You are on page 1of 11

Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2018) 53:1361–1370

https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-018-1607-x

ORIGINAL PAPER

Violent interpersonal trauma predicts aggressive thoughts


and behaviors towards self and others: findings from the National
Comorbidity Survey-Adolescent Supplement
Teah‑Marie Bynion1 · Renee Cloutier2 · Heidemarie Blumenthal2 · Emily R. Mischel1 · Sasha M. Rojas1 ·
Ellen W. Leen‑Feldner1

Received: 28 November 2017 / Accepted: 18 September 2018 / Published online: 25 September 2018
© Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2018

Abstract
Purpose The purpose of the current study is to examine the relation between adolescent traumatic event exposure and aggres-
sion directed towards the self and others. Theoretical perspectives underscore the particularly pernicious effects of violent
traumas intentionally perpetrated by others in terms of negative posttraumatic outcomes. However, a careful comparison
of trauma type in relation to aggression has not been done with youth. The current project, therefore, examined differences
between youth with a history of violent interpersonal trauma (VIT) compared to those with a non-violent trauma type his-
tory in terms of aggressive behavior directed towards others as well as aggressive self-directed thoughts (suicidal ideation).
Method The sample was drawn from the National Comorbidity Survey-Replication and consisted of 1928 adolescents
(M = 15.4; SD = 1.47 years), each assigned to one of four age- and gender-matched trauma history groups.
Results Consistent with hypotheses, among trauma-exposed youth, those who had experienced or witnessed a VIT (com-
pared to those reporting a non-VIT only) evidenced elevated aggression, even after controlling for a number of theoretically
relevant covariates (e.g., gender, age, household income, demographic factors).
Conclusion Results indicate that youth with a history of VIT may be at unique risk for experiencing aggression directed
towards others and the self compared to those without this specific trauma history. This is important because trauma type
is an easily assessed indicator of potentially elevated risk for these types of deleterious outcomes. Findings are situated in a
research agenda aimed at continuing to refine our understanding of the link between interpersonal trauma and aggression.

Keywords Adolescents · Violent interpersonal trauma · Aggression · Suicidal ideation

Introduction mental health treatment) and indirect (e.g., law enforcement


expenditures) costs related to aggression. Such costs do not
Aggression perpetrated by youth, whether in the context of always include intangible costs related to pain and suffering
events such as school shootings (other-directed aggression) or death-related losses [1]. Indeed, recent estimates indicate
or suicidal behavior (self-directed aggression), remains a that both self-harm and interpersonal violence remain one of
pressing public health matter. In addition to human suf- the leading causes of death among adolescents [2]. One fac-
fering, billions are spent annually in direct (e.g., medical/ tor that may be linked to both types of aggression is a history
of violent interpersonal trauma (VIT) [3]. Given the unique
opportunities and challenges that characterize adolescence,
* Teah‑Marie Bynion including high rates of traumatic event exposure [4], the
trbynion@uark.edu
current study examined whether the experience of a violent
* Ellen W. Leen‑Feldner interpersonal traumatic event would relate to elevated self
eleenfe@uark.edu
and other aggression among youth.
1
Department of Psychological Science, University
of Arkansas, 216 Memorial Hall, Fayetteville, AR 72701,
USA
2
University of North Texas, Denton, USA

13
Vol.:(0123456789)
1362 Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2018) 53:1361–1370

Traumatic event exposure Current study

Traumatic event exposure, defined by witnessing or expe- The current study aimed to address this gap in the litera-
riencing a life-threatening event or sexual violation [5], is ture by examining the relation between traumatic event
common among adolescents. Nationally representative find- type and aggression towards the self and others within a
ings suggest 60–70% of youth report experiencing at least large sample of adolescents. It was predicted that youth who
one traumatic event [5, 6]. Although most people recover experienced a VIT, relative to those who experienced a non-
from traumatic event exposure without developing problems, violent interpersonal trauma (non-VIT) and those with no
there is a great deal of heterogeneity in outcomes among history of trauma (No Trauma), would evidence elevated
those who do develop problems [7]. That is, traumatic event aggressive behavior towards others (e.g., hitting others) and
exposure is linked to an array of negative outcomes, includ- self-directed aggressive thoughts (i.e., suicidal ideation).
ing mood disorders, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), Because both gender [24, 25] and age [25, 26] relate posi-
and substance use problems [8–10]. Further, traumatic event tively to these outcomes, case control matching procedures
exposure is linked with increased risk for both suicidal were utilized to equate groups on these factors.
behavior [11] and aggression towards others [12]. Thus,
trauma type is an important factor to consider in relation to
traumatic event exposure and aggression towards the self Method
and others.
Design overview
Trauma type
Data were drawn from the National Comorbidity Survey-
Extant data indicate traumatic events of a violent, interper- Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A), a nationally representa-
sonal nature (e.g., abuse, sexual assault), compared to events tive cross-sectional survey designed to assess the prevalence
such as car accidents or natural disasters are associated with and psychosocial correlates of the Diagnostic and Statisti-
elevated rates of PTSD among adults [13, 14] and youth [4]. cal Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) [5] diagnoses
Indeed, traumatic events perpetrated by others, particularly among adolescents 13–18 years of age. Study background,
when they occur in childhood [15], are linked with anxiety, methods, and sampling procedures for the complete sample
substance use, and other stress-related problems [16, 17]. (N = 10,148) is comprehensively described elsewhere [27,
From a theoretical perspective, traumatic events involving 28]. Briefly, the survey was based on a dual-frame design
intentional violence by others runs counter to the impetus that recruited adolescents from households who had par-
to form and maintain social bonds as a means of ensuring ticipated in the National Comorbidity Survey-Replication
survival [18]. Accordingly, traumatic experiences in which (NCS-R; n = 879), and from a representative sample of
another person violates accepted behavioral norms may be schools in the same communities as the NCS-R households
more insidious than traumatic experiences involving inter- (n = 9,244). Adolescents completed face-to-face, computer-
personal loss, natural disasters, or accidents, and by virtue assisted interviews [including a modified Composite Inter-
of disrupting social bonding systems, facilitate aggression national Diagnostic Interview (CIDI)] [28, 29] in their home
towards others and the self. In line with this view, Kisiel and with professional interviewers.
colleagues [19], in a large sample of children entering the
state child welfare custody system (M = 5.2 years), found Participants
repeated exposure to physical or sexual abuse or family vio-
lence was linked with a twofold increase in anger control Given the large sample size differences across trauma his-
problems, poor self-regulation, and danger to others com- tory, we age- and gender-matched adolescents who experi-
pared to “other” traumatic events (e.g., single incident, non- enced a VIT with those who experienced a non-VIT, Both,
violent trauma). Such findings are consistent with studies and No Trauma to create four groups of participants with
that link sexual assault [20], childhood abuse [21], family 482 cases each. Participants endorsing “Other” or “Private
violence [22], and peer victimization [23] with self and other Events” were excluded (n = 982). Private events were meas-
aggression among youth. To the best of our knowledge, how- ured by asking, “… did you ever have an extremely upset-
ever, no studies have specifically compared VIT exposure, ting or life-threatening event that you did not tell me about
defined as traumatic events characterized by intentional vio- because you did not want to talk about it?”. After match-
lence perpetrated by others, to other types of trauma in terms ing, the final sample included 1,928 adolescents (M = 15.4,
of associations with forms of aggression among adolescents. SD = 1.47; 51.9% girls). Please see Tables 1 and 2 for

13
Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2018) 53:1361–1370 1363

Table 1  Sample descriptive No trauma IPV only Non-IPV only Both Total
statistics n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%) N (%)

Demographics
Age (M, SD) 15.4 (1.47) 15.4 (1.47) 15.4 (1.47) 15.4 (1.47) 15.4 (1.47)
Gender (female) 250 (51.9) 250 (51.9) 250 (51.9) 250 (51.9) 250 (51.9)
Race/ethnicity
Hispanic 82 (17.0) 116 (24.1) 67 (13.9) 112 (23.2) 377 (19.6)
Black 63 (13.1) 89 (18.5) 116 (24.1) 120 (24.9) 388 (20.1)
Other 25 (5.2) 33 (6.8) 30 (6.2) 29 (6.0) 117 (6.1)
White 312 (64.7) 244 (50.6) 269 (55.8) 221 (45.9) 1046 (54.3)
Household Income
Median $40–44,999 $25–29,999 $40–44,999 $30–34,999 $35–39,999
Below poverty ratio 83 (17.2) 96 (19.9) 91 (18.9) 99 (20.5) 369 (19.1)
DSM-IV diagnoses
PTSD – 30 (6.2) 12 (2.5) 54 (11.2) 96 (5.0)
Major depressive episode 40 (8.3) 98 (20.3) 46 (9.5) 115 (23.9) 299 (15.5)
Externalizing disorders 36 (7.5) 97 (20.1) 46 (9.5) 161 (33.4) 340 (17.6)
CD 9 (1.9) 44 (9.1) 12 (2.5) 85 (17.6) 150 (7.8)
ODD 28 (5.8) 71 (14.7) 38 (7.9) 110 (22.8) 247 (12.8)
Substance use disorder 32 (6.6) 90 (18.7) 41 (8.5) 115 (23.9) 278 (14.4)
Alcohol abuse 16 (3.3) 36 (7.5) 27 (5.6) 61 (12.7) 140 (7.3)
Alcohol dependence 3 (0.6) 7 (1.5) 1 (0.2) 12 (2.5) 23 (1.2)
Drug abuse 20 (4.1) 55 (11.4) 27 (5.6) 61 (12.7) 163 (8.5)
Drug dependence 2 (0.4) 10 (2.1) 3 (0.6) 29 (6.0) 44 (2.3)

n = 482 in each group; N = 1928 total. Poverty ratio was defined using household family income for the
past-year relative to the federally defined poverty line based on family size. Adolescents with a household
family income of three or more times the poverty level were considered ‘above’ (reference) and adolescents
with a household family income less than three times the poverty line were considered ‘below’
IPV interpersonal violence, PTSD post-traumatic stress disorder, DSM-IV diagnostic criteria, CD/ODD
meeting criteria for either conduct or oppositional defiance disorder

descriptive statistics and specific traumatic events observed with theoretical models discussed in the introduction, these
in the current sample. events reflect the perpetration of intentional violence by oth-
ers. Eight items corresponding to non-VIT events were also
Measures identified (e.g., serious illness, major disaster). Here, the
non-VIT category combines categories from McLaughlin
Traumatic events and colleagues [4], who distinguished between “accidents”
(e.g., car accidents, accidentally hurting another person)
Participants were administered a modified version of the and “network/witnessing” events (e.g., unexpected death of
World Health Organization Composite International Diag- a loved one). Because “other” and “private” events were
nostic Interview (CIDI) [28, 29], a structured diagnostic not defined and could conceivably include VIT or non-VIT
interview with excellent psychometric properties [30]. For events, these events were excluded from analyses.
the purpose of the NCS-A study, the CIDI was modified to
include multiple indicators of emotional functioning (e.g., Suicidal ideation
suicidality) and for use with youth (e.g., language, compre-
hension) [31]. Lifetime exposure to traumatic events was Self-directed aggressive thoughts were assessed with a sin-
assessed via the PTSD module of the CIDI. The PTSD mod- gle yes/no item from the suicidality module of the CIDI that
ule has demonstrated good test–retest and inner-rater reli- was administered to all adolescents (i.e., have you ever seri-
ability in previous samples [30, 32] and is consistent with ously thought about killing yourself?). Skip logic employed
DSM-IV criteria. Following McLaughlin and colleagues in the interview required participants to first endorse suicidal
[4], nine items corresponding to nine VIT event types were ideation before being asked about plans or attempts. Due
identified (e.g., robbery, physical abuse, rape). Consistent to the low number of participants endorsing suicidal plans

13
1364 Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2018) 53:1361–1370

Table 2  Prevalence of exposure Traumatic event VIT Only Non-VIT Only Both Total
to trauma by group n = 482 n = 482 n = 482 N = 1446
n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%)

VIT
Kidnapped 4 (0.09) – 17 (3.5) 21(1.1)
Physical abuse by caregiver 10 (2.1) – 37 (7.7) 73 (5.0)
Beaten up by partner 9 (1.8) – 19 (3.9) 28 (1.9)
Beaten up by anyone else 84 (17.4) – 93 (19.3) 177 (12.2)
Mugged 108 (22.4) – 170 (35.4) 278 (19.2)
Raped 50 (10.4) – 56 (11.6) 106 (7.3)
Other sexual molestation 72 (14.9) – 84 (17.4) 156 (10.8)
Stalked 95 (19.7) – 94 (19.5) 189 (13.1)
Witnessed domestic violence 176 (36.5) – 169 (35.1) 345 (23.9)
Non-VIT
Car accident – 80 (16.6) 69 (14.3) 149 (10.3)
Other serious accident – 50 (10.4) 98 (20.3) 148 (10.2)
Major disaster – 116 (24.1) 94 (19.5) 210 (14.5)
Serious illness – 48 (9.9) 65 (13.5) 113 (7.8)
Unexpected death of loved one – 262 (54.4) 280 (58.1) 542 (37.5)
Someone else stressful experience – 57 (11.8) 103 (21.5) 160 (11.1)
Saw bad injury/death – 74 (15.4) 151 (31.3) 225 (15.6)
Accidentally hurt another person – 5 (1.0) 16 (3.3) 21 (1.5)

The total (N = 1446) represents the percentage of traumatic event type endorsed given event in the VIT or
non-VIT only plus the number who endorsed the event in the “Both” group; non-VIT, reported at least one
non violent interpersonal trauma only; VIT, reported at least one violent interpersonal trauma only; Both,
reported at least one non-VIT and one VIT

(n = 69) and attempts (n = 64), the current study focused Household income was based on the ratio of income to
exclusively on aggressive thoughts related to killing oneself. poverty line and was used as a proxy for socioeconomic
status (SES). Poverty ratio was defined using household
Other‑focused aggression family income for the past year relative to the federally
defined poverty line based on family size. To evaluate the
Aggression towards others was assessed with three yes/no incremental predictive validity of trauma type on self/other
items of the CIDI that addressed attacks of anger which led aggression, lifetime history of theoretically-relevant dis-
to breaking items of value (have you ever in your life had orders was selected as indices of psychiatric risk. These
attacks of anger when all of a sudden you lost control and included any substance use disorders, externalizing disor-
broke or smashed something worth more than a few hun- ders (oppositional defiant disorder or conduct disorder),
dred dollars?), attacking another person (have you ever had major depressive episode, and PTSD [37]. To provide
attacks of anger when all of a sudden you lost control and stable parameter estimates and avoid ‘overfitting’ [38],
hit or tried to hurt someone?), or, if responding ‘no’ to any we elected to merge conceptually similar diagnoses with
actual attacks, threats towards another person (have you ever low sample prevalence rates (< 15%) thereby reducing the
had attacks of anger when all of a sudden you lost control total number of additional predictors in the model. Specifi-
and threatened to hit or hurt someone?). cally, alcohol abuse, alcohol dependence, drug abuse, and
drug dependence were consolidated to represent ‘any life-
Covariates time substance use disorder’. Conduct disorder and ODD
were consolidated to represent ‘any lifetime externalizing
To compute adjusted odds ratios (OR), several factors disorder’. Finally, lifetime MDE (15.5% in sample) and
were considered as covariates due to their association PTSD (5%) were retained as individual predictors given
with aggression. Specifically, race/ethnicity [33], house- their conceptual relationship to the outcomes (e.g., suici-
hold income [34], and relevant lifetime DSM-IV diagno- dality) and primary predictors. Although age and gender
ses [35, 36] were considered as additional predictors to are also related to aggression [22, 39, 40], these variables
include in the final conditional logistic regression models. were already methodologically addressed by the age- and

13
Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2018) 53:1361–1370 1365

gender- matching procedure and, therefore, were not negative outcomes, two sets of analyses were conducted—
included as additional predictors. the first included No Trauma as the reference group and
excluded PTSD as a predictor, the second included non-VIT
Data analytic approach event as the reference group while excluding the No Trauma
group and included PTSD as a predictor. These analyses pro-
Case control matching vide comparison groups which include a relevant ‘baseline’
for what a low-risk (i.e., non-trauma exposed) population is
Adolescents exposed to VIT events only (n = 482) were likely to experience.
exact matched with adolescents in three different compari-
son groups on relevant demographics (i.e., age and gender).
The larger NCS-A dataset includes a substantially larger pro- Results
portion of adolescents outside of the VIT group with com-
plete gender and age data making this an ideal application Descriptive/preliminary analyses
of a 1:1 exact matching procedure [41]. Exact matches were
selected at random, without replacement using the ‘Case- Following the coding scheme for classifying potentially
Control Matching’ Python extension command in SPSS 22 traumatic events outlined by McLaughlin and colleagues
[42, 43]. Sampling weights were not applied.1 [4], prevalence of trauma exposure by group was examined
(Table 2). Unadjusted odds ratios indicated a significant
Preliminary and primary analyses difference in the proportion of cases from the four groups
endorsing serious thoughts of suicidality, having attacks of
Descriptive data for the final sample of 1928 participants anger where they broke something, having attacks of anger
were examined (see Table 1). The sample consisted of four where they hit/attempted to hit someone, or an anger attack
equal groups: those who experienced a VIT, those who where they threatened to hit another person. Specifically,
experienced a non-VIT, Both, or No trauma. A series of adolescents in the VIT and Both groups endorsed signifi-
cross-tabulations and Chi-square tests were conducted to cantly more (a) serious thoughts of suicidality, (b) anger
examine the relation between these four groups and aggres- attacks where valuables were broken, and (c) anger attacks
sion-related outcomes. Unadjusted ORs were then calculated where someone else was hit compared to those in the No
to determine the extent to which the two groups exposed to Trauma group and non-VIT groups.2 Please see Fig. 1 for
VIT or Both were associated with increased odds of each the percentage of each outcome by trauma group.
outcome occurring in comparison to No Trauma and non- Among the subsample of youth responding to the ques-
VIT exposure. Cross-tabulations and Chi-square analyses tion of anger attacks including the threat of hitting someone,
testing the associations of potential covariates with each out- threats were endorsed by a significantly smaller proportion
come also were conducted. To ensure that the overall model of adolescents in the No Trauma group than the VIT only
explains more variance in our outcomes than the null, we and Both group, but not the non-VIT only group. Accord-
examined whether there was a statistically significant change ingly, we did not include threatening aggression as an out-
in variance accounted for by the overall model than what come in primary analyses; we return to this issue in the
would be expected by chance. Variables related to at least discussion.
one of the outcomes were included in the final conditional Of the six covariates tested, only household income
logistic regression analyses [44]. was unrelated to any outcomes. Therefore, the final set of
Finally, adjusted ORs and associated 95% confidence analyses include race/ethnicity, substance use disorder,
intervals (CIs) were calculated using logistic regression to
determine the extent to which VIT exposure was associated
with increased odds of each outcome in comparison to No 2
Chi square tests of independence indicated a significant difference
Trauma and non-VIT exposure, after adjusting for identified in the proportion of cases from the four groups endorsing serious
covariates. To examine the extent to which trauma expo- thoughts of suicidality [χ2 (3, N = 1876) = 59.469, p < .001], hav-
sure type is associated with increased risk for experiencing ing attacks of anger where they broke something [χ2 (3, N = 1928)
= 102.618, p < .001], having attacks of anger where they hit/
attempted to hit someone [χ2 (3, N = 1927) = 117.242, p < .001], or
an anger attack where they threatened to hit another person [χ2 (3,
1
Sampling weights for the NCS-A dataset are often used to ensure N = 1090) = 13.381, p = .004]. Specifically, (a) serious thoughts of
analyses are conducted on a nationally representative sample [28]. suicidality, (b) anger attacks where valuables were broken, and (c)
However, because we selected a sub-sample of adolescents based on anger attacks where someone else was hit each were endorsed by
history of exposure to a traumatic event, our sample is not nationally fewer adolescents in the No Trauma and non-VIT groups, and signifi-
representative of the US adolescent population thus sampling weights cantly more adolescents in the VIT and Both groups, than expected
were not applied. by chance.

