You are on page 1of 8

Received: 21 July 2020 Accepted: 21 January 2021

DOI: 10.1002/aur.2485

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Alexithymia is related to poor social competence in autistic and


nonautistic children

Nichole E. Scheerer1,2 | Troy Q. Boucher1 | Grace Iarocci1

1
Department of Psychology, Simon Fraser Abstract
University, Burnaby, British Columbia, Canada
2
Alexithymia is characterized by difficulties identifying and describing one’s own
The Brain and Mind Institute, Western
Interdisciplinary Research Building, University
emotions and the emotions of others. These challenges with understanding emo-
of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada tions in people with alexithymia may give rise to difficulties with social interac-
tions. Given that alexithymia frequently co-occurs with autism spectrum disorder
Correspondence (ASD), and that both populations have difficulties with social interactions, it is of
Nichole E. Scheerer, The Brain and Mind
Institute, Western Interdisciplinary Research
interest to determine whether alexithymia can help to parse some of the heteroge-
Building, University of Western Ontario, 1151 neity in social competence in autistic and nonautistic individuals. The caregivers
Richmond Street, London, Ontario N6A 3K7, of 241 children (6–14 years old), including 120 autistic, and 121 nonautistic, rated
Canada.
Email: nikkischeerer@gmail.com
their child’s social competence using the Multidimensional Social Competence
Scale (MSCS), autism traits using the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ), and
Funding information alexithymia traits using the Children’s Alexithymia Measure (CAM). Regression
Canada First Research Excellence Fund; analyses indicated that age, IQ, sex, AQ, and CAM scores accounted for 40.2% of
Natural Sciences and Engineering Research the variance in autistic children’s, and 68.2% of the variance in nonautistic chil-
Council of Canada
dren’s, parent-reported social competence. Importantly, after controlling for age,
IQ, sex, and AQ scores, CAM scores alone accounted for an additional 16.2% of
the variance in autistic children’s, and 17.4% of the variance in nonautistic chil-
dren’s social competence. These results indicate that higher alexithymia traits pre-
dict lower levels of social competence, suggesting that increased difficulty in
identifying and describing one’s own emotions and the emotions of others is asso-
ciated with poorer social competence. Furthermore, CAM scores were found to
partially mediate the relationship between autistic traits and social competence,
suggesting that comorbid alexithymia traits may be partially responsible for poor
social competence in individuals with high autistic traits. This research contributes
to the understanding of the factors associated with the development of social com-
petence and highlights alexithymia as a potential target for identification and
intervention to improve social competence.

Lay Summary: Alexithymia is a condition where people find it hard to think and
talk about their (and others’) feelings. About 50% of autistic people have
alexithymia. This might be why they have social and emotional difficulties. Par-
ents answered questions about alexithymia and social difficulties their children
have. Children with more alexithymia problems had poorer social skills. Thus,
alexithymia may be related to social problems faced by autistic and nonautistic
children.

KEYWORDS
alexithymia, autism, autism spectrum disorder, emotional processing, social competence

[Correction added on 26 February 2021, after first online publication: The


affiliation of Dr. Boucher and Dr. Iarocci were incorrect in the initial publication.
It has been corrected.]

© 2021 International Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals LLC.

Autism Research. 2021;1–8. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/aur 1


2 SCHEERER ET AL.

INTRODUCTION “alexithymia hypothesis”, individual differences in emo-


tional processing that are common, but not universal, to
Alexithymia is a condition that is characterized by diffi- autistic individuals can be explained by co-occurring
culties identifying and describing one’s own emotions alexithymia. For example, alexithymia, rather than autis-
and the emotions of others. Alexithymia has a high co- tic traits, was found to predict variability in emotion rec-
occurrence with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and ognition and empathy in a sample of autistic individuals
other disorders such as schizophrenia and bipolar disor- (Bird et al., 2010; Cook et al., 2013). Thus, alexithymia
der (Bird & Cook, 2013; Ospina et al., 2019; Trevisan may be the root of the emotion recognition and related
et al., 2019). The compromised understanding of others’ challenges in a subgroup of autistic people (Foulkes
emotions in individuals with alexithymia has been et al., 2015).
suggested to give rise to problems with social interactions To develop effective interventions to improve the
(Poquérusse et al., 2018). Given that alexithymia fre- social skills of autistic individuals, it is important to first
quently co-occurs with ASD, and that both populations identify which of the many social competencies
have difficulties with social interactions, it is of interest to (e.g., emotion understanding) are affected in specific
determine whether alexithymia can help to parse some of groups of autistic people. The aim of this study was to
the heterogeneity in social competence seen not only in explore whether there is a link between alexithymia and
individuals with ASD, but also those without ASD. social competence in autistic and nonautistic children,
Alexithymia traits can be measured continuously which could account for the heterogeneity in social com-
within the general population (Bagby et al., 1994; petence observed in both autistic and nonautistic chil-
Trevisan et al., 2019), with alexithymia occurring in dren. Given the previously reported association between
approximately 10% of the nonautistic population social functioning and alexithymia in individuals with
(Berthoz et al., 2011), relative to approximately 50% of bipolar disorder and schizophrenia (Ospina et al., 2019;
autistic individuals (Berthoz & Hill, 2005; Kinnaird Kimhy et al., 2012, respectively), we expected that
et al., 2019). Alexithymic individuals struggle to recog- higher scores on the children’s alexithymia measure
nize and articulate their emotional experiences (CAM), indicating more alexithymia traits, would be
(Sifneos, 1972). Alexithymic individuals are characterized related to lower scores on the multidimensional social
as having difficulties identifying and describing their competence scale (MSCS), indicating lower social
emotions, attributing bodily sensations to emotional competence.
experiences, thinking symbolically, and self-reflecting on
their inner experiences (Kooiman et al., 2002; Nemiah
et al., 1976; Ospina et al., 2019; Trevisan et al., 2019). METHODS
Poor emotional awareness can contribute to difficulties
in social communication (e.g., expressing one’s emotions) Caregivers of 241 children between the ages of 6 and
and social interaction (e.g., understanding emotional cues 14 years old participated: 120 autistic (95 boys,
from others). Emotional awareness and understanding Mage = 10.20; SD = 1.91), and 121 nonautistic (71 boys,
are essential aspects of social engagement and under- Mage = 9.50; SD = 1.73) children. Inclusion criteria for
standing (Blair et al., 2007; Van Kleef, 2009). Thus, the current study were an ASD diagnosis (ASD group
alexithymia may interfere with the development of social only) and Full Scale IQ > 80 assessed using the Wechsler
competence. Accordingly, in both bipolar disorder Abbreviated Scales of Intelligence, Second Edition
(Ospina et al., 2019) and schizophrenia (Kimhy (WASI-II; Wechsler, 2011). Children with ASD and
et al., 2012), alexithymia has been found to predict social intellectual disability should be considered as a separate
functioning. Given the high prevalence of alexithymia in subgroup. Sample characteristics are presented in
the autistic population, it is possible that certain emo- Table 1. Caregivers rated their child’s social competence
tional and social difficulties experienced by some autistic on the Multidimensional Social Competence Scale
individuals may be due to co-occurring alexithymia. (MSCS; Yager & Iarocci, 2013), and alexithymia traits
Although emotional deficits are not a core diagnostic on the Children’s Alexithymia Measure (CAM; Way
feature of ASD, a difficulty perceiving emotions in et al., 2010). A diagnosis of ASD in the province of Brit-
others, a lack of empathy, and other emotional ish Columbia, Canada involves a standardized procedure
processing difficulties are often used as diagnostic using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule
markers of ASD (APA, 2013; Bird & Cook, 2013; (ADOS) and the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised
Gaigg, 2012). However, not all autistic individuals have (ADI-R) conducted by a trained clinician. Confirmation
these emotional processing difficulties (Kinnaird of ASD diagnosis was provided by caregivers in the form
et al., 2019). Recent studies have suggested that it is of a clinical diagnostic report or government funding eli-
alexithymia, not autism, that is driving these emotional gibility report. All caregivers provided informed consent
processing difficulties (Bird & Cook, 2013; Cook prior to participation. This study was approved by the
et al., 2013; Trevisan et al., 2019). According to the Office of Research Ethics at Simon Fraser University.
SCHEERER ET AL. 3

TABLE 1 Participant characteristics

TD children (n = 121) ASD children (n = 120)


N (%) Mean (SD) N (%) Mean (SD)

Age 9.50 (1.73) 10.20 (1.91)


