You are on page 1of 62

eTextbook 978-0199731763 The Oxford

Handbook of Sport and Performance


Psychology (Oxford Library of
Psychology)
Visit to download the full and correct content document:
https://ebookmass.com/product/etextbook-978-0199731763-the-oxford-handbook-of-s
port-and-performance-psychology-oxford-library-of-psychology/
O X F O R D L I B R A R Y O F P S YC H O L O G Y

The Oxford Library of Psychology, a landmark series of handbooks, is published by


Oxford University Press, one of the world’s oldest and most highly respected pub-
lishers, with a tradition of publishing significant books in psychology. The ambi-
tious goal of the Oxford Library of Psychology is nothing less than to span a vibrant,
wide-ranging field and, in so doing, to fill a clear market need.
Encompassing a comprehensive set of handbooks, organized hierarchically, the
Library incorporates volumes at different levels, each designed to meet a distinct
need. At one level are a set of handbooks designed broadly to survey the major sub-
fields of psychology; at another are numerous handbooks that cover important cur-
rent focal research and scholarly areas of psychology in depth and detail. Planned
as a reflection of the dynamism of psychology, the Library will grow and expand as
psychology itself develops, thereby highlighting significant new research that will
impact on the field. Adding to its accessibility and ease of use, the Library will be
published in print and, later on, electronically.
The Library surveys psychology’s principal subfields with a set of handbooks
that capture the current status and future prospects of those major subdisciplines.
This initial set includes handbooks of social and personality psychology, clinical
psychology, counseling psychology, school psychology, educational psychology,
industrial and organizational psychology, cognitive psychology, cognitive neuro-
science, methods and measurements, history, neuropsychology, personality assess-
ment, developmental psychology, and more. Each handbook undertakes to review
one of psychology’s major subdisciplines with breadth, comprehensiveness, and
exemplary scholarship. In addition to these broadly conceived volumes, the Library
also includes a large number of handbooks designed to explore in depth more spe-
cialized areas of scholarship and research, such as stress, health and coping, anxiety
and related disorders, cognitive development, or child and adolescent assessment.
In contrast to the broad coverage of the subfield handbooks, each of these latter
volumes focuses on an especially productive, more highly focused line of scholar-
ship and research. Whether at the broadest or most specific level, however, all of the
Library handbooks offer synthetic coverage that reviews and evaluates the relevant
past and present research and anticipates research in the future. Each handbook in
the Library includes an introductory and concluding chapters written by its editor
to provide a roadmap to the handbook’s table of contents and to offer informed
anticipations of significant future developments in that field.
An undertaking of this scope calls for handbook editors and chapter authors who
are established scholars in the areas about which they write. Many of the nation’s and
world’s most productive and best-respected psychologists have agreed to edit Library
handbooks or write authoritative chapters in their areas of expertise.

vii
For whom has the Oxford Library of Psychology been written? Because of its
breadth, depth, and accessibility, the Library serves a diverse audience, including
graduate students in psychology and their faculty mentors, scholars, researchers,
and practitioners in psychology and related fields. Each will find in the Library the
information he or she seeks on the subfield or focal area of psychology in which he
or she works or are interested.
Befitting its commitment to accessibility, each handbook includes a compre-
hensive index, as well as extensive references to help guide research. And because
the Library was designed from its inception as an online as well as a print resource,
its structure and contents will be readily and rationally searchable online. Further,
once the Library is released online, the handbooks will be regularly and thoroughly
updated.
In summary, the Oxford Library of Psychology will grow organically to provide a
thoroughly informed perspective on the field of psychology, one that reflects both
psychology’s dynamism and its increasing interdisciplinarity. Once published elec-
tronically, the Library is also destined to become a uniquely valuable interactive
tool, with extended search and browsing capabilities. As you begin to consult this
handbook, we sincerely hope you will share our enthusiasm for the more than 500-
year tradition of Oxford University Press for excellence, innovation, and quality, as
exemplified by the Oxford Library of Psychology.

Peter E. Nathan
Editor-in-Chief
Oxford Library of Psychology

viii ox f o rd l i bra ry o f ps yc h olog y


A B O U T T H E E D I TO R

Shane M. Murphy
Shane M. Murphy, Ph.D., is a licensed psychologist and Professor in Psychology at
Western Connecticut State University in Danbury, Connecticut, where he teaches
sport, health, and performance psychology. Dr. Murphy served as head of the
United States Olympic Committee’s Sport Psychology Department from 1987 to
1994, and from 1992 to 1994 he was Associate Director of its Division of Sport
Science & Technology.
Shane is a past-President and also a fellow of the Division of Sport and Exercise
Psychology of the American Psychological Association. His books include Sport
Psychology Interventions, The Sport Psych Handbook, The Achievement Zone, and The
Tears and the Cheers. He has published 17 book chapters on sport psychology and
human performance, and over 30 refereed articles. Shane serves on the Editorial
Boards of the International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, the Journal of
Clinical Sport Psychology, and the Journal of Media Psychology. His research includes
the study of imagery and performance, the assessment of psychological skills for
managing high performance, and analysis of the psychology of participation in
video game play.
In addition to working with the world’s top athletes, Shane is a performance
consultant to many companies and organizations and has worked with Royal Bank
of Scotland, Pepsi, Bristol-Meyers-Squibb, Siemens-Rolm, Deutsche Bank and
with a number of performing artists, directors, musicians, and arts groups. He is
married to wife Annemarie, and has two children.

ix
This page intentionally left blank
CO N T R I B U TO R S

Bruce Abernethy Packianathan Chelladurai


Faculty of Health Sciences School of Physical Activity and Educational
The University of Queensland Services
Brisbane, Australia The Ohio State University
Mark B. Andersen Columbus, Ohio
Institute of Sport, Exercise and Active Dave Collins
Living Institute of Coaching and Performance
Victoria University University of Central Lancashire
Melbourne, Australia Grey Matters Performance
Mark R. Beauchamp Lancashire, United Kingdom
School of Kinesiology Jean Côté
University of British Columbia School of Kinesiology and Health Studies
Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada Queen’s University
Stuart J. H. Biddle Kingston, Ontario, Canada
School of Sport, Exercise, and Health Jennifer Cumming
Sciences School of Sport and Exercise Sciences
Loughborough University University of Birmingham
Loughborough, United Kingdom Birmingham, United Kingdom
Nicole D. Bolter Julie P. Douglas
Department of Kinesiology School of Sport, Exercise, and Health
San Francisco State University Sciences
San Francisco, California Loughborough University
Charles Brown Loughborough, United Kingdom
Director, Get Your Head in the Game Kate I. Goodger
Charlotte, North Carolina English Institute of Sport
Christopher M. Carr The High Performance Centre
St. Vincent Sports Performance Bisham Abbey National Sports Centre
Indianapolis, Indiana Nr Marlow, Buckinghamshire
Sam Carr Trish Gorely
Department of Education School of Sport, Exercise, and Health
University of Bath Sciences
Bath, United Kingdom Loughborough University
Albert V. Carron Loughborough, United Kingdom
University of Western Ontario Daniel Gould
London, Ontario, Canada Institute for the Study of Youth Sports
Kathleen V. Casto Michigan State University
Department of Graduate Psychology East Lansing, Michigan
James Madison University
Harrisonburg, Virginia

xi
Christy Greenleaf Martin I. Jones
Department of Kinesiology, Health Department of Sport and Exercise
Promotion, and Recreation Psychology
University of North Texas University of Gloucestershire
Denton, Texas Gloucestershire, United Kingdom
Douglas M. Hankes Sara Kamin
Student Counseling Services From The Heart Psychotherapy
Auburn University Toronto, Ontario, Canada
Auburn, Alabama Maria Kavussanu
Robert J. Harmison School of Sport and Exercise Sciences
Department of Graduate Psychology University of Birmingham
James Madison University Birmingham, United Kingdom
Harrisonburg, Virginia Lindsay E. Kipp
Chris G. Harwood School of Kinesiology
School of Sport, Exercise, and Health University of Minnesota, Twin Cities
Sciences Minneapolis, Minnesota
Loughborough University Anthony P. Kontos
Loughborough, United Kingdom UPMC Sports Medicine Concussion
Antonis Hatzigeorgiadis Program
Department of Physical Education and Department of Orthopaedic Surgery
Sport Sciences University of Pittsburgh School of
University of Thessaly Medicine
Trikala, Greece Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania
Kate F. Hays Alan S. Kornspan
Director, The Performing Edge Department of Sport Science and Wellness
Toronto, Ontario, Canada Education
John Heil University of Akron
Psychological Health Roanoke Akron, Ohio
Roanoke, Virginia Justine K. Lai
Tom Hildebrandt Appearance and Performance Enhancing
Appearance and Performance Enhancing Drug Use Program
Drug Use Program Mount Sinai School of Medicine
Mount Sinai School of Medicine New York, New York
New York, New York Barbi Law
Ben Jackson Schulich School of Education
School of Sport Science, Exercise and Nipissing University
Health North Bay, Ontario, Canada
University of Western Australia Todd Loughead
Perth, Australia Department of Kinesiology
Graham Jones University of Windsor
Top Performance Consulting, Ltd. Windsor, Ontario, Canada
Wokingham, United Kingdom Luc Martin
Marc V. Jones Department of Kinesiology
Centre for Sport Health and Exercise University of Western Ontario
Research London, Ontario, Canada
Staffordshire University
Staffordshire, United Kingdom

xii co n t r i bu tors
Rich Masters Kirsten Peterson
Institute of Human Performance Performance Psychology
University of Hong Kong Australian Institute of Sport
Hong Kong SAR, China Canberra, Australia
Sean McCann Albert J. Petitpas
Senior Sport Psychologist Department of Psychology
United States Olympic Committee Springfield College
Colorado Springs, Colorado Springfield, Massachusetts
Penny McCullagh Trent A. Petrie
Department of Kinesiology Department of Psychology
California State University, East Bay University of North Texas
Hayward, California Denton, Texas
Nikola Medic Leslie Podlog
School of Exercise and Health Sciences Department of Exercise and Sport Science
Edith Cowan University University of Utah
Perth, Australia Salt Lake City, Utah
Joseph P. Mills Emily A. Roper
Faculty of Physical Education and Department of Health and Kinesiology
Recreation Sam Houston State University
University of Alberta Huntsville, Texas
Edmonton, Alberta, Canada Martyn Standage
Antoinette M. Minniti Department for Health
School of Science and Technology University of Bath
Nottingham Trent University Bath, United Kingdom
Nottingham, United Kingdom Diane Ste-Marie
Zella E. Moore School of Human Kinetics
Department of Psychology University of Ottawa
Manhattan College Ottawa, Ontario, Canada
New York, New York Joachim Stoeber
Aidan Moran School of Psychology
School of Psychology University of Kent
University College Dublin Canterbury, United Kingdom
Dublin, Ireland William B. Strean
Katie L. Morton Faculty of Physical Education and
Department of Public Health and Recreation
Primary Care University of Alberta
University of Cambridge Edmonton, Alberta, Canada
Cambridge, United Kingdom Yannis Theodorakis
Bryan P. Murphy Department of Physical Education and
University of Connecticut Sport Sciences
Storrs, Connecticut University of Thessaly
Shane M. Murphy Trikala, Greece
Department of Psychology Taunya Marie Tinsley
Western Connecticut State University Department of Counselor Education
Danbury, Connecticut California University of Pennsylvania
Sanna M. Nordin-Bates California, Pennsylvania
Department of Performance and Training
Swedish School of Sport and Health Sciences
Stockholm, Sweden

cont ri bu tors xiii


Eleanna Varangis Mark R. Wilson
Appearance and Performance Enhancing University of Exeter
Drug Use Program Exeter, United Kingdom
Mount Sinai School of Medicine E. Missy Wright
New York, New York Institute for the Study of Youth Sports
Amy S. Walker Michigan State University
Independent Practice Success in a Different East Lansing, Michigan
Language Bradley W. Young
Rockville, Maryland School of Human Kinetics
Maureen R. Weiss University of Ottawa
School of Kinesiology Ottawa, Canada
University of Minnesota, Twin Cities Nikos Zourbanos
Minneapolis, Minnesota Department of Physical Education and
Sarah E. Williams Sport Sciences
University of Birmingham University of Thessaly
Birmingham, United Kingdom Trikala, Greece

xiv co n t r i bu tors
CONTENTS

Preface xix

Part One • The Nature and Scope of Sport and


Performance Psychology
1. History of Sport and Performance Psychology 3
Alan S. Kornspan
2. The Psychology of Performance in Sport and Other Domains 24
Kate F. Hays
3. Sport and Performance Psychology: Ethical Issues 46
Douglas M. Hankes
4. The Role of Superior Performance Intelligence in Sustained Success 62
Graham Jones
5. Performance Psychology in the Performing Arts 81
Sanna M. Nordin-Bates

Part Two • Individual Psychological Processes in Performance


6. Concentration: Attention and Performance 117
Aidan Moran
7. Conscious and Unconscious Awareness in Learning and Performance 131
Rich Masters
8. Emotion Regulation and Performance 154
Marc V. Jones
9. Anxiety: Attention, the Brain, the Body, and Performance 173
Mark R. Wilson
10. Cognitions: Self-Talk and Performance 191
Yannis Theodorakis, Antonis Hatzigeorgiadis, and Nikos Zourbanos
11. The Role of Imagery in Performance 213
Jennifer Cumming and Sarah E. Williams
12. Motivation: Self-Determination Theory and Performance in Sport 233
Martyn Standage
13. Modeling and Performance 250
Penny McCullagh, Barbi Law, and Diane Ste-Marie
14. Efficacy Beliefs and Human Performance: From Independent Action
to Interpersonal Functioning 273
Mark R. Beauchamp, Ben Jackson, and Katie L. Morton
15. Perfectionism and Performance 294
Joachim Stoeber

xv
Part Three • Social Psychological Processes in Performance
16. Teamwork and Performance 309
Albert V. Carron, Luc J. Martin, and Todd M. Loughead
17. Leadership and Manifestations of Sport 328
Packianathan Chelladurai
18. The Psychology of Coaching 343
Daniel Gould and E. Missy Wright
19. Moral Behavior in Sport 364
Maria Kavussanu
20. Gender, Identity, and Sport 384
Emily A. Roper
21. Relationships and Sport and Performance 400
Sam Carr
22. Culture/Ethnicity and Performance 418
Anthony P. Kontos

Part Four • Human Development and Performance


23. A Developmental Approach to Sport Expertise 435
Jean Côté and Bruce Abernethy
24. Training for Life: Optimizing Positive Youth Development through
Sport and Physical Activity 448
Maureen R. Weiss, Lindsay E. Kipp, and Nicole D. Bolter
25. Talent Development: The Role of the Family 476
Chris G. Harwood, Julie P. Douglas, and Antoinette M. Minniti
26. Expert Masters Sport Performers: Perspectives on Age-Related
Processes, Skill Retention Mechanisms, and Motives 493
Bradley W. Young and Nikola Medic
27. Transitions: Ending Active Involvement in Sports and Other
Competitive Endeavors 513
Albert J. Petitpas, Taunya Marie Tinsley, and Amy S. Walker

Part Five • Interventions in Sport and Performance Psychology


28. Counseling Performers in Distress 527
Zella E. Moore
29. Appearance- and Performance-Enhancing Drug Use 545
Thomas B. Hildebrandt, Eleanna Varangis, and Justine K. Lai
30. Burnout: A Darker Side to Performance 562
Kate I. Goodger and Martin I. Jones
31. The Body and Performance 581
William B. Strean and Joseph P. Mills
32. Injury and Performance 593
John Heil and Leslie Podlog
33. Pain and Performance 618
John Heil and Leslie Podlog

x vi co n t e n ts
34. Eating Disorders in Sport 635
Trent A. Petrie and Christy Greenleaf
35. Physical Activity Interventions 660
Stuart J. H. Biddle and Trish Gorely
36. The Role of the Sport and Performance Psychologist with the
Coach and Team: Implications for Performance Counseling 676
Christopher M. Carr
37. The Performance Coach 692
Dave Collins and Sara Kamin
38. Optimal Performance: Elite Level Performance in “The Zone” 707
Robert J. Harmison and Kathleen V. Casto
39. Supervision and Mindfulness in Sport and Performance Psychology 725
Mark B. Andersen

Part Six • Future Directions


40. Sport and Performance Psychology: A Look Ahead 741
Kirsten Peterson, Charles Brown, Sean McCann, and Shane M. Murphy

Index 755

cont e nts xvii


This page intentionally left blank
P R E FA C E

From 2009 to 2011, my scholarly life [SM] has revolved around editing this volume, The
Oxford Handbook of Sport and Performance Psychology. It has been a very exciting time. From
the beginning, the Handbook was built on the principle of examining the broad spectrum
of research and practice on the psychology of excellent performance, in sport and in many
other domains. To my knowledge, this is the first volume to attempt such an enterprise,
and I owe my contributing authors an enormous debt of gratitude for tackling this project
with such enthusiasm, perseverance and, yes, excellence. I am sure they began to tire of my
hands-on approach as I questioned, probed, and pushed them to address the broadest pos-
sible range of performance psychology issues. They answered the challenge magnificently,
and my excitement grew as I read each new chapter and realized the great depth and scope
of the research being presented. There are so many chapters in this Handbook that are truly
groundbreaking, bringing together knowledge from a variety of domains and sources and
looking at the question of the psychology of excellent performance in innovative ways. I am
confident that you will experience this tangible sense of excitement as you read the chapters
of the Handbook.
As I look back over the past two decades of work in sport and performance psychology,
it is interesting to note the gradual focusing of attention on performance issues. As the field
has matured, there has been a surge of interest in the psychological factors that influence
performance and enable high levels of performance to occur. As a result, there has been much
debate over the essence of the field and appropriate areas of emphasis. Where once an inter-
est in sport and physical activity chiefly defined the field, a movement has been under way
to instead define sport and performance psychology as primarily reflecting interest in issues
of performance, in sport and other domains. This trend can clearly be seen in several recent
developments. For example, within the Oxford Library of Psychology, a separate volume is
being published under the editorship of my colleague Ed Acevedo, The Oxford Handbook of
Exercise Psychology. Publishing separate volumes on exercise and on sport and performance
reflects the reality that research in these fields has focused on different issues and that there
has been such a great amount of research in both areas that trying to combine them would
do a disservice to both. Another example is the appearance of the new Journal of Sport,
Exercise and Performance Psychology, due to begin publication by the American Psychological
Association in 2012, under the editorship of Jeffrey Martin. This is the first academic jour-
nal to use the title “performance psychology,” and although it includes all three interest
areas, it acknowledges that: “The third and unique branch of Sport, Exercise and Performance
Psychology reflects the growing interest in performance with strong physical elements by indi-
viduals who cannot be viewed as either athletes or exercisers” (Martin, 2011, p. 1).
In researching the debate on the appropriate focus for our field, I find it fascinat-
ing that I first addressed this issue in print 16 years ago, when I edited Sport Psychology
Interventions (Murphy, 1995) for Human Kinetics Publishers. In my introductory chapter
to that volume, I discussed proposals that practitioners move away from defining them-
selves as “sport psychologists” and establish a separate field of “performance enhance-
ment.” I concluded that there was little to be gained from such an approach and much to
be lost. My summary of the argument is still convincing to my modern self:
It seems unwise to deny the historical and theoretical roots of such [performance enhancement]
approaches. The danger of “reinventing the wheel” is apparent. As this book illustrates, there

xix
is much more to be gained from recognizing the intellectual roots of the field and drawing on
them in fresh and creative ways. The variety of intervention models described in this book is a
testament to the vibrancy of the field of psychology and to the opportunities for incorporating a
variety of approaches into the applied practice of sport psychology.
(p. 11)

It seems clear that the resolution of this issue, established in the following years, has
indeed been to reject dividing sport psychology and performance psychology into separate
fields, and instead a consensus has emerged among researchers and practitioners that the
best approach is to regard sport and performance as complementary foci of attention. Thus,
the term “sport and performance psychology” has gained widespread acceptance. For those
readers who wish to consider these issues in greater depth, many excellent discussions are
contained in this volume. I especially recommend the wonderful overview chapter by Kate
Hays, and the critical analysis offered by Dave Collins and Sara Kamin in The Performance
Coach chapter.
I believe we still have work to do in bringing clarity to these issues. One issue I encourage
researchers and practitioners to consider is the question of the primacy of performance as a
focus of both research and intervention. There are those who argue that a focus on improv-
ing performance is the defining feature of sport and performance psychology (Newburg,
1992), those who see performance as just one possible focus of intervention (along with
others such as health or happiness, c.f., Whelan, Meyers, & Donovan, 1995), and those
who see a focus on performance as a dead end for the field, one that distracts us from help-
ing the whole person (Andersen, 2009). Having spent much of the past 2 years considering
this issue, I believe that there may be more agreement among these competing viewpoints
than is recognized. Simply put, I think that a focus on performance is the central focus of
sport and performance psychology, but that there is no reason to think that such a focus
is antithetical to a consideration of the needs of the individual in the context of his or her
entire life. Sport and performance psychology has evolved toward a holistic approach that
considers the life of the whole person while nonetheless maintaining the central focus on
performance itself. There are no life issues than cannot potentially impact performance
for good or ill, and therefore there are no issues that are out of bounds for consideration
by the effective sport and performance psychology practitioner. For example, important
issues such as injury, an eating disorder, a troubled family relationship, or a deleterious ten-
dency toward perfectionism (all of which are covered in chapters in this Handbook) may
all interfere with an athlete’s or performing artist’s performance. Thus, the skilled sport
and performance psychology practitioner will identify the key issues and intervene appro-
priately in each such situation. In some cases, this may result in a referral out or bringing
a colleague into the consultation. In some cases, the resulting interventions may include
some or many aspects of traditional psychotherapy. Yet, I would maintain that performance
issues can remain the focus of intervention. There are many intervention approaches and
strategies that the skilled performance consultant can utilize to effect change—but it is not
the techniques and strategies that define performance psychology. I have seen the argument
that consultations can move from a performance focus to a clinical focus and back again,
and that such moves are reflected in changes from using psychological skills training (PST)
to doing psychotherapy, but I reject this argument. Psychological skills training is simply
a collection of intervention techniques, not a framework for consultation. If issues (e.g.,
alcoholism, obsessive-compulsive disorder, marital distress) arise that interfere with perfor-
mance, they must be dealt with, and psychotherapy may be the best technique to employ
to improve performance. Thus, it is not a matter of moving from a focus on performance to
a focus on health or vice versa, but a matter of moving between different techniques in the
pursuit of improved performance.
We believe there are strong theoretical and historical justifications for this approach.
From the beginning, history’s great thinkers have always considered worldly performance

xx p re fac e
as an important part of a complete human life; it is difficult to find a great thinker who
has scorned performance. Even the Eastern traditions, often regarded as antithetical to
achievement and worldly striving, offer support for one who believes in the importance
of human performance. Siddhartha Guatama, the Buddha, who is widely misperceived in
the West as advocating a recluse’s detachment from worldly things, joyfully propounded
the central importance of human performance. Right Effort, the energetic will to per-
form rightly in the world, is one of the spokes of the central Eightfold Path. Further, the
Buddha maintained that one of the four keys to a layperson’s happiness in life is excellent
accomplishment born of persistent effort—that by “whatsoever activity a householder earns
his living [ . . . ] at that he becomes skillful [ . . . and knows and performs] the proper ways
and means.” Not even the Buddha, then, who said that all attachment and desire is dukkha
[suffering] and should be eliminated (which seems so shocking to anyone with the Western
“thirst to win” mentality) was opposed to the study and improvement of human perfor-
mance (Rahula, 1974).
The great scholar Confucius, whose ideals have shaped world history in inestimable ways,
was not a particular fan of athletic performance, castigating in his Analects two Chinese
princes who focused their efforts on the disciplines of archery and rowing, respectively
(Confucius, 1995). Yet, although he disdained sport, Confucius considered human perfor-
mance so important that he flatly decreed that “if a man has reached forty or fifty without
being heard of [acquired a good reputation through his performance in proper duties], he,
indeed, is incapable of commanding respect!”
If we look to the Western philosophical traditions, it is even harder to find a great thinker
who would say that human performance is not of central importance. Perhaps the closest we
can come are the Greco-Roman philosophers of the Stoic school, famous for their assertion
that virtue alone is sufficient for happiness and that all worldly goods, honors, and accolades
are unnecessary. But even among the Stoics, the absolute importance of hard-won excep-
tional performance was accepted. Epictetus, the liberated slave whose handbook is one of the
most widely read reviews of Stoic philosophy, tells us that we must not only engage in but
succeed in what we turn our efforts toward in this world, for it is “impious for a man to with-
draw himself from being useful to those who have need of our services.” The key, Epictetus
asserts, is that we must execute our performances mindfully, as he makes clear when he says,
“A man wishes to conquer at the Olympic Games. I also wish indeed, for it is a fine thing.
But observe both the things which come first, and the things which follow; and then begin
to act.” In fact, Epictetus the Stoic delivers one of the most remarkable defenses of hard-won,
true-grit performance in the history of early Western thought. Epictetus tells us that we must
pursue our loftiest, most difficulty won ideals “as if they were laws, as if you would be guilty
of impiety if you transgressed any of them.” He then goes on to tell his reader that:

You are no longer a youth, but already a full grown man. If then you are negligent and slothful,
and are continually making procrastination after procrastination, and proposal after proposal,
and fixing day after day, after which you will attend to yourself, you will not know that you are
not making improvement, but you will continue ignorant both while you live and till you die.
Immediately then think it right to live as a full-grown man, and one who is making proficiency,
and let every thing which appears to you to be the best be to you a law which must not be
transgressed. And if anything laborious, or pleasant or glorious or inglorious be presented to you,
remember that now is the contest, now are the Olympic Games, and they cannot be deferred;
and that it depends on one defeat and one giving way that progress is either lost or maintained.
(Epictetus, 2004, p. 21)

