You are on page 1of 9

Psychological Assessment Copyright 2002 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

2002, Vol. 14, No. 3, 365–373 1040-3590/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//1040-3590.14.3.365

The Multidimensional Structure of Perfectionism in Clinically Distressed


and College Student Samples
Brian J. Cox, Murray W. Enns, and Ian P. Clara
University of Manitoba

Confirmatory factor analysis was used to evaluate 2 multidimensional measures of perfectionism (R. O.
Frost, P. Marten, C. Lahart, & R. Rosenblate, 1990; P. L. Hewitt & G. L. Flett, 1991). On a first-order
level, support was found for Hewitt and Flett’s (1991) original 3-factor conceptualization of perfection-
ism, although only for an empirically derived 15-item subset. Support was also obtained for 5 of the 6
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

dimensions proposed by R. O. Frost et al. (1990), but the model only displayed good fit when a refined
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

scale containing 22 of the original 35 items was used. A second-order analysis found evidence for 2
higher-order factors of adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism. Perfectionism dimensions correlated in
expected directions with personality domains, symptom distress, and academic achievement. The brief
measures of perfectionism also retained the construct-related validity displayed by the full-item versions.

The potential harmfulness of perfectionism has been recognized fectionistic motivation for one’s self), Socially Prescribed Perfec-
for many years in clinical writings, particularly in the context of tionism (the perception that others hold excessively high standards
depression (e.g., Adler, 1956; Beck, 1967; Hamachek, 1978; Hol- for one’s self), and Other-Oriented Perfectionism (holding unreal-
lender, 1965). Recurrent themes in these early descriptions of istic standards of performance for significant others). The HF MPS
maladaptive perfectionism include unrealistically high and inflex- contains 45 items in total, with 15 items assigned to each subscale.
ible standards for performance, self-criticism and fear of failure, Reliability and validity indices evidenced for the HF MPS have
inability to experience satisfaction from performance, and a per- been impressive (e.g., Hewitt, Flett, Turnbull-Donovan, & Mikail,
ceived discrepancy between performance and standards. In recent 1991). In general, the Socially Prescribed subscale has been the
years there has been considerable growth in the perfectionism dimension most consistently implicated with several forms of
literature, due in large part to developments in the operationaliza- distress, including depressed and anxious mood states, hopeless-
tion and measurement of the perfectionism construct. Working ness, and even suicidal ideation. Exploratory factor analysis (EFA)
from different conceptual frameworks, two groups of investigators was used to identify subscales in a college student sample in the
independently developed instruments entitled Multidimensional initial development of the HF MPS (Hewitt & Flett, 1991). How-
Perfectionism Scales (MPS; Frost, Marten, Lahart, & Rosenblate, ever, despite the clear and well-developed model of perfectionism
1990; Hewitt & Flett, 1991). The following paragraphs provide an captured in the HF MPS, there have been no confirmatory studies
overview of these popular instruments and summarize a more to evaluate its proposed factor structure in either clinical or non-
exhaustive review on the assessment of perfectionism recently clinical samples.
completed (Enns & Cox, 2002).

Frost et al.’s (1990) MPS


Hewitt and Flett’s (1991) MPS
Hewitt and Flett (HF; 1991) described three dimensions of The Frost MPS has the following six dimensions based largely
perfectionism based on interpersonal source and direction, and on the nature and potential origins of perfectionism: Concern Over
they are assessed by three subscales of the MPS: Self-Oriented Mistakes is viewed as the major dimension in this conceptualiza-
Perfectionism (the setting of excessively high standards and per- tion. It involves setting high standards of personal performance
“which are accompanied by tendencies for overly critical evalua-
tions of one’s own behavior” (Frost et al., 1990, p. 450). The
remaining dimensions are Personal Standards (the setting of high
Brian J. Cox and Murray W. Enns, Department of Psychiatry, University standards for one’s performance), Doubts About Actions (doubts
of Manitoba, Winnipeg, Manitoba, Canada; Ian P. Clara, Department of about the quality of one’s performance), and Organization (em-
Psychology, University of Manitoba. phasizing order and precision). Two more subscales, Parental
This work was supported by an operating grant from the Social Sciences Expectations and Parental Criticism, address perceptions about the
and Humanities Research Council of Canada and an infrastructure grant
standards parents have for the individual and the belief that a
from the Canada Foundation for Innovation. Brian J. Cox was supported by
the Canada Research Chairs program, and Ian P. Clara was supported by
failure to meet these standards can result in a loss of acceptance,
the Manitoba Health Research Council. respectively.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Brian J. Although generally supportive, there has been considerably less
Cox, PZ-430 PsycHealth Centre, 771 Bannatyne Avenue, Winnipeg, Mani- empirical work using the Frost MPS compared to the HF MPS.
toba R3E 3N4, Canada. E-mail: coxbj@cc.umanitoba.ca Available evidence suggests that Doubts About Actions and espe-

365
366 COX, ENNS, AND CLARA

cially Concern Over Mistakes are the dimensions most consis- item content and wording to suggest that some items could be
tently associated with emotional distress, including data from a eliminated. For example, several items in the Self-Oriented Per-
clinically depressed sample (Enns & Cox, 1999). EFA was used to fectionism subscale all refer to the subjective need for “perfection”
derive subscales in a college student sample in the initial devel- or being “perfect,” and several items in the Socially Prescribed
opment of the Frost MPS (Frost et al., 1990). However, subsequent Perfectionism subscale all refer to people’s “expectations” or what
studies using EFA have either (a) found a substantial number of people “expect.”
items with cross-loadings in the six-factor solution (Parker & The second objective was to investigate a second-order, two-
Adkins, 1995; Rheaume, Freeston, Dugas, Letarte, & Ladouceur, factor model of perfectionism using a subset of scales from the two
1995), or (b) failed to identify a six-factor solution and instead MPS instruments and evaluated through the use of a second-order
have generated three-factor solutions (Purdon, Antony, & Swin- CFA. On the basis of previous findings, our hypothesized model
son, 1999) as well as four-factor (Stober, 1998; Stumpf & Parker, contained two higher-order factors that were believed to represent
2000) solutions. Given this apparent uncertainty in factor structure, adaptive and maladaptive aspects of perfectionism.
the paucity of confirmatory factor analytic studies of the Frost A final objective was to examine associations between adaptive
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