MSCS 287.94 (44.81) 208.07 (31.01)
CAM 7.69 (7.52) 16.71 (8.80)
AQ 15.64 (7.83) 32.41 (6.78)
WASI-II FSIQ-2 109.39 (13.60) 100.63 (18.46)
Ethnicity
Asian 53 (44) 44 (37)
Asian-Canadian 9 (7) 16 (13)
White 41 (34) 56 (47)
Other 18 (15) 4 (3)
Child’s school type
Public school 116 (96) 100 (83)
Private school 5 (4) 13 (11)
Home school 0 7 (6)
Parent marital status
Single 4 (3) 4 (3)
Married/common-law 106 (88) 97 (80)
Separated/divorced 11 (9) 19 (16)
Family income
Less than $20,000 2 (2) 3 (3)
$20,000 to $49,999 11 (9) 18 (15)
$50,000 to $79,999 33 (27) 23 (19)
$80,000 to $109,999 22 (18) 23 (19)
$110,000 to $139,999 22 (18) 23 (19)
Greater than $140,000 28 (23) 22 (18)
Prefer not to say 3 (3) 8 (7)
Abbreviations: AQ, autism quotient; CAM, children’s alexithymia measure; MSCS, Multidimensional Social Competence Scale; WASI-II FSIQ-2, Wechsler abbreviated
scales of intelligence, second edition, full scale IQ-2.

Social competence Alexithymia

The MSCS is a 77-item parent-report questionnaire The CAM is a 14-item parent-report questionnaire that
that assesses childrens’ social competence, answered assesses childrens’ alexithymia traits using a 4-point
using a five-point rating scale (1 = Not True or Almost Likert scale (Almost Never, Sometimes, Often, and
Never True, 5 = Very True or Almost Always True). Almost Always). Higher total scores indicate higher
Questions on the MSCS assess the social functioning levels of alexithymia. Factor analysis of the CAM shows
of the child in everyday life across seven domains: strong internal reliability (0.92; Way et al., 2010).
social motivation, social inferencing, demonstrating
empathic concern, social knowledge, emotion regula-
tion, verbal conversation skills, and nonverbal sending Autistic traits
skills. The MSCS total score, which is the sum of
scores for all seven domains, was used as the measure The AQ is a 50-item parent-report questionnaire that
of social competence in the current study. Higher assesses autistic traits in children using a 4-point scale
MSCS total scores reflect better social competence of (definitely agree, slightly agree, slightly disagree, and def-
the child. The reliability coefficients for the internal initely agree). The AQ-Child (ages 4–11; Auyeung
consistency of MSCS subscales are high (range et al., 2008) or the AQ-Adolescent (ages 12–15; Baron-
0.84–0.90), as is the MSCS total score (0.95; Yager & Cohen et al., 2006) were completed by parents of children
Iarocci, 2013). depending on the child’s age. Both versions yield a total
4 SCHEERER ET AL.