If we can find such a rousing defense of the importance of performance in the writings
of Epictetus, how much more support can a proponent of performance find in the writings
of Aristotle? Aristotle indisputably places the centrality of performing well in all the activ-
ities of our lives at the very center of his philosophy. Action and performance is key to the

p re face xxi
Aristotelian worldview; “activities are, as we have said, what really matters in life,” Aristotle
writes in his Nicomachean Ethics, and Aristotle means not only activities, but excellently
performed activities. Aristotle is willing to say that “without qualification, in the same way
in every case” the only path to goodness for a man is to excel in performance. He explains,
“for the characteristic activity of the lyre-player is to play the lyre, and that [the character-
istic activity] of the good lyre-player to play it well.” For Aristotle, mere possession of supe-
rior skills is an irrelevant distinction; one must perform well in the world to win happiness.
Echoing our quote of Epictetus, Aristotle writes that, “As in the Olympic Games it is not the
most attractive and the strongest who are crowned, but those who compete (since it is from
this group that winners come), so in life it is those who act rightly who will attain what is
noble and good” (Aristotle, 1962).
The Greco-Roman philosophical tradition has strongly influenced European and American
psychology, and the Aristotelian concept of doing well and living well (eudaimonia, sometimes
translated as human “flourishing”) as the goal of life permeates many psychological theories.
Abraham Maslow placed the esteem needs, such as achievement, on the second highest rung
of his hierarchy of needs, below only that of self-actualization. In recent times, the positive
psychology movement has re-emphasized the centrality of achievement and success within
psychology: “[Positive psychology] provides a means of looking at the people in organiza-
tions that is grounded in the strengths that enable those people to succeed in what they do,
the well-being that they achieve through doing so, and the meaning and fulfilment that gives
coherence and direction to their activities along the way” (Linley, Harrington, & Garcea,
2010, p. xxiii).
But perhaps the simplest argument we can propose is that human performance is still the
essential feature that determines the success of our society. Our performance as individu-
als, families, organizations, communities, and as a nation will determine our success in
dealing with the most important issues of our day, such as social justice, global warming,
the fossil fuel crisis, the health threat of pollution, and developing a sustainable approach
to resource and ecosystem management. It is not hyperbole to suggest that our success or
failure in tackling these challenges will determine the future of our culture and, perhaps,
our very existence as a species.
The organization of this Handbook is, I hope, simple and straightforward. Part one, The
Nature and Scope of Sport and Performance Psychology, contains the most general overview
chapters, addressing the wide range of performance psychology issues being studied today.
Alan Kornspan provides an interesting overview of the historical development of the field;
Kate Hays has written the foundational chapter of the Handbook, incisively delineating
the nature and scope of sport and performance psychology; Doug Hankes contributes an
immensely practical guide to the many ethical issues that arise from practice in our field;
the chapter from Graham Jones is unique, an original piece of research examining the pro-
posed concept of Superior Performance Intelligence as an explanatory and unifying concept
in performance psychology; and the final chapter in this section is a wonderful contribution
to the field, Sanna Nordin-Bates’ truly comprehensive review of the psychological literature
on performance psychology in the performing arts.
Part two of the Handbook deals with Individual Psychological Processes in Performance.
It includes chapters on most of the individual difference variables that have been widely
studied for their relationship with performance. Aidan Moran’s chapter on concentration
is typically thoughtful and complete; Rich Masters provides a fascinating and provoca-
tive chapter studying the role of awareness in consciousness and performance; Marc Jones
contributes a concise and up-to-the-minute report on the role of emotional control in per-
formance; Mark Wilson tackles the issue of anxiety by examining its role in attentional
processes; Yannis Theodorakis, Antonis Hatzigeorgiadis, and Nikos Zourbanos give us
a splendid overview of the research on the concept of self-talk; Jennifer Cumming and
Sarah E. Williams have written a timely review of the role of imagery in performance, uti-
lizing the latest research from cognitive neuroscience to bring together the many research

x x ii p re fac e
strands in this area; the meta-theory of self-determination theory (SDT) provides the uni-
fying framework for Martyn Standage’s excellent critique of current research and practice
in motivation; Penny McCullagh, Barbi Law, and Diane Ste-Marie have contributed an
important chapter on the too-often understudied topic of modeling and performance;
Mark R. Beauchamp, Ben Jackson, and Katie L. Morton provide an informative synthesis
of the research on self-efficacy and performance; and Joachim Stoeber concludes this sec-
tion with his own research and that of many others on the role that perfectionism plays in
excellent performance.
Part three of the Handbook addresses Social Psychological Processes in Performance,
covering those topics that deal with performance in groups, teams, relationships, and even
cultures. Albert V. Carron, Luc Martin, and Todd Loughead lead off this section with a
definitive chapter on the issue of teamwork; Packianathan Chelladurai provides a fresh look
at the issue of leadership with his chapter; Dan Gould and E. Missy Wright have written an
extensive and eminently accessible chapter on the psychology of coaching; Maria Kavussanu
brings clarity to the very important issues of moral behavior in sport; Emily Roper makes
a superb contribution, bringing together scholarship from a wide variety of sources in her
chapter on gender, identity, and sport; Sam Carr provides a much-needed examination of
the role of relationships in performance, offering several fascinating approaches to this area,
including attachment theory, transference, and actor–partner models; and Anthony Kontos
concludes this section by discussing the many significant issues raised when a cultural/
ethnic perspective is taken toward performance.
Part four of the Handbook, Human Development and Performance, looks at human
performance from a developmental perspective, considering such topics as expertise, talent
development, and training throughout the lifespan. Jean Côté and Bruce Abernethy pro-
vide their usual masterful review of the varied literature on sport expertise; Maureen Weiss,
Lindsay Kipp, and Nicole Bolter give us a thoughtful, well-supported chapter on positive
youth development via the pathways of sport and physical activity; Chris Harwood, Julie
Douglas, and Antoinette Minniti provide a thorough and thought-provoking examination
of the role of the family in talent development across performance domains; Bradley Young
and Nikola Medic review a sometimes-neglected literature on expert master-level athletes
and provide a clear pathway for future researchers to follow; and Albert Petitpas, Taunya
Tinsley, and Amy Walker give us a thoughtful reflection on the issue of career transitions in
sports and other competitive endeavors.
Part five of the Handbook addresses Interventions in Sport and Performance Psychology.
Too often, review handbooks of this type offer up the same predictable menu of interven-
tion chapters. This Handbook instead offers a wide-ranging set of chapters that deal with
the real issues faced by consultants who work in high-performance fields in sports, the
arts, and business. Each author is not only a recognized leader in his or her field, but has
also faced the hands-on issues engendered by working in his or her field of expertise. Zella
Moore bats lead-off in this section and delivers a hit, a concise and lucid roadmap to coun-
seling performers in distress, containing a practical dimensional classification system for
performers; Thomas Hildebrandt, Eleanna Varangis, and Justine Lai next provide a com-
pelling account of appearance- and performance-enhancing drug use in sport, describing a
research base that many psychologists are unfamiliar with; Kate Goodger and Martin Jones
examine the issue of burnout in sport and performance, highlighting key areas in which
new applied research is desperately needed; Billy Strean and Joe Mills contribute a ground-
breaking chapter that steps outside the prevailing zeitgeist to examine the phenomenology
of the body in physical activity and suggests how adopting this perspective offers a pathway
to better health and happiness; next comes two chapters authored by John Heil and Leslie
Podlog—the first is a comprehensive overview of the psychological issues of injury and
performance, while the second is another innovative and groundbreaking chapter, one of
the first ever to deal with the psychology of pain and performance; Trent Petrie and Christy
Greenleaf next contribute a sensitive and practical chapter dealing with eating disorders in

p re face xxiii
sport; Stuart Biddle and Trish Gorely review the research on behavior change in physical
activity in a terrific chapter that bridges the gap between exercise and sport psychology;
Chris Carr has written a very practical, systems theory–based guide to organizational con-
sulting with high-performance teams, utilizing his extensive experience in this area; Dave
Collins and Sara Kamin took on one of the toughest assignments in the Handbook, dis-
cussing the objectives, knowledge base, and best practices for “performance coaches”; Bob
Harmison and Kathleen Casto do a great job describing the extensive research on optimal
performance; and Mark Andersen concludes this section in his inimitable style, exploring
supervision issues in sport and performance psychology from a mindfulness perspective.
The final section of the Handbook, Part six, Future Directions, includes only one chap-
ter, but it is a special surprise. Three of the most experienced sport and performance psy-
chologists in the world, Kirsten Peterson, Charles Brown, and Sean McCann, gamely agreed
to peek into the crystal ball and provide a look at the future of sport and performance psy-
chology. Their answers to the questions I posed yield some of the most thought-provoking
discussions in the entire Handbook.
Finally, a very big thank you to all who made this Handbook possible. Peter Nathan,
the Editor-in-Chief of the Oxford Psychology Library, has my gratitude for asking me to
guide such an ambitious project. All the staff at Oxford have been wonderfully support-
ive during this long and tiring process; I wish to thank Mariclaire Cloutier, who provided
invaluable feedback during the planning process, and especially my editor-extraordinaire,
Chad Zimmerman, without whose quiet encouragement and dry wit I might not have
survived this experience. My colleagues at Western Connecticut State University have been
understanding and encouraging throughout, and granting me a sabbatical for a semester
was a godsend. And, of course, my wonderful family: my caring wife Annemarie, the best
psychologist I know; my son Bryan, who co-wrote this Preface; and my wonderful daughter
Theresa, the best scientist in the family. I love you very much.

Shane M. Murphy
Bryan P. Murphy
Danbury, Connecticut
November 2011

References
Andersen, M. (2009). Performance enhancement as a bad start and a dead end: A parenthetical comment on
Mellalieu and Lane. The Sport and Exercise Scientist, 20 (June), 12–14, British Association of Sport and Exercise
Sciences.
Aristotle. (1962). Nicomachean ethics. Translated by Martin Ostwald. Indianapolis, IN: The Bobbs-Merrill
Company.
Confucius. (1995). The Analects. Translated by William Soothill. New York: Dover Publications.
Epictetus. (2004). Enchiridion. Translated by George Long. Minneola, NY: Dover Publications.
Linley, P. A., Harrington, S., & Garcea, N. (2010). Preface. Oxford handbook of positive psychology and work.
New York: Oxford University Press.
Martin, J. (2011). Welcome. Sport, Exercise and Performance Psychology. 1(Suppl), 1–2.
Murphy, S. M. (1995). Introduction to sport psychology interventions. In S. M. Murphy (Ed.), Sport psychology
interventions (pp. 1–15). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Newburg, D. (1992). Performance enhancement: Toward a working definition. Contemporary Thought on
Performance Enhancement, 1, 10–25.
Rahula, W. (1974). What the Buddha taught (2nd edition). New York: Grove Press.
Whelan, J. P., Meyers, A. W., & Donovan, C. (1995). Competitive recreational athletes: A multisystemic model. In
S. M. Murphy (Ed.), Sport psychology interventions (pp. 71–116). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

x x iv p re fac e
PA RT
1
The Nature and
Scope of Sport and
Performance Psychology
This page intentionally left blank
CH APT E R

History of Sport and Performance


1 Psychology

Alan S. Kornspan

Abstract
This chapter presents an overview of the history of sport and performance psychology. First, the
chapter will highlight the beginnings of sport psychology in the 1890s, when psychology was first
applied to sport in laboratory settings. The applications of the “new psychology” applied to sport and
the first sport psychology congress held in Lausanne, Switzerland, in 1913, are described. The chapter
describes how psychologists began testing athletes in psychology laboratories using experimental
psychology methods during 1920–1940. The development of sport psychology laboratories worldwide
and the work of Coleman Griffith are highlighted. Next, the chapter explains the beginnings of the
use of psychology for performance enhancement purposes between 1940 and 1965. The growth
of sport psychology literature during this time is discussed. The chapter then focuses on sport
and performance psychology from 1965 to 1980, including the development of sport psychology
organizations. The chapter concludes by providing an overview of sport and performance psychology
from 1980 to today. The modern period is characterized by an increasing application of sport and
performance psychology knowledge across a variety of performance domains.
Key Words: History, sport psychology, time periods, application, historical development

Researchers in the fields of sport psychology and help enhance athletic performance. First, a histor-
the history of psychology are increasingly interested ical overview of the development of sport psychol-
in the historical development of sport and perfor- ogy from 1890 to 1920 will be detailed. This will
mance psychology (Green & Benjamin, 2009). be followed by information about how psycholog-
Work on the historical development of the field of ical testing began to be used by psychologists and
sport psychology has been published since at least coaches between 1920 and 1940, and this section
the late 1970s (Feige, 1977), but recent analyses also presents an overview of the laboratories devel-
have provided a more thorough understanding of oped specifically for sport psychology research. The
the development of the field. third section, focused on 1940–1965, provides
Recent literature in the history of sport psy- a discussion of the development and use of psy-
chology has discussed the development of sport chology and mental skills training to help athletes
psychology from 1890 to1965, focusing on the enhance performance. Historical developments
development of the profession of sport psychol- that occurred in the field of sport and performance
ogy during this time. Although the field of sport psychology from 1965 to 1980 are discussed, and
psychology compromises various areas, including the chapter concludes by providing an overview of
exercise, health, and participation and motivation, the development of sport and performance psy-
the focus of this chapter is on the historical aspects chology between 1980 and 1989, and from 1990
of using psychology and mental skills training to to the present.