MPS in either clinical or nonclinical samples is noteworthy. Parker and maladaptive dimensions of perfectionism and indices of per-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and Stumpf (1995) did use confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) in sonality, symptom distress, and academic performance in an at-
their evaluation of the Frost MPS with academically talented tempt to establish construct-related validity. It is important to
sixth-grade children. They were able to obtain an adequate fit for understand how specific individual differences that may confer
the model, but only after nine models were evaluated and modified psychological vulnerabilities to emotional disorders relate to broad
by freeing up parameter estimates that suggested poor fit. personality dimensions (Lilienfeld, 1997). For example, higher-
There has been little factor analytic work involving direct com- order personality dimensions such as neuroticism can be viewed as
parisons of the two MPS instruments, and none with respect to “source” traits (Cattell, 1977), which in turn give rise to more
clinical samples. Using an undergraduate sample, Frost, Heimberg, specific “surface” traits (e.g., maladaptive perfectionism), and can
Holt, Mattia, and Neubauer (1993) conducted an EFA of the nine therefore shed light on the origins of the latter. In this regard,
combined MPS subscales. The results indicated a two-factor so- conscientiousness and neuroticism were selected from the Big Five
lution that the authors interpreted as reflecting maladaptive eval- taxonomy of personality (Costa & McCrae, 1992), similar to the
uation concerns and positive achievement striving. This interpre- approach taken in previous perfectionism research (e.g., Stumpf &
tation was largely based on the profile of correlations between the Parker, 2000). Neuroticism is characterized by the general ten-
individual subscales that comprised the two factors and measures dency to experience psychological distress and maladjustment, and
of negative affect and positive affect. The Frost MPS dimensions high levels convey proneness to “irrational ideas” (Costa & Mc-
of Concern Over Mistakes, Doubts About Actions, and Parental Crae, 1992, p. 14). In contrast, conscientiousness is characterized
Criticism and the HF MPS dimension of Socially Prescribed by purposefulness, organization, and “will to achieve,” and is
Perfectionism were significantly associated with negative affect. associated with academic and occupational achievement (Costa &
The HF MPS dimension of Self-Oriented Perfectionism and the McCrae, 1992, p. 16). It was hypothesized that maladaptive di-
Frost MPS dimensions of Personal Standards and Organization mensions of perfectionism would be significantly correlated with
were significantly associated with positive affect. The Other- neuroticism, whereas adaptive dimensions would be correlated
Oriented Perfectionism (HF MPS) and Parental Expectations with conscientiousness.
(Frost MPS) subscales were not associated with either type of In addition to relationships with higher-order personality dimen-
affect (correlations in all cases were very close to zero). In a sions, we also evaluated the construct-related validity of the brief
similar fashion, Stumpf and Parker (2000) recently used EFA of and full perfectionism measures, and their adaptive and maladap-
factor scores to obtain a higher-order structure of the Frost MPS tive dimensions, with a measure of depressed mood used in pre-
that they labeled healthy and unhealthy perfectionism. The former vious perfectionism research for this purpose (Frost et al., 1993).
dimension was significantly correlated with the broad personality Finally, we also followed an approach used in some previous
domain of conscientiousness, whereas the latter was correlated research on adaptive aspects of perfectionism (Brown et al., 1999)
with neuroticism. It would be useful to evaluate a hierarchical and examined associations with recent and anticipated academic
model of perfectionism with a second-order CFA in which the performance. A third, independent sample consisting of medical
relationships among the items and the latent factors could be school students was used for this purpose.
explicitly specified.
The present study therefore had three objectives. The first was Method
to evaluate the first-order structure of the two MPS instruments in
both clinically distressed and college student samples through the Participants
use of first-order confirmatory factor analysis. As part of this task
we also sought to determine whether the factor structure could be The clinically distressed sample consisted of 412 adult outpatients (241
women, 171 men; mean age ⫽ 40.83 years, SD ⫽ 12.31) seen for
recovered with a smaller set of items, especially for the HF MPS.
assessment at a mood disorders program in a university-affiliated teaching
In a clinical context, such as in the assessment of depressed hospital. All patients were diagnosed according to criteria from the Diag-
individuals, this issue is particularly relevant and becomes even nostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed.; American
more pronounced if an attempt is made to simultaneously assess Psychiatric Association, 1994), following a semistructured 1–2 hr clinical
the domains from both MPS instruments (i.e., a total of 80 Likert- interview conducted by an experienced psychiatrist. Patients gave informed
scale items of perfectionism). There is sufficient repetitiveness in written consent that the information they provided be used for research
MULTIDIMENSIONAL PERFECTIONISM SCALES 367

purposes. The breakdown of primary diagnoses in the sample included questions a 5-point, Likert-type scale was used where 1 ⫽ bottom 10% of
major depressive disorder (48.8%), dysthymia (13.1%), panic disorder the class, 2 ⫽ bottom third of the class, 3 ⫽ middle third of the class, 4 ⫽
(11.1%), and bipolar disorder (6.8%), other Axis I disorders, such as an top third of the class, and 5 ⫽ top 10% of the class. These anchors were
eating disorder (8.7%), other anxiety disorder (4.9%), substance abuse chosen to reflect the fact that these students do not receive letter grades and
(2.4%), depressive disorder not otherwise specified (2.4%), and personality instead are provided information on their performance relative to the rest of
disorder (1.7%). The mean score for the sample on the Beck Depression the class.
Inventory (BDI; Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emery, 1979) was 25.86 (SD ⫽
11.69), a level considered to represent moderate to severe depression Statistical Procedure
(Beck, Steer, & Garbin, 1988). A subset of the patients with major
depressive disorder (n ⫽ 145) was used in a previous correlational study of All confirmatory models were assessed using the AMOS 4.0 package
perfectionism dimensions and depression symptom severity (Enns & Cox, (Arbuckle & Wothke, 1999) with maximum likelihood estimation. Several
1999). indices of fit were used to evaluate the models. The goodness-of-fit index
The college sample consisted of 288 first-year university students re- (GFI; Jöreskog & Sörbom, 1986) looks at the proportion of variance
cruited from an introductory psychology course (182 women, 106 men; accounted for in the sample covariance matrix by the estimated population
mean age ⫽ 19.06 years, SD ⫽ 3.19). The BDI score for this nonclinical covariance matrix. The adjusted goodness-of-fit index (AGFI; Jöreskog &
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