score ranging from 0 to 50 and follow a similar structure, RESULTS


assessing social and nonsocial characteristics of ASD
across five domains: social skills, attention switching, Group differences
attention to details, communication skills, and imagina-
tion. Typically, a score of 32 is considered the cutoff for Independent samples t tests indicated that autistic chil-
significant autistic traits (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001). dren were significantly older than nonautistic children (t
Internal consistency is good for both the AQ-Child (0.97; (239) = −2.998, p = 0.003). Nonautistic children had sig-
Auyeung et al., 2008) and the AQ-Adolescent (0.79; nificantly higher MSCS (t(213.62) = 16.102, p < 0.001)
Baron-Cohen et al., 2006). and IQ (t(218.75) = 4.190, p < 0.001) scores, while autis-
tic children had significantly higher CAM (t
(232.72) = −8.552, p < 0.001) and AQ (t(239) = −17.758,
Intelligence quotient (IQ) p < 0.001) scores. Chi-square results indicate that the dis-
tribution of males and females differed across autistic
The IQ of participants was estimated using the WASI-II. and nonautistic groups (χ 2(1) = 11.799, p = 0.001).
The WASI-II provides an accurate estimate of cognitive The goal of this study was to see if alexithymia traits
ability comparable to other measures of intelligence, could help to parse some of the heterogeneity in social
developed for quick administration with individuals aged competence in both our samples of autistic and non-
6–90 years old (Wechsler, 2011). The WASI-II Full-Scale autistic children. Given we found group differences in
IQ-2 (FSIQ-2) composite scale was administered with CAM and MSCS scores, we opted to run subsequent
participants by clinical psychology graduate students and analyses separately for each group in order to investigate
is reported in the current study. The FSIQ-2 is composed the relationship between alexithymia traits and social
of scores from two subtests: Vocabulary and Matrix Rea- competence within each group, without worrying about
soning. The reliability coefficients for the FSIQ-2 com- the between group differences driving our effects. How-
posite scale is excellent (0.93) for children ages 6–16 years ever, results of the combined analyses can be found in the
old, and the interrater reliability for the subtests is high supplemental information.
for both Matrix Reasoning (0.99) and for Vocabu-
lary (0.95).
ASD group
Statistical analyses Pearson’s correlational analyses indicate that CAM scores
were significantly correlated with total MSCS (r
Independent samples t tests were conducted to investigate (119) = −0.520, p < 0.001) and AQ (r(119) = 0.271,
group differences in age, MSCS, CAM, AQ, and IQ, p = 0.003) scores. CAM scores were also significantly corre-
while a chi-square analysis was conducted to look for dif- lated with all seven subscales of the MSCS (p < 0.05). MSCS
ferences in the gender distribution across groups. Next, scores were also significantly correlated with AQ scores (r
correlational analyses were conducted to look at the rela- (119) = −0.520, p < 0.001). However, IQ scores were not sig-
tions between MSCS, CAM, AQ, and IQ, separately for nificantly correlated with CAM, MSCS, or AQ scores.
autistic and nonautistic children. Hierarchical regressions Step 1 of the regression model indicated that age, sex,
were conducted separately for autistic children and non- and IQ did not account for significant variance in MSCS
autistic children to see whether after controlling for age, scores (R2 = 0.017, F(3, 116) = 0.685, p = 0.563). With
sex, IQ, and autism characteristics, alexithymia could the addition of AQ scores in the second step of the model
account for a significant amount of variance in MSCS an additional 22.3% of the variance in MSCS scores was
scores. A three-step procedure was used whereby the first accounted for, resulting in a significant model
step contained the independent variables of age, sex, and (R2 = 0.241, F(4, 115) = 9.111, p < 0.001). Similarly, in
IQ in order to control for their potential influence on step 3 of the model CAM scores were added, accounting
social competence. Given social competence has been for an additional 16.2% of variance in MSCS scores,
shown to vary with autistic traits (Scheerer et al., 2020), resulting in a significant model (R2 = 0.402, F
the second step contained AQ scores to parse out the var- (5, 114) = 15.347, p < 0.001; see Table 2).
iance in social competence accounted for by autistic Given AQ scores were significantly correlated with
traits. The third step contained CAM scores to determine both MSCS and CAM scores, and CAM scores were also
how much additional variance in the dependent variable, correlated with MSCS scores, we conducted a mediation
MSCS scores, could be accounted for by alexithymia analysis to determine whether social deficits in children
traits. Finally, regression-based mediation analyses were with high levels of autistic traits are mediated by
conducted using the bootstrapping method (Hayes & alexithymia traits. As Figure 1 illustrates, the standard-
Rockwood, 2017), to examine alexithymia traits as a ized regression coefficient between autistic traits and
potential mediator of the relationship between autistic alexithymia traits was statistically significant, as was the
traits and social competence. standardized regression coefficient between alexithymia
SCHEERER ET AL. 5

TABLE 2 Hierarchical regression analysis for autistic participants

Step 1 Step 2 Step 3


Variable B β B β B β

Constant 207.500** - 277.486** - 288.349** -


IQ 0.09 0.053 0.092 0.055 −0.028 −0.017
Age −0.998 −0.061 −1.002 −0.062 −0.126 −0.008
Sex 8.302 0.109 7.511 0.099 8.24 0.108
AQ - - −2.160** −0.473 −1.627** −0.356
CAM - - - - −1.507** −0.428
R2 0.017 0.241 0.402
F 0.563 9.111** 15.347**
ΔR 2
- 0.223 0.162
ΔF - 33.810** 30.836**
Note: n = 120; **p < 0.001.