3
The New Psychology: Sport and Performance ways (Fuchs, 1998) using examples of his work and
Psychology from 1890 to 1920 the research that he conducted in the psychology
Few details about sport and exercise psychol- laboratory. One example of his research included
ogy between 1890 and 1920 appeared in the liter- an experiment on the reaction time of fencers, con-
ature until now, when recently published literature ducted in December 1893. Interestingly, at about
has provided a more detailed understanding of this the same time, at Harvard University, G. W. Fitz
period (e.g., Bäumler, 2009). For example, influen- was also studying the reaction time of athletes
tial physical educators and psychologists began to (Kornspan, 2007a).
teach psychology as part of the training of physi- In perhaps one of the first experiments conducted
cal directors;William James, author of the 1892 studying sport in a psychology laboratory, Scripture
Principles of Psychology taught a psychology course to designed an experiment to determine if there were
students in the physical training program at Harvard differences between expert and novice fencers
(Kornspan, 2007a). Additionally, in 1891, Luther (Scripture, 1894a). It is noteworthy that Scripture’s
Gulick, the director of the YMCA training school work was conducted with W. G. Anderson; Anderson
in Springfield, Massachusetts, began teaching a new was an influential physical educator and the founding
seminar in psychology for students studying to be president of the Association for the Advancement of
physical training directors (Naismith, 1941). This Physical Education, in 1885 (Kornspan, 2007a). In
first psychology seminar included James Naismith addition to the sport of fencing, Scripture also stud-
and legendary football coach Amos Alonzo Stagg. ied runners and boxers in the Yale psychology labora-
During this seminar, Gulick challenged the tory during the 1890s (Scripture, 1894b).
physical directors he was training to create a sport Clearly, from Scripture’s writings, he believed
or game that would be interesting, fun, and could that psychology could have an influence on sport
be played inside during the winter; James Naismith performance. He also believed that his work could
rose to the challenge and presented his ideas for a influence physical educators to investigate and
new indoor sport. Months later, Gulick assigned apply psychological findings to the field of sport and
Naismith the task of developing this new game physical training (Kornspan, 2007a). Apparently,
by stating, “Naismith, now would be a good time Scripture also had the opportunity to influence
for you to work on that new game that you said physical educators through the teaching of psychol-
could be invented” (Naismith, 1941, p. 37). Thus, ogy, since he was part of the faculty of the Anderson
it was during the psychology seminar of 1891 that Normal School for Gymnastics (Anderson School
Naismith originally was influenced to develop the of Gymnastics, 1896). Additionally, as a professor
new game of basketball. Additionally, it appears of psychology at Yale University, Scripture taught
that Naismith was so influenced by the field of the course “Psychology (Elementary Laboratory
psychology that he gave a commencement address Course),” which was specifically recommended to
at the YMCA Training School, in 1891, titled the students who had an interest in the psychology
“Psychology of Exercise,” and he taught a psychol- of exercise or physical training (Yale University,
ogy course in the YMCA training school after grad- 1898/1899). For an in-depth description of
uation (Slayer, 2009). Scripture’s work at the Yale psychology laboratory,
In addition to Gulick, James, and Naismith teach- see Kornspan (2007a) and Goodwin (2009).
ing psychology courses in physical director training In addition to recent literature discussing the
schools during this period, E. W. Scripture became work of E. W. Scripture and the “new psychology”
interested in the applications of psychology to daily as applied to sport, recent researchers have written
life (Scripture, 1895). Scripture referred to this area about the work of Norman Triplett (e.g., Davis,
of the field as the “New Psychology.” Historically, Huss, & Becker, 2009; Strube, 2005; Stroebe,
psychology was at a crossroads: some individuals 2012; Vaughan & Guerin, 1997), who published an
believed that psychology should be based on philo- experimental investigation related to the psychology
sophical thought, whereas others, such as Scripture, of sport and social psychology (Triplett, 1898). This
believed that psychology should be based in scien- study, often referred to as the first experimental study
tific evidence gained from studies conducted in the in sport psychology, has been considered by some as
laboratory (Kornspan, 2007a). Scripture brought the birth of the social psychology discipline (Brehm,
attention to these ideas in his 1895 text, The New Kassin, & Fein, 1999). Triplett’s (1898) study
Psychology, which provided examples of how psy- was a part of his master’s degree thesis at Indiana
chology could improve life in a variety of different University (Strube, 2005) and was completed under

4 h i s to ry o f s po rt a n d per fo rma n c e p s ych olog y


the direction of professors W. L. Bryan and J. A. Throughout the early 1900s, Coubertin wrote
Bergstrom (Triplett, 1898). Triplett was interested philosophical essays related to the psychology of
in studying the performance of cyclists: He wanted sport, and a collection of these articles would later
to know if cyclists were more likely to cycle faster if be published as a book, Essais de psychologie sport-
they were riding against a competitor, if they were ive, in 1913 (Coubertin, 1913). These essays were
paced, or if they were riding alone. Interestingly, mainly developed and published in the Olympic
one of the first Europeans to study the psychology Review to help individuals consider the psychology
of sport during the same period, Phillipe Tissie, was of sport as a lead-up to the International Congress of
also studying the psychological aspects of cycling the Psychology and Physiology of Sports. A review
(Bäumler, 2009). of this book appeared in the American Physical
Researchers have recently reanalyzed the results Education Review (Meylan, 1917).
of the Triplett (1898) study (Strube, 2005). Strube Coubertin described the sports psychology con-
has suggested that, based on the results of Triplett’s gress in his Olympic Memoirs. He organized the
study, there was very little evidence to suggest that congress because he believed that the medical pro-
social facilitation existed based on his (2005) re- fession had been too focused on the physiology of
analysis of statistical data using contemporary sta- sports, and he wanted to see the medical profession
tistical methods. Fascinatingly, Triplett’s study of focus additionally on psychology related to sport.
cyclists would not be the only study of athletics and Coubertin presented the idea to develop a sport psy-
games completed in the psychology department chology congress in 1909 (Coubertin, 1936/1979),
at Indiana University during this period. Claude and he received permission to hold the congress in
E. Price completed a master’s degree thesis titled, 1911, at the Budapest meetings. After receiving
“The relation of athletics to scholarship at Indiana permission to assemble the congress, Coubertin
University: A statistical study.” Additionally, Robert focused on convincing the medical profession of its
E. Newland completed a master’s degree thesis titled, importance (Coubertin, 1936/1979).
“The psychology of a game—Checkers” (Indiana The International Congress of the Psychology
University, 1905; Hearst & Capshew, 1988). and Physiology of Sport, or what Coubertin has
Recent sport psychology literature has also referred to in his Olympic memoirs as the Sports
begun to discuss one of the first sport psychology Psychology Congress, took place from May 8 to May
events to be organized by the International Olympic 10, 1913 (Kornspan, 2007b). Approximately 400
Committee, the International Congress of the individuals from throughout the world attended the
Psychology and Physiology of Sport (Bäumler, 2009; congress. Many of the reports were related to phil-
Kornspan, 2007b; Kornspan, 2009b; Silva, 2002). osophical or introspective psychology. For example,
This event was developed through the efforts of the presentations were provided by elite athletes who
founder of the modern Olympic Games, Pierre de discussed what they were thinking during high-level
Coubertin, who organized the modern Games in the performance. Paul Rousseau, a famous cyclist, dis-
early 1890s. He believed that the Olympic Games cussed how positive thoughts could help him over-
should include an educational component and not come fatigue while performing. In addition to these
be simply focused on watching and participating introspective reports, a presentation was provided
in sports (Kornspan, 2007b). Thus, he developed by Jean Phillipe (Kornspan, 2007b), who called
educational congresses to be part of the Olympic for the psychology of sport to be studied in psy-
Games. The first congress to have a psychological chological laboratories. Although many of the pre-
component was held in Le Havre, France, in 1897. sentations focused on the mental aspects of sports,
A part of the congress held in Le Havre focused on a main critique of the congress by those in atten-
the “Psychology of Exercise” (Kornspan, 2007b). dance was related to insufficient scientific evidence
A comparison of the works by Scripture and presented to support the statements made. Despite
Coubertin shows that, whereas Scripture was very the critics, Coubertin would later refer to this event
interested in bringing psychology into the laboratory as “the birth of the psychology of sport” (Müller,
and taking scientific measurements in order to make 1997, p. 54).
data-based recommendations, Coubertin viewed As recent literature in the history of sport psy-
psychology from a philosophical point of view. This chology demonstrates, the works of E. W. Scripture,
would be a significant distinction as Coubertin Pierre de Coubertin, and Norman Triplett are an
developed and planned the International Congress important part of the early history of the field. An
of the Psychology and Physiology of Sport. additional important aspect of this period is the

korns pa n 5
interest psychologists showed in the area of sport, for the American Physical Education Association
physical activity, and human performance. Wilhelm during this time. The Committee of Nineteen (orig-
Wundt, who is considered the father of modern inally the Committee of Fifteen) was formed by the
experimental psychology, was the major professor Advancement of the American Physical Education
for 16 American doctoral students who went on Association in 1899. The purpose of this commit-
to work as professors of psychology in the United tee was to focus on helping elementary, secondary,
States (Benjamin, Durkin, Link, Vestal, & Acord, and higher education institutions understand the
1992). Eight of the 16 American psychologists important role of physical training in education
whom Wundt trained would go on to either con- (American Physical Education Association, 1900).
duct research in physical training/athletics or write Psychologists who were a part of the Committee of
or work in the field of athletics. These psycholo- Nineteen included G. Stanley Hall, Joseph Jastrow,
gists included Frank Angell (who became an ath- E. W. Scripture, James M. Cattell, and William
letic director at Stanford University), James Cattell, James.
Lightner Witmer, Charles Judd, Edward A. Pace, In summary, the early years of sport psychology
Walter Dill Scott, E. W. Scripture, and Harry Kirke saw psychologists and physical educators becom-
Wolfe. ing interested in applying psychology to physi-
James McKeen Cattell conducted a study of how cal training and sport. From the beginning of the
individuals practiced running 3 miles (Fuchs, 1998). experimental study of psychology, psychologists and
E. W. Scripture, along with writing various books physical educators collaborated to determine how
and conducting studies related to sport and physical psychology could be applied to sport and physical
activity, was an active presenter at American Physical education. It is noteworthy that many individuals
Education Association conferences. An abstract trained directly by Wilhelm Wundt had an inter-
of his presentation, “The Psychological Aspects of est in writing about, researching, or working in
Physical Education” was published in the American sport-related jobs. It should be noted that other
Physical Education Review in 1901 (Scripture, 1901). areas of performance psychology also began to be
Lightner Witmer, the founder of the first psychology studied and written about during this period (e.g.,
clinic and the first individual to use the term “clin- Carl E. Seashore’s study of the psychology of music
ical psychology” (Watson, 1956), presented a paper [Seashore, 1919]; Walter Dill Scott’s application of
at the National Education Association conference psychology to business [Scott, 1917]).
and at the Philadelphia Physical Education Society in As this period progressed, suggestions were made
1898 titled, “The Mental Factor in Physical Training” to create laboratories to study the psychology of
(Kuhn, 1898; Stoneroad, 1898). Witmer also pub- football and to study athletes directly in psychology
lished an experimental study of his work titled “The laboratories. The next phase of the development of
Special Class for Backward Children.” A main part of the field of sport psychology saw the actual crea-
this study was to observe how physical training and tion of laboratories specifically devoted to sport
exercise affected those children who participated in psychology.
the study (Witmer, 1911).
Additionally, Edward A. Pace was part of the 1920–1940: Development of Sport
gymnasium committee at Catholic University Psychology Laboratories and Psychological
(“Report of the Temporary Committee on the Testing Applied to Sport
Gymnasium,” Catholic University Bulletin vol- During the decades between 1920 and 1940,
umes 21–23). Walter Dill Scott completed labo- psychologists and physical educators began to study
ratory studies of athletes (Scott, 1917); Scott was the psychological aspects of athletics utilizing exper-
interested in whether coaching could improve the imental laboratory methods. Individuals with an
performance of athletes. Harry Kirke Wolfe had an interest in applying psychology focused on the use
interest in athletics because he served on the fac- of psychological testing and measurement to iden-
ulty athletics committee helping to oversee athletics tify highly skilled athletes. Thus, testing that took
at the University of Nebraska. Additionally, Wolfe place at psychology laboratories involving athletes
may have been one of the first to propose the crea- often focused on identifying those psychological
tion of a laboratory specifically to study the psychol- attributes that helped athletes become successful
ogy of football (Brannon, 1913). (e.g., Fuchs, 1998).
It also should be noted that early psychologists There was a worldwide development of sport
were actively involved in committee membership psychology laboratories in which athletes were

6 h i s to ry o f s po rt a n d per fo rma n c e p s ych olog y


tested during this time. In 1920, Robert Werner laboratory was in existence from 1925 until 1932.
Schulte, a former student of Wilhelm Wundt, Early reports of Coleman Griffith’s work with the
became the director of the sport psychology lab- University of Illinois athletic department began in
oratory at the German High School for Physical the fall of 1921, when it was reported that Coach
Exercise in Charlottenburg, Germany (Bäumler, Zuppke changed the colors of the school’s foot-
2009; Seiler & Wylleman, 2009; Tenenbaum, ball team uniforms from traditional blue to orange
Morris, & Hackfort, 2008). This laboratory tested based on his consultations with Griffith (Eckersall,
athletes in order to analyze their psychological skills 1921). Griffith appears to have begun teaching a
and various attributes (Bäumler, 2009). In addi- psychology of athletics class in the early 1920s. By
tion to his research, Schulte began to provide lec- 1922, a description of Griffith’s course is provided
tures on sport psychology to students, and he also in the University of Illinois Bulletin (University
published Body and Mind in Sport in 1921 and of Illinois, 1922). In March 1922, it was reported
Aptitude and Performance Testing in Sport in 1925 that Coleman Griffith had been added to the fac-
(Bäumler, 2009). Schulte’s work was documented ulty advisory staff for the University of Illinois foot-
in the Grandenwitz (1923) Scientific American arti- ball team (“Psychologist to Aid Selection of Illini
cle, “Making Sport a Science” and in a 1922 article Athletes,” 1922; “Psychology of Athletics Will be
titled, “The Psychology of Sport” in the Journal of Tried,” 1922). As part of the faculty advisory staff,
the American Medical Association. Griffith hoped to aid coaches in selecting athletes for
In Russia, the first sport psychology department the team. Apparently, having Griffith as part of the
was developed by P. A. Rudik, in 1920, in Moscow, advisory staff was effective: according to Brichford
at the State Central Institute of Physical Culture (2008, p. 48), “the Zuppke-Griffith collaboration
(Rudik, 1962; Seiler & Wylleman, 2009). Bäumler contributed to the national championships the Illini
(2009) reported that the type of research conducted won in 1923 and 1927.”
by Rudik used experimental methods to test various Griffith viewed his research as important since
aptitudes. Rudik conducted studies related to reac- it provided him with scientific evidence to teach
tion times of runner, boxers, and wrestlers (Rudik, his course, “Psychology and Athletics.” It appears
1962), and he also investigated whether specific also that Griffith was very proud of his research
types of training could help athletes react more work and of how it had influenced the teaching of
quickly. In explaining the type of work that he and sport psychology. In fact, Griffith stated: “When
his colleagues were conducting during this period, this course was first started, it was the only one of
Rudik (1962, p. 557) stated, “Special attention of its kind; but other schools have seen the advantage
the psychologists in this period of the development of such a course and the idea has now spread to
of the psychology of sport was directed to the study other parts of the country. Illinois still has, how-
of the psychological laws connected with the latent ever, the only laboratory devoted exclusively to the
period of motor reactions.” For a detailed descrip- study of psychological problems in athletic compe-
tion of the history of sport psychology in Russia, see tition” (Griffith, 1928, p. 10). In addition to sport
Rodionov (2010). psychology classes being taught at the University
In addition to the advent of sport psychology of Illinois in the 1920s, the Daily Illini reported
departments and laboratories in Germany and that courses related to the psychology of sport
Russia, in Japan, in 1924, the National Institute of were being taught at the State University of Iowa
Physical Education was established by the ministry and at Cornell University and that these universi-
of Education in Tokyo (Fujita, 1987). A specific ties were also using Griffith’s book, the Psychology
section of the institute was devoted to the study of of Coaching (“Griffith to teach coaching course,”
psychology applied to sport and physical education 1926). Not only was Griffith’s work influential in
(Fujita, 1987). Mitsuo Matsui, who had majored the United States, it appears that it was also influ-
in aviation psychology at Tokyo University, was ential worldwide. According to Lidor, Morris,
appointed director of the sport psychology section Bardaxoglou, and Becker (2001), Herbert Niiler,
for the institute (Fujita, 1987). Matsui completed an instructor at Tartu University in Estonia, was
sport psychology research from 1924 until the influenced by Griffith’s work on various publica-
beginning of World War II (Fujita, 1987). tions he produced about sport between 1930 and
In the United States, the first sport psychol- 1932. Griffith was also aware of the work in sport
ogy laboratory opened in September 1925, at the psychology being conducted worldwide. According
University of Illinois (Kroll & Lewis, 1970). The to Kremer and Moran (2008), Griffith visited both

