between A,-B2 and B3-Q infants. results provide evidence of a link between physiological pro-
The meaning of the positive relation between heart-rate vari- cesses and the behaviors that index infant-mother attachment.
ability and attachment insecurity is not yet clear. The idea that The evidence indicating that psychobiological factors contrib-
children with high heart-rate variability (in this case, V) are ute to the development of the infant-mother attachment is in
more behaviorally reactive to distressing situations (DiPietro. accordance with the findings of a number of temperament re-
Larson, & Porges, 1987; Porter et al., 1988) is partially sup- searchers (Campos, Barrett, Lamb, Goldsmith, & Stenberg,
ported in that insecure/resistant (C) children have high heart- 1983; Chess & Thomas, 1982; Kagan, 1982). Future research
rate variability, and it was type C infants who showed the great- should be directed toward resolving the issues raised by these
est amount of negative emotion in the strange situation (Shiller, findings and developing a more complete understanding of
Izard, & Hembree, 1986). The idea is not supported by our how the concepts of heart-rate variability, attachment, and tem-
finding relating to msecure/avoidant (A) children. They had perament are interrelated.
higher heart-rate variability than secure (B) children at all ages
except 6 months, yet they also expressed significantly less nega-
tive emotion in the strange situation than insecure/resistant References
children (Termine & Izard, 1989). Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of
Since ambivalent/resistant (C) children have a long latency to attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. HiHsdale,
sooth and resume play during the final reunion episode (Ains- NJ: Erlbaum.
worth et al., 1978; Shiller et al., 1986), their behavior might Appelbaura, M. I., & McCall, R. B. (1983). Design and analysis in
contribute to the cardiac activity monitored at 13-months. How- developmental psychology. In P. H. Mussen(Ed.), Handbook of child
psychology (pp. 415-476). New York: Wiley.
ever, the findings do not support this contention, because an
Belsky, J., & Rovine, M. (1987). Temperament and attachment security
increase in physical activity (crying, clinging, resisting, and rest- in the strange situation: An empirical rapproachment. Child Devel-
less behavior) following the strange situation and a break would opment, 58, 787-795.
not produce the observed higher vagal tone. Rather, increased Billman, G. E., & Dujardin, J. P. (in press). Dynamic changes in cardiac
metabolic demands associated with increased activity results in vagal tone as measured by time series analysis. American Journal of
lower vagal tone. Thus, any increase in motor activity would Physiology.
INFANT CARDIAC ACTIVITY 439
Bretherton, I., O'Connell, B., & Tracy, R. (1980, March). Styles of Myers, J. L. (1979). Fundamentals of experimental design. Boston, MA:
mother-infant and stranger-infant interaction. Paper presented at the Allyn & Bacon.
International Conference on Infant Studies, New Haven, CT. Porges, S. W. (1976). Peripheral and neurochemical parallels of psycho-
Campos, J. J., Barrett, K. C, Lamb, M. E., Goldsmith, H. H., & Sten- pathology: A psychophysiological model relating autonomic imbal-
berg, C. (1983). Socioemotional development. In M. M. Haith & J. J. ance in hyperactivity, psychopathology, and autism. In H. Reese
Campos (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 2. Infancy and (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior: Vol. 2 (pp. 35-65).
developmental psychobiology (pp. 783-915). New York: Wiley. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Chess, S., & Thomas, A. (1982). Infant bonding: Mystique and reality. Porges, S. W (1985). Method and apparatus for evaluating rhythmic
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 52, 213-222. oscillations in aperiodic response systems. (U.S. Patent No. 4,510,944)
Cohen, B. (1988). Relations among autonomic responding, infant and Washington, DC: US. Government Printing Office.
adult emotions, temperament and personality. Unpublished doctoral Porges, S. W (in press). Vagal tone: A mediator of affect. In J. A. Garber
dissertation, University of Delaware, Newark, DE. & K. A. Dodge (Eds.), The development of affect regulation anddysre-
Dellinger, J. A., Taylor, H. L., & Porges, S. W (1987). Atropine sulfate gulation. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
effects on aviator performance and on respiratory-heart period in- Porges, S. W, 8L Fox, N. A. (1986). Developmental psychophysiology. In
teractions. Aviation Space Environment Medicine, 58. 333-338. M. Coles, E. Donchin, & S. W Porges (Eds.), Psychophysiology. New
DiPietro, J. A., Larson, S. K., & Porges, S. W. (1987). Behavioral and York: Guilford Press.
heart-rate pattern differences between breast-fed and bottle-fed neo- Porter, F. L., Porges, S. W, & Marshall, R. E. (1988). Newborn pain
nates. Developmental Psychology, 23, A61-A14. cries and vagal tone: Parallel changes in response to circumcision.