13
1366 Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2018) 53:1361–1370

Fig. 1  Percentage of reported 40

Percent Endorsed by Youth


self and other aggression by 35
trauma group 30
25
20
15
10
5
0
Suicidal Ideation (n=473) Breaking Items (n=700) Hitting Others (n=838)
Types of Aggression by Trauma Group
No Trauma non-VIT VIT Both

externalizing disorders, major depressive episode, and PTSD risk for aggression-related outcomes. As expected, results
(for trauma exposed sample only) as covariates. suggest that exposure to events that involve the intentional
perpetration of violence by others is uniquely associated
Primary analyses/logistic regression/adjusted odds with endorsement of both self-directed aggression (suicidal
ratios ideation) and aggression towards others.

In the total sample with No Trauma used as the reference Self‑directed aggression
group, the conditional logistic regression models fit the
data significantly better than the null model for: suicidal First, with regard to suicidal ideation, analyses from the
ideation, anger where objects are broken, and anger involv- total sample indicated that, compared to adolescents who
ing hitting others. Overall, VIT and Both group member- had not been exposed to trauma, those with a history of
ship was associated with greater endorsements of suicidal VIT were more than twice as likely to endorse having
ideation (OR = 2.28, 95% CI = 1.50–3.46; OR = 2.08; seriously thought about killing themselves. This was not
95% CI = 1.36–3.20), breaking items (OR = 2.21, 95% the case for those who were exposed to events such as a
CI = 1.65–2.96; OR = 2.30; 95% CI = 1.71–3.11), and hit- car accident or serious illness—there was no difference
ting others (OR = 2.08; 95% CI = 1.57–2.75; OR = 2.42; 95% between youth with non-VIT history and those with no
CI = 1.82–3.23) compared to membership in the No Trauma trauma in terms of suicidal ideation. A similar pattern was
group. In contrast, there were no differences on any of these observed within the trauma-exposed groups; those with
outcomes between the non-VIT and No Trauma groups. Sim- a history of VIT were significantly more likely to report
ilarly, in the trauma exposed sample with non-VIT trauma suicidal ideation compared to those whose trauma his-
as the reference group, the conditional logistic regression tory was not characterized by violent events intentionally
models fit the data significantly better than the null model for perpetrated by others (i.e., non-VIT history). Importantly,
each outcome and the VIT only and Both groups were asso- groups were matched on age and gender, and observed
ciated with greater endorsements on each of the outcomes. findings held after controlling for a number of theoreti-
Please see Table 3 for predictor-specific adjusted odds ratios. cally relevant covariates. These results accord with previ-
ous work linking trauma generally [11] and interpersonal
events such as sexual assault specifically [20] to suicide
Discussion risk behavior among youth. The current findings extend
the literature by comparing specific types of trauma as
The current study is the first to compare violent interper- they relate to both suicidal ideation as well as aggression
sonal traumatic events and other trauma types in relation to towards others among youth, and underscore the particu-
both aggressive self-directed thoughts, as well as aggres- larly destructive effects of traumatic event exposure that
sive behaviors towards others within a large sample of ado- involves the intentional perpetration of violence by oth-
lescents. Although previous empirical research indicates ers. Specifically, such experiences are correlated with an
aggression is elevated following traumatic event exposure increased likelihood of seriously considering taking one’s
generally [19, 21], less is known about the role of trauma life. An important next step will be to extend these findings
type and its link to various forms of aggression in this rela- to other indicators of suicidal behavior. Although suicidal
tion, particularly among adolescents. This is important ideation is predictive of other suicidal behaviors [45], the
because of the relative ease with which traumatic event manner in which self-directed aggression was operational-
type can be assessed, thereby identifying youth at elevated ized here (i.e., suicidal ideation) does not include active

13
Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2018) 53:1361–1370 1367

Table 3  Odds ratios of group Variables Suicidal ideation Breaking items Hitting others
on outcomes adjusting for
sociodemographics and relevant AOR 95% CI AOR 95% CI AOR (95% CI)
DSM-IV diagnoses (No Trauma
and non-VIT as reference) No Trauma as reference (N = 1928)
Race/ethnicity
NH White (ref) 1.0 (ref) [0.52–1.05] 1.0 (ref) 1.0 (ref)
Hispanic 0.739 0.907 [0.69–1.18] 0.85 [0.66–1.10]
NH Black 0.510* [0.34–0.765] 1.07 [0.82–1.39] 1.35* [1.05–1.74]
Other 0.531 [0.28–1.00] 1.34 [0.89–2.02] 1.15 [0.76–1.74]
Lifetime diagnosis
Any SUD 2.19* [1.57–3.06] 1.67* [1.26–2.23] 1.61* [1.20–2.16]
CD/ODD 1.48* [1.07–2.06] 2.28* [1.75–2.98] 3.00* [2.28–3.96]
MDE 3.98* [2.95–5.37] 2.03* [1.56–2.65] 1.77 [1.34–2.33]
Group
No Trauma 1.0 (ref) 1.0 (ref) 1.0 (ref)
Non-VIT 1.137 [0.71–1.82] 1.28 [0.95–1.73] 1.24 [0.94–1.65]
VIT 2.28* [1.50–3.46] 2.21* [1.65–2.96] 2.08* [1.57–2.75]
Both 2.08* [1.36–3.20] 2.30* [1.71–3.11] 2.42* [1.82–3.23]
Non-VIT as reference (n = 1446)
Race/ethnicity
NH White (ref) 1.0 (ref) 1.0 (ref) 1.0 (ref)
Hispanic 0.75 [0.51–1.09] 0.995 [0.74–1.33] 0.98 [0.73–1.31]
NH Black 0.53* [0.35–0.81] 1.04 [0.78–1.38] 1.42* [1.08–1.87]
Other 0.57 [0.29–1.11] 1.39 [0.88–2.21] 1.36 [0.85–2.15]
Lifetime diagnosis
Any SUD 2.07* [1.45–2.95] 1.80* [1.32–2.45] 1.78* [1.30–2.45]
CD/ODD 1.40 [0.99–1.99] 2.09* [1.57–2.78] 2.56* [1.90–3.45]
MDE 3.51* [2.53–4.88] 2.05* [1.52–2.75] 1.71* [1.26–2.31]
PTSD 1.55 [0.94–2.57] 0.811 [0.51–1.29] 0.80 [0.50–1.27]
Group
Non-VIT 1.0 (ref) 1.0 (ref) 1.0 (ref)
VIT 2.00* [1.34–3.00] 1.72* [1.30–2.28] 1.68* [1.28–2.20]
Both 1.82* [1.20–2.75] 1.83* [1.37–2.44] 1.99* [1.51–2.63]

Non-VIT as Reference group excludes No Trauma; AOR, adjusted odds ratio; NH, non-Hispanic; any SUD,
meeting DSM-IV criteria for either alcohol or other substance use disorder (excluding nicotine); any CD/
ODD, meeting criteria for either conduct or oppositional defiance disorder; any MDE, meeting criteria for
major depressive episodes
*OR values are statistically different from reference group at p < .05

suicidal ideation or suicide attempts. Future studies should [49] as well as deliberate self-harm [50] via longitudinal
consider assessing the link between trauma type and fac- designs. Further, theoretical and empirical work suggests
tors further along the continuum from ideation to action that intrapersonal and interpersonal distress in response
[46]. Additionally, the current cross-sectional study design to a traumatic event is also linked to an increased risk for
precludes temporal conclusions. Notably, empirical work intentional self-harm [51, 52] and mediates the association
which includes longitudinal studies suggests interpersonal between psychopathology (e.g., depression) and suicidal
trauma (e.g., victimization, abuse) typically precedes sui- behaviors [53]. Therefore, potential mediators of the rela-
cidal ideation [47, 48]. Indeed, suicidal ideation has not tion between specific types of traumatic event exposure
been examined as a precipitant risk factor for experienc- and aggression towards self and others will be important
ing a violent interpersonal trauma. Nonetheless, an impor- to consider in future research. Examples of such media-
tant next step will be to disentangle temporal patterning tors include subjective levels of distress, emotion regula-
between traumatic event exposure and suicidal ideation tion difficulties, quality and quantity of social support, and
negative belief systems [54–56].

13
1368 Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2018) 53:1361–1370

Aggression towards others at play, such as a dispositional tendency toward impulsiv-


ity, which may elevate risk for both traumatic event expo-
Similarly, with regard to aggression towards others, analyses sure and aggression [60]. Laboratory-based experimental
of the total sample indicated that those with a history of VIT work (e.g., manipulation of imaginal exposure to various
were more than twice as likely to endorse breaking items or trauma types) would increase confidence in the role of spe-
hitting others compared to those who had not been exposed cific trauma types as they relate to aggression towards self
to trauma. Further, there was no difference between youth and others. Second, it will be important to examine study
with non-VIT history and those with no trauma in terms hypotheses using approaches other than exclusively self-
of other-focused aggression. Within the trauma exposed report. Future work may usefully incorporate multi-modal
groups, those who endorsed VIT were more than twice as assessments such as a death/suicide implicit association
likely to endorse breaking things and hitting others when test [44] or multi-informant report to bolster confidence
compared to those with a non-VIT history. These results are that findings are not unduly influenced by memory or
consistent with work linking traumatic event exposure gener- mood-related biases. Third, an exact-case matching pro-
ally [12], and intentional harm inflicted by another person cedure was employed to reduce bias [61]. As suggested,
specifically (e.g., bullying) [23], with displays of aggression. the current sample was matched without replacement [42]
The current study extends this body of work by providing and sample weights were not applied [27, 62]. Two poten-
evidence that youth exposed to a VIT, even compared to tial drawbacks of the matching approach include loss of
youth with other types of traumatic event exposure, report statistical power from excluding available cases and the
elevated aggressive behavior towards others. From a theo- inability to directly test the estimates on which cases are
retical perspective, surviving an act of violence intentionally matched (here, age and gender). Because the findings are
perpetuated by another person may disrupt the formation consistent when the analyses are conducted in the entire
of normal social bonds and predict aggression [18]. This sample with age/gender included as covariates, the con-
study lays the foundation for future research examining why cerns with the matching approach are minimized. Finally,
traumatic experiences involving intentional perpetration of the current study separately examined self- and other-
violence correlate with aggression. For instance, perhaps directed aggression. However, research points toward a
violent interpersonal trauma results in greater distress during third category of “combined” aggression [63–65]. This
the trauma itself, which contributes to increased aggressive co-morbidity is less common than either form of aggres-
behavior. Another possibility is the negative effects on one’s sion alone. For example, data from a large national sam-
beliefs about the world secondary to such experiences [57]. ple indicates about 3% of adolescents reported both being
Future work could examine the role of theoretically-rele- in a physical fight and attempting suicide during the past
vant mechanisms, including peri-traumatic stress levels and year. Substance use, particularly binge drinking, along
posttraumatic cognitions, in driving the current observed with depressive symptoms were elevated in this combined
differences. group. It is not currently clear what drives the target of
A different pattern was observed for threatening aggres- aggression (e.g., self versus other), although theorists have
sion among the trauma-exposed groups. Here, preliminary suggested specific endogenous factors such as depres-
analyses indicated VIT and Both groups did not differ from sion or resentment may be implicated [66]. Future work
the non-VIT group. Methodological factors may be at play comparing different forms of aggression as a function of
here, as subjects had to say “no” to the question related to trauma history should consider including the comorbidity
hitting someone before the threat question was posed. Fur- between self-and-other aggression as an additional form
ther, developmental scholars contend that youth typically of aggression.
hold relatively accepting beliefs around mild aggression
such as verbal threat, suggesting it may not be a sensitive
outcome indicator in terms of the effects of experiencing a
VIT trauma [58, 59]. This represents an important area for Conclusion
future work.
These limitations notwithstanding, the findings link expe-
Limitations riences of violent interpersonal trauma with elevated dis-
plays of aggression towards others as well as aggressive
Although the current findings extend the literature by self-directed thoughts among a large sample of youth.
underscoring the importance of comparing specific types Important next steps will be to better clarify the nature of
of trauma as they relate to aggression towards self and oth- this relation, as such data can usefully inform interventions
ers, there are a number of important limitations. First, this targeting youth at risk for aggression by virtue of traumatic
study cannot speak to causality. Additional factors may be event history.

