You are on page 1of 12

Journal of Criminal Justice 30 (2002) 89 – 100

Public views toward crime and correctional policies


Is there a gender gap?
Brandon K. Applegatea,*, Francis T. Cullenb, Bonnie S. Fisherb
a
Department of Criminal Justice and Legal Studies, University of Central Florida, Post Office Box 161600, 311 HPA, Orlando,
FL 32816-1600, USA
b
Division of Criminal Justice, University of Cincinnati, 600 Dyer Hall, Cincinnati, OH 45221-0389, USA

Abstract

Differences between men and women in their proximity to crime, moral development, and attitudes toward an
array of social issues suggest that a gender gap in crime views may exist. Investigations of this possibility,
however, are in short supply. Using a statewide data set and a variety of global and specific questions about crime
policy, punishment, and rehabilitation, this study found that men and women tend to hold moderately divergent
views. Women tend to express greater support for offender treatment and less support for punishment than men.
Implications of these results for the future of correctional and crime policy are discussed. D 2002 Elsevier
Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

Introduction research on crime opinions. Gender simply has not


been a focal issue in most examinations of attitudes
For more than six decades, political scientists have toward crime policies, punishment, and offender
devoted considerable attention to differences between rehabilitation. This study sought to provide greater
men and women in policy preferences and voting insight into gender differences by investigating, in a
behavior. Even greater efforts have been made to single analysis, men’s and women’s views across
understand gender differences since the 1980 pres- several aspects of crime policy and correctional policy.
idential election. In fact, it has become commonplace
to refer to a ‘‘gender gap’’ in citizens’ candidate
choices and preferences for social policies (Borquez, Gender and crime views
Goldenberg, & Kahn, 1988). Substantially less atten-
tion has been focused on possible differences between Theoretical perspectives
men’s and women’s views on crime and corrections.
Despite salient theoretical perspectives that might be It is somewhat surprising that criminologists have
applied, only a few studies have explored male – not devoted more attention to assessing whether a
female differences in crime attitudes. This is not to gender gap exists in crime views. As noted above, the
say that gender has been completely absent from existence of a gender gap is well established in a wide
variety of other areas. Women are more supportive of
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +1-407-823-3739; fax: social welfare, equal rights for homosexuals (Petersen
+1-407-823-5360. & Donnenwerth, 1998), equal opportunities for
E-mail address: bapplega@mail.ucf.edu women (Petersen & Donnenwerth, 1998; Studlar,
(B.K. Applegate). McAllister, & Hayes, 1998), and education, and they

0047-2352/02/$ – see front matter D 2002 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 0 4 7 - 2 3 5 2 ( 0 1 ) 0 0 1 2 7 - 1
90 B.K. Applegate et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 30 (2002) 89–100

are more concerned about healthcare and about greater support for offender treatment among women.
protecting the environment (Shapiro & Mahajan, Similarly, a concern with alleviating harm would
1986; Steger & Witt, 1989). As Shapiro and Mahajan logically lead women to oppose capital punishment;
(1986) report, women also tend to oppose issues obviously, the death penalty irreversibly harms the
involving force or violence (e.g., national defense, person being executed. Predictions about gender
military presence outside the United States, and the differences in support for other types of punishment,
death penalty), oppose abortion, and support com- however, are less clear. Women may favor greater
passionate policies for disadvantaged groups (e.g., punitiveness in an effort to deter or incapacitate
African Americans, the poor, and the elderly). Fur- criminals, if this might be seen as a means of
thermore, since the 1980 presidential election, Amer- protecting potential victims. Deterrence and inca-
ican women consistently have voted for Democratic pacitation, however, inherently visit harm on the
candidates more often than have men. In a recent offender. An alternative prediction stemming from
analysis of gender differences in voting, Studlar et al. Gilligan’s (1982) theory of ‘‘different voices,’’ there-
(1998) found that the largest gap since 1964 was in fore, seems more likely. Women who are in Gilligan’s
1992, when the percentage of women voting Democrat (1977) second stage of moral reasoning emphasize
was ten percentage points higher than the percentage self-sacrifice in the pursuit of care and protection, and
of men voting Democrat. The researchers’ empirical those at the third and highest stage act on a principle
examination of possible explanations for this differ- of nonviolence. Seeking to minimize harm, women
ence revealed that men’s and women’s voting was might be oriented toward lesser punitiveness, instead
indistinguishable once their views on several political preferring preventative crime policies that build
issues were considered. Thus, women’s voting patterns stronger families and communities.
were different from men’s because of gender differ- The aspects of the gender gap discussed thus far
ences in attitudes toward defense spending, spending suggest that women would be less punitive and more
on poverty, women’s equality, abortion, and the death supportive of preventative and rehabilitative ap-
penalty (Studlar et al., 1998). proaches to crime. A third consideration, however,
The divide between men and women on these paints a different picture: proximity to crime. Miller,
social and political issues might be expected to spur Rossi, and Simpson (1986) suggest that diverse social
criminologists to investigate gender differences in groupings may hold different views about punish-
crime views. Taken together, the findings on wom- ment because of their proximity to crime and to
en’s views suggest that women, compared to men, are criminal justice practices. Differentiating objective
more compassionate and more concerned about the proximity from subjective proximity, these authors
well-being of others, especially the socially disadvan- contend that objective proximity to crime refers to
taged. If these orientations also extend to attitudes ‘‘the empirically determined probabilities of the
toward offenders, greater support for rehabilitation occurrence of activities, events, or situations in which
and less support for punishment might also be ex- crime is implicated’’ (Miller et al., 1986, p. 316). Men
pected among women. and women, therefore, may disagree in their attitudes
Gilligan’s (1982) propositions about moral differ- about crime because of their differential likelihood of
ences between men and women suggest similar expect- offending, being victimized, being arrested, and so
ations about gender differences in crime attitudes. on. Miller et al. (1986, p. 316) use subjective prox-
Gilligan (1977, 1982) proposes a theory of differential imity to crime to mean ‘‘the perceived probabilities of
moral reasoning in which she argues that, although the same experiences.’’ Gender differences in atti-
men base their decisions about right and wrong on an tudes may be based on such considerations as fear of
‘‘ethic of justice,’’ women’s morality has a different crime or perceived likelihood of arrest.
foundation. Women do not emphasize individual rights Regarding prior victimization as an indicator of
and fairness. Instead, their moral decisions are based objective proximity, the existing literature is largely
on ‘‘sensitivity to the needs of others and the assump- consistent. Those who have been victimized by crime
tion of responsibility for taking care’’ (Gilligan, 1982, are no more punitive than those who have not been
p. 16). This ‘‘ethic of care’’ springs from an under- victimized (Blumstein & Cohen, 1980; McCorkle,
standing of the interconnectedness of human relation- 1993; Ouimet & Coyle, 1991; Stinchcombe et al.,
ships and a desire to minimize harm to oneself, to 1980; Tyler & Weber, 1982; cf. Keil & Vito, 1991).
others, and to the relationships between individuals. Victimization also appears to have no significant
This position predicts a particular pattern of men’s influence on support for rehabilitation (Cullen, Clark,
and women’s attitudes toward crime policies. To the Cullen, & Mathers, 1985; Langworthy & Whitehead,
extent that people believe that rehabilitation helps 1986; McCorkle, 1993).
offenders become more productive citizens and pro- In the realm of subjective proximity to crime, the
tects future victims, an ethic of care would predict empirical evidence on the relationship between fear
B.K. Applegate et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 30 (2002) 89–100 91

of crime and attitudes toward crime is mixed. The Applegate, Wright, Cullen, & Dunaway, 1993; Tyler &
weight of the evidence, however, seems to favor fear Weber, 1982). For example, in a particularly detailed
as a significant predictor of correctional attitudes. investigation of gender and attitudes toward capital
Several researchers have failed to uncover a relation- punishment, Whitehead and Blankenship (2000)
ship between fear and punitive crime views (Barkan found that 80 percent of men stated that they favored
& Cohn, 1994; Kelley & Braithwaite, 1990; Ouimet the death penalty, compared to only 65 percent of the
& Coyle, 1991). Furthermore, McCorkle (1993) women in their sample. Furthermore, a larger percent-
found an insignificant relationship between fear and age of women (26 percent) than men (12 percent)
support for rehabilitation. Other scholars, however, indicated a preference that murderers be sentenced to a
have reported that fear of crime is significantly full life term in prison rather than execution. Men and
related to greater support for capital punishment (Keil women also differed in the reasons that they gave for
& Vito, 1991; Tyler & Weber, 1982), increased their support or opposition, and women who initially
harshness in preferred sentences (Hough & Moxon, favored the death penalty were more willing to change
1988), and greater punitiveness (McCorkle, 1993). their position in the face of empirical evidence (White-
Langworthy and Whitehead (1986) also explored fear head & Blankenship, 2000).
as an influence on citizens’ choices between punish- The research on men’s and women’s views of
ment and teaching as the main purpose of prisons. capital punishment not withstanding, the evidence for
Their findings revealed that respondents who were a gender gap in other areas of crime attitudes is rather
more afraid of crime were less likely to state that the inconsistent. Some research shows that gender is
purpose of prisons is ‘‘to teach them [prisoners] to be unrelated to support for rehabilitation (Haghighi &
law-abiding citizens’’ (Langworthy & Whitehead, Lopez, 1998; Hurwitz & Smithey, 1998; Johnson,
1986, p. 580). 1994; McCorkle, 1993; Reichel & Gauthier, 1990;
The fear of crime literature, therefore, suggests a Warr & Stafford, 1984). Other investigations reveal
gender gap in crime views. The direction of the that women’s and men’s views toward correctional
relationship between gender and attitudes toward treatment are significantly different, with women
criminal justice policies, however, is opposite what reporting more favorable attitudes (Cullen et al.,
is predicted by the literature on other social and 1985; Cullen, Golden, & Cullen, 1983; Haghighi &
political issues and Gilligan’s (1977, 1982) moral Lopez, 1998; Langworthy & Whitehead, 1986). Fur-
development theory. A consistent finding in the fear thermore, several studies of punitive opinions show
of crime literature is that women tend to be more that men are less punitive than women (Cohn, Bar-
fearful of victimization than men (Ferraro, 1995; kan, & Halteman, 1991; Farnworth et al., 1996;
Langworthy & Whitehead, 1986; Sprott, 1999). Thus, Haghighi & Lopez, 1998; Hurwitz & Smithey,
crime fear suggests greater punitiveness and less 1998; Kelley & Braithwaite, 1990; McCorkle,
support for rehabilitation among women than men. 1993; Miller et al., 1986; Samuel & Moulds, 1986;
Thomas, Cage, & Foster, 1976). Evidence also exists,
Empirical evidence of a gender gap however, showing that men are more punitive than
women (Blumstein & Cohen, 1980; Grasmick &
The empirical research on attitudes toward pun- McGill, 1994; Sprott, 1999), or that there is no
ishment and rehabilitation sheds little light on the relationship between gender and punitiveness (Apple-
extent and nature of a gender gap. Although several gate, Cullen, Link, Richards, & Lanza-Kaduce, 1996;
studies of preferences for punishments and attitudes Applegate, Cullen, Turner, & Sundt, 1996; Grasmick
toward rehabilitation have included sex as an inde- et al., 1993; Grasmick, Davenport, Chamlin, & Bur-
pendent variable, only a few authors have paid sik, 1992; Halim & Stiles, 2001; Hurwitz & Smithey,
particular attention to gender issues. Moreover, most 1998; Osborne & Rappaport, 1985; Ouimet & Coyle,
of these studies examined only a limited set of 1991; Singh & Jayewardene, 1978; Sprott, 1999;
criminal justice attitudes. For all issues except capital Taylor & Kleinke, 1992).
punishment, the results are quite equivocal. The existing research, therefore, suggests that
The evidence on support for capital punishment is women are less likely to support the death penalty
fairly clear. Almost invariably, researchers have than men. It also indicates that women may be more
reported that women are less favorable toward the supportive of rehabilitation than men, or men’s and
death penalty than men (Barkan & Cohn, 1994; women’s views may not differ significantly. Further-
Bohm, 1991; Farnworth, Bennett, & West, 1996; more, women may be less punitive than men, more
Grasmick, Cochran, Bursik, & Kimpel, 1993; Halim punitive than men, or may hold nearly the same
& Stiles, 2001; Keil & Vito, 1991; Kelley & attitudes as men toward punishment. Some of these
Braithwaite, 1990; Sandys & McGarrell, 1995; Skov- differences might be explained by the way crime
ron, Scott, & Cullen, 1989; Zeisel & Gallup, 1989; cf. attitudes were measured. Sprott (1999), for example,
92 B.K. Applegate et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 30 (2002) 89–100

recently showed that among a sample of Canadians, and those with higher incomes tend to be more
the type of question asked affected the relationship punitive and less supportive of rehabilitation; those
between gender and punitiveness. In response to a who are more educated, on the other hand, tend to
global question about whether the courts are harsh favor treatment. Although the effects of these variables
enough in sentencing offenders, men and women did on attitudes typically are slight, the existing research
not differ significantly in a multivariate analysis. In suggests that the sample might overestimate punitive-
contrast, women were significantly less punitive than ness and underestimate support for rehabilitation.
men when they were asked to sentence a specific The predominance of White respondents in the
juvenile offender. sample also restricted the ability to explore possible
interactions between race and gender. The existing
Research strategy research is equivocal on whether race and gender
interact in predicting crime views. On the one hand,
The present study sought to explore whether ques- Miller et al. (1986) report that Black women, on
tion type matters by asking a variety of different types average, preferred harsher sentences than Black men,
of questions about citizens’ views toward crime policy, White women, or White men for a variety of crimes.
punishment, and rehabilitation within a single study. On the other hand, Haghighi and Lopez (1998) found
Drawing on the theoretical and empirical discussion that non-White males and females were consistently
above, it was expected that compared to men, women less harsh in their views on parole and sentencing
would be more supportive of rehabilitation, more policies than White males and females. Similarly,
supportive of liberal approaches to crime, and more Halim and Stiles (2001) failed to find any significant
supportive of conservative approaches to crime. It was race – gender interactions in public preferences for
also predicted that women would hold less favorable capital punishment or for harsher courts. Due to the
attitudes toward capital punishment. Regarding other limited number of minorities in the present sample,
forms of punishment, the existing literature provides stable estimates of interactions are not possible.1
more ambiguous guidance, but it was hypothesized Thus, this study was unable to offer evidence that
that women would be less punitive than men when might resolve this issue.
they were asked to consider a specific offender and
more punitive when asked about general policies. Dependent variables

Support for rehabilitation and punishment


Methodology Attitudes toward offender treatment and punish-
ment were assessed several ways. First, attitudes
Sample toward offender treatment were measured by a com-
posite index of ten statements about rehabilitation
For this study, Survey Sampling Inc. provided a (Rehabilitation Policy Index). Examples of the indi-
random sample of 1000 Ohio residents. Question- vidual items included ‘‘It is a good idea to provide
naires were mailed to each member of the sample treatment for offenders who are in prison’’ and ‘‘It is
following Dillman’s (1978) total design method. The important to try to rehabilitate juveniles who have
first mailing was distributed in May 1996. Reminder committed crimes and are now in the correctional
letters were mailed one, three, and seven weeks later. system.’’ The respondents assigned each state-
Completed questionnaires were returned by 559 ment one of six points on a Likert scale that ranged
respondents. In addition, 105 questionnaires were from disagree strongly (coded 1) to agree strongly
returned unanswered because the intended respondent (coded 6). For each respondent, the individual scores
could not be located, had moved out of the state, was were summed and divided by the number of items
too ill to complete the survey, or was deceased. The answered, producing an index of the mean of valid
resulting response rate for those members of the responses (Cronbach’s a = 0.90). Higher index scores
sample who received a questionnaire and were cap- indicated more favorable views toward the rehabilita-
able of completing it was 64.4 percent (559/895). tion of offenders.2
Based on comparisons with data from the 1990 As a second measure of attitudes toward offender
census, it appears that the sample overrepresents men treatment, the respondents were presented with a
(66.7 percent), Whites (92.4 percent), people who are vignette and asked for their reactions to the hypothet-
older (mean = 53.5 years), people who have a college ical offender described. Each respondent read one
education (27.9 percent), and people with higher vignette that described a specific offender – age, race,
household incomes (median = US$35,000 – 49,000) sex – and several aspects of his or her situation – cri-
(U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1994). Prior research minal history, drug use, employment history, current
indicates that older respondents, White respondents, offense, sentence, and type of rehabilitation program.
B.K. Applegate et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 30 (2002) 89–100 93

The scenarios were constructed as factorial survey of Rehabilitation Goal,’’ ‘‘Importance of Protection
vignettes (Rossi & Nock, 1982). Thus, each of the Goal,’’ and ‘‘Importance of Punishment Goal.’’
attributes was randomly varied in the vignettes. The Attitudes toward punishment also were assessed
following is an example of the vignettes provided: by obtaining each respondent’s position on the death
penalty (Capital Punishment). The respondents were
Gary, a thirty year old White male threatened a asked whether they favored, opposed, or had no
victim with a weapon and demanded money. The
opinion on capital punishment: ‘‘Are you in favor
victim gave him US$10 and was not harmed. After
of the death penalty for a person convicted of
being convicted for this crime, the court discovered
that Gary had been employed off and on for several
murder?’’ This question replicates one used in pre-
years, and did not use drugs. His prior record showed vious national polls by the Gallup Organization (see
that he had been convicted once before for a crime in Warr, 1995). In logistic regression analysis reported
which he drove a car while drunk and caused a below, the respondents who favored capital punish-
traffic accident. No one was seriously hurt. ment were coded 1 and those who were unsupportive
For his current offense, Gary was sentenced to or had no opinion were coded 0.
probation, where he will continue to live in the An additional item, which replicates a question
community but must meet with a probation officer used since the early 1970s in the U.S. General Social
once per month. As a part of his sentence, Gary is in
Survey (Smith, 1998), sought the respondents’ atti-
a rehabilitation program. He is enrolled in a psycho-
tudes toward their local courts: ‘‘In general, do you
logical treatment program that teaches offenders to
give up criminal values and encourages good behav-
think the courts in this area deal too harshly or not
ior through a system of rewards and punishments. harshly enough with criminals?’’ The respondents
chose ‘‘too harshly,’’ ‘‘about right,’’ or ‘‘not harshly
Following the vignettes, the respondents were enough.’’ For the logit analyses, a dummy variable
asked to rate each of five statements regarding was created for Court Harshness with those who
treatment (e.g., ‘‘I support the use of rehabilitation responded not harshly enough coded 1 and all other
with Lisa/Gary’’) on the same six-point Likert scale responses coded 0.
described above. A single mean response index To measure somewhat more specific attitudes
(Vignette Rehabilitation Index) was created, with toward punishment, a composite index of four items
higher scores indicating more favorable attitudes taken from Cullen, Cullen, and Wozniak (1988) was
toward rehabilitation (Cronbach’s a = 0.87). created (Punishment Policy Index). An example of
The survey also replicated a question previously the statements included in this index was ‘‘Since
included in several polls conducted by Louis Harris most criminals will commit crimes over and over
and Associates (see Cullen, Skovron, Scott, & Bur- again, the only way to protect society is to put these
ton, 1990; Hindelang, Dunn, Aumick, & Sutton, criminals in jail and throw away the key.’’ The
1975, p. 218; McGarrell & Flanagan, 1985, p. 233; respondents indicated, on the same six-point Likert
Sundt, Cullen, & Applegate, Turner, 1998): scale described above, how strongly they agreed or
disagreed with each statement. The individual scores
Now what do you think should be the main emphasis were summed and divided by the number of items
in most prisons — punishing the individual convicted answered, producing an index of the mean of valid
of a crime, trying to rehabilitate the individual so
responses for each respondent (Cronbach’s a = 0.71).
that he might return to society as a productive
citizen, or protecting society from future crimes he More punitive responses produced higher scores on
might commit? this index.
Finally, punitiveness was measured in response to
The respondents were instructed to choose one the offender described in the vignette. Following the
main goal from these possibilities: ‘‘punish,’’ ‘‘rehab- five statements about rehabilitation described above,
ilitate,’’ ‘‘protect society,’’ and ‘‘not sure.’’ four punitive statements were provided (e.g., ‘‘It is
An additional set of questions allowed for the important to make sure Lisa/Gary gets the punishment
possibility that the respondents would prefer that that she/he deserves’’). The respondents expressed
prisons pursue more than one goal (Thomson & their agreement or disagreement on the same six-point
Ragona, 1987; Warr & Stafford, 1984). To assess Likert scale, and the items were combined to create a
men’s and women’s support for each goal of prisons, single index (Cronbach’s a = 0.72). Higher scores on
the survey asked them to indicate the importance of this Vignette Punitiveness Index indicate more pun-
each of the goals mentioned in the previous question. itive attitudes.
The respondents rated each goal as very important
(coded 4), important (coded 3), a little important Support for liberal and conservative crime policies
(coded 2), or not important (coded 1). In the tables The questionnaire also included several items that
that follow, these items are identified as ‘‘Importance tapped the respondent’s views on traditionally liberal
94 B.K. Applegate et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 30 (2002) 89–100