sample (M ⫽ 10.15, SD ⫽ 7.69) was in the none to minimal depression Sörbom, 1986) is an adjustment of the GFI for the number of parameters
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

range (Beck et al., 1988). Students were given written feedback about the in the model. Values of .90 (GFI) and .80 (AGFI) are indicative of good fit
purpose of the study and about recognizing the symptoms of depression. (Marsh, Balla, & McDonald, 1988). Both of these indices show downward
They were also provided with information about student counseling ser- biases with smaller sample sizes (Fan & Wang, 1998). The comparative fit
vices and clinical services in the community. Data from this student sample index (CFI; Bentler, 1988) compares the hypothesized model against an
were used in a separate study of personality correlates of depressed mood independence model and is normed between a 0 and 1 range. Values at or
(Enns, Cox, & Borger, 2001). greater than .90 are indicative of good fitting models (Tabachnick & Fidell,
The third sample was used for the purpose of further establishing 1996), and there is little influence due to sample size effects (Fan & Wang,
construct-related validity of the brief versus original perfectionism mea- 1998). The Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI; Tucker & Lewis, 1973) is a type of
sures and consisted of 96 medical school students (56 men, 40 women; comparative fit index that can be used to compare models in different
mean age ⫽ 25.07 years, SD ⫽ 4.57) enrolled in the undergraduate medical samples of unequal size, and it can range outside of the 0 –1 range with
training program at the University of Manitoba. Data from this sample values at or over .90 being indicative of good fit (Marsh et al., 1988). The
were used in a separate study of perfectionism in medical students (Enns, root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) assesses the amount of
Cox, Sareen, & Freeman, 2001). model misfit, and values less than .05 are considered to be indicative of
good fitting models (Fan & Wang, 1998; Raykov, 1998).
Accordingly, the following frequently used criteria were used in evalu-
Materials ating the adequacy of the models: GFI ⬎ .90, AGFI ⬎ .80, CFI ⬎ .90,
TLI ⬎ .90, RMSEA ⬍ .05. No item was allowed to load on more than one
All participants completed both the HF MPS (Hewitt & Flett, 1991) and factor. Correlated errors were not specified in the models. For the HF MPS
the Frost MPS (Frost et al., 1990). The Hewitt and Flett MPS uses a 7-point and for the Frost MPS, all latent factors were allowed to covary. No
Likert scale ranging from 1 (disagree) to 7 (agree) and assesses three negative variances or correlations greater than one were present in any of
subscales: Self-Oriented Perfectionism [SOP; ␣s ⫽ .92 (clinical), .91 the CFA solutions.
(university)], Other-Oriented Perfectionism [OOP; ␣s ⫽ .80 (clinical), .74
(university)], and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism [SPP; ␣s ⫽ .88 (clin-
ical), .88 (university)]. Results
The Frost MPS uses a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly First-Order Confirmatory Analysis
disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) and assesses six perfectionism subscales
that contain between four to nine items each: Concern Over Mistakes HF MPS. The three-factor model proposed by Hewitt and Flett
[COM; ␣s ⫽ .91 (clinical), .90 (university)], Doubts About Actions [DA; (1991) was tested in each sample. As can be seen in Table 1, none
␣s ⫽ .77 (clinical), .71 (university)], Parental Criticism [PC; ␣s ⫽ .84 of the corresponding fit indices fulfilled the evaluative criteria for
(clinical), .84 (university)], Parental Expectations [PE; ␣s ⫽ .87 (clinical), good fit for this model.
.85 (university)], Personal Standards [PS; ␣ ⫽ .81 (clinical), .83 (univer-
To evaluate the potential of a brief HF MPS, we first conducted
sity)] and Organization [O; ␣s ⫽ .65 (clinical), .93 (university)]. Although
the emphasis is on individual subscales, the Frost MPS can be used to
an EFA using the college student sample and then attempted to
generate a total Perfectionism score by using only five of the subscales. cross-validate the three-factor model with our reduced item con-
The Organization subscale is omitted from this score because of a low tent using CFA in the clinical sample. Because the number of HF
correlation with the other subscales (Frost et al., 1990). MPS factors is known and because of limitations imposed by
Participants also completed the NEO-Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI; sample size, each of the 15-item HF MPS subscales was factor-
Costa & McCrae, 1992), a 60-item personality measure that assesses the analyzed separately. A single factor was extracted in each case and
Big Five personality dimensions. In order to examine the correlates of the 5 items from each subscale with the highest factor loadings
potentially adaptive and maladaptive aspects of perfectionism, we selected were chosen for inclusion in the briefer measure. This procedure
the factors of Conscientiousness [␣s ⫽ .86 (clinical), .80 (university)] and also ensured that there was a sufficient number of indicators (5) for
Neuroticism [␣s ⫽ .83 (clinical), .88 (university)]. The BDI was used to
each of the factors (Marsh, Hau, Balla, & Grayson, 1998). The
assess depressed mood [␣s ⫽ .90 (clinical), .86 (university)].
Academic functioning in the medical students was assessed by two
items chosen for each of the three subscales were as follows:
questions: “In the past academic year, how do you think your medical Items 13, 31, 33, 35, 39 for SPP [␣s ⫽ .85 (university), .81
school performance compared to your classmates?” (recent performance), (clinical)]; Items 6, 14, 28, 40, 42 for SOP [␣s ⫽ .84 (university),
and “What level of academic performance do you think you will be able to .84 (clinical)]; and Items 10, 19, 24, 43, and 45 for OOP [␣s ⫽ .66
achieve in medical school this year?” (anticipated performance). For both (university), .52 (clinical)]. The results of the CFA are presented in
368 COX, ENNS, AND CLARA

Table 1
Summary of Fit Indices for the Hewitt and Flett (1991) and Frost et al. (1990) Multidimensional
Perfectionism Scales (MPS) in University and Clinical Samples

Measure and sample GFI AGFI CFI TLI RMSEA ␹2 (df)

Hewitt & Flett (HF) MPS


Three-factor model
University .71 .68 .74 .72 .07 2,207.48 (942)
Clinical .71 .68 .75 .74 .07 2,806.35 (942)
Three-factor model (brief HF MPS)
Clinical .94 .92 .94 .93 .05 190.99 (87)
Frost MPS
Six-factor model
University .80 .77 .89 .88 .06 1,174.24 (545)
Clinical .80 .77 .86 .85 .07 1,648.28 (545)
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Stober (1998) four-factor model


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

University .75 .71 .84 .83 .08 1,453.68 (554)


Clinical .74 .70 .82 .80 .08 2,064.82 (554)
Purdon et al. (1999) three-factor model
University .74 .70 .83 .81 .08 1,286.27 (431)
Clinical .75 .71 .82 .80 .09 1,750.70 (431)
Five-factor model (Brief Frost MPS)
Clinical .91 .88 .94 .93 .06 462.12 (199)

Note. GFI ⫽ Goodness-of-fit index; AGFI ⫽ Adjusted goodness-of-fit index; CFI ⫽ Comparative fit index;
TLI ⫽ Tucker–Lewis Index; RMSEA ⫽ Root-mean-square error of approximation.

Table 1. In contrast to the full HF MPS, the brief HF MPS model criterion of eigenvalues greater than 1, we generated a five-factor
satisfied almost all of the evaluative criteria for good fit.1 solution. The pattern of item loadings closely matched that of the
To perform a more rigid test of covariance structure generali- original scale, with two exceptions. Item 18 from the COM scale
zation, we took the parameter estimates from the confirmatory loaded highest on the Personal Standards factor, and the items for
analysis of the brief HF MPS in the university sample and treated PC and PE loaded on a single parental factor (which is also a
them as fixed parameters in the same model with the clinical characteristic of four-factor models of the Frost MPS; e.g., Stumpf
sample. In other words, following the procedure outlined by Ban- & Parker, 2000). Seven items that had cross-loadings higher than
dalos (1993) and Floyd and Widaman (1995) all common factor .30 were removed. From the COM and PP factors the 5 items with the
loadings were restricted to determine whether the covariance ma- highest factor loadings that did not cross-load were chosen, from the
trix in the cross-validation sample could be reproduced by the O factor the top 4 items that did not cross-load were chosen, from PS
factor loadings from the initial sample. The fit indices for this
the 5 items that did not cross-load were chosen, and from the DA
cross-validation model in the clinical sample were as follows:
factor the 3 items which did not cross-load were chosen. This process
GFI ⫽ .93, AGFI ⫽ .91, CFI ⫽ .92, TLI ⫽ .92, RMSEA ⫽ .06,
yielded a 22-item set and five subscales: COM [Items 9, 13, 14, 23,
and ␹2(99) ⫽ 248.42.
34; ␣s ⫽ .86 (university), .86 (clinical)], O [Items 7, 27, 29, 31; ␣s ⫽
Frost MPS. Table 1 also contains the fit statistics for the Frost
MPS models. The original six-factor model failed to meet any of .90 (university), .88 (clinical)], PS [Items 6, 12, 19, 24, 30; ␣s ⫽
the evaluative criteria although some indices (e.g., CFI), ap- .85 (university), .83 (clinical)], PP [Items 3, 20, 22, 26, 35; ␣s ⫽ .84
proached the level of good fit. The situation did not improve for (university), .86 (clinical)], and DA [Items 17, 28, 32; ␣s ⫽
either a four-factor model (Stober, 1998; Stumpf & Parker, 2000) .63 (university), .68 (clinical)].
or the Purdon et al. (1999) three-factor model. The factor structure was then cross-validated using confirmatory
At this point we decided to attempt to identify a Frost MPS analysis in the clinical sample, and the results are presented near
model through the use of EFA with the college student sample, and the bottom of Table 1. The model satisfied four of the five
then to cross-validate the model with CFA in the clinical sample. goodness-of-fit indicators. Together with the brief HF MPS, the
Previous EFA analyses with the Frost MPS (e.g., Purdon et al.,
1999) have found that some items do not load very highly on their
respective subscales or factors. Therefore, similar to the approach
taken in the HF MPS analysis, we attempted to cull a set of items 1
In response to reviewers’ requests, all 45 items of the HF MPS were
that best represented their respective factors. Because the number also examined simultaneously in a single EFA. A very similar set of items
of factors or subscales in the Frost MPS is less clear than with the emerged when the 45-item factor analysis was used. The 5 OOP items
HF MPS, and because our sample size permitted it, we performed matched exactly, 4 of the 5 SPP items matched, and 3 of the 5 SOP items
an exploratory analysis utilizing principal-components analysis matched. In accordance with this close similarity in item composition, the
with all 35 items. Using an oblique (oblimin) rotation and a CFA produced very similar fit indices.
MULTIDIMENSIONAL PERFECTIONISM SCALES 369