F I G U R E 1 Standardized regression coefficients for the relationship between autistic traits and social competence as mediated by alexithymia
traits. Standardized regression coefficients for the relationship between autistic traits and social competence for (a) autistic and (b) nonautistic
children. Followed by standardized regression coefficients for the mediation of the relationship between autistic traits and social competence by
alexithymia traits for (c) autistic and (d) nonautistic children

traits and social competence. The standardized indirect (120) = −0.761, p < 0.001) and AQ (r(120) = 0.596,
effect was (0.27)(−0.42) = −0.12. We tested the signifi- p < 0.001) scores. CAM scores were also significantly
cance of this indirect effect using bootstrapping proce- correlated with all seven subscales of the MSCS
dures (Hayes, 2013). Unstandardized indirect effects were (p < 0.05). MSCS scores were also significantly correlated
computed for each of 10,000 bootstrapping samples. The with AQ scores (r(120) = −0.694, p < 0.001). However,
bootstrapped unstandardized indirect effect was −0.52 IQ scores were not significantly correlated with CAM,
with a 95% confidence interval that ranged from −0.97 to MSCS, or AQ scores.
−0.18. Thus, the indirect effect was statistically signifi- Step 1 of the model indicated that age, sex, and IQ did
cant. However, given that the relationship between autis- not account for significant variance in MSCS scores
tic traits and social competence was reduced but still (R2 = 0.049, F(3, 117) = 2.014, p = 0.116). With the addi-
significant with alexithymia traits added to the model, tion of AQ scores in the second step of the model an addi-
alexithymia traits only partially mediated this tional 45.8% of variance in MSCS scores was accounted
relationship. for, resulting in a significant model (R2 = 0.508, F
(4, 116) = 29.864, p < 0.001). In step 3 of the model CAM
scores were added, accounting for an additional 17.4% of
Nonautistic group variance in MSCS scores, resulting in a significant model
(R2 = 0.668, F(5, 115) = 49.330, p < 0.001; see Table 3).
Pearson’s correlational analyses indicate that CAM Given AQ scores were significantly correlated with
scores were significantly correlated with total MSCS (r both MSCS and CAM scores, and CAM scores were also
6 SCHEERER ET AL.

TABLE 3 Hierarchical regression analysis for nonautistic participants

Step 1 Step 2 Step 3


Variable B β B β B β

Constant 243.716** - 293.994** - 311.909** -


IQ 0.589 0.179 0.369 0.112 0.14 0.042
Age −2.575 −0.1 1.371 0.053 1.777 0.069
Sex 10.336 0.114 6.631 0.073 6.118 0.068
AQ - - −3.975 −0.694 −2.209 −0.386
CAM - - - - −3.137 −0.526
R2 0.049 0.508 0.682
F 2.014 29.894** 49.330**
ΔR 2
- 0.458 0.174
ΔF - 108.009** 63.080**
Note: n = 121; **p < 0.001.

correlated with MSCS scores, we conducted a mediation found to partially mediate the relationship between autis-
analysis to determine whether social deficits in children tic traits and social competence, suggesting that comor-
with high levels of autistic traits are mediated by bid alexithymia may be partially responsible for the poor
alexithymia traits. As Figure 1 illustrates, the standard- social competence observed in individuals with high
ized regression coefficient between autistic traits and autistic traits.
alexithymia traits was statistically significant, as was the Social communication difficulties are one of the diag-
standardized regression coefficient between alexithymia nostic criteria for ASD, and this association is reflected
traits and social competence. The standardized indirect in the relation between autistic traits and social compe-
effect was (0.60)(−0.54) = −0.32. We tested the signifi- tence seen in our samples. However, alexithymia
cance of this indirect effect using bootstrapping proce- accounted for additional variance in social competence,
dures (Hayes, 2013). Unstandardized indirect effects were supporting the idea that alexithymia may explain some of
computed for each of 10,000 bootstrapping samples. The the heterogeneity in social competence seen in the autistic
bootstrapped unstandardized indirect effect was −1.84 population. The ability to recognize and articulate one’s
with a 95% confidence interval that ranged from −2.59 to emotional experiences, and the experiences of others, is
−1.17. Thus, the indirect effect was statistically signifi- crucial for many social functions such as producing and
cant. However, given the relationship between autistic perceiving nonverbal signals like facial affect and tone of
traits and social competence was reduced, but still signifi- voice, as well as understanding sarcasm. Many of these
cant, with alexithymia traits added to the model, functions appear to be impaired in individuals with
alexithymia traits only partially mediated this alexithymia. For example, Heaton et al. (2012) found
relationship. that in autistic individuals, differences in the ability to
recognize the emotion expressed by changes in vocal
affect were accounted for by alexithymia traits. If co-
DISCUSSION occurring alexithymia makes it difficult for individuals
to produce and perceive social cues, social interactions
The aim of this study was to explore the link between are likely to be confusing, uncomfortable, and ulti-
alexithymia and social competence in autistic and non- mately unsuccessful. When social interactions are per-
autistic children. The results indicate that autistic traits ceived as unsuccessful, they become less rewarding and
accounted for significant variance in social competence pleasurable (Jobe & White, 2007). Accordingly, previ-
in both children with and without ASD, 22.3% and ous studies of alexithymia in the general population
45.8% respectively. However, scores on the childhood have shown that alexithymia is related to social anhedo-
alexithymia measure accounted for an additional 16.2% nia (Prince & Berenbaum, 1993) and decreased satisfac-
and 17.4% of the variance in social competence for autis- tion in intimate relationships (Humphreys et al., 2009).
tic and nonautistic children, respectively, after controlling Furthermore, frequent social blunders are likely to lead
for age, IQ, gender, and autistic traits. These results sug- to social withdrawal, which then limits future opportu-
gest that over and above autistic traits, alexithymia is nities to practice and improve one’s social abilities
related to parent reports of social competence in autistic (Foulkes et al., 2015). Accordingly, a recent study has
and nonautistic children. Notably, alexithymia was shown that alexithymia, not autism, predicted the num-
related to total MSCS scores, as well as each domain of ber of social interactions autistic adults engaged in
the MSCS. Interestingly, alexithymia traits were also (Gerber et al., 2019).
SCHEERER ET AL. 7