and traditionally conservative approaches to crime (vandalism, noisy neighbors, litter, teens hanging
control. Six liberal items focused on preventative out, insulting or bothersome people) were a problem
measures such as cleaning up neighborhoods, pro- in their neighborhood. Although they are not direct
viding job assistance, and early intervention with measures of one’s fear of becoming a crime victim,
troubled youths (e.g., ‘‘During the summer, the state they do assess a more general sense of insecurity. The
government should help provide jobs for inner-city responses to these items were combined to form an
youths from poor families’’). Six conservative items additive index, where higher scores indicated percep-
concentrated on harsher sentencing and enhancing the tions of a more civil neighborhood (a = 0.79).
ability of the police and courts to arrest and convict Victimization was measured by asking the re-
offenders (e.g., ‘‘Build more prisons so that longer spondents to indicate whether they had been the
sentences could be given to criminals’’ and ‘‘Allow victim of three property and three personal crimes
the courts to use evidence that shows an offender’s in the past twelve months. Those who responded
guilt, even if it was obtained illegally’’). The respond- ‘‘yes’’ to any of the crimes were coded 1, and those
ents assigned each statement one of six points on the who had not been victimized were coded 0.
same Likert scale that ranged from disagree strongly Contact with offenders was assessed with three
(coded 1) to agree strongly (coded 6). Two summated questions that asked each respondent whether he or
indexes were created: a Liberal Crime Policy Index she had ever ‘‘visited any prisons or jails,’’ ‘‘worked
(Cronbach’s a = 0.76) and a Conservative Crime for pay or done volunteer work with people who had
Policy Index (Cronbach’s a = 0.68). Index scores broken the law,’’ or ‘‘known someone who was in
indicate greater support for liberal and conservative prison or jail.’’ A scale of contact was created by
policies, respectively. summing the number of affirmative responses.
Additional control variables also were included in
Independent variables the multivariate analyses. These variables were race,
age, income, education, political conservatism, polit-
The main independent variable of interest, gender, ical party, salience of religion, adherence to a literal
was measured in a straightforward way. The respond- interpretation of the Bible, holding a punitive image of
ents indicated on the survey whether they were male God, and intensity of a religiously based attitude of
or female. In the analyses that follow, male respond- forgiveness. Prior studies have found these variables
ents are coded 1 and female respondents are coded 0. to be related to social attitudes and/or to gender (Britt,
Earlier, this article suggested that a gender gap in 1998; Grasmick et al., 1993; Haghighi & Lopez, 1998;
attitudes might be explained by differences in men’s Miller et al., 1986; Petersen & Donnenwerth, 1998).
and women’s moral perspectives (Gilligan, 1982), or
by differential proximity to crime (Miller et al.,
1986). Unfortunately, measures of the respondents’ Results
moral bases were not available in this data set. Thus,
Gilligan’s (1982) explanation of gender differences Bivariate analyses
could not be assessed directly. In an effort to deter-
mine whether differences in men’s and women’s Table 1 presents descriptive statistics for male and
views of crime and corrections might be due to their female respondents on each of the continuous depend-
subjective or objective proximity to crime, two meas- ent variables. The first three variables measure atti-
ures of fear, one measure of victimization, and one tudes toward rehabilitation. As shown in the table,
measure of neutral contact with offenders were women recorded significantly more favorable attitudes
included in the multivariate models that follow. toward the rehabilitation policies and toward the
First, the survey asked the respondents, ‘‘At one rehabilitation of the offender described in the vignette,
time or another, most people have experienced fear compared to men. Women also indicated that they
about becoming the victim of a crime. Think back to believed rehabilitation was a more important goal of
those times when you might have felt afraid or prisons than did men. Despite the statistical signific-
worried that you might be a crime victim. How many ance of these differences, both men’s and women’s
times have you felt afraid of crime in the last average scores indicated support for rehabilitation. The
month?’’ The respondents were then afforded the average response of both groups fell between ‘‘agree’’
response options ‘‘none,’’ ‘‘once,’’ ‘‘two or three,’’ and ‘‘agree slightly’’ for the statements comprising the
‘‘four or five,’’ and ‘‘more than five.’’ Rehabilitation Policy Index and the Vignette Rehab-
The second measure of fear attempted to assess ilitation Index. The figures displayed in Table 1 also
the respondents’ perceptions of the safety of their indicate that both men and women tended to rate
neighborhoods. A set of items asked the respondents rehabilitation as an ‘‘important’’ goal of prisons. Thus,
to indicate whether any of five incivil activities the gap between men’s and women’s views toward
B.K. Applegate et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 30 (2002) 89–100 95