brief Frost MPS was the most successful of the six different of PP items from the brief Frost MPS originated from the PC
models evaluated in the clinical sample.2 subscale, it was conceptualized as an element of maladaptive
We performed a similar test of covariance structure generaliza- perfectionism. The results in Table 5 show that the model success-
tion on the Frost MPS. The parameter estimates from the univer- fully passed three of the five fit indicators in each sample.
sity sample for the 22-item five-factor Frost model were treated as The third and final set of results consists of correlations between
fixed parameters in the same model with the clinical sample. The brief and full-scale perfectionism dimensions, higher-order mal-
fit indices for this cross-validation model were as follows: GFI ⫽ adaptive and adaptive dimensions of perfectionism (from the com-
.89, AGFI ⫽ .87, CFI ⫽ .93, TLI ⫽ .92, RMSEA ⫽ .04, and posite subscales), and the broad personality domains of Neuroti-
␹2(219) ⫽ 575.73. cism and Conscientiousness, as well as a symptom measure of
Correlations between the subscales of the HF MPS and brief HF distress and indices of academic achievement. Table 6 presents
MPS are presented in Table 2, and the correlations between the these correlational results. The conservative ( p ⬍ .01) alpha is
subscales of the Frost MPS and brief Frost MPS are presented in more appropriate given the large number of correlation coeffi-
Table 3. In the majority of cases the correlations exceeded .90 and cients. The largest correlations were in expected directions (e.g.,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

all correlations exceeded .70. Table 4 displays the correlations indices of maladaptive perfectionism and tendency to experience
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

between the brief HF MPS and the brief Frost MPS. A high degree distress), although there were some small correlations that were
of redundancy among the subscales was not indicated. unexpected (e.g., adaptive perfectionism and neuroticism). A very
similar pattern of correlational findings based on brief measures
Second-Order Confirmatory Analysis versus full-scale measures of perfectionism was evident, indicating
that a reduction in scale length did not come at the expense of
To determine whether a higher-order, two-factor structure of reduced construct-related validity.
adaptive and maladaptive perfectionism could be identified from
the HF MPS and Frost MPS items, we conducted a second-order
confirmatory factor analysis. Using the correlational findings of
Discussion
Frost et al. (1993), we defined maladaptive perfectionism as a
composite of the items that constituted the COM, DA, and PC The purposes of this study were to evaluate the first-order and
subscales from the Frost MPS and the SPP subscale items from the second-order structure of two popular multidimensional measures
HF MPS. Adaptive perfectionism was defined as a composite of of perfectionism. This study is the first to use CFA with several
the PS and O subscale items from the Frost MPS and the SOP different goodness-of-fit indicators in both nonclinical and clini-
subscale items from the HF MPS. Consistent with their observed cally distressed samples. The latter group is especially important
lack of relationship with either negative affect or positive affect in because although perfectionism has potentially broad implications
the Frost et al. (1993) study, the OOP subscale from the HF MPS for a number of forms of psychopathology, much of the existing
and the PE subscale from the Frost MPS were not included in the factor-analytic literature is confined to college student samples. In
maladaptive–adaptive model. Table 5 presents the CFA results for general, the findings were very consistent across the two different
this higher order, two-factor model based on the original, full item samples, and this consistency attests to the robustness and appli-
sets from both MPS subscales. None of the indices indicated good cability of the perfectionism models for a wide range of current
fit. An adaptive–maladaptive model based on the brief HF MPS level of psychological functioning.
and brief Frost MPS was also evaluated. The same subscales used Strong support was found for the three-factor model of the HF
in the full-scale versions were again selected. Because the majority MPS (Hewitt & Flett, 1991), although most of the evidence for
good fit was obtained with a reduced item set (i.e., 15 instead of

Table 2 2
Intercorrelations Between Corresponding Full (45-Item) and It is possible that the brief measures of perfectionism displayed such
good fit to the data simply because of the reduced number of items and
Brief (15-Item) Subscales of the Hewitt and Flett (1991)
corresponding degrees of freedom in the models compared with the full-
Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale for Clinical and item versions. This possibility was investigated by constructing brief item
University Samples sets that were not empirically derived. A random selection of 5 items from
the remaining 10 items of each subscale for the HF MPS were chosen to
Brief Brief Brief
construct an alternative 15-item scale which was subjected to a three-factor
Self- Socially Other-
Subscale Oriented Prescribed Oriented CFA in both the university (U) and clinical (C) samples. The fit statistics
were GFI (.91 U, .87 C), AGFI (.88 U, .83 C), CFI (.82 U, .76 C), RMSEA
Clinical (n ⫽ 412) (.07 U, .09 C), and ␹2 (df ⫽ 87; 207.05 U, 384.35 C). For the Frost MPS
the remaining 13 items were used to construct an alternative 22-item scale,
Self-Oriented .96 with additional items randomly selected from the brief Frost MPS to bring
Socially Prescribed .91
the total item numbers within each subscale up to equality with the brief
Other-Oriented .73
Frost MPS. This alternative scale was subjected to a five-factor CFA and
University (n ⫽ 288) resulted in the following fit statistics: GFI (.85 U, .84 C), AGFI (.81 U, .80
C), CFI (.88 U, .85 C), RMSEA (.08 U, .08 C), and ␹2 (df ⫽ 199; 517.74
Self-Oriented .95 U, 776.63 C). To summarize, almost all of the goodness-of-fit indicators
Socially Prescribed .94 suggested neither the alternative brief item set for the HF MPS nor the
Other-Oriented .77
Frost MPS satisfied criteria for good fit.
370 COX, ENNS, AND CLARA

Table 3
Intercorrelations Between Corresponding Full (35-Item) and Brief (22-Item) Subscales of the
Frost et al. (1990) Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale for Clinical and University Samples

Brief Concern Brief Personal Brief Parental Brief Doubts Brief


Subscale Over Mistakes Standards Perceptions About Actions Organization

Clinical (n ⫽ 412)
Concern Over Mistakes .96
Personal Standards .97
Parental Expectations .86
Parental Criticism .91
Doubts About Actions .97
Organization .97
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

University (n ⫽ 288)
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Concern Over Mistakes .96


Personal Standards .96
Parental Expectations .87
Parental Criticism .92
Doubts About Actions .96
Organization .98

45). This finding suggests that not only might a reduced item set We were also able to obtain good fit for the Frost MPS (Frost et
be advantageous in clinical and other assessment settings where al., 1990), but only after refining the scale. In the process, and
time and effort on the part of respondents is an important concern, perhaps not surprisingly, the data necessitated that parental criti-
but also that the underlying factor structure might be better cap- cism and expectations be collapsed into one parental perceptions
tured by focusing on the most salient or marker-type items. We domain. This refined Frost MPS containing five first-order factors
would encourage the developers of the HF MPS to consider the successfully passed several goodness-of-fit indicators, whereas
construction of a brief HF MPS and hope that our findings help this was not the case for other previously identified models (Pur-
point to directions that appear most promising with respect to item don et al., 1999; Stober, 1998; Stumpf & Parker, 2000). These
selection. findings suggest that the factor structure uncertainty and instability

Table 4
Correlations Between the Brief Hewitt and Flett (1991) Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale (MPS) and Brief Frost et al. (1990)
MPS Subscales in the Clinical and University Samples

Brief Brief Brief


Brief Socially Brief Brief Concern Personal Parental Brief Doubts
Subscale Self-Oriented Prescribed Other-Oriented Over Mistakes Standards Perceptions About Actions

Clinical (n ⫽ 412)
Brief Socially Prescribed .51** —
Brief Other-Oriented .15** .12* —
Brief Concern Over Mistakes .39** .54** .00 —
Brief Personal Standards .71** .39** .15** .36** —
Brief Parental Perceptions .36** .56** ⫺.02 .43** .34** —
Brief Doubts About Actions .32** .43** ⫺.08 .59** .27** .34** —
Brief Organization .28** .04 .08 ⫺.03 .24** .11* ⫺.08

University (n ⫽ 288)
Brief Socially Prescribed .38** —
Brief Other-Oriented .12* .08 —
Brief Concern Over Mistakes .44** .57** .06 —
Brief Personal Standards .70** .37** .21** .41** —
Brief Parental Perceptions .24** .69** ⫺.02 .49** .25** —
Brief Doubts About Actions .36** .43** ⫺.01 .53** .31** .41** —
Brief Organization .20** ⫺.03 ⫺.08 .08 .23** .02 .22**

* p ⬍ .05, two-tailed. ** p ⬍ .01, two-tailed.


MULTIDIMENSIONAL PERFECTIONISM SCALES 371

Table 5
Summary of Fit Indices for the Second-Order Analysis in the University and Clinical Samples

Measure and sample GFI AGFI CFI TLI RMSEA ␹2 (df)

Adaptive–Maladaptive Perfectionisma
University .71 .68 .81 .80 .06 2876.24 (1370)
Clinical .69 .67 .79 .78 .06 3833.74 (1370)
Adaptive–Maladaptive Perfectionismb
University .83 .81 .91 .90 .06 918.82 (457)
Clinical .85 .83 .91 .90 .06 1075.18 (457)

Note. GFI ⫽ Goodness-of-fit index; AGFI ⫽ Adjusted goodness-of-fit index; CFI ⫽ Comparative fit index;
TLI ⫽ Tucker–Lewis Index; RMSEA ⫽ Root-mean-square error of approximation.
a
From original set of items. b Items from the Brief Hewitt and Flett (1991) Multidimensional Perfectionism
Scale (MPS) and the brief Frost et al. (1990) MPS.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

might be put to rest with the adoption of the model and item instruments. In general, correlations between the two higher-order
composition identified in the present study. Our findings were perfectionism dimensions and the broad personality domains of
consistent with previous research (e.g., Purdon et al., 1999; Parker neuroticism and conscientiousness were in the expected directions,
& Adkins, 1995) which has shown that many Frost MPS items as were associations with symptom distress versus academic
have substantial cross-loadings or do not load highly on their achievement. The strongest correlations, representing medium to
respective subscales. Specifically, the present results highlight the large effect sizes (Cohen, 1992), were the positive associations
potential advantage of culling a stronger and cleaner set of items between maladaptive perfectionism with neuroticism and symp-
from the larger item set, in order to assess a robust and replicable tom distress, as well as adaptive perfectionism with conscientious-
model. A brief 22-item subset was used to achieve this purpose, ness and academic achievement. However, the relationships were
and the subscales correlated very highly with the original not as unambiguous as they could be and there were some unex-
subscales. pected, albeit small, correlations (adaptive perfectionism and
Content validity has been defined as the “degree to which neuroticism).
elements of an assessment instrument are relevant to and repre- A limitation of the present study and of the perfectionism
sentative of the targeted construct for a particular assessment literature is the sole reliance on self-report measures. The devel-
purpose” (Haynes, Richard, & Kubany, 1995, p. 238). On the basis opment of a valid and reliable structured interview of multiple
of this definition and the description of “elements” provided by dimensions of perfectionism is a clear direction for future research.
Haynes et al., the factor analytically derived subscales of the two Another limitation is that all of the data were gathered from the
brief MPS instruments look favorable. The instructions and re- same respondents, and an interesting question for further study is
sponse formats remain unchanged. More important, the content of the degree to which participants’ responses and those of their
the individual items that constitute the factors or subscales is very spouses or significant others converge. Similarly, measurement
consistent with the original descriptions of the various facets of the formats that would complement verbal and self-report assessment
perfectionism construct set out in the development of the two MPS are also needed. Examples include behavioral observation or psy-
instruments (Frost et al., 1990; Hewitt & Flett, 1991). Using the chophysiological indices in laboratory-task situations.
framework for judging content validity described by Murphy and
In conclusion, the present study was able to obtain strong
Davidshofer (1998), we found that in each case the subscale items
support for two different conceptually derived, multidimensional
appear to provide a representative sample for each of the content
measures of perfectionism as well as a hierarchical model of the
domains described in seminal articles (Frost et al., 1990; Hewitt &
structure of perfectionism. The results suggest these measures of
Flett, 1991).3
Our results also build on previous literature that has suggested a
higher-order, two-factor model of perfectionism involving posi- 3
In fact, the “parental perceptions” factor of the brief Frost et al. MPS
tive/healthy aspects and negative/unhealthy aspects (Frost et al., identified in the present study can be viewed as more consistent with the
1990; Stumpf & Parker, 2000). The model used in the second- original construct defined by Frost et al. (1990) compared with the two
order CFA consisted of adaptive versus maladaptive dimensions of separate parental subscales that subsequently emerged from empirical analysis
perfectionism, and it was able to satisfy several goodness-of-fit in the Frost et al. MPS. In the Frost et al. article each of the facets of the
indicators. A difference from previous studies is that we were able perfectionism construct were explicitly defined in separate sections, and pa-
to identify a model that provided good fit to the data that actually rental influences were discussed as a single facet. Frost et al. subsequently
divided the parental facet into separate PE and PC subscales on the basis of the
relied on fewer items. In fact, a two-factor, second-order model of
results from a factor analysis, but there was some ambiguity in content. For
perfectionism was empirically supported only when items from example, the item “I never felt like I could meet my parents’ expectations”
the two brief versions of the MPS instruments were used. By emerged as part of the PC subscale rather than the PE subscale. Therefore, the
not including other-oriented perfectionism in the adaptive–mal- content validity of the parental perceptions factor in the present study appears
adaptive model, this resulted in a composite item length of only 32 to be more consistent with the original description of a single parental dimen-
items—fewer items than contained in either of the individual MPS sion of perfectionism as delineated in the Frost et al. (1990) article.
372 COX, ENNS, AND CLARA

Table 6
Correlations Between Perfectionism Dimensions, Higher-Order Personality Domains, Distress, and Academic Achievement

Neuroticism Conscientiousness BDI Recent academic Anticipated academic


achievement achievement
Scale and subscale Univ. Clin. Univ. Clin. Univ. Clin. (Med. school) (Med. school)

Brief HF MPS (1991)


Self-Oriented Perfectionism .18** .35** .38** .24** .11 .19** .31** .38**
(.18**) (.16**) (.41**) (.24**) (.13*) (.22**) (.30**) (.41**)
Socially Prescribed Perfectionism .41** .14** ⫺.07 ⫺.08 .41** .39** .15 .23*
(.45**) (.40**) (⫺.04) (⫺.13*) (.46**) (.44**) (.13) (.23*)
Brief Frost MPS (1990)
Concern Over Mistakes .53** .56** .06 ⫺.22** .53** .48** .15 .17
(.52**) (.54**) (.06) (⫺.18**) (.52**) (.48**) (.17) (.18)
Doubts About Actions .51** .58** .01 ⫺.36** .41** .39** ⫺.05 ⫺.09
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

(.53**) (.56**) (.01) (⫺.39**) (.43**) (.38**) (⫺.06) (⫺.09)


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Personal Standards .15** .09 .35** .25** .15* .10 .27** .29**
(.15**) (.08) (.39**) (.31**) (.13*) (.09) (.25*) (.30**)
Parental Perceptions .31** .26** ⫺.10 ⫺.03 .35* .19** ⫺.04 .07
(.33**) (.30**) (⫺.10) (⫺.05) (.38**) (.20**) (⫺.13) (⫺.00)
Organization .10 ⫺.15** .50** .60** .03 ⫺.06 .03 ⫺.13
(.11) (⫺.16**) (.50**) (.63**) (.05) (⫺.08) (.02) (⫺.12)
Composite Scales
Maladaptive Perfectionism .52** .49** ⫺.04 ⫺.10* .52** .45** .10 .17
(.54**) (.52**) (⫺.02) (⫺.19**) (.54**) (.49**) (.10) (.18)
Adaptive Perfectionism .19** .16** .52** .32** .13* .18** .28** .27**
(.20**) (.10) (.51**) (.38**) (.14*) (.17**) (.28**) (.34**)

Note. Neuroticism and Conscientiousness scores are from the NEO Five Factor Inventory. Correlations using the original, full item sets are given in
parentheses. Parental Perceptions consists mainly of Parental Criticism items. Maladaptive Perfectionism is the sum of scores on the Doubts About Actions,
Parental Perceptions (Parental Criticism for full-scale computations), Concern Over Mistakes, and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism scales. Adaptive
Perfectionism is the sum of scores on the Personal Standards, Organization and Self-Oriented Perfectionism scales. BDI ⫽ Beck Depression Inventory;
HF ⫽ Hewitt & Flett; MPS ⫽ Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale; Univ. ⫽ University sample (n ⫽ 288); Clin. ⫽ Clinical sample (n ⫽ 412); Med.
school ⫽ Medical students (n ⫽ 96).
* p ⬍ .05, two-tailed. ** p ⬍ .01, two-tailed.

