You are on page 1of 10

This article was downloaded by: [University of Toronto Libraries]

On: 04 December 2014, At: 11:30


Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954
Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

American Journal of Clinical


Hypnosis
Publication details, including instructions for authors and
subscription information:
http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ujhy20

Attributions, Involuntariness, and


Hypnotic Rapport
a a
Steven Jay Lynn Ph.D. , Michael Snodgrass , Judith W.
a b a c a d
Rhue , Michael R. Nash & David C. Frauman
a
Ohio University , USA
b
University of Toledo , Toledo, Ohio, USA
c
University of Tennessee , Knoxville, Tennessee, USA
d
St. Vincent Stress Center and the Indiana University School
of Medicine , Indianapolis, Indiana, USA
Published online: 21 Sep 2011.

To cite this article: Steven Jay Lynn Ph.D. , Michael Snodgrass , Judith W. Rhue , Michael
R. Nash & David C. Frauman (1987) Attributions, Involuntariness, and Hypnotic Rapport,
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 30:1, 36-43

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00029157.1987.10402720

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information
(the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor
& Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties
whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the
Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and
views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The
accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently
verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable
for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages,
and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in
connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any
substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,
systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.
Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/
page/terms-and-conditions
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 11:30 04 December 2014
AMERICAN JOURNAL OF CLINICAL HYPNOSIS
VOLUME 30. NUMBER I. JULY 1987

Attributions, Involuntariness, and Hypnotic


Rapport

Steven Jay Lynn, Michael Snodgrass, Judith W. Rhue, Michael R.


Nash, and David C. Frauman'
Ohio University
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 11:30 04 December 2014

Subjects' attributions of their hypnotic responsiveness to the hypnotist's abil-


ities and efforts were associated with reports of suggestion-related involuntar-
iness. In addition, rapport with the hypnotist was found to correlate with
experienced involuntariness. As predicted, high-susceptible subjects expressed
greater positive rapport and liking for the hypnotist and experienced greater
involuntariness than their low-susceptible counterparts; they also attributed more
of their responsiveness to the hypnotist's ability and efforts. Medium suscep-
tibles were distinguishable from both highs and lows on all measures except
liking for the hypnotist, in which case they were similar to the highs. Subjects
viewed ability and effort as independent sources of hypnotic responsiveness.

In 1974 Weitzenhoffer used the term communication" into behavior that is ex-
"classical suggestion effect" to describe perienced as involuntary. However, more
the subject's "transformation of the es- than 30 years before Weitzenhoffer pro-
sential, manifest, ideational content of a vided this characterization, White (1941)
maintained that suggestion-related invol-
untariness was so central to the experi-
Received May 16, 1985; revised February 5, ence of hypnosis that it was incumbent
1986; second revision September 5, 1986; ac- upon theorists to address this domain of
cepted for publication September 8, 1986. experience. Hypnosis theorists (Arnold,
I The authors wish to thank Frank Bellezza for 1946; Bowers, 1976; Coe, 1978; Hilgard,
his statistical consultation. Judith W. Rhue is now 1977,1981; Sarbin & Coe, 1972; Spanos,
at the University of Toledo, Toledo, Ohio; Mi- Rivers, & Ross, 1977; Spanos 1982;
chael R. Nash is now at University of Tennessee,
Knoxville, Tennessee; and David C. Frauman is
Weitzenhoffer, 1974) have risen to the
now at the St. Vincent Stress Center and the In- challenge of explaining the well-docu-
diana University School of Medicine, Indianap- mented finding (e.g., K. Bowers, 1981;
olis, Indiana. P. Bowers, 1982; Farthing, Brown, &
Venturino, 1983; Spanos, Rivers, & Ross,
For reprints write to Steven Jay Lynn, 1977) that hypnotizable subjects who pass
Ph.D., Psychology Department, Ohio test suggestions characterize their re-
University, Athens, Ohio 45701.
sponses as more automatic or nonvoli-

36
ATIRIBUTIONS AND INVOLUNTARINESS 37

tional than subjects who fail suggestions. 1984; Spanos, Weekes, & deGroh, 1984),
Diverse theoretical accounts have var- and with recent survey data indicating that
iously emphasized dissociative processes the majority of subjects endorse the state-
(Bowers, 1976; Hilgard, 1977), vivid and ment, "The extent to which hypnosis is
sustained suggestion-related imagery (Ar- successful depends on the skill of the hyp-
nold, 1946), and contextual and social notist" (Wilson, Greene, & Loftus, 1986).
psychological variables (Coe, 1978; Sar- The present study examines the rela-
bin & Coe, 1979; Spanos, Rivers, & Ross, tionship between subjects' reports of in-
1977; Spanos, 1982). voluntariness and their attributions
Sarbin and Coe (1979) have observed concerning the source of their responsive-
that subjects' interpretations of their ex- ness on the HGSHS:A (Shor & Orne,
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 11:30 04 December 2014

periences may reflect on implicit distinc- 1962), that is, how much of their respon-
tion between "doings" (seeing themselves siveness was due to their own ability and!
as agents of goal-directed, purposeful ac- or efforts or to the hypnotist's ability and!
tions) and "happenings" (viewing them- or efforts. Ability and effort were the fo-
selves as passive respondents). Along cