You are on page 1of 6

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2014, 47, 1–6 NUMBER 3 (FALL)

THE USE OF LINKED ACTIVITY SCHEDULES TO TEACH CHILDREN


WITH AUTISM TO PLAY HIDE-AND-SEEK
MATTHEW T. BRODHEAD, THOMAS S. HIGBEE, JOY S. POLLARD,
JESSICA S. AKERS, AND KRISTINA R. GERENCSER
UTAH STATE UNIVERSITY

Linked activity schedules were used to establish appropriate game play in children with autism
during a game of hide-and-seek. All 6 participants demonstrated acquisition of appropriate play
skills in the presence of the activity schedules and maintained responding during subsequent phases.
When the schedules were removed, responding decreased to baseline levels, demonstrating that the
schedules controlled responding. Implications for future research on the use of activity schedules to
teach social behavior are discussed.
Key words: activity schedules, autism, social interactions

Activity schedules, which contain visual or find the corresponding game and repeat the script,
auditory cues that serve as discriminative stimuli “Let’s play [name of game]” to initiate the
to engage in a sequence of activities (McClanna- interaction with his or her peer. The joint activity
han & Krantz, 1999), are often useful in schedule increased appropriate interactive peer
promoting independent responding and on-task play, and this behavior was maintained at high
behavior in individuals with autism and other levels during maintenance and novel location
disabilities across a range of skills and contexts (see phases for all three pairs of participants.
Koyama & Wang, 2011, for a review). Although Based on the success of Betz et al. (2008), we
multiple studies have demonstrated the effective- sought to investigate whether photographic
ness of activity schedules on maintaining an activity schedules could be used to promote
individual’s on-task behavior (e.g., MacDuff, appropriate play between children with autism
Krantz, & McClannahan, 1993), researchers during an activity that was less structured (e.g.,
have only recently begun to investigate the involved greater movement around the environ-
potential for activity schedules to promote social ment with fewer specific visual cues for playing
interactions between peers. For example, Betz, the game) and required more social interaction
Higbee, and Reagon (2008) used a joint activity than the games used in Betz et al. Specifically, we
schedule (i.e., one activity schedule used by two extended Betz et al. by using activity schedules to
peers) to improve social interactions between promote appropriate peer play during a game of
children with autism during a series of board hide-and-seek between pairs of preschool-aged
games. Each page of the joint activity schedule children with autism. Because hide-and-seek
contained a picture of a participant, a picture of a requires children to play different roles (i.e., the
game, and a written script to prompt a social hider and the seeker), we created individual
interaction between the two children. Betz et al. linked activity schedules with pages sequenced so
taught the participant depicted in the picture to that one child played the role of the hider while
the other played the role of the seeker.
We thank Kylee Lewis for her help throughout this METHOD
project.
Address correspondence to Thomas S. Higbee, Utah Participants and Setting
State University, 2865 Old Main Hill, Logan, Utah 84332
(e-mail: tom.higbee@usu.edu). We recruited six participants, Arlo (5 years
doi: 10.1002/jaba.145 old), Isis (5 years old), Brad (5 years old),

1
2 MATTHEW T. BRODHEAD et al.

Michelle (5 years old), Gus (3 years old), and ment in discrete game-play behaviors using a
Olivia (4 years old), all of whom had a diagnosis per-opportunity measure (Cooper, Heron, &
of autism and came from Caucasian, English- Heward, 2007). Table 1 displays a sequential
speaking households. Participants had received list of target responses. For the baseline and no-
intensive behavior-analytic instruction in a schedule probe phases, we scored only hide-and-
university-based preschool program for 1 to seek behaviors because the activity schedule was
2 years and were fluent in following individual not available during these phases; if a participant
activity schedules. We considered participants to hid or searched in any of the eight correct
be fluent if they followed their independent locations (see teaching and novel location phase
activity schedules with 80% or better accuracy, descriptions), it was counted as a correct response
without response prompts, in their preschool (defined as any instance when the participant
program for three consecutive prestudy sessions. independently engaged in the specified response
Participants also were able to make choices in the appropriate order at the appropriate time).
between two activities and had picture–location An independent observer coded at least 30% of
correspondence skills (see McClannahan & sessions for each condition for all participants.
Krantz, 1999, for a list of additional prerequisite Interobserver agreement was calculated by
skills for activity schedules). We grouped dividing agreements by disagreements plus agree-
participants into three dyads (Arlo and Isis, ments and converting the result to a percentage.
Brad and Michelle, Gus and Olivia) and Interobserver agreement was 96% for Arlo
conducted one to two sessions per day with (range, 88% to 100%), 96% for Isis (range,
each dyad. Sessions occurred at least 30 min 91% to 100%), 97% for Brad (range, 89% to
apart, and each session took place in the 100%), 99% for Michelle (range, 97% to 100%),
participants’ classroom (that included tables 97% for Gus (range, 86% to 100%), and 96% for
and chairs), hallways, and two offices. Olivia (range, 91% to 100%).

Measurement Materials
During every session, we videotaped partic- Each participant had his or her own linked
ipants from at least 2 m away. After the session, activity schedule that consisted of a small three-
the first author scored each participant’s engage- ring binder that contained four laminated pages.

Table 1
Hider and Seeker Response Chain

Condition Hider Seeker


a a
Beginning Open schedule Open schedule
Middle Attend to hiding placea Say, “go hide!”
Put “oh, no!” script on wrist Look at peer
Arrive at hiding place Count from 1 to 20
Wait at hiding place Put “I found you!” script on wrista
Grab searching cuea
Search for peer
Say, “oh, no!” Say, “I found you!”
Return to schedulea Return to schedulea
Place “oh, no!” script on schedulea Place “I found you!” script on schedulea
Return searching cuesa
Turn pagea Turn pagea
End Say, “Thanks for playing!” Say, “Thanks for playing!”

Note. Each participant played each role twice.


a
Denotes schedule behavior only.
LINKED ACTIVITY SCHEDULES 3

Figure 1. The pages for the hider (left) and seeker (right).

Two pages contained discriminative stimuli for During all sessions, participants wore small
hider behavior, and two pages contained stimuli plastic watches with Velcro attached. The watches
for seeker behavior (see Figure 1). Therefore, each allowed them to place the “oh, no!” and “I found
participant had the opportunity to play the hider you!” scripts on their wrists while they engaged in
and seeker roles twice each, for a total of four hider or seeker roles, respectively.
independent games per session. The hider pages
had a green background, and the seeker pages had Procedure
a blue background. We alternated the pages in Before baseline, we taught picture–location
each binder so that one participant had a hider correspondence to each participant by placing
role and one participant had a seeker role each each location picture in front of the participant
time the game was played. The first hider page and saying, “go here.” The participant was
contained a picture of a hiding place and the physically prompted to engage in a correct
script “oh, no!” to promote a reciprocal social response if he or she did not respond within 3 s
interaction between participants when the hider or responded incorrectly. We established appro-
was found. The second hider page contained two priate vocal responding to each script by teaching
pictures of possible hiding places with the word the responses “I found you!,” “oh, no!,” “go
“choice” along with the script “oh, no!” Both hide!,” and “thanks for playing” with a 3-s
seeker pages contained the script “go hide!” They constant delay vocal prompt. We also taught each
also contained a script with the numbers 1 participant to touch or count out loud from 1 to
through 20, the script “I found you,” and a 20, in order, using physical or vocal prompting,
cardboard strip that contained two picture respectively. Baseline data collection began when
locations to cue searching behavior. The pictures participants responded correctly to each of these
and scripts were affixed to each page with Velcro. target skills with 80% accuracy or better, without
4 MATTHEW T. BRODHEAD et al.

response prompts, for three consecutive training Then, the hider was physically prompted to go to
sessions. his or her hiding location. Next, the seeker was
The baseline, no-schedule probe, and schedule vocally prompted to count from 1 to 20 (Arlo, Isis,
probe phases lasted 10 min each. All other Michelle, Gus, and Olivia) or physically prompted
sessions lasted 3 to 7 min (session duration to touch each number (Brad). Next, the seeker was
decreased as participants learned to respond physically prompted to put the script “I found
correctly and thus required less prompting). All you!” on his or her watch and to take the cardboard
sessions began with the instruction, “It is time to strip with the searching cues. The seeker was
play hide and seek.” Instruction for the baseline physically prompted to arrive at each location on
and no-schedule probe conditions also included the searching cue, starting with the left picture
the instructions, “[Child’s name], you are the first. If the hider was not in that location, the
hider; [child’s name], you are the seeker. Go!” seeker was physically prompted to touch the next
These additional instructions were necessary picture and arrive at that location. When the
because the schedules were not available to seeker located the hider, the seeker was vocally
determine hider and seeker roles during these prompted to say “I found you!” and the hider was
conditions. We used a nonconcurrent multiple vocally prompted to say “oh, no!” (in that order).
baseline design across dyads to assess the effects of Then, both participants were physically prompted
the linked activity schedules. For teaching, to return to their schedules, return their scripts
resequencing, and the novel location phases, and searching cues to the binder, and turn the
stability was defined as at least three sessions at page. At this point, the previous hider now had
80% accuracy or better for both participants. For the seeker role, and the previous seeker now had
all conditions, one teacher was assigned to each the hider role. This sequence continued until each
participant for prompting purposes. After com- participant engaged in each role twice. The session
pletion of each session, reinforcement was ended when each participant turned to the final
provided in the form of praise and a small edible page and engaged in the vocal scripted response
item (e.g., candy, chips). “Thanks for playing!”
Baseline. Each session began with the instruc- During teaching sessions, instructors faded
tion to play the game (as described above). physical prompting as participant responding
Linked activity schedules were not available, and improved. They also began fading the “I found
experimenters ignored problem behavior (e.g., you!” and “oh, no!” scripts, beginning with the
lying on the ground, throwing toys) and attempts last word, after a participant independently read
to interact with adults. the scripts for two consecutive sessions. A word
Schedule probe. Linked activity schedules were was faded from the script after each session in
available, but no prompts were provided. Given which the participant independently read the
that participants were fluent followers of inde- script. The next phase did not begin until all
pendent activity schedules, the purpose of this scripts were faded (i.e., scripts no longer appeared
phase was to measure whether they would follow in the binder). If at any time the participant
the linked activity schedules without additional responded incorrectly or out of order, prompts
instruction. were provided until the correct response
Teaching. Graduated guidance (MacDuff et al., occurred.
1993) was used to teach the appropriate sequence Resequencing. This phase was identical to the
of behaviors and vocal prompting to occasion teaching phase, except that a random-number
scripted responding. After the session started and generator was used to designate hider and seeker
the participants had opened their schedules, the locations. Which participant started as the hider
seeker was vocally prompted to say “go hide!” or seeker was also alternated. If a participant
LINKED ACTIVITY SCHEDULES 5

engaged in an incorrect response, physical Baseline Probe Teaching Resequence N.S. Novel Location Assessment

100
prompts were provided until the correct response Arlo

occurred. The purpose of this phase was to 80

demonstrate that the pictures had discriminative


60
control over responding.
No-schedule probe. This phase was identical to 40
baseline. The purpose of this phase was to Isis

measure responding in the absence of the activity 20

schedules and to act as a brief reversal within the 0


Arlo and Isis

multiple baseline design. 1 6 11 16 21 26 31

Percentage of Correct Responses


Michelle
Novel location assessment. This phase was 100

identical to the resequencing phase, except that 80


a random-number generator was used to desig-
nate pictures with up to four novel hider and 60

seeker locations. No pictures from previous Brad


40
phases were used.
20

Brad and Michelle


RESULTS AND DISCUSSION 0
1 6 11 16 21 26 31
Olivia

For all participants, appropriate game play did 100


not occur during baseline or schedule probe
sessions (see Figure 2). Instead, participants 80

engaged in off-task behaviors (e.g., crawling on 60


the floor, taking toys from other students) that Gus

did not resemble appropriate hide-and-seek 40

interactions. When graduated guidance and vocal


20
prompting were introduced during the teaching
Gus and Olivia
phase, Arlo and Isis met the 80% stability criteria 0
1 6 11 16 21 26 31
in eight sessions, Brad and Michelle in 10 Sessions
sessions, and Gus and Olivia in seven sessions.
During the resequencing phase, responding was Figure 2. The results for Arlo and Isis (top), Brad and
maintained above 80% for all participants except Michelle (middle), and Gus and Olivia (bottom). N.S. ¼ the
Brad, whose responding dropped to 79% for the no-schedule probe phase.
second and fourth sessions. During the no-
schedule probe phase, responding resembled that strated that linked activity schedules can control
of baseline conditions for all participants (e.g., social interactions during a less structured play
crawling on the floor, refusing to play with the activity. The fact that responding was not
other participant, etc.). Target responding was maintained when the schedule was removed
maintained above 80% for the novel location could be problematic if the primary goal of the
assessment for all participants. intervention is for participants to play the game in
Our study extends the research on activity the absence of the schedule. On the other hand,
schedules by demonstrating that two indepen- for many individuals with autism and related
dent activity schedules may be linked to promote disabilities, activity schedules could be considered
appropriate play between children with autism a type of permanent support, such as a planner,
during a social game. In addition, we demon- that is not meant to be faded. Although no efforts
6 MATTHEW T. BRODHEAD et al.

were made to fade the activity schedules and be useful to examine the effects of these
transfer stimulus control to the natural environ- procedures on more complex social games and
ment in the current study, this is an obvious activities that require back-and-forth interaction
avenue for future research. between participants. Investigation of the under-
There are some limitations of our study that are lying mechanisms that account for the effective-
worth mentioning. First, we did not collect ness of activity schedules also will be a useful line
fidelity data on the implementation and fading of of research. Finally, future researchers may
prompts, because this was a dynamic process. examine strategies to establish game play itself
Because we used various instructors over the as a reinforcing activity.
course of the study, it is possible that slight
procedural variations occurred. Second, response
measures during baseline might have been REFERENCES
artificially low due to the sequenced nature of Betz, A., Higbee, T. S., & Reagon, K. A. (2008). Using joint
the behavior. Specifically, if the participant did not activity schedules to promote peer engagement in
engage in the first correct response in the response preschoolers with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 41, 237–241. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2008.41-237
chain, he or she did not have the opportunity to Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007).
engage in subsequent responses in the chain, even Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River,
if those skills were present in the repertoire. Third, NJ: Pearson Education.
we tested the intervention in only one setting (a Koyama, T., & Wang, H. (2011). Use of activity schedule to
promote independent performance of individuals with
classroom), so the extent to which behavior would autism and other intellectual disabilities: A review.
generalize to other play settings is unknown. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 2235–2242.
Anecdotally, a few participants occasionally doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.05.003
MacDuff, G. S., Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. (1993).
engaged in novel social interactions (e.g., “Ready Teaching children with autism to use photographic
or not, here I come”), so future researchers may activity schedules: Maintenance and generalization of
wish to examine how to promote novel responses complex response chains. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 26, 89–97. doi: 10.1901/jaba.1993.26-89
when children are using activity schedules. McClannahan, L. E., & Krantz, P. J. (1999). Activity
Researchers may also wish to examine the utility schedules for children with autism: Teaching independent
of linked activity schedules to promote social behavior. Bethesda, MD: Woodbine House.
interactions among more than two peers as well as
whether schedules can be used to promote social
Received May 15, 2013
interaction over more extended time periods than Final acceptance February 27, 2014
were examined in the current study. It also would Action Editor, Jennifer Austin

You might also like