Table 1
Men’s and women’s mean attitudes toward crime policies, punishment, and rehabilitation
Significance
Female mean Male mean t (one-tailed)
Rehabilitation Policy Index 4.61 4.25 4.79 .000
Vignette Rehabilitation Index 4.46 4.20 2.85 .002
Importance of Rehabilitation Goal 3.37 3.07 4.18 .000
Importance of Protection Goal 3.60 3.51 1.47 .072
Importance of Punishment Goal 3.53 3.47 1.14 .128
Punishment Policy Index 4.43 4.41 0.29 .384
Vignette Punitiveness Index 3.59 3.81 2.56 .006
Conservative Crime Policy Index 4.37 4.22 1.93 .027
Liberal Crime Policy Index 4.64 4.32 4.03 .000
All indexes were measured on a six-point Likert scale, ranging from agree strongly (coded 6) to disagree strongly (coded 1). The
importance of each goal was measured on a four-point scale, where very important was coded 4 and not important was coded 1.

treatment was a matter of the degree of support, rather with the few studies of rehabilitation attitudes and
than support versus opposition. gender, women in the present study appeared to be
Men and women did not differ significantly, how- more supportive of treatment for offenders. Also
ever, on the following three variables shown in reflecting prior analyses, more men than women
Table 1. On average, both genders believed that favored capital punishment. As noted, there was less
protecting society and punishment were ‘‘important’’ consistency in the findings of prior studies on the
to ‘‘very important’’ goals of prisons. Men and women relationship between other measures of punitiveness
also tended to report very similar attitudes toward the and gender. Equivocal results also are apparent across
items that made up the Punishment Policy Index. the various measures of punishment attitudes
In contrast, the final three variables displayed in included in this study. Men and women were nearly
Table 1 indicate a gender gap. Male and female equally supportive of general punishment policies,
respondents’ punitiveness toward the hypothetical but women seemed to be less punitive than men when
vignette offender differed significantly. On average, they were asked to consider a particular offender.
women were less punitive than men. Compared to
men, women also indicated greater support for con-
servative crime policies and for liberal crime policies.
Table 2 presents descriptive statistics, separately Table 2
for men and women, for the remaining dependent Comparison of men’s and women’s attitudes toward capital
variables. As shown in the table, approximately 70 punishment, the harshness of courts, and the main emphasis
percent of both genders reported a belief that the of prisons
courts are not harsh enough on offenders. Their views Percentage Percentage
on the death penalty and on the main emphasis of of women of men
prisons, however, were significantly different. In Court harshness
terms of capital punishment for murderers, men were Too harsh 2.2 2.5
considerably more likely to favor this sentence About right 27.5 28.9
(81.9 percent) than women (63.9 percent). Women, Not harsh enough 70.2 68.6
however, more often stated that they had ‘‘no opinion.’’ c2 = .154, df = 2, P = .926
Less dramatic, though still significant, differences
were uncovered for the preferred goal of prisons. Capital punishment for murder
Favor 63.9 81.9
Substantially, fewer women than men chose protec-
Oppose 14.8 11.8
tion of society as the appropriate emphasis of prison. No opinion 21.3 6.3
As Table 2 shows, a larger percentage of women c2 = 30.309, df = 2, P = .000
reported that rehabilitation should be the main goal.
The percentage of men was only slightly greater than Main emphasis of prisons should be
the percentage of women choosing ‘‘punishment.’’ Punishment 18.0 21.6
The bivariate analyses shown in Tables 1 and 2 Protection of society 23.5 35.9
suggest that a gender gap in crime attitudes exists. Rehabilitation 49.2 37.0
More so than men, women favored efforts, both Not sure 9.3 5.5
liberal and conservative, to reduce crime. Consistent c2 = 13.709, df = 3, P = .003
96 B.K. Applegate et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 30 (2002) 89–100

Multivariate analyses equations was the amount of variation explained


greater than 2 percent, and even at its largest, R2
To further investigate the gender gap in crime was only .039.
views, several multivariate models were estimated. Table 3 also reports the results for gender from the
As noted earlier, the apparent distinction between regression models that included the control variables.
men’s and women’s attitudes might result from their As shown by the increased R2 values, including these
different levels of proximity to crime. The multi- additional variables substantially increased the
variate models, therefore, control for aspects of this explanatory power of the models. Still, little more
factor, as well as several other attitudinal and socio- than 10 percent of the variation was explained for
demographic variables. most of the dependent variables. More salient to the
The analyses were conducted in two stages. First, focus of this study, however, is whether including
in an effort to assure that the method of analysis these variables was able to explain away the influence
would not alter the results from what was reported in of gender on crime attitudes. As shown in Table 3,
Tables 1 and 2, regression equations in which gender gender remained significant for most of the models.
was the only independent variable — OLS for the Compared to men, women reported significantly
continuous dependent variables and logit for the more favorable attitudes toward the Rehabilitation
dichotomous dependent variables — were computed. Policy Index. They also thought that rehabilitation
These results mirrored those shown in Tables 1 and 2. was a more important goal of prisons, and women
Secondly, regression equations that included gender were more likely to choose rehabilitation as the main
and the other independent variables described earlier goal of prisons. Men, on the other hand, were more
were computed. likely to favor capital punishment, were more pun-
Table 3 shows the results of these analyses.3 The itive toward the offender described in the vignette,
first column of coefficients reports the bivariate and were more likely to select protection as the
regressions. These models clearly show that women appropriate main emphasis of prisons. Men also were
were more supportive of rehabilitation and of con- less supportive of conservative crime control efforts
servative and liberal approaches to crime control, but and of liberal crime policies, though gender was not
they were less supportive of capital punishment, and statistically significant for these variables. These
they were less punitive than men toward specific results suggest that for the most part, the influence
offenders. This table also shows that the amount of of gender on attitudes toward crime and corrections
variation in these attitudes that was explained by was not explained away by fear, victimization, con-
gender alone was modest. In only four of the eight tact with offenders, race, age, income, education,

Table 3
Regression of crime policy attitudes on gender with and without control variables
Without controls With controls
2
Dependent variable Gender b (significance) R Gender b (significance) R2
Rehabilitation Policy Index .361 (.000) .038 .199 (.010) .214
Vignette Rehabilitation Index .259 (.002) .013 .066 (.476) .209
Importance of Rehabilitation Goal .297 (.000) .029 .190 (.014) .117
Main emphasis should be rehabilitation .500 (.003) .013 .490 (.028) .136
(1 = rehabilitation, 0 = all others)
Main emphasis should be protection .600 (.002) .016 .550 (.023) .074
(1 = protection, 0 = all others)
Support for capital punishment .938 (.000) .036 .908 (.000) .115
(1 = favor, 0 = oppose/no opinion)
Vignette Punitiveness Index .221 (.006) .010 .264 (.005) .149
Conservative Crime Policy Index .143 (.027) .005 .130 (.140) .148
Liberal Crime Policy Index .318 (.000) .027 .150 (.065) .226
Gender is coded such that 0 = female and 1 = male. All models include a constant and are statistically significant ( P .05). In all
models, control variables were age, race, education, income, fear of crime, perceived neighborhood incivility, victimization,
conservatism, political party, biblical literalness, religious salience, punitive image of God, and religious forgiveness. The
vignette analyses also included the offender’s race, sex, age, employment history, drug use, offense history, current offense,
sentence type, and type of rehabilitation program as control variables. R2 is the adjusted R2 for the continuous dependent
variables and pseudo-R2 (Aldrich & Nelson, 1984) for the dichotomous dependent variables.
B.K. Applegate et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 30 (2002) 89–100 97