korns pa n 7
Schulte’s and Sippel’s sport psychology laborato- tests, such as tests of reaction time, Brown tested var-
ries in Berlin, Germany. ious psychological variables including intelligence,
Although systematic research in sport psychol- aggression, personality, and character (Graef et al.,
ogy appears to have begun in laboratories started 2009).
in the 1920s in Germany, Russia, Japan, and the By the end of the 1930s, psychologists were con-
United States, historians of sport psychology have sulting with coaches and providing psychological
recently published detailed accounts of other labo- testing for athletes. A noteworthy example of con-
ratories and individuals conducting sport psychol- sultation during this period is the work of Coleman
ogy research during this period. Fuchs provided an Griffith and Jack Sterrett with the Chicago Cubs
example of the testing of elite-level athletes at the professional baseball team. For a detailed account
Columbia University Laboratory (Fuchs, 2009), of Griffith and Sterrett’s work, see Green (2003;
and he recounted the story of Babe Ruth, an elite 2011).
athlete in the sport of baseball and playing for the An important aspect of the new psychology dur-
New York Yankees, being tested in the Columbia ing this period was to create a scientific discipline of
University Psychology Laboratory. After a base- the field of sport psychology. Because this was such
ball game during the 1921 season, Hugh Fullerton a significant aspect of this period, it is important
(a well-known sportswriter) took Ruth to the to recognize the International Federation of Sports
Columbia University Psychology laboratory to be Medicine’s defining the purpose of their organiza-
tested by psychologists Albert Johanson and Joseph tion in their first constitution in 1928. This orga-
Holmes (Fuchs, 2009). Ruth was tested on a variety nization proposed that one of its main purposes
of psychological tests that included tests of reaction was “to inaugurate scientific research on biology,
time, intelligence, auditory speed, visual processing, psychology, and sociology in relation to sports”
the speed his swing, and perception. In most of the (LaCava, 1956, pp. 1109–1110). Additionally, in
tests, the Babe scored well above average (Fullerton, 1928, Coubertin formed the International Bureau
1921). of Sport Pedagogy; one of the purposes of this asso-
Another example of research studying the psy- ciation was to provide a sport-related organization
chology of human sports performance was the work that would welcome psychologists as members of
conducted by Walter Miles and John Lawther. Miles, the sport science team interested in the study of
a psychology professor at Stanford University, had sport (Kornspan, 2007b).
a graduate student in the psychology department, As Benjamin and Baker (2003) pointed out, dur-
B. C. Graves who was also a coach with the school’s ing the 1920s, the general public became more inter-
football team. Miles and Graves published two ested in consulting with psychologists. Businesses
studies together during the early 1930s related to and families wanted the services of psychologists
tests of the charging time of athletes on the Stanford to help improve their situations. Donald Laird,
football team, which was coached by Pop Warner director of the Colgate Psychology Laboratory, con-
(Baugh & Benjamin, 2009). Lawther, while coach- sulted with large corporations and businesses and
ing basketball and serving as the chair of the depart- promoted psychology to improve business (Laird,
ment of psychology at Westminster College, studied 1936).
athletes’ reaction times in his school’s psychology Additionally, by 1939, the field of psychol-
laboratory during the early 1930s (“Big Athletes ogy in the United States was organizing psycholo-
Alert,” 1931). gists to prepare for the possibility of psychology
Psychologists during the 1920s and 1930s were professionals being needed to assist the military
interested in providing psychological tests to ath- during World War II (Benjamin & Baker, 2003).
letes related to speed of movement, reaction time, Consultation with the military would continue
vision, concentration, tapping, and physical strength. into the next decade as psychologists and physical
Additionally, some psychologists began to use inven- educators contributed their knowledge to helping
tories that measured psychological qualities such address military problems. For example, individual
as the personality, intelligence, and aggression of psychologists who contributed to the development
athletes (Graef, Kornspan, & Baker, 2009). Paul of sport psychology also consulted with the mili-
Brown, in consultation with Ross Stagner and Robert tary during World War II. Walter Miles investigated
Henderson, designed a battery of tests that Brown adaptation to darkness and found that a pilot who
could utilize to gain more knowledge about his ath- wore red goggles under normal light conditions was
letes (Graef et al., 2009). In addition to psychomotor able to adapt to working in the dark immediately

8 h i s to ry o f s po rt a n d per fo rma n c e p s ych olog y


(Goodwin, 2005). Also, Coleman Griffith served with the exception of one have had no previous
as the director for specialized training programs for experience, and yet all are fighting like veterans.
military students attending the University of Illinois (p. 95)
(Franch, 2007).
Dewitt Portal told his friend Bud Winter, the
Psychology of Sport 1940–1965 track and field coach at San Jose State College,
In addition to recent sport psychology liter- about the work that Yates was conducting with
ature that has provided insight into the historical the boxing team (Kornspan, 2009a). Winter had
development of the field between 1890 and 1940, planned to have his athletes start to work with Yates,
recent sport psychology research has provided more but enlisted in the Navy before this could begin.
detail about how psychology was used with ath- When Navy officials learned that he knew about
letes between 1940 and 1965. In the 1940s and the work that Yates was conducting at San Jose
1950s, psychologists and physical educators began State College, they asked Winter to take charge of a
to discuss ways that psychology could be applied relaxation research committee at the Navy preflight
specifically to work with athletes to help improve school in Del Monte, California (Winter, 1981).
performance. During this period, individuals such The Navy had been having issues with aviators not
as Dorothy Yates, Richard Paynter, and Bud Winter being relaxed enough while flying combat missions.
were involved in researching and applying psy- Thus, Yates was part of a committee that researched
chology to sport to enhance athletic performance the effects of relaxation on naval aviators. Edmund
(Kornspan, 2009b). What is interesting about the Jacobson, the developer of progressive relaxation,
literature from the 1940s is that individuals began a commonly used technique in sport psychology
to discuss their consultations with athletes in today, was also part of the same research commit-
which they applied cognitive-behavioral methods. tee (Jacobson, 1962; Winter, 1981). According to
Additionally, professionals during this period, such Winter (1981), the research was very successful, and
as Anna Espenschade and Franklin Henry, suggested it showed the importance of relaxation for the suc-
ways that mental practice and visualization could be cessful performance of both athletic and nonsport
helpful to athletes (Kornspan, 2009b). activities.
Research into the history of sport psychology Another example of psychology applied to sport
has begun to examine the application of psychol- during this period was the work of David F. Tracy
ogy to sport during this period, including the work (Kornspan & MacCracken, 2003). Tracy was inter-
of Dorothy Yates with boxers at San Jose State viewed by a reporter in 1949, and he suggested that
College (Kornspan & MacCracken, 2001; Vealey, he could help the St. Louis Browns improve their
2006), the work of David F. Tracy with the St. performance. The St. Louis Browns hired him to
Louis Browns (Kornspan & MacCracken, 2003), work with the team during the 1950 spring train-
the use of hypnosis with athletes during the 1950s ing session. Tracy gained much media attention
and 1960s (Kornspan, 2009a), and the work of A. for applying psychology with the St. Louis Browns
C. Puni in applying psychology to sport in Russia baseball team, and he worked with the team through
in the 1950s and 1960s (Ryba, Stambulova, & the end of May 1950 (Kornspan & MacCracken,
Wrisberg, 2005). 2003). Tracy’s work received a great deal of attention
In the early 1940s, Dorothy Yates, a clinical psy- throughout the United States; thus it would seem
chologist at San Jose State College, became inter- reasonable to hypothesize that many psychologists
ested in applying psychology to athletics (Kornspan heard about Tracy’s work and may have begun to
& MacCracken, 2001). Yates, who was a professor consider applying psychology to work with athletes
of clinical psychology at San Jose State College, was (Kornspan, 2009a).
asked by an athlete on the boxing team to help him In the 1950s, other individuals who began apply-
with the mental aspects of boxing. Thus, she began ing psychology in their work with athletes included
helping athletes by using a combination of pro- Bruce Ogilvie and Burt Giges (Simons & Andersen,
gressive relaxation and positive affirmations (Yates, 1995), both of whom used their clinical back-
1943). Riley (1943) wrote: grounds to work with individual athletes. In Brazil,
a psychologist was working with the national soc-
Dr. Yates has applied psychological treatment to cer team as early as 1958 (Salmela, 1992). Antonelli
every man coming out for boxing. The team is was involved with the Italian Olympic committee
undefeated this year . . . . All of the men on the team, in helping to select athletes for the 1956 Olympic

korns pa n 9
Games in Melbourne, Australia. In Japan, another that high-level athletes in the United States were
example of applying psychology to athletic perfor- being provided with hypnosis interventions to aid
mance was provided by Naruse (1965), who, in a them in enhancing performance. He describes how
published article, described examples of helping a hypnotist and a psychologist provided a hypnosis
athletes deal with performance anxiety. intervention to Bill Faul, a rookie baseball pitcher
In addition to examples of psychology being with the Detroit Tigers. Moser (1963) suggested
applied in the United States, Italy, Japan, and Brazil, that the use of hypnosis in athletics was becom-
reports of the use of psychology to enhance sport ing much more common, citing the University of
performance were seen in England (Kornspan, Arizona football team and professional golfers as
2009b), Australia (Phillips, 2000), and the Soviet among those who were using hypnosis for perfor-
Union (“Russia eyes hypnotism in sports,” 1957) mance enhancement purposes.
during this same period. In Australia, Phillips In addition to the beginnings of the applica-
(2000) provides credit to Forbes and Ursula Carlile tion of sport psychology to help athletes improve
for applying psychology with Olympic swimmers. performance, researchers were continuing to study
According to Phillips (2000), Forbes Carlile was performance and to produce research related to
one of the first coaches to use hypnosis in Australia the psychology of sport during these decades.
to prepare athletes for competition. Additionally, An effort was made to encourage researchers to
the Soviet Olympic team at the 1952 Olympics become actively involved in conducting psycholog-
worked with sport science professionals including ical research related to sport and physical education.
sport psychologists (Ryba et al., 2005). According Johnson (1943), a professor of physical education at
to Seiler and Wylleman (2009), the use of psychol- the University of Denver, published an article titled,
ogy to help enhance athletic performance was also “The Opportunity for Psychological Research in the
being applied in Germany during this period. Field of Physical Education”; in analyzing the cur-
Coaches in the United States and other parts rent state of the psychology of physical education
of the world learned that sport science and sport and sport literature, Johnson believed that there
psychology methods were being used in the Soviet was great opportunity for those in the physical
Union to train athletes during this period (“Russ education profession to study psychological issues.
clinic at Stanford,” 1961). Payton Jordan, coach of Additionally, researchers made an effort during this
the Stanford University track and field team, main- time to summarize current knowledge in the field of
tained a correspondence with the national coach for sport psychology.
the Russian track-and-field team, Gabriel Korobkov. Cureton (1949) and Henry, Espenschade,
Korobkov described to Jordan the various training Hodgson, Karpovich, Steinhaus, and Jarrett (1949)
techniques they were using to train their athletes reviewed the literature on psychology related to
and that the Russian athletic teams had psycholo- physical education and sport. Cureton (1949), a
gists who were analyzing sport performance. Jordan professor at the University of Illinois, analyzed the
stated, “By studying these things, including the research in the field of physical education between
particular emotions of the individual athletes, 1930 and 1946. Of the 3,426 master’s degree the-
they often avoid the trial and error which many of ses completed in this period, Cureton coded 129 or
our athletes often go through before they find the 3.8% of these studies to be related to psychological
events for which they are best suited” (“Russ clinic issues. Of the 416 doctoral dissertations completed,
at Stanford,” 1961, p. 22). Ryba, Stambulova, and Cureton identified 22 or 6.3% of the physical edu-
Wrisberg (2009) have recently detailed A. C. Puni’s cation doctoral dissertations as having a focus on
theoretical concepts of preparing athletes mentally, psychology. Finally, Cureton analyzed Research
which were used to guide the work of sport psy- Quarterly between 1930 and 1946 and identified
chology professionals working with Russian athletes 830 published articles. Of these, Cureton identified
during the period. Rodionov (2010) also described 21 or 2.5% as being focused on psychology related
the growing use of psychology in preparing athletes to sport and physical education.
for competition. In addition to describing the amount of stud-
The research on psychological factors involved ies related to psychological issues in physical edu-
in successful performance was moving out of the cation, Cureton (1949) also provided a narrative
research laboratory and was being applied in the of what he referred to as “applied psychological
field. In a 1963 Life magazine article, “Athletes Get studies” (Cureton, 1949, p. 39). He noted that
Help from Hypnotists,” Moser (1963) explained the research literature contained reports on the

10 h i s to ry o f s po rt a n d per fo rma n c e p s ych olog y


Another random document with
no related content on Scribd:
DANCE ON STILTS AT THE GIRLS’ UNYAGO, NIUCHI

Newala, too, suffers from the distance of its water-supply—at least


the Newala of to-day does; there was once another Newala in a lovely
valley at the foot of the plateau. I visited it and found scarcely a trace
of houses, only a Christian cemetery, with the graves of several
missionaries and their converts, remaining as a monument of its
former glories. But the surroundings are wonderfully beautiful. A
thick grove of splendid mango-trees closes in the weather-worn
crosses and headstones; behind them, combining the useful and the
agreeable, is a whole plantation of lemon-trees covered with ripe
fruit; not the small African kind, but a much larger and also juicier
imported variety, which drops into the hands of the passing traveller,
without calling for any exertion on his part. Old Newala is now under
the jurisdiction of the native pastor, Daudi, at Chingulungulu, who,
as I am on very friendly terms with him, allows me, as a matter of
course, the use of this lemon-grove during my stay at Newala.
FEET MUTILATED BY THE RAVAGES OF THE “JIGGER”
(Sarcopsylla penetrans)

The water-supply of New Newala is in the bottom of the valley,


some 1,600 feet lower down. The way is not only long and fatiguing,
but the water, when we get it, is thoroughly bad. We are suffering not
only from this, but from the fact that the arrangements at Newala are
nothing short of luxurious. We have a separate kitchen—a hut built
against the boma palisade on the right of the baraza, the interior of
which is not visible from our usual position. Our two cooks were not
long in finding this out, and they consequently do—or rather neglect
to do—what they please. In any case they do not seem to be very
particular about the boiling of our drinking-water—at least I can
attribute to no other cause certain attacks of a dysenteric nature,
from which both Knudsen and I have suffered for some time. If a
man like Omari has to be left unwatched for a moment, he is capable
of anything. Besides this complaint, we are inconvenienced by the
state of our nails, which have become as hard as glass, and crack on
the slightest provocation, and I have the additional infliction of
pimples all over me. As if all this were not enough, we have also, for
the last week been waging war against the jigger, who has found his
Eldorado in the hot sand of the Makonde plateau. Our men are seen
all day long—whenever their chronic colds and the dysentery likewise
raging among them permit—occupied in removing this scourge of
Africa from their feet and trying to prevent the disastrous
consequences of its presence. It is quite common to see natives of
this place with one or two toes missing; many have lost all their toes,
or even the whole front part of the foot, so that a well-formed leg
ends in a shapeless stump. These ravages are caused by the female of
Sarcopsylla penetrans, which bores its way under the skin and there
develops an egg-sac the size of a pea. In all books on the subject, it is
stated that one’s attention is called to the presence of this parasite by
an intolerable itching. This agrees very well with my experience, so
far as the softer parts of the sole, the spaces between and under the
toes, and the side of the foot are concerned, but if the creature
penetrates through the harder parts of the heel or ball of the foot, it
may escape even the most careful search till it has reached maturity.
Then there is no time to be lost, if the horrible ulceration, of which
we see cases by the dozen every day, is to be prevented. It is much
easier, by the way, to discover the insect on the white skin of a
European than on that of a native, on which the dark speck scarcely
shows. The four or five jiggers which, in spite of the fact that I
constantly wore high laced boots, chose my feet to settle in, were
taken out for me by the all-accomplished Knudsen, after which I
thought it advisable to wash out the cavities with corrosive
sublimate. The natives have a different sort of disinfectant—they fill
the hole with scraped roots. In a tiny Makua village on the slope of
the plateau south of Newala, we saw an old woman who had filled all
the spaces under her toe-nails with powdered roots by way of
prophylactic treatment. What will be the result, if any, who can say?
The rest of the many trifling ills which trouble our existence are
really more comic than serious. In the absence of anything else to
smoke, Knudsen and I at last opened a box of cigars procured from
the Indian store-keeper at Lindi, and tried them, with the most
distressing results. Whether they contain opium or some other
narcotic, neither of us can say, but after the tenth puff we were both
“off,” three-quarters stupefied and unspeakably wretched. Slowly we
recovered—and what happened next? Half-an-hour later we were
once more smoking these poisonous concoctions—so insatiable is the
craving for tobacco in the tropics.
Even my present attacks of fever scarcely deserve to be taken
seriously. I have had no less than three here at Newala, all of which
have run their course in an incredibly short time. In the early
afternoon, I am busy with my old natives, asking questions and
making notes. The strong midday coffee has stimulated my spirits to
an extraordinary degree, the brain is active and vigorous, and work
progresses rapidly, while a pleasant warmth pervades the whole
body. Suddenly this gives place to a violent chill, forcing me to put on
my overcoat, though it is only half-past three and the afternoon sun
is at its hottest. Now the brain no longer works with such acuteness
and logical precision; more especially does it fail me in trying to
establish the syntax of the difficult Makua language on which I have
ventured, as if I had not enough to do without it. Under the
circumstances it seems advisable to take my temperature, and I do
so, to save trouble, without leaving my seat, and while going on with
my work. On examination, I find it to be 101·48°. My tutors are
abruptly dismissed and my bed set up in the baraza; a few minutes
later I am in it and treating myself internally with hot water and
lemon-juice.
Three hours later, the thermometer marks nearly 104°, and I make
them carry me back into the tent, bed and all, as I am now perspiring
heavily, and exposure to the cold wind just beginning to blow might
mean a fatal chill. I lie still for a little while, and then find, to my
great relief, that the temperature is not rising, but rather falling. This
is about 7.30 p.m. At 8 p.m. I find, to my unbounded astonishment,
that it has fallen below 98·6°, and I feel perfectly well. I read for an
hour or two, and could very well enjoy a smoke, if I had the
wherewithal—Indian cigars being out of the question.
Having no medical training, I am at a loss to account for this state
of things. It is impossible that these transitory attacks of high fever
should be malarial; it seems more probable that they are due to a
kind of sunstroke. On consulting my note-book, I become more and
more inclined to think this is the case, for these attacks regularly
follow extreme fatigue and long exposure to strong sunshine. They at
least have the advantage of being only short interruptions to my
work, as on the following morning I am always quite fresh and fit.
My treasure of a cook is suffering from an enormous hydrocele which
makes it difficult for him to get up, and Moritz is obliged to keep in
the dark on account of his inflamed eyes. Knudsen’s cook, a raw boy
from somewhere in the bush, knows still less of cooking than Omari;
consequently Nils Knudsen himself has been promoted to the vacant
post. Finding that we had come to the end of our supplies, he began
by sending to Chingulungulu for the four sucking-pigs which we had
bought from Matola and temporarily left in his charge; and when
they came up, neatly packed in a large crate, he callously slaughtered
the biggest of them. The first joint we were thoughtless enough to
entrust for roasting to Knudsen’s mshenzi cook, and it was
consequently uneatable; but we made the rest of the animal into a
jelly which we ate with great relish after weeks of underfeeding,
consuming incredible helpings of it at both midday and evening
meals. The only drawback is a certain want of variety in the tinned
vegetables. Dr. Jäger, to whom the Geographical Commission
entrusted the provisioning of the expeditions—mine as well as his
own—because he had more time on his hands than the rest of us,
seems to have laid in a huge stock of Teltow turnips,[46] an article of
food which is all very well for occasional use, but which quickly palls
when set before one every day; and we seem to have no other tins
left. There is no help for it—we must put up with the turnips; but I
am certain that, once I am home again, I shall not touch them for ten
years to come.
Amid all these minor evils, which, after all, go to make up the
genuine flavour of Africa, there is at least one cheering touch:
Knudsen has, with the dexterity of a skilled mechanic, repaired my 9
× 12 cm. camera, at least so far that I can use it with a little care.
How, in the absence of finger-nails, he was able to accomplish such a
ticklish piece of work, having no tool but a clumsy screw-driver for
taking to pieces and putting together again the complicated
mechanism of the instantaneous shutter, is still a mystery to me; but
he did it successfully. The loss of his finger-nails shows him in a light
contrasting curiously enough with the intelligence evinced by the
above operation; though, after all, it is scarcely surprising after his
ten years’ residence in the bush. One day, at Lindi, he had occasion
to wash a dog, which must have been in need of very thorough
cleansing, for the bottle handed to our friend for the purpose had an
extremely strong smell. Having performed his task in the most
conscientious manner, he perceived with some surprise that the dog
did not appear much the better for it, and was further surprised by
finding his own nails ulcerating away in the course of the next few
days. “How was I to know that carbolic acid has to be diluted?” he
mutters indignantly, from time to time, with a troubled gaze at his
mutilated finger-tips.
Since we came to Newala we have been making excursions in all
directions through the surrounding country, in accordance with old
habit, and also because the akida Sefu did not get together the tribal
elders from whom I wanted information so speedily as he had
promised. There is, however, no harm done, as, even if seen only
from the outside, the country and people are interesting enough.
The Makonde plateau is like a large rectangular table rounded off
at the corners. Measured from the Indian Ocean to Newala, it is
about seventy-five miles long, and between the Rovuma and the
Lukuledi it averages fifty miles in breadth, so that its superficial area
is about two-thirds of that of the kingdom of Saxony. The surface,
however, is not level, but uniformly inclined from its south-western
edge to the ocean. From the upper edge, on which Newala lies, the
eye ranges for many miles east and north-east, without encountering
any obstacle, over the Makonde bush. It is a green sea, from which
here and there thick clouds of smoke rise, to show that it, too, is
inhabited by men who carry on their tillage like so many other
primitive peoples, by cutting down and burning the bush, and
manuring with the ashes. Even in the radiant light of a tropical day
such a fire is a grand sight.
Much less effective is the impression produced just now by the
great western plain as seen from the edge of the plateau. As often as
time permits, I stroll along this edge, sometimes in one direction,
sometimes in another, in the hope of finding the air clear enough to
let me enjoy the view; but I have always been disappointed.
Wherever one looks, clouds of smoke rise from the burning bush,
and the air is full of smoke and vapour. It is a pity, for under more
favourable circumstances the panorama of the whole country up to
the distant Majeje hills must be truly magnificent. It is of little use
taking photographs now, and an outline sketch gives a very poor idea
of the scenery. In one of these excursions I went out of my way to
make a personal attempt on the Makonde bush. The present edge of
the plateau is the result of a far-reaching process of destruction
through erosion and denudation. The Makonde strata are
everywhere cut into by ravines, which, though short, are hundreds of
yards in depth. In consequence of the loose stratification of these
beds, not only are the walls of these ravines nearly vertical, but their
upper end is closed by an equally steep escarpment, so that the
western edge of the Makonde plateau is hemmed in by a series of
deep, basin-like valleys. In order to get from one side of such a ravine
to the other, I cut my way through the bush with a dozen of my men.
It was a very open part, with more grass than scrub, but even so the
short stretch of less than two hundred yards was very hard work; at
the end of it the men’s calicoes were in rags and they themselves
bleeding from hundreds of scratches, while even our strong khaki
suits had not escaped scatheless.