Autistic individuals often have fewer social relation- lower levels of social competence regardless of ASD diag-
ships than nonautistic individuals (Locke et al., 2010). nosis. Thus, co-occurring alexithymia may be one rea-
However, ASD has been associated with high levels of son for poor social competence in a subgroup of autistic
loneliness (Jobe & White, 2007; Whitehouse et al., 2009), children. This research highlights emotion recognition
which suggests that autistic individuals desire social inter- and understanding as an important target for identifica-
actions, but these desires are not being fulfilled (Foulkes tion and intervention to improve social competence.
et al., 2015). The current results suggest that the poor With this in mind, when investigating and developing
emotional awareness caused by co-occurring alexithymia interventions for emotional processing difficulties in
may contribute to the social difficulties that make for- autistic individuals, it is important that researchers and
ming and maintaining relationships difficult for autistic clinician measure and control for co-occurring
individuals. Approximately 50% of the autistic popula- alexithymia.
tion has been shown to also have alexithymia, thus these
individuals represent a specific subgroup in the autistic A C K N O W L E D G M EN T S
population that may benefit from interventions targeting Thank you to the families who participated in this
emotional awareness and understanding to improve their research, and the research assistants in the Autism and
social success (Kinnaird et al., 2019). Given alexithymia Developmental Disorders Lab at SFU who helped collect
was also related to social competence in the nonautistic and analyze this data. I would also like to thank Dr.
sample, these findings suggest that emotional processing Ryan Stevenson, Dr. Fakhri Shafai, and Samantha
difficulties may also contribute to social difficulties in Schultz for comments and feedback on the manuscript
nonautistic children, and also represent a target for inter- drafts. This research was supported by Nichole Scheerer’s
vention in nonautistic individuals with social difficulties. NSERC Postdoctoral Fellowship (PDF) held at Simon
However, the other core cluster of symptoms in autism Fraser University, as well as her BrainsCAN Postdoc-
related to restricted and repetitive patterns of behavior toral Fellowship at Western University, funded by the
may create challenges that are unique to autistic individ- Canada First Research Excellence Fund (CFREF).
uals. Additional research is necessary to determine
whether alexithymia traits influence social functioning in CONFLICT OF INTEREST
a similar manner in autistic and nonautistic individuals. The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare,
The results of this study suggest a strong link between financial or otherwise.
alexithymia and social competence, yet we must acknowl-
edge a few limitations of the current study. Anxiety and OR CID
depression are both constructs that have been associated with Nichole E. Scheerer https://orcid.org/0000-0003-0070-
alexithymia and are also highly comorbid with ASD 6559
(Foulkes et al., 2015; Kinnaird et al., 2019), thus it is possible
that anxiety and depression also play a mediating or moder- REF ER ENCE S
ating role in the relations between alexithymia and social American Psychiatric Association (APA). Diagnostic and statistical
competence. In addition, given that the results of this study manual of mental disorders. 5th Ed. Washington: APA; 2013.
Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & Allison, C. (2008).
were obtained by parent report, these findings are limited by
The autism spectrum quotient: Children’s version (AQ-Child).
the extent to which parents can accurately assess their child’s Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(7), 1230–1240.
emotional awareness and understanding. We also cannot Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D., & Taylor, G. J. (1994). The twenty-item
confirm the direction of influence whether emotional difficul- Toronto Alexithymia Scale—I. Item selection and cross-validation
ties lead to poor social competence or vice versa. of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38(1),
23–32.
Baron-Cohen, S., Hoekstra, R. A., Knickmeyer, R., & Wheelwright, S.
(2006). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ)—adolescent version.
CONCLUSION Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(3), 343, 350.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., &
Alexithymia has been associated with a wide range of Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence
emotional processing difficulties, including emotional from asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, malesand
females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and
interoception (Murphy et al., 2018; Silani et al., 2008), Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17.
empathy (Bird et al., 2010), and the recognition of emo- Berthoz, S., & Hill, E. L. (2005). The validity of using self-reports to
tion in others (Cook et al., 2013). Many of these difficul- assess emotion regulation abilities in adults with autism spectrum
ties are often attributed to an ASD diagnosis, but recent disorder. European Psychiatry, 20(3), 291–298.
Berthoz, S., Pouga, L., & Wessa, M. (2011). Alexithymia from the
work suggests that these difficulties may actually be the
social neuroscience perspectiveThe oxford handbook of social neu-
byproduct of co-morbid alexithymia (e.g., Shah roscience. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
et al., 2016; but also c.f. Nicholson et al., 2018). The cur- Bird, G., & Cook, R. (2013). Mixed emotions: The contribution of
rent findings support this hypothesis, indicating that chil- alexithymia to the emotional symptoms of autism. Translational
dren with higher alexithymia traits also have similarly Psychiatry, 3(7), e285
8 SCHEERER ET AL.

Bird, G., Silani, G., Brindley, R., White, S., Frith, U., & Singer, T. impairments do not lie at the heart of autism or alexithymia. Jour-
(2010). Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels nal of Abnormal Psychology, 127(6), 612, 622.
of alexithymia but not autism. Brain, 133(5), 1515–1525. Ospina, L. H., Shanahan, M., Perez-Rodriguez, M. M., Chan, C. C.,
Blair, K. S., Smith, B. W., Mitchell, D. G., Morton, J., Clari, R., & Burdick, K. E. (2019). Alexithymia predicts poorer
Vythilingam, M., Pessoa, L., Fridberg D, Zametkin A, social and everyday functioning in schizophrenia and bipolar dis-
Nelson EE, Drevets WC & Pine, D. S. (2007). Modulation of emo- order. Psychiatry Research, 273, 218–226.
tion by cognition and cognition by emotion. NeuroImage, 35(1), Poquérusse, J., Pastore, L., Dellantonio, S., & Esposito, G. (2018).
430–440. Alexithymia and autism spectrum disorder: A complex relation-
Cook, R., Brewer, R., Shah, P., & Bird, G. (2013). Alexithymia, not ship. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1196.
autism, predicts poor recognition of emotional facial expressions. Prince JD, & Berenbaum H (1993). Alexithymia and hedonic capacity.
Psychological Science, 24(5), 723–732. Journal of Research in Personality 27: 15–22.
Foulkes, L., Bird, G., Gökçen, E., McCrory, E., & Viding, E. (2015). Scheerer, N. E., Aime, H., Boucher, T., & Iarocci, G. (2020). The asso-
Common and distinct impacts of autistic traits and alexithymia on ciation between self-reported camouflaging of autistic traits and
social reward. PLoS One, 10(4), e0121018. social competence in nonautistic young adults. Autism in Adult-
Gaigg, S. B. (2012). The interplay between emotion and cognition in hood , 2(4), 298–306.
autism spectrum disorder: Implications for developmental theory. Shah, P., Hall, R., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2016). Alexithymia, not
Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6, 113. autism, is associated with impaired interoception. Cortex, 81,
Gerber, A. H., Girard, J. M., Scott, S. B., & Lerner, M. D. (2019). 215-220.
Alexithymia–not autism–is associated with frequency of social Sifneos, P. E. (1972). Is dynamic psychotherapy contraindicated for a
interactions in adults. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 123, large number of patients with psychosomatic diseases?. Psycho-
103477. therapy and Psychosomatics, 21(1–6), 133–136.
Hayes, A. F.(2013). Methodology in the social sciences. Introduction to Silani, G., Bird, G., Brindley, R., Singer, T., Frith, C., & Frith, U.
mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A (2008). Levels of emotional awareness and autism: An fMRI
regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press. study. Social Neuroscience, 3(2), 97–112.
Hayes, A. F., & Rockwood, N. J. (2017). Regression-based statistical Trevisan, D. A., Altschuler, M. R., Bagdasarov, A., Carlos, C.,
mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: Observa- Duan, S., Hamo, E., Kala S, McNair ML, Parker T, Stahl D &
tions, recommendations, and implementation. Behaviour Research Winkelman, T. (2019). A meta-analysis on the relationship
and Therapy, 98, 39–57. between interoceptive awareness and alexithymia: Distinguishing
Heaton, P., Reichenbacher, L., Sauter, D., Allen, R., Scott, S., & interoceptive accuracy and sensibility. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
Hill, E. (2012). Measuring the effects of alexithymia on perception chology, 128, 765, 776
of emotional vocalizations in autistic spectrum disorder and typi- Van Kleef, G. A. (2009). How emotions regulate social life: The emo-
cal development. Psychological Medicine, 42(11), 2453–2459. tions as social information (EASI) model. Current Directions in
Humphreys TP, Wood LM, Parker JD (2009) Alexithymia and satisfac- Psychological Science, 18(3), 184–188.
tion in intimate relationships. Personality and Individual Differ- Way, I. F., Applegate, B., Cai, X., Franck, L. K., Black-Pond, C.,
ences 46: 43–47. Yelsma, P., Roberts E., Hyter Y. & Muliett, M. (2010). Children’s
Jobe, L. E., & White, S. W. (2007). Loneliness, social relationships, and Alexithymia Measure (CAM): A new instrument for screening dif-
a broader autism phenotype in college students. Personality and ficulties with emotional expression. Journal of Child & Adolescent
Individual Differences, 42(8), 1479–1489. Trauma, 3(4), 303–318.
Kimhy, D., Vakhrusheva, J., Jobson-Ahmed, L., Tarrier, N., Wechsler, D. (2011). WASI-II: Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelli-
Malaspina, D., & Gross, J. J. (2012). Emotion awareness and reg- gence. San Antonio, TX: NCS Pearson.
ulation in individuals with schizophrenia: Implications for social Whitehouse, A. J., Durkin, K., Jaquet, E., & Ziatas, K. (2009). Friend-
functioning. Psychiatry Research, 200(2–3), 193–201. ship, loneliness and depression in adolescents with Asperger’s Syn-
Kinnaird, E., Stewart, C., & Tchanturia, K. (2019). Investigating drome. Journal of Adolescence, 32(2), 309–322.
alexithymia in autism: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Yager, J., & Iarocci, G. (2013). The development of the multi-
European Psychiatry, 55, 80–89. dimensional social competence scale: A standardized measure of
Kooiman, C. G., Spinhoven, P., & Trijsburg, R. W. (2002). The assess- social competence in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research,
ment of alexithymia: A critical review of the literature and a psy- 6(6), 631–641.
chometric study of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20. Journal of
Psychosomatic Research, 53(6), 1083–1090.
SUPPORTING INFORMATION
Locke, J., Ishijima, E. H., Kasari, C., & London, N. (2010). Loneliness,
friendship quality and the social networks of adolescents with
Additional supporting information may be found online
high-functioning autism in an inclusive school setting. Journal of in the Supporting Information section at the end of this
Research in Special Educational Needs, 10(2), 74–81. article.
Murphy, J., Brewer, R., Hobson, H., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2018). Is
alexithymia characterised by impaired interoception? Further evi-
dence, the importance of control variables, and the problems with How to cite this article: Scheerer NE, Boucher TQ,
the Heartbeat Counting Task. Biological Psychology, 136, 189–197.
Iarocci G. Alexithymia is related to poor social
Nemiah, J., Freyberger, H., & Sifneos, P. E. (1976). Alexithymia: A
view of the psychosomatic process. Modern Tends in Psychoso- competence in autistic and nonautistic children.
matic Medicine 3: 430–439. Autism Research. 2021;1–8. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Nicholson, T. M., Williams, D. M., Grainger, C., Christensen, J. F., aur.2485
Calvo-Merino, B., & Gaigg, S. B. (2018). Interoceptive

You might also like