Field, T., Healy, B., Goldstein, S., Perry, S., & Bendell, D. (1988). Infants Child Development, 59, 495-505.
of depressed mothers show "depressed" behavior even with nonde- Richters, J. E, Waters, E., & Vaughn, B. E. (1988). Empirical classifica-
pressed adults. Child Development, 59,1569-1579. tion of infant-mother relationships from interactive behavior and
Finn, J. D. (1974). A general model for multivariate analysis. New York: crying during reunion. Child Development, 59, 512-522.
Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. Rothbart, M. K., &Derryberry, D. (1981). Development of individual
Fox, N. A. (1989). The psychophysiological correlates of emotional differences in temperament. In M. E. Lamb & A. L. Brown (Eds.),
reactivity during the first year of life. Developmental Psychology, 25, Advances in developmental psychology: Vol. I (pp. 37-86). Hillsdale,
364-372. NJ: Erlbaum.
Fox, N. A., & Field, X (1989). "foung children's responses to entry into Saul, J. P., Rea, R. F., Eckberg, D. L., Berger, R. D., & Cohen, R. J.
preschool: Psychophysiological and behavioral findings. Journal of (1990). Heart rate and muscle sympathetic nerve variability during
Applied Developmental Psychology, 10, 527-540. reflex changes of autonomic activity. American Journal of Physiol-
Goldsmith, H. H., & Campos, J. X (1982). Toward a theory of infant ogy, 258, HI 13-W21.
temperament. In R. N. Emde & R. J. Harmon (Eds.), The develop- Shiller, V M., Izard, C. E., & Hembree, E. A. (1986). Patterns of emo-
ment ofattachment andaffiHative systems. New York: Plenum Press. tion expression during separation in the strange-situation proce-
Healy, B. X (1989). Autonomic nervous system correlates of tempera- dure. Developmental Psychology, 22, 378-382.
ment. Infant Behavior and Development, 12, 289-304. Siegel, L. J. (1981). Naturalistic study of coping strategies in children
Izard, C. E. (1990). Facial expressions and the regulation of emotions. facing medical procedures. Paper presented at the meeting of the
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 487-498. Southeastern Psychological Association, Atlanta, GA.
Jansen, H. X, & Dellinger, J. A. (1988). Effects of atropine on respira- Stifter, C. A., & Fox, N. A. (1990). Infant reactivity: Physiological corre-
tory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) in the rhesus macaque. Neurotoxicology latesofnewborn and 5-month temperament. Developmental Psychol-
and Teratology, 10,169-174. ogy, 26, 582-588.
Kagan, J. (1982). Heart rate and heart rate variability as signs of a Stifter, C.» Fox, N. A., & Porges, S. W (1989). Facial expressivity and
temperamental dimension in infants. In C. E. Izard (Ed.), Measuring vagal tone in five and ten month old infants. Infant Behavior and
emotion in infants and children (pp. 38-66). Cambridge, England: Development, 12,127-137.
Cambridge University Press. Suomi, S. J. (1987). Genetic and maternal contributions to individual
Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Snidman, N. (1987). The physiology and differences in rhesus monkey biobehavioral development. In N. A.
psychology of behavioral inhibition in children. Child Development, Krasnagor, E. M. Blass, M. A. Hoffer, & W R Smotherman (Eds.),
55,1459-1473. Perinatal development: A psychobiological perspective. San Diego,
CA: Academic Press.
Knight, A. B., Atkins, A., Eagle, C, Evans, N., Finkelstein, J. W, Fuku-
shima, D, Katz, J., & Weiner, H. (1979). Psychological stress, ego Termine, N. X, & Izard, C. E. (1989). The effects of anger and sadness
defenses, and cortisol production in children hospitalized for elec- expressions on the attachment behavior of 13-month olds. Manuscript
in preparation.
tive surgery. Psychosomatic Medicine, 41, 40-49.
Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. New
Main, M., & Weston, D. K. (1982). Avoidance of the attachment figure
York: Brunner/Mazel.
in infancy: Descriptions and interpretations. In C. M. Parkes & J.
Yeh, S., Forsythe, A., & Hon, E. H. (1973). Quantification of fetal heart
Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), The place ofattachment in human behavior
beat-to-beat interval differences. Obstetrics and Gynecology, 41,
(pp. 31-59). New \ork: Basic Books. 355-363.
Miyake, K., Chen, S. X, & Campos, J. J. (1985). Infant temperament,
mother's mode of interaction, and attachment in Japan: An interim
report. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points in attach- Received January 20,1990
ment theory and research. Monographs ofthe Society for Research in Revision received October 24,1990
Child Development, 50(1-2, Serial No. 209), 276-297. Accepted October 24,1990 •

You might also like