political conservatism, political party, religious views, similar on some of their views about crime. Women,
or, in the case of the vignette, the specific character- for example, were just as likely as men to say that the
istics of the offense or the offender. courts in their area do not operate harshly enough, a
As a final step in the analyses, regression models finding that mirrors results from prior studies (Halim
were computed for each dependent variable separately & Stiles, 2001; Sprott, 1999). This study also failed
for men and women.4 The goal was to explore possible to identify significant differences on the perceived
interactions between gender and the other independent importance of punishment as a goal of prisons or on
variables. The equation suggested by Brame, Pater- the Punishment Policy Index. Even on those variables
noster, Mazerolle, and Piquero (1998) was used to test that did show significantly different views among
for significant differences between the regression men and women, the divergence was a matter of
coefficients of the male subsample and those of the degree; men’s and women’s attitudes were not oppos-
female subsample. Of the 184 pairs of coefficients ite. For example, the average score on the Liberal
compared, only thirty-six (19.6 percent) were signific- Crime Policy Index for men (4.32) was significantly
antly different, and few clear patterns emerged. different from the average score for women (4.64),
Proximity seemed to be implicated in many of the but both groups of respondents tended to express
instances where men’s and women’s coefficients were favorable attitudes. Women were more favorable by
significantly different. Prior victimization, fear of approximately 0.3 points on a six-point scale.
crime, and perceptions of neighborhood incivility Furthermore, gender accounted for only a modest
had significantly greater impacts on women than on percentage of the variation in attitudes. The relatively
men, though not in every model. Compared to its weak correlations between gender and attitudes may
effects on men, victimization experience for women help to explain the equivocal results produced by
led to greater reductions in support for rehabilitation in previous studies. If gender has a limited influence on
two models (Rehabilitation Policy Index and Import- crime views, the likelihood of detecting such effects
ance of Rehabilitation Goal) and for the liberal crime in any single study would be fairly low (Cohen,
control policies. Victimization also affected men and 1988). Thus, some samples would identify a signific-
women differently in their views of capital punish- ant relationship while others would not.
ment. For men, having been the victim of a crime was On the other hand, even if differences are not
positively related to support, but for women, prior always large, a gender gap in crime attitudes seems to
victimization was negatively related to support. Fear exist across most outcomes. Differences are espe-
of crime, either direct or because of perceived incivil- cially evident on support for capital punishment and
ity in one’s neighborhood, led to greater support for support for rehabilitation. These differences tend to
capital punishment, greater support for conservative be among the largest. In selecting the main emphasis
crime control measures, lesser support for liberal of prison, for example, more than 12 percent more
crime control measures, and reduced perceptions that women than men chose rehabilitation. The split was
rehabilitation is an important goal of prisons among even larger for capital punishment, where approxi-
women compared to men. Finally, women also were mately 64 percent of the female respondents said that
influenced more strongly by the gender of the offender they favored the death penalty, compared to 82 per-
described in the vignette. The female respondents cent of male respondents — a difference of 18 percent.
tended to be more supportive of rehabilitation and Moreover, differences in these two conceptual areas
less punitive when the vignette described a woman; tended to persist even after controls for other varia-
this was not the case for the male respondents. bles were introduced. Women continued to be less
supportive of capital punishment, and more support-
ive of rehabilitation. Women also were notably less
Discussion punitive when they were asked to respond to the
particular offender described in the vignette.
At the outset, this article proposed to explore These findings suggest the need to probe more
whether men’s attitudes on crime and corrections deeply into why these gender differences persist. In
were different from women’s. In this regard, a variety large part, they are not explained by other demo-
of measures were used to examine support for pun- graphic characteristics — race, age, income, or edu-
ishment, including capital punishment, support for cation — by political views, or by religious views.
rehabilitation, and support for liberal and conservat- Miller et al.’s (1986) suggestion that divergent pun-
ive crime control strategies. Similar to past evalua- ishment preferences might be explained by objective
tions, the results are not completely consistent. The and subjective proximity to crime largely was not
pattern of these findings, however, is understandable. supported by the data presented here. Although the
On the one hand, there is a risk of overestimating concept of proximity could be operationalized more
gender differences. Men and women were strikingly fully, this study included two measures of objective
98 B.K. Applegate et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 30 (2002) 89–100

proximity — victimization and neutral contact with whether an increase in the number of women in
offenders — and two measures of subjective prox- political office matters in regard to the concerns of
imity — fear of crime and perceived neighborhood women, Mezey (1994) reviewed the research on the
incivility. As noted, gender effects were not explained attitudinal and behavioral differences between male
away by these variables in most cases. Relatedly, and female politicians. The topics studied under the
differences between the separate-gender models dem- umbrella of ‘‘women’s issues’’ frequently include
onstrated that objective and subjective proximity had such considerations as reproductive rights, child
greater effects on the attitudes of women than men. abuse, welfare reform, and education. Based on
Proximity, however, does not explain away the gen- studies from the past three decades, Mezey (1994,
der gap in correctional attitudes. p. 267) concluded that ‘‘sex, along with party affili-
An alternative theoretical position may be useful to ation and political ideology, plays an important role
understand the present study’s results. Unfortunately, in determining legislative support for women’s
this study was unable to directly address an explana- issues’’ (also see Reingold, 1992).
tion drawn from Gilligan’s (1982) work. Still, her The qualified finding of sex differences among
delineation of ‘‘different voices’’ seems to resonate policymakers combined with the results of the present
with the present findings. The ethic of care that study suggest that a gender gap may affect crime
Gilligan contends predominates among women would policy, as well as other issue areas. The gender
seem to suggest greater concern for the well being of differences revealed here were small but showed
others, which would explain their greater support for consistency. It would be easy to claim too much
rehabilitation, as well as their hesitance toward capital regarding the potential impact of women on correc-
punishment. This framework also makes sense of the tions and crime policies, but there is also a risk in
finding for the conservative crime policy issues. Men dismissing modest differences. Even if only incre-
would be reacting to these countermeasures based on mentally and intermittently, women may exert a
an ethic of justice, a concern for individual rights. humanizing influence on correctional and crime pol-
Several of the items that made up this index suggested icymaking in the United States.
that efforts to protect citizens from victimization from
crime should supersede concerns for individual rights,
men might be expected to express less-favorable Acknowledgments
attitudes. Introducing a direct assessment of people’s
moral bases into analyses of gender and crime views
This project was supported by Grant No. 96-IJ-
would likely prove enlightening.
CX-0007 awarded by the National Institute of Justice,
Office of Justice Programs, U.S. Department of
Justice, and a grant awarded by the University
Conclusion
Research Council, University of Cincinnati. Points
of view in this document are those of the authors and
The results reported here show a relatively small
do not necessarily represent the official position or
but potentially important gap between men’s and
policies of the U.S. Department of Justice or the
women’s attitudes toward crime, punishment, and
University of Cincinnati.
corrections. To the extent that women have a different
perspective on what should be done about crime and
criminals, they tend to be more compassionate and Notes
more protective. This article has already noted that
past studies have revealed women are more support- 1. Analyses that included Gender  Race interaction
ive of a variety of policies that assist the disadvan- effects revealed that such effects were not statistically
taged, provide for protection of the environment and significant for most of the dependent variables in the study.
unborn children, and avoid use of force. When the Because of the limited number of non-White respondents
current findings are placed in the context of differ- in the sample, however, these results are unreliable and
ential gender attitudes toward other social and polit- cannot be taken as evidence that race and gender do
ical policies, a salient conclusion emerges: women, not interact.
more so than men, seem to hold a general view that 2. In the interest of parsimony, this article only
provides examples of the items in each of the indexes
the government should not simply be an instrument
included in this study. Complete lists of all items for each
of punishment and accountability but also should index are available from the lead author.
provide assistance to people with needs. 3. Table 3 does not list the gender coefficients for the
As women take a greater role in public policy following dependent variables: Punishment Policy Index,
making, their orientation toward crime policies may Importance of Protection as a Goal, Importance of Punish-
become increasingly important. In an effort to answer ment as a Goal, Court Harshness, or Punishment as the Main
B.K. Applegate et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 30 (2002) 89–100 99