13
Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2018) 53:1361–1370 1369

Compliance with ethical standards 15. Zlotnick C, Johnson J, Kohn R, Vicente B, Rioseco P, Saldivia
S (2008) Childhood trauma, trauma in adulthood, and psy-
chiatric diagnoses: results from a community sample. Compr
Conflict of interest On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author
Psychiatry 49(2):163–169. https​: //doi.org/10.1016/j.compp​
states that there is no conflict of interest.
sych.2007.08.007
16. Edgardh K, Ormstad K (2000) Prevalence and characteristics of
sexual abuse in a national sample of Swedish seventeen-year-old
boys and girls. Acta Paediatr 89(3):310–319
References 17. Kilpatrick DG, Acierno R, Saunders B, Resnick HS, Best CL,
Schnurr PP (2000) Risk factors for adolescent substance abuse and
1. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Cost of injury reports. dependence: data from a national sample. J Consult Clin Psychol
Web-based statistics and query reporting system (WISQARS). 68(1):19–30
National Center for Injury Prevention and Control. https​://www. 18. Charuvastra A, Cloitre M (2008) Social bonds and posttrau-
cdc.gov/injur​y/wisqa​rs/index​.html. Retrieved on 8 Aug 2018 matic stress disorder. Annu Rev Psychol 59:301–328. https​://doi.
2. Mokdad AH, Forouzanfar MH, Daoud F, Mokdad AA, Bcher- org/10.1146/annur​ev.psych​.58.11040​5.08565​0
aoui E, Moradi-Lakeh C, M. et al (2016) Global burden of dis- 19. Kisiel CL, Fehrenchab T, Torgersen E, Stolbach B, McClelland
eases, injuries, and risk factors for young people’s health dur- G, Griffin G, Burkman K (2014) Constellations of interpersonal
ing 1990–2013: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of trauma and symptoms in child welfare: Implications for a devel-
Disease Study 2013. The Lancet 387(2):383–2401. https​://doi. opmental trauma framework. J Fam Violence 29:1–14. https:​ //doi.
org/10.1016/S0140​6736(16)00648​-6 org/10.1007/s1089​6-013-9559-0
3. Madfis E (2016) In search of meaning: are school rampage shoot- 20. Tomasula JL, Anderson LM, Littleton HL, Riley-Tillman TC
ings random and senseless violence? J Psychol 151(1):21–35. (2012) The association between sexual assault and suicidal activ-
https​://doi.org/10.1080/00223​980.2016.11961​61 ity in a national sample. School Psychol Q 27(2):109–119. https​
4. McLaughlin KA, Koenen KC, Hill ED, Petukhova M, Sampson ://doi.org/10.1037/a0029​162
NA, Zaslavsky AM, Kessler RC (2013) Trauma exposure and 21. Harford TC, Yi HY, Grant BF (2014) Associations between child-
posttraumatic stress disorder in a national sample of adolescents. hood abuse and interpersonal aggression and suicide attempt
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 52(8):815–830. https​://doi. among US adults in a national study. Child Abuse Neglect
org/10.1016/j.jaac.2013.05.011 38(8):1389–1398. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiab​u.2014.02.011
5. American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and statisti- 22. Sternberg KJ, Baradaran LP, Abbott CB, Lamb ME, Guterman
cal manual of mental disorders, 4th edn (DSM-IV). American E (2006) Type of violence, age, and gender differences in the
Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC effects of family violence on children’s behavior problems: a
6. Copeland WE, Keeler G, Angold A, Costello EJ (2007) Traumatic mega-analysis. Dev Rev 26(1):89–112. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.
events and posttraumatic stress in childhood. Arch Gen Psychiatry dr.2005.12.001
64:577–584. https​://doi.org/10.1037//0022-006X.70.5.1067 23. Burgess AW, Garbarino C, Carlson MI (2006) Pathological teas-
7. Cicchetti D, Toth SL (2009) The past achievements and future ing and bullying turned deadly: shooters and suicide. Victims
promises of developmental psychopathology: the coming of age Offenders 1(1):1–13
of a discipline. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 50(1–2):16–25. https​ 24. Becker DF, Grilo CM (2007) Prediction of suicidality and vio-
://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01979​.x lence in hospitalized adolescents: comparisons by sex. Can J Psy-
8. Kearney CA, Wechsler A, Kaur H, Lemos-Miller A (2010) Post- chiatry 52(9):572–580
traumatic stress disorder in maltreated youth: a review of con- 25. Gvion Y, Apter A (2012) Suicide and suicidal behavior. Public
temporary research and thought. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev Health Rev 34(2):1–20
13(1):46–76. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1056​7-009-0061-4 26. Boeninger DK, Masyn KE, Feldman BJ, Conger RD (2010) Sex
9. Liu H, Petukhova MV, Sampson NA, Aguilar-Gaxiola S, Alonso differences in developmental trends of suicide ideation, plans,
J, Andrade LH et al (2017) Association of DSM-IV posttraumatic and attempts among European American adolescents. Suicide
stress disorder with traumatic experience type and history in the Life Threat Behav 40(5):451–464. https ​ : //doi.org/10.1521/
World Health Organization World Mental Health Surveys. JAMA suli.2010.40.5.451Br​y
Psychiatry 74(3):270–281. https​://doi.org/10.1001/jamap​sychi​ 27. Kessler RC, Avenevoli S, Costello EJ, Green JG, Gruber MJ,
atry.2016.3783 Heeringa S et al (2009) National comorbidity survey replication
10. McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky adolescent supplement (NCS-A): II. Overview and design. J Am
AM, Kessler RC (2012) Childhood adversities and first onset of Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 48(4):380–385
psychiatric disorders in a national sample of US Adolescents. 28. Merikangas KR, Avenevoli S, Costello EJ, Koretz D, Kessler
Arch Gen Psychiatry 69(11):1151–1160. https​://doi.org/10.1001/ RC (2009) National comorbidity survey replication adolescent
archg​enpsy​chiat​ry.2011.2277 supplement (NCS-A): I. Background and measures. J Am Acad
11. Ford JD, Hartman JK, Hawke J, Chapman JC (2008) Traumatic Child Adolesc Psychiatry 48(4):367–379. https:​ //doi.org/10.1097/
victimization posttraumatic stress disorder, suicidal ideation, and CHI.0b013​e3181​9996f​1
substance abuse risk among juvenile justice-involved youths. J 29. Kessler RC, Üstün TB (2004) The world mental health (WMH)
Child Adolesc Trauma 1:75–92 survey initiative version of the world health organization (WHO)
12. Shields A, Cicchetti D (1998) Reactive aggression among mal- composite international diagnostic interview (CIDI). Int J Meth-
treated children: the contributions of attention and emotion dys- ods Psychiatr Res 13(2):93–121
regulation. J Clin Child Psychol 27(4):381–395 30. Reed V, Gander F, Pfister H, Steiger A, Sonntag H, Trenkwalder
13. Frans Ö, Rimmö PA, Åberg L, Fredrikson M (2005) Trauma expo- C et al (1998) To what degree does the Composite International
sure and post-traumatic stress disorder in the general population. Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) correctly identify DSM-IV disor-
Acta Psychiatr Scand 111(4):290–291. https​://doi.org/10.111 ders? Testing validity issues in a clinical sample. Int J Methods
1/j.1600-0447.2004.00463​.x Psychiatr Res 7(3):142–155
14. Shalev AY, Freedman S (2005) PTSD following terrorist attacks: 31. Jensen PS, Watanabe HK, Richters JE (1999) Who’s up first?
A prospective evaluation. Am J Psychiatry 162(6):1188–1191 Testing for order effects in structured interviews using a

13
1370 Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology (2018) 53:1361–1370