perfectionism could be successfully shortened through selective of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation.
reduction of the number of items. The correlational findings sug- Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 77–100.
gest that construct-related validity is not compromised in the brief Bentler, P. M. (1988). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psy-
measures of perfectionism. The study also provided empirical chological Bulletin, 107, 238 –246.
Brown, E. J., Heimberg, R. G., Frost, R. O., Makris, G. S., Juster, H. R.,
support for composite MPS indices of negative or maladaptive
& Leung, A. W. (1999). Relationship of perfectionism to affect, expec-
perfectionism and positive or adaptive perfectionism. This dem-
tations, attributions, and performance in the classroom. Journal of Social
onstrated ability is a valuable one given the recent emphasis in and Clinical Psychology, 18, 98 –120.
differentiating “good” and “bad” perfectionism (e.g., Terry-Short, Cattell, R. B. (1977). A more sophisticated look at structure: Perturbation,
Owens, Slade, & Dewey, 1995) and that the MPS instruments were sampling, role, and observer trait-view theories. In R. B. Cattell & R. M.
not specifically designed for this purpose. Dreger (Eds.), Handbook of modern personality theory (pp. 166 –220).
New York: Wiley.
References Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.
Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory
Adler, A. (1956). The neurotic disposition. In H. L. Ansbacher & R. R. (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI): Professional
Ansbacher (Eds.), The individual psychology of Alfred Adler (pp. 239 – manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
262). New York: Harper. Enns, M. W., & Cox, B. J. (1999). Perfectionism and depression symptom
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical man- severity in major depressive disorder. Behaviour Research and Ther-
ual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. apy, 37, 783–794.
Arbuckle, J. L., & Wothke, W. (1999). Amos 4.0 user’s guide. Chicago, IL: Enns, M. W., & Cox, B. J. (2002). The nature and assessment of perfec-
Smallwaters Corporation. tionism: A critical analysis. In G. Flett & P. Hewitt (Eds.), Perfection-
Bandalos, D. L. (1993). Factors influencing cross-validation of confirma- ism: Theory, research, and treatment (pp. 33– 62). Washington, DC:
tory factor analysis models. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 28, 351– American Psychological Association.
374. Enns, M. W., Cox, B. J., & Borger, S. C. (2001). Correlates of analogue
Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental and theoretical and clinical depression: A further test of the phenomenological conti-
aspects. New York: Harper & Row. nuity hypothesis. Journal of Affective Disorders, 66, 175–183.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive Enns, M. W., Cox, B. J., Sareen, J., & Freeman, P. (2001). Adaptive and
therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press. maladaptive perfectionism in medical students: A longitudinal investi-
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric properties gation. Medical Education, 35, 1034 –1042.
MULTIDIMENSIONAL PERFECTIONISM SCALES 373

Fan, X., & Wang, L. (1998). Effects of potential confounding factors on fit Murphy, K. R., & Davidshofer, C. O. (1998). Psychological testing:
indices and parameter estimates for true and misspecified SEM models. Principles and applications (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 58, 701–735. Hall.
Floyd, F. J., & Widaman, K. F. (1995). Factor analysis in the development Parker, W. D., & Adkins, K. K. (1995). A psychometric evaluation of the
and refinement of clinical assessment instruments. Psychological As- Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale. Journal of Psychopathology and
sessment, 7, 286 –299. Behavioral Assessment, 17, 323–334.
Frost, R. O., Heimberg, R. G., Holt, C. S., Mattia, J. I., & Neubauer, A. L. Parker, W. D., & Stumpf, H. (1995). An examination of the Multidimen-
(1993). A comparison of two measures of perfectionism. Personality sional Perfectionism Scale with a sample of academically talented chil-
and Individual Differences, 14, 119 –126. dren. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 13, 372–383.
Frost, R. O., Marten, P., Lahart, C., & Rosenblate, R. (1990). The dimen- Purdon, C., Antony, M. M., & Swinson, R. P. (1999). Psychometric
sions of perfectionism. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 14, 449 – 468. properties of the Frost Multidimensional Perfectionism scale in a clinical
Hamachek, D. E. (1978). Psychodynamics of normal and neurotic perfec- anxiety disorders sample. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, 1271–
tionism. Psychology, 15, 27–33. 1286.
Haynes, S. N., Richard, D. C. S., & Kubany, E. S. (1995). Content validity Raykov, T. (1998). On the use of confirmatory factor analysis in person-
in psychological assessment: A functional approach to concepts and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

ality research. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 291–293.


methods. Psychological Assessment, 7, 238 –247.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Rheaume, J., Freeston, M. H., Dugas, M. J., Letarte, H., & Ladouceur, R.
Hewitt, P. L., & Flett, G. L. (1991). Perfectionism in the self and social
(1995). Perfectionism, responsibility, and obsessive– compulsive symp-
contexts: Conceptualization, assessment, and association with psycho-
toms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 785–794.
pathology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 456 – 470.
Stober, J. (1998). The Frost Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale revis-
Hewitt, P. L., Flett, G. L., Turnbull-Donovan, W., & Mikail, S. F. (1991).
ited: More perfect with four (instead of six) dimensions. Personality and
The Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale: Reliability, validity, and
Individual Differences, 24, 481– 491.
psychometric properties in psychiatric samples. Psychological Assess-
Stumpf, H., & Parker, W. D. (2000). A hierarchical structural analysis of
ment, 3, 464 – 468.
perfectionism and its relation to other personality characteristics. Per-
Hollender, M. H. (1965). Perfectionism. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 6,
94 –103. sonality and Individual Differences, 28, 837– 852.
Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1986). LISREL VI: Analysis of linear Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics (3rd
structural relationships by maximum likelihood, instrumental variables, ed.). New York: HarperCollins.
and least squares methods. Mooresville, IN: Scientific Software. Terry-Short, L. A., Owens, R. G., Slade, P. D., & Dewey, M. E. (1995).
Lilienfeld, S. O. (1997). The relation of anxiety sensitivity to higher and Positive and negative perfectionism. Personality and Individual Differ-
lower order personality dimensions: Implications for the etiology of ences, 18, 663– 668.
panic attacks. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 539 –544. Tucker, L. R., & Lewis, C. (1973). The reliability coefficient for maximum
Marsh, H. W., Balla, J. R., & McDonald, R. P. (1988). Goodness-of-fit likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika, 38, 1–10.
indexes in confirmatory factor analysis: The effect of sample size.
Psychological Bulletin, 103, 391– 410.
Marsh, H. W., Hau, K. T., Balla, J. R., & Grayson, D. (1998). Is more ever Received September 28, 2000
too much? The number of indicators per factor in confirmatory factor Revision received March 27, 2002
analysis. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 33, 181–220. Accepted April 4, 2002 䡲

You might also like