NATIVE PATH THROUGH THE MAKONDE BUSH, NEAR


MAHUTA

I see increasing reason to believe that the view formed some time
back as to the origin of the Makonde bush is the correct one. I have
no doubt that it is not a natural product, but the result of human
occupation. Those parts of the high country where man—as a very
slight amount of practice enables the eye to perceive at once—has not
yet penetrated with axe and hoe, are still occupied by a splendid
timber forest quite able to sustain a comparison with our mixed
forests in Germany. But wherever man has once built his hut or tilled
his field, this horrible bush springs up. Every phase of this process
may be seen in the course of a couple of hours’ walk along the main
road. From the bush to right or left, one hears the sound of the axe—
not from one spot only, but from several directions at once. A few
steps further on, we can see what is taking place. The brush has been
cut down and piled up in heaps to the height of a yard or more,
between which the trunks of the large trees stand up like the last
pillars of a magnificent ruined building. These, too, present a
melancholy spectacle: the destructive Makonde have ringed them—
cut a broad strip of bark all round to ensure their dying off—and also
piled up pyramids of brush round them. Father and son, mother and
son-in-law, are chopping away perseveringly in the background—too
busy, almost, to look round at the white stranger, who usually excites
so much interest. If you pass by the same place a week later, the piles
of brushwood have disappeared and a thick layer of ashes has taken
the place of the green forest. The large trees stretch their
smouldering trunks and branches in dumb accusation to heaven—if
they have not already fallen and been more or less reduced to ashes,
perhaps only showing as a white stripe on the dark ground.
This work of destruction is carried out by the Makonde alike on the
virgin forest and on the bush which has sprung up on sites already
cultivated and deserted. In the second case they are saved the trouble
of burning the large trees, these being entirely absent in the
secondary bush.
After burning this piece of forest ground and loosening it with the
hoe, the native sows his corn and plants his vegetables. All over the
country, he goes in for bed-culture, which requires, and, in fact,
receives, the most careful attention. Weeds are nowhere tolerated in
the south of German East Africa. The crops may fail on the plains,
where droughts are frequent, but never on the plateau with its
abundant rains and heavy dews. Its fortunate inhabitants even have
the satisfaction of seeing the proud Wayao and Wamakua working
for them as labourers, driven by hunger to serve where they were
accustomed to rule.
But the light, sandy soil is soon exhausted, and would yield no
harvest the second year if cultivated twice running. This fact has
been familiar to the native for ages; consequently he provides in
time, and, while his crop is growing, prepares the next plot with axe
and firebrand. Next year he plants this with his various crops and
lets the first piece lie fallow. For a short time it remains waste and
desolate; then nature steps in to repair the destruction wrought by
man; a thousand new growths spring out of the exhausted soil, and
even the old stumps put forth fresh shoots. Next year the new growth
is up to one’s knees, and in a few years more it is that terrible,
impenetrable bush, which maintains its position till the black
occupier of the land has made the round of all the available sites and
come back to his starting point.
The Makonde are, body and soul, so to speak, one with this bush.
According to my Yao informants, indeed, their name means nothing
else but “bush people.” Their own tradition says that they have been
settled up here for a very long time, but to my surprise they laid great
stress on an original immigration. Their old homes were in the
south-east, near Mikindani and the mouth of the Rovuma, whence
their peaceful forefathers were driven by the continual raids of the
Sakalavas from Madagascar and the warlike Shirazis[47] of the coast,
to take refuge on the almost inaccessible plateau. I have studied
African ethnology for twenty years, but the fact that changes of
population in this apparently quiet and peaceable corner of the earth
could have been occasioned by outside enterprises taking place on
the high seas, was completely new to me. It is, no doubt, however,
correct.
The charming tribal legend of the Makonde—besides informing us
of other interesting matters—explains why they have to live in the
thickest of the bush and a long way from the edge of the plateau,
instead of making their permanent homes beside the purling brooks
and springs of the low country.
“The place where the tribe originated is Mahuta, on the southern
side of the plateau towards the Rovuma, where of old time there was
nothing but thick bush. Out of this bush came a man who never
washed himself or shaved his head, and who ate and drank but little.
He went out and made a human figure from the wood of a tree
growing in the open country, which he took home to his abode in the
bush and there set it upright. In the night this image came to life and
was a woman. The man and woman went down together to the
Rovuma to wash themselves. Here the woman gave birth to a still-
born child. They left that place and passed over the high land into the
valley of the Mbemkuru, where the woman had another child, which
was also born dead. Then they returned to the high bush country of
Mahuta, where the third child was born, which lived and grew up. In
course of time, the couple had many more children, and called
themselves Wamatanda. These were the ancestral stock of the
Makonde, also called Wamakonde,[48] i.e., aborigines. Their
forefather, the man from the bush, gave his children the command to
bury their dead upright, in memory of the mother of their race who
was cut out of wood and awoke to life when standing upright. He also
warned them against settling in the valleys and near large streams,
for sickness and death dwelt there. They were to make it a rule to
have their huts at least an hour’s walk from the nearest watering-
place; then their children would thrive and escape illness.”
The explanation of the name Makonde given by my informants is
somewhat different from that contained in the above legend, which I
extract from a little book (small, but packed with information), by
Pater Adams, entitled Lindi und sein Hinterland. Otherwise, my
results agree exactly with the statements of the legend. Washing?
Hapana—there is no such thing. Why should they do so? As it is, the
supply of water scarcely suffices for cooking and drinking; other
people do not wash, so why should the Makonde distinguish himself
by such needless eccentricity? As for shaving the head, the short,
woolly crop scarcely needs it,[49] so the second ancestral precept is
likewise easy enough to follow. Beyond this, however, there is
nothing ridiculous in the ancestor’s advice. I have obtained from
various local artists a fairly large number of figures carved in wood,
ranging from fifteen to twenty-three inches in height, and
representing women belonging to the great group of the Mavia,
Makonde, and Matambwe tribes. The carving is remarkably well
done and renders the female type with great accuracy, especially the
keloid ornamentation, to be described later on. As to the object and
meaning of their works the sculptors either could or (more probably)
would tell me nothing, and I was forced to content myself with the
scanty information vouchsafed by one man, who said that the figures
were merely intended to represent the nembo—the artificial
deformations of pelele, ear-discs, and keloids. The legend recorded
by Pater Adams places these figures in a new light. They must surely
be more than mere dolls; and we may even venture to assume that
they are—though the majority of present-day Makonde are probably
unaware of the fact—representations of the tribal ancestress.
The references in the legend to the descent from Mahuta to the
Rovuma, and to a journey across the highlands into the Mbekuru
valley, undoubtedly indicate the previous history of the tribe, the
travels of the ancestral pair typifying the migrations of their
descendants. The descent to the neighbouring Rovuma valley, with
its extraordinary fertility and great abundance of game, is intelligible
at a glance—but the crossing of the Lukuledi depression, the ascent
to the Rondo Plateau and the descent to the Mbemkuru, also lie
within the bounds of probability, for all these districts have exactly
the same character as the extreme south. Now, however, comes a
point of especial interest for our bacteriological age. The primitive
Makonde did not enjoy their lives in the marshy river-valleys.
Disease raged among them, and many died. It was only after they
had returned to their original home near Mahuta, that the health
conditions of these people improved. We are very apt to think of the
African as a stupid person whose ignorance of nature is only equalled
by his fear of it, and who looks on all mishaps as caused by evil
spirits and malignant natural powers. It is much more correct to
assume in this case that the people very early learnt to distinguish
districts infested with malaria from those where it is absent.
This knowledge is crystallized in the
ancestral warning against settling in the
valleys and near the great waters, the
dwelling-places of disease and death. At the
same time, for security against the hostile
Mavia south of the Rovuma, it was enacted
that every settlement must be not less than a
certain distance from the southern edge of the
plateau. Such in fact is their mode of life at the
present day. It is not such a bad one, and
certainly they are both safer and more
comfortable than the Makua, the recent
intruders from the south, who have made USUAL METHOD OF
good their footing on the western edge of the CLOSING HUT-DOOR
plateau, extending over a fairly wide belt of
country. Neither Makua nor Makonde show in their dwellings
anything of the size and comeliness of the Yao houses in the plain,
especially at Masasi, Chingulungulu and Zuza’s. Jumbe Chauro, a
Makonde hamlet not far from Newala, on the road to Mahuta, is the
most important settlement of the tribe I have yet seen, and has fairly
spacious huts. But how slovenly is their construction compared with
the palatial residences of the elephant-hunters living in the plain.
The roofs are still more untidy than in the general run of huts during
the dry season, the walls show here and there the scanty beginnings
or the lamentable remains of the mud plastering, and the interior is a
veritable dog-kennel; dirt, dust and disorder everywhere. A few huts
only show any attempt at division into rooms, and this consists
merely of very roughly-made bamboo partitions. In one point alone
have I noticed any indication of progress—in the method of fastening
the door. Houses all over the south are secured in a simple but
ingenious manner. The door consists of a set of stout pieces of wood
or bamboo, tied with bark-string to two cross-pieces, and moving in
two grooves round one of the door-posts, so as to open inwards. If
the owner wishes to leave home, he takes two logs as thick as a man’s
upper arm and about a yard long. One of these is placed obliquely
against the middle of the door from the inside, so as to form an angle
of from 60° to 75° with the ground. He then places the second piece
horizontally across the first, pressing it downward with all his might.
It is kept in place by two strong posts planted in the ground a few
inches inside the door. This fastening is absolutely safe, but of course
cannot be applied to both doors at once, otherwise how could the
owner leave or enter his house? I have not yet succeeded in finding
out how the back door is fastened.

MAKONDE LOCK AND KEY AT JUMBE CHAURO


This is the general way of closing a house. The Makonde at Jumbe
Chauro, however, have a much more complicated, solid and original
one. Here, too, the door is as already described, except that there is
only one post on the inside, standing by itself about six inches from
one side of the doorway. Opposite this post is a hole in the wall just
large enough to admit a man’s arm. The door is closed inside by a
large wooden bolt passing through a hole in this post and pressing
with its free end against the door. The other end has three holes into
which fit three pegs running in vertical grooves inside the post. The
door is opened with a wooden key about a foot long, somewhat
curved and sloped off at the butt; the other end has three pegs
corresponding to the holes, in the bolt, so that, when it is thrust
through the hole in the wall and inserted into the rectangular
opening in the post, the pegs can be lifted and the bolt drawn out.[50]

MODE OF INSERTING THE KEY

With no small pride first one householder and then a second


showed me on the spot the action of this greatest invention of the
Makonde Highlands. To both with an admiring exclamation of
“Vizuri sana!” (“Very fine!”). I expressed the wish to take back these
marvels with me to Ulaya, to show the Wazungu what clever fellows
the Makonde are. Scarcely five minutes after my return to camp at
Newala, the two men came up sweating under the weight of two
heavy logs which they laid down at my feet, handing over at the same
time the keys of the fallen fortress. Arguing, logically enough, that if
the key was wanted, the lock would be wanted with it, they had taken
their axes and chopped down the posts—as it never occurred to them
to dig them out of the ground and so bring them intact. Thus I have
two badly damaged specimens, and the owners, instead of praise,
come in for a blowing-up.
The Makua huts in the environs of Newala are especially
miserable; their more than slovenly construction reminds one of the
temporary erections of the Makua at Hatia’s, though the people here
have not been concerned in a war. It must therefore be due to
congenital idleness, or else to the absence of a powerful chief. Even
the baraza at Mlipa’s, a short hour’s walk south-east of Newala,
shares in this general neglect. While public buildings in this country
are usually looked after more or less carefully, this is in evident
danger of being blown over by the first strong easterly gale. The only
attractive object in this whole district is the grave of the late chief
Mlipa. I visited it in the morning, while the sun was still trying with
partial success to break through the rolling mists, and the circular
grove of tall euphorbias, which, with a broken pot, is all that marks
the old king’s resting-place, impressed one with a touch of pathos.
Even my very materially-minded carriers seemed to feel something
of the sort, for instead of their usual ribald songs, they chanted
solemnly, as we marched on through the dense green of the Makonde
bush:—
“We shall arrive with the great master; we stand in a row and have
no fear about getting our food and our money from the Serkali (the
Government). We are not afraid; we are going along with the great
master, the lion; we are going down to the coast and back.”
With regard to the characteristic features of the various tribes here
on the western edge of the plateau, I can arrive at no other
conclusion than the one already come to in the plain, viz., that it is
impossible for anyone but a trained anthropologist to assign any
given individual at once to his proper tribe. In fact, I think that even
an anthropological specialist, after the most careful examination,
might find it a difficult task to decide. The whole congeries of peoples
collected in the region bounded on the west by the great Central
African rift, Tanganyika and Nyasa, and on the east by the Indian
Ocean, are closely related to each other—some of their languages are
only distinguished from one another as dialects of the same speech,
and no doubt all the tribes present the same shape of skull and
structure of skeleton. Thus, surely, there can be no very striking
differences in outward appearance.
Even did such exist, I should have no time
to concern myself with them, for day after day,
I have to see or hear, as the case may be—in
any case to grasp and record—an
extraordinary number of ethnographic
phenomena. I am almost disposed to think it
fortunate that some departments of inquiry, at
least, are barred by external circumstances.
Chief among these is the subject of iron-
working. We are apt to think of Africa as a
country where iron ore is everywhere, so to
speak, to be picked up by the roadside, and
where it would be quite surprising if the
inhabitants had not learnt to smelt the
material ready to their hand. In fact, the
knowledge of this art ranges all over the
continent, from the Kabyles in the north to the
Kafirs in the south. Here between the Rovuma
and the Lukuledi the conditions are not so
favourable. According to the statements of the
Makonde, neither ironstone nor any other
form of iron ore is known to them. They have
not therefore advanced to the art of smelting
the metal, but have hitherto bought all their
THE ANCESTRESS OF
THE MAKONDE
iron implements from neighbouring tribes.
Even in the plain the inhabitants are not much
better off. Only one man now living is said to
understand the art of smelting iron. This old fundi lives close to
Huwe, that isolated, steep-sided block of granite which rises out of
the green solitude between Masasi and Chingulungulu, and whose
jagged and splintered top meets the traveller’s eye everywhere. While
still at Masasi I wished to see this man at work, but was told that,
frightened by the rising, he had retired across the Rovuma, though
he would soon return. All subsequent inquiries as to whether the
fundi had come back met with the genuine African answer, “Bado”
(“Not yet”).
BRAZIER

Some consolation was afforded me by a brassfounder, whom I


came across in the bush near Akundonde’s. This man is the favourite
of women, and therefore no doubt of the gods; he welds the glittering
brass rods purchased at the coast into those massive, heavy rings
which, on the wrists and ankles of the local fair ones, continually give
me fresh food for admiration. Like every decent master-craftsman he
had all his tools with him, consisting of a pair of bellows, three
crucibles and a hammer—nothing more, apparently. He was quite
willing to show his skill, and in a twinkling had fixed his bellows on
the ground. They are simply two goat-skins, taken off whole, the four
legs being closed by knots, while the upper opening, intended to
admit the air, is kept stretched by two pieces of wood. At the lower
end of the skin a smaller opening is left into which a wooden tube is
stuck. The fundi has quickly borrowed a heap of wood-embers from
the nearest hut; he then fixes the free ends of the two tubes into an
earthen pipe, and clamps them to the ground by means of a bent
piece of wood. Now he fills one of his small clay crucibles, the dross
on which shows that they have been long in use, with the yellow
material, places it in the midst of the embers, which, at present are
only faintly glimmering, and begins his work. In quick alternation
the smith’s two hands move up and down with the open ends of the
bellows; as he raises his hand he holds the slit wide open, so as to let
the air enter the skin bag unhindered. In pressing it down he closes
the bag, and the air puffs through the bamboo tube and clay pipe into
the fire, which quickly burns up. The smith, however, does not keep
on with this work, but beckons to another man, who relieves him at
the bellows, while he takes some more tools out of a large skin pouch
carried on his back. I look on in wonder as, with a smooth round
stick about the thickness of a finger, he bores a few vertical holes into
the clean sand of the soil. This should not be difficult, yet the man
seems to be taking great pains over it. Then he fastens down to the
ground, with a couple of wooden clamps, a neat little trough made by
splitting a joint of bamboo in half, so that the ends are closed by the
two knots. At last the yellow metal has attained the right consistency,
and the fundi lifts the crucible from the fire by means of two sticks
split at the end to serve as tongs. A short swift turn to the left—a
tilting of the crucible—and the molten brass, hissing and giving forth
clouds of smoke, flows first into the bamboo mould and then into the
holes in the ground.
The technique of this backwoods craftsman may not be very far
advanced, but it cannot be denied that he knows how to obtain an
adequate result by the simplest means. The ladies of highest rank in
this country—that is to say, those who can afford it, wear two kinds
of these massive brass rings, one cylindrical, the other semicircular
in section. The latter are cast in the most ingenious way in the
bamboo mould, the former in the circular hole in the sand. It is quite
a simple matter for the fundi to fit these bars to the limbs of his fair
customers; with a few light strokes of his hammer he bends the
pliable brass round arm or ankle without further inconvenience to
the wearer.
SHAPING THE POT

SMOOTHING WITH MAIZE-COB

CUTTING THE EDGE


FINISHING THE BOTTOM

LAST SMOOTHING BEFORE


BURNING

FIRING THE BRUSH-PILE


LIGHTING THE FARTHER SIDE OF
THE PILE

TURNING THE RED-HOT VESSEL

NYASA WOMAN MAKING POTS AT MASASI


Pottery is an art which must always and everywhere excite the
interest of the student, just because it is so intimately connected with
the development of human culture, and because its relics are one of
the principal factors in the reconstruction of our own condition in
prehistoric times. I shall always remember with pleasure the two or
three afternoons at Masasi when Salim Matola’s mother, a slightly-
built, graceful, pleasant-looking woman, explained to me with
touching patience, by means of concrete illustrations, the ceramic art
of her people. The only implements for this primitive process were a
lump of clay in her left hand, and in the right a calabash containing
the following valuables: the fragment of a maize-cob stripped of all
its grains, a smooth, oval pebble, about the size of a pigeon’s egg, a
few chips of gourd-shell, a bamboo splinter about the length of one’s
hand, a small shell, and a bunch of some herb resembling spinach.
Nothing more. The woman scraped with the
shell a round, shallow hole in the soft, fine
sand of the soil, and, when an active young
girl had filled the calabash with water for her,
she began to knead the clay. As if by magic it
gradually assumed the shape of a rough but
already well-shaped vessel, which only wanted
a little touching up with the instruments
before mentioned. I looked out with the
MAKUA WOMAN closest attention for any indication of the use
MAKING A POT. of the potter’s wheel, in however rudimentary
SHOWS THE a form, but no—hapana (there is none). The
BEGINNINGS OF THE embryo pot stood firmly in its little
POTTER’S WHEEL
depression, and the woman walked round it in
a stooping posture, whether she was removing
small stones or similar foreign bodies with the maize-cob, smoothing
the inner or outer surface with the splinter of bamboo, or later, after
letting it dry for a day, pricking in the ornamentation with a pointed
bit of gourd-shell, or working out the bottom, or cutting the edge
with a sharp bamboo knife, or giving the last touches to the finished
vessel. This occupation of the women is infinitely toilsome, but it is
without doubt an accurate reproduction of the process in use among
our ancestors of the Neolithic and Bronze ages.
There is no doubt that the invention of pottery, an item in human
progress whose importance cannot be over-estimated, is due to
women. Rough, coarse and unfeeling, the men of the horde range
over the countryside. When the united cunning of the hunters has
succeeded in killing the game; not one of them thinks of carrying
home the spoil. A bright fire, kindled by a vigorous wielding of the
drill, is crackling beside them; the animal has been cleaned and cut
up secundum artem, and, after a slight singeing, will soon disappear
under their sharp teeth; no one all this time giving a single thought
to wife or child.
To what shifts, on the other hand, the primitive wife, and still more
the primitive mother, was put! Not even prehistoric stomachs could
endure an unvarying diet of raw food. Something or other suggested
the beneficial effect of hot water on the majority of approved but
indigestible dishes. Perhaps a neighbour had tried holding the hard
roots or tubers over the fire in a calabash filled with water—or maybe
an ostrich-egg-shell, or a hastily improvised vessel of bark. They
became much softer and more palatable than they had previously
been; but, unfortunately, the vessel could not stand the fire and got
charred on the outside. That can be remedied, thought our
ancestress, and plastered a layer of wet clay round a similar vessel.
This is an improvement; the cooking utensil remains uninjured, but
the heat of the fire has shrunk it, so that it is loose in its shell. The
next step is to detach it, so, with a firm grip and a jerk, shell and
kernel are separated, and pottery is invented. Perhaps, however, the
discovery which led to an intelligent use of the burnt-clay shell, was
made in a slightly different way. Ostrich-eggs and calabashes are not
to be found in every part of the world, but everywhere mankind has
arrived at the art of making baskets out of pliant materials, such as
bark, bast, strips of palm-leaf, supple twigs, etc. Our inventor has no
water-tight vessel provided by nature. “Never mind, let us line the
basket with clay.” This answers the purpose, but alas! the basket gets
burnt over the blazing fire, the woman watches the process of
cooking with increasing uneasiness, fearing a leak, but no leak
appears. The food, done to a turn, is eaten with peculiar relish; and
the cooking-vessel is examined, half in curiosity, half in satisfaction
at the result. The plastic clay is now hard as stone, and at the same
time looks exceedingly well, for the neat plaiting of the burnt basket
is traced all over it in a pretty pattern. Thus, simultaneously with
pottery, its ornamentation was invented.
Primitive woman has another claim to respect. It was the man,
roving abroad, who invented the art of producing fire at will, but the
woman, unable to imitate him in this, has been a Vestal from the
earliest times. Nothing gives so much trouble as the keeping alight of
the smouldering brand, and, above all, when all the men are absent
from the camp. Heavy rain-clouds gather, already the first large
drops are falling, the first gusts of the storm rage over the plain. The
little flame, a greater anxiety to the woman than her own children,
flickers unsteadily in the blast. What is to be done? A sudden thought
occurs to her, and in an instant she has constructed a primitive hut
out of strips of bark, to protect the flame against rain and wind.
This, or something very like it, was the way in which the principle
of the house was discovered; and even the most hardened misogynist
cannot fairly refuse a woman the credit of it. The protection of the
hearth-fire from the weather is the germ from which the human
dwelling was evolved. Men had little, if any share, in this forward
step, and that only at a late stage. Even at the present day, the
plastering of the housewall with clay and the manufacture of pottery
are exclusively the women’s business. These are two very significant
survivals. Our European kitchen-garden, too, is originally a woman’s
invention, and the hoe, the primitive instrument of agriculture, is,
characteristically enough, still used in this department. But the
noblest achievement which we owe to the other sex is unquestionably
the art of cookery. Roasting alone—the oldest process—is one for
which men took the hint (a very obvious one) from nature. It must
have been suggested by the scorched carcase of some animal
overtaken by the destructive forest-fires. But boiling—the process of
improving organic substances by the help of water heated to boiling-
point—is a much later discovery. It is so recent that it has not even
yet penetrated to all parts of the world. The Polynesians understand
how to steam food, that is, to cook it, neatly wrapped in leaves, in a
hole in the earth between hot stones, the air being excluded, and
(sometimes) a few drops of water sprinkled on the stones; but they
do not understand boiling.
To come back from this digression, we find that the slender Nyasa
woman has, after once more carefully examining the finished pot,
put it aside in the shade to dry. On the following day she sends me
word by her son, Salim Matola, who is always on hand, that she is
going to do the burning, and, on coming out of my house, I find her
already hard at work. She has spread on the ground a layer of very
dry sticks, about as thick as one’s thumb, has laid the pot (now of a
yellowish-grey colour) on them, and is piling brushwood round it.
My faithful Pesa mbili, the mnyampara, who has been standing by,
most obligingly, with a lighted stick, now hands it to her. Both of
them, blowing steadily, light the pile on the lee side, and, when the
flame begins to catch, on the weather side also. Soon the whole is in a
blaze, but the dry fuel is quickly consumed and the fire dies down, so
that we see the red-hot vessel rising from the ashes. The woman
turns it continually with a long stick, sometimes one way and
sometimes another, so that it may be evenly heated all over. In
twenty minutes she rolls it out of the ash-heap, takes up the bundle
of spinach, which has been lying for two days in a jar of water, and
sprinkles the red-hot clay with it. The places where the drops fall are
marked by black spots on the uniform reddish-brown surface. With a
sigh of relief, and with visible satisfaction, the woman rises to an
erect position; she is standing just in a line between me and the fire,
from which a cloud of smoke is just rising: I press the ball of my
camera, the shutter clicks—the apotheosis is achieved! Like a
priestess, representative of her inventive sex, the graceful woman
stands: at her feet the hearth-fire she has given us beside her the
invention she has devised for us, in the background the home she has
built for us.
At Newala, also, I have had the manufacture of pottery carried on
in my presence. Technically the process is better than that already
described, for here we find the beginnings of the potter’s wheel,
which does not seem to exist in the plains; at least I have seen
nothing of the sort. The artist, a frightfully stupid Makua woman, did
not make a depression in the ground to receive the pot she was about
to shape, but used instead a large potsherd. Otherwise, she went to
work in much the same way as Salim’s mother, except that she saved
herself the trouble of walking round and round her work by squatting
at her ease and letting the pot and potsherd rotate round her; this is
surely the first step towards a machine. But it does not follow that
the pot was improved by the process. It is true that it was beautifully
rounded and presented a very creditable appearance when finished,
but the numerous large and small vessels which I have seen, and, in
part, collected, in the “less advanced” districts, are no less so. We
moderns imagine that instruments of precision are necessary to
produce excellent results. Go to the prehistoric collections of our
museums and look at the pots, urns and bowls of our ancestors in the
dim ages of the past, and you will at once perceive your error.
MAKING LONGITUDINAL CUT IN
BARK

DRAWING THE BARK OFF THE LOG

REMOVING THE OUTER BARK


BEATING THE BARK

WORKING THE BARK-CLOTH AFTER BEATING, TO MAKE IT


SOFT

MANUFACTURE OF BARK-CLOTH AT NEWALA


To-day, nearly the whole population of German East Africa is
clothed in imported calico. This was not always the case; even now in
some parts of the north dressed skins are still the prevailing wear,
and in the north-western districts—east and north of Lake
Tanganyika—lies a zone where bark-cloth has not yet been
superseded. Probably not many generations have passed since such
bark fabrics and kilts of skins were the only clothing even in the
south. Even to-day, large quantities of this bright-red or drab
material are still to be found; but if we wish to see it, we must look in
the granaries and on the drying stages inside the native huts, where
it serves less ambitious uses as wrappings for those seeds and fruits
which require to be packed with special care. The salt produced at
Masasi, too, is packed for transport to a distance in large sheets of
bark-cloth. Wherever I found it in any degree possible, I studied the
process of making this cloth. The native requisitioned for the
purpose arrived, carrying a log between two and three yards long and
as thick as his thigh, and nothing else except a curiously-shaped
mallet and the usual long, sharp and pointed knife which all men and
boys wear in a belt at their backs without a sheath—horribile dictu!
[51]
Silently he squats down before me, and with two rapid cuts has
drawn a couple of circles round the log some two yards apart, and
slits the bark lengthwise between them with the point of his knife.
With evident care, he then scrapes off the outer rind all round the
log, so that in a quarter of an hour the inner red layer of the bark
shows up brightly-coloured between the two untouched ends. With
some trouble and much caution, he now loosens the bark at one end,
and opens the cylinder. He then stands up, takes hold of the free
edge with both hands, and turning it inside out, slowly but steadily
pulls it off in one piece. Now comes the troublesome work of
scraping all superfluous particles of outer bark from the outside of
the long, narrow piece of material, while the inner side is carefully
scrutinised for defective spots. At last it is ready for beating. Having
signalled to a friend, who immediately places a bowl of water beside
him, the artificer damps his sheet of bark all over, seizes his mallet,
lays one end of the stuff on the smoothest spot of the log, and
hammers away slowly but continuously. “Very simple!” I think to
myself. “Why, I could do that, too!”—but I am forced to change my
opinions a little later on; for the beating is quite an art, if the fabric is
not to be beaten to pieces. To prevent the breaking of the fibres, the
stuff is several times folded across, so as to interpose several
thicknesses between the mallet and the block. At last the required
state is reached, and the fundi seizes the sheet, still folded, by both
ends, and wrings it out, or calls an assistant to take one end while he
holds the other. The cloth produced in this way is not nearly so fine
and uniform in texture as the famous Uganda bark-cloth, but it is
quite soft, and, above all, cheap.
Now, too, I examine the mallet. My craftsman has been using the
simpler but better form of this implement, a conical block of some
hard wood, its base—the striking surface—being scored across and
across with more or less deeply-cut grooves, and the handle stuck
into a hole in the middle. The other and earlier form of mallet is
shaped in the same way, but the head is fastened by an ingenious
network of bark strips into the split bamboo serving as a handle. The
observation so often made, that ancient customs persist longest in
connection with religious ceremonies and in the life of children, here
finds confirmation. As we shall soon see, bark-cloth is still worn
during the unyago,[52] having been prepared with special solemn
ceremonies; and many a mother, if she has no other garment handy,
will still put her little one into a kilt of bark-cloth, which, after all,
looks better, besides being more in keeping with its African
surroundings, than the ridiculous bit of print from Ulaya.
MAKUA WOMEN

You might also like