Goal of Prisons. Gender was not a significant predictor of tenacity of rehabilitative ideology. Criminal Justice
public views on any of these measures either in the bivariate and Behavior, 17, 6 – 18.
or in the multivariate analyses. Dillman, D. A. (1978). Mail and telephone surveys: the total
4. Tables displaying the results of these analyses are design method. New York: Wiley.
available from the lead author. Farnworth, M., Bennett, K., & West, V. M. (1996). Mail vs.
telephone surveys of criminal justice attitudes: a compa-
rative analysis. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 12,
References 113 – 133.
Ferraro, K. (1995). Fear of crime: interpreting victim risk.
Aldrich, J. H., & Nelson, F. D. (1984). Linear probability, Albany: State University of New York Press.
logit, and probit models. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Gilligan, C. (1977). Concepts of the self and of morality.
Applegate, B. K., Cullen, F. T., Link, B. G., Richards, P. J., Harvard Educational Review, 47, 481 – 517.
& Lanza-Kaduce, L. (1996). Determinants of public pu- Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: psychological
nitiveness toward drunk driving: a factorial survey ap- theory and women’s development. Cambridge, MA:
proach. Justice Quarterly, 13, 57 – 79. Harvard University Press.
Applegate, B. K., Cullen, F. T., Turner, M. G., & Sundt, J. L. Grasmick, H. G., Cochran, J. K., Bursik Jr., R. J., & Kimpel,
(1996). Assessing public support for three-strikes-and- M. (1993). Religion, punitive justice, and support for the
you’re-out laws: global versus specific attitudes. Crime death penalty. Justice Quarterly, 10, 289 – 314.
and Delinquency, 42, 517 – 534. Grasmick, H. G., Davenport, E., Chamlin, M. B., & Bursik
Applegate, B. K., Wright, J. P., Cullen, F. T., & Dunaway, Jr., R. J. (1992). Protestant fundamentalism and the retrib-
R. G. (1993). Victim – offender race and support for cap- utive doctrine of punishment. Criminology, 30, 21 – 45.
ital punishment: a factorial design approach. American Grasmick, H. G., & McGill, A. L. (1994). Religion, attribu-
Journal of Criminal Justice, 18, 95 – 115. tion style, and punitiveness toward juvenile offenders.
Barkan, S. E., & Cohn, S. F. (1994). Racial prejudice and Criminology, 32, 23 – 46.
support for the death penalty by whites. Journal of Haghighi, B., & Lopez, A. (1998). Gender and perception of
Research in Crime and Delinquency, 31, 202 – 209. prisons and prisoners. Journal of Criminal Justice, 26,
Blumstein, A., & Cohen, J. (1980). Sentencing of convicted 453 – 464.
offenders: an analysis of the public’s view. Law and Halim, S., & Stiles, B. L. (2001). Differential support for
Society Review, 14, 223 – 261. police use of force, the death penalty, and perceived
Bohm, R. M. (1991). American death penalty opinion, harshness of the courts: effects of race, gender, and
1936 – 1986: a critical examination of the Gallup polls. region. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 28, 3 – 23.
In: Bohm R. M. (Ed.), The death penalty in America: Hindelang, M. J., Dunn, C. S., Aumick, A. L., Sutton, L. P.
current research. Cincinnati, OH: Anderson. (Eds.) (1975). Sourcebook of criminal justice statistics —
Borquez, J., Goldenberg, E. N., & Kahn, K. F. (1988). Press 1974. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
portrayals of the gender gap. In: C. M. Mueller (Ed.), Hough, M., & Moxon, D. (1988). Dealing with offenders:
The politics of the gender gap: the social construction of popular opinion and the views of victims in England and
political influence. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Wales. In: N. Walker, & M. Hough (Eds.), Public atti-
Brame, R., Paternoster, R., Mazerolle, P., & Piquero, A. tudes to sentencing: surveys from five countries. Brook-
(1998). Testing for the equality of maximum-likeli- field, VT: Gower.
hood regression coefficients between two independent Hurwitz, J., & Smithey, S. (1998). Gender and differences
equations. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 14, on crime and punishment. Political Research Quarterly,
245 – 261. 51, 89 – 115.
Britt, C. L. (1998). Race, religion and support for the death Johnson, B. (1994, November/December). To rehabilitate or
penalty: a research note. Justice Quarterly, 15, 175 – 191. punish: results of a public opinion poll. American Jails,
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behav- 8, 41 – 43.
ioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Keil, T. J., & Vito, G. F. (1991). Fear of crime and attitudes
Cohn, S. F., Barkan, S. E., & Halteman, W. A. (1991). toward capital punishment: a structural equations model.
Punitive attitudes toward criminals: racial consensus or Justice Quarterly, 8, 447 – 464.
racial conflict? Social Problems, 38, 287 – 296. Kelley, J., & Braithwaite, J. (1990). Public opinion and the
Cullen, F. T., Clark, G. A., Cullen, J. B., & Mathers, R. A. death penalty in Australia. Justice Quarterly, 7, 529 – 563.
(1985). Attribution, salience, and attitudes toward crim- Langworthy, R. H., & Whitehead, J. T. (1986). Liberalism
inal sanctioning. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 12, and fear as explanations of punitiveness. Criminology,
305 – 331. 24, 575 – 591.
Cullen, F. T., Cullen, J. B., & Wozniak, J. F. (1988). Is McCorkle, R. C. (1993). Research note: punish and rehabil-
rehabilitation dead? The myth of the punitive public. itate? Public attitudes toward six common crimes. Crime
Journal of Crime and Justice, 16, 303 – 317. and Delinquency, 39, 240 – 252.
Cullen, F. T., Golden, K. M., & Cullen, J. B. (1983). Is child McGarrell, E. F., T. J. Flanagan (Eds.) (1985). Sourcebook of
saving dead? Attitudes toward juvenile rehabilitation in criminal justice statistics—1984. Washington, DC: U.S.
Illinois. Journal of Criminal Justice, 11, 1 – 13. Government Printing Office.
Cullen, F. T., Skovron, S. E., Scott, J. E., & Burton Jr., V. S. Mezey, S. G. (1994). Increasing the number of women in
(1990). Public support for correctional treatment: the office: does it matter? In: E. A. Cook, S. Thomas, &
100 B.K. Applegate et al. / Journal of Criminal Justice 30 (2002) 89–100

A. M. Gallup (Eds.), The year of the woman: myths Sprott, J. B. (1999). Are members of the public tough on
and realities. San Francisco: Westview. crime? The dimensions of public ‘‘punitiveness’’. Jour-
Miller, J. L., Rossi, P. H., & Simpson, J. E. (1986). Percep- nal of Criminal Justice, 27, 467 – 474.
tions of justice: race and gender differences in judgments Steger, M. A. E., & Witt, S. L. (1989). Gender differences in
of appropriate prison sentences. Law and Society Re- environmental orientations: a comparison of publics and
view, 20, 313 – 334. activists in Canada and the US. Western Political Quar-
Osborne, Y. H., & Rappaport, N. B. (1985). Sentencing terly, 42, 627 – 649.
severity with mock jurors: predictive validity of three Stinchcombe, A. L., Adams, R., Heimer, C. A., Scheppele,
variable categories. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, K. L., Smith, T. W., & Taylor, D. G. (1980). Crime and
3, 467 – 473. punishment: changing attitudes in America. San Fran-
Ouimet, M., & Coyle, E. J. (1991). Fear of crime and sen- cisco: Jossey-Bass.
tencing punitiveness: comparing the general public and Studlar, D. T., McAllister, I., & Hayes, B. C. (1998). Ex-
court practitioners. Canadian Journal of Criminology, plaining the gender gap in voting: a cross-national anal-
33, 149 – 162. ysis. Social Science Quarterly, 79, 779 – 798.
Petersen, L. R., & Donnenwerth, G. V. (1998). Religion and Sundt, J. L., Cullen, F. T., Applegate, B. K., & Turner, M. G.
declining support for traditional beliefs about gender (1998). The tenacity of the rehabilitative ideal revisited:
roles and homosexual rights. Sociology of Religion, 59, have attitudes toward offender treatment changed? Crim-
353 – 371. inal Justice and Behavior, 25, 426 – 442.
Reichel, P. L., & Gauthier, A. K. (1990). Boot camp correc- Taylor, C., & Kleinke, C. L. (1992). Effects of severity of
tions: a public reaction. In: R. Muraskin (Ed.), Issues in accident, history of drunk driving, intent, and remorse on
justice: exploring policy issues in the criminal justice judgments of a drunk driver. Journal of Applied Social
system. Bristol, IN: Wyndham Hall. Psychology, 22, 1641 – 1655.
Reingold, B. (1992). Concepts of representation among fe- Thomas, C. W., Cage, R. J., & Foster, S. C. (1976). Public
male and male state legislators. Legislative Studies opinion on criminal law and legal sanctions: an exami-
Quarterly, 17, 509 – 537. nation of two conceptual models. Journal of Criminal
Rossi, P. H., S. L. Nock (Eds.) (1982). Measuring social Law and Criminology, 67, 110 – 116.
judgments: the factorial survey approach. Beverly Thomson, D. R., & Ragona, A. J. (1987). Popular moder-
Hills: Sage. ation versus governmental authoritarianism: an interac-
Samuel, W., & Moulds, E. (1986). The effect of crime se- tionist view of public sentiments toward criminal sanc-
verity on perceptions of fair punishment: a California tions. Crime and Delinquency, 33, 337 – 357.
case study. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, Tyler, T. R., & Weber, R. (1982). Support for the death
77, 931 – 948. penalty: instrumental response to crime or symbolic
Sandys, M., & McGarrell, E. F. (1995). Attitudes toward attitude? Law and Society Review, 17, 21 – 45.
capital punishment: preference for the penalty or mere U.S. Bureau of the Census. (1994). County and city data-
acceptance? Journal of Research in Crime and Delin- book: 1994. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Print-
quency, 32, 191 – 213. ing Office.
Shapiro, R. Y., & Mahajan, H. (1986). Gender differences in Warr, M. (1995). Public opinion on crime and punishment.
policy preferences: a summary of trends from the 1960s Public Opinion Quarterly, 59, 296 – 310.
to the 1980s. Public Opinion Quarterly, 50, 42 – 61. Warr, M., & Stafford, M. (1984). Public goals of punishment
Singh, A., & Jayewardene, C. H. S. (1978). Philosophical and support for the death penalty. Journal of Research in
consistency in public attitudes on crime and justice. Aus- Crime and Delinquency, 21, 95 – 111.
tralian and New Zealand Journal of Criminology, 11, Whitehead, J. T., & Blankenship, M. B. (2000). The gender
182 – 184. gap in capital punishment attitudes: an analysis of sup-
Skovron, S. E., Scott, J. E., & Cullen, F. T. (1989). The port and opposition. American Journal of Criminal Jus-
death penalty for juveniles: an assessment of public sup- tice, 25, 1 – 13.
port. Crime and Delinquency, 35, 546 – 561. Zeisel, H., & Gallup, A. M. (1989). Death penalty sentiment
Smith, T. W. (1998, August). Trendlets: crime and punish- in the United States. Journal of Quantitative Criminol-
ment: an update. GSS News, 12, 5. ogy, 5, 285 – 296.

You might also like