counterbalanced experimental design. J Abnorm Child Psychol 50. Liu RT, Miller I (2014) Life events and suicidal ideation and
27(6):439–445 behavior: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev 34:181–192. https​
32. Wittchen HU, Lachner G, Wunderlich U, Pfister H (1998) Test- ://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.01.006
retest reliability of the computerized DSM-IV version of the 51. Giletta M, Scholte RHJ, Engels RCME, Ciairano S, Prinstein MJ
Munich-Composite International Diagnostic Interview (M-CIDI). (2012) Adolescent non-suicidal self-injury: a cross-national study
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 33(11):568–578 of community samples from Italy, the Netherlands and the United
33. Blum RW, Beuhring T, Shew ML, Bearinger LH, Sieving RE, States. Psychiatry Res 197(0):66–72. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Resnick MD (2000) The effects of race/ethnicity, income, and psych​res.2012.02.009
family structure on adolescent risk behaviors. Am J Public Health 52. Nock MK, Prinstein MJ (2005) Contextual features and behavioral
90(12):1879–1884 functions of self-mutilation among adolescents. J Abnorm Psychol
34. Santiago CD, Wadsworth ME, Stump J (2011) Socioeconomic 114(1):140–146. https​://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.114.1.140
status, neighborhood disadvantage, and poverty-related stress: 53. Yates TM, Tracy AJ, Luthar SS (2008) Nonsuicidal self-injury
prospective effects on psychological syndromes among diverse among “privileged” youths: longitudinal and cross-sectional
low-income families. J Econ Psychol 32(2):218–230. https​://doi. approaches to developmental process. J Consult Clin Psychol
org/10.1016/j.joep.2009.10.008 76(1):52–62. https​://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.76.1.52
35. Nock MK, Green JG, Hwang I, McLaughlin KA, Sampson 54. Adrian M, Zeman J, Erdley C, Lisa L, Sim L (2011) Emotional
NA, Zaslavsky AM, Kessler RC (2013) Prevalence, correlates, dysregulation and interpersonal difficulties as risk factors for non-
and treatment of lifetime suicidal behavior among adolescents: suicidal self-injury in adolescent girls. J Abnorm Child Psychol
results from the national comorbidity survey replication adoles- 39(3):389–400. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1080​2-010-9465-3
cent supplement. JAMA Psychiatry 70(3):300–310. https​://doi. 55. Buckholdt KE, Parra GR, Jobe-Shields L (2009) Emotion regula-
org/10.1001/2013.jamap​sychi​atry.55 tion as a mediator of the relation between emotion socialization
36. Nock MK, Borges G, Bromet EJ, Alonso J, Angermeyer M, Beau- and deliberate self-harm. Am J Orthopsychiatry 79(4):482–490.
trais A et al (2008) Cross-national prevalence and risk factors for https​://doi.org/10.1037/a0016​735
suicidal ideation, plans and attempts. Br J Psychiatry 192(2):98– 56. Rojas SM, Feldner MT, Bilsky SA, Dutton C, Badour CL, Leen-
105. https​://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.107.04011​3 Feldner EW (2017) The association between lifetime histories
37. Verona E, Sachs-Ericsson N, Joiner TE Jr (2004) Suicide attempts of PTSD and suicidal ideation among a national sample of ado-
associated with externalizing psychopathology in an epidemio- lescents: the roles of parent and family related social support.
logical sample. Am J Psychiatry 161:444–451 J Anxiety Disord 49:95–103. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxd​
38. Bagley SC, White H, Golomb BA (2001) Logistic regression is.2017.04.006
in the medical literature: standards for use and reporting, with 57. Horowitz MJ (1993) Stress-response syndromes. In: Wilson JP,
particular attention to one medical domain. J Clin Epidemiol Raphael B (eds) International handbook of traumatic stress syn-
54(10):979–985 dromes. Plenum, New York, pp 49–60
39. Crick NR, Grotpeter JK (1995) Relational aggression, gender, and 58. Lim SH, Ang RP (2009) Relationship between boys’ normative
social-psychological adjustment. Child Dev 66(3):710–722 beliefs about aggression and their physical, verbal, and indirect
40. Rhodes AE, Boyle MH, Bridge JA, Sinyor M, Links PS, Tonmyr aggressive behaviors. Adolescence 44(175):635–650
L et al (2014) Antecedents and sex/gender differences in youth 59. Nelson DA, Springer MM, Nelson LJ, Bean NH (2008) Norma-
suicidal behavior. World J Psychiatry 4(4):120–132. https​://doi. tive beliefs regarding aggression in emerging adulthood. Soc Dev
org/10.5498/wjp.v4.i4.120 17(3):638–660
41. Althauser RP, Rubin D (1970) The computerized construction of 60. Jang KL, Stein MB, Taylor S, Asmundson GJ, Livesley WJ (2003)
a matched sample. Am J Sociol 76(2):325–346 Exposure to traumatic events and experiences: aetiological rela-
42. IBM (2013) Python reference guide for IBM SPSS statistics. http:// tionships with personality function. Psychiatry Res 120(1):61–69
www-01.ibm.com/suppo​r t/docvi​ew.wss?uid=swg27​03840​7#en. 61. Rubin DB (1973) Matching to remove bias in observational stud-
Retrieved on 8 Aug 2018 ies. Biometrics 29(1):159–183
43. Stuart EA (2010) Matching methods for causal inference: a review 62. Rudolph KE, Stuart EA, Glass TA, Marques AH, Duncko R,
and a look forward. Stat Sci Rev J Inst Math Stat 25(1):1–21. https​ Merikangas KR (2014) The association between cortisol and
://doi.org/10.1214/09-STS31​3 neighborhood disadvantage in a US population-based sample of
44. Peng CYJ, So TSH (2002) Logistic regression analysis and report- adolescents. Health Place 25:68–77
ing: a primer. Issues Psychol Educ Soc Sci 1(1):31–70 63. Giaconia RM, Reinherz HZ, Silverman AB, Pakiz B, Frost AK,
45. Nock MK, Park JM, Finn CT, Deliberto TL, Dour HJ, Banaji Cohen E (1995) Traumas and posttraumatic stress disorder in a
MR (2010) Measuring the suicidal mind: Implicit cognition pre- community population of older adolescents. J Am Acad Child
dicts suicidal behavior. Psychol Sci 21(4):511–517. https​://doi. Adolesc Psychiatry 34:1369–1380
org/10.1177/09567​97610​36476​2 64. Heilbron N, Prinstein MJ (2010) Adolescent peer victimization,
46. Van Orden KA, Witte TK, Cukrowicz KC, Braithwaite SR, Selby peer status, suicidal ideation, and nonsuicidal self-injury: exam-
EA, Joiner TE Jr (2010) The interpersonal theory of suicide. Psy- ining concurrent and longitudinal associations. Merrill-Palmer Q
chol Rev 117:575–600. https​://doi.org/10.1037/a0018​697 56(3):388–419. https​://doi.org/10.1353/mpq.0.0049
47. Bryan CJ, McNaugton-Cassill M, Osman A, Hernandez AM (2013) 65. Harford TC, Yi H, Freeman RC (2012) A typology of violence
The associations of physical and sexual assault with suicide risk in against self and others and its associations with drinking and other
nonclinical military and undergraduate samples. Suicide Life Threat drug use among high school students in a U.S. general population
Behav 43(2):223–234. https​://doi.org/10.1111/sltb.12011​ survey. J Child Adolesc Subst Abuse 21(4):349–366. https​://doi.
48. Lereya ST, Winsper C, Heron J, Lewis G, Gunnell D, Fisher HL, org/10.1080/10678​28X.2012.71002​8
Wolke D (2013) Being bullied during childhood and the prospec- 66. O’Donnell O, House A, Waterman M (2015) The co-occurrence
tive pathways to self-harm in late adolescence. J Am Acad Child of aggression and self-harm: systematic literature review. J Affect
Adolesc Psychiatry 52(6):608–618. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j. Disord 17(5):325–350. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2014.12.051
jaac.2013.03.012
49. Chang CE (2002) Predicting suicide ideation in an adolescent
population: examining the role of social problem solving as a
moderator and a mediator. Personal Individ Differ 32:1279–1291

13
Social Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemiology is a copyright of Springer, 2018. All Rights
Reserved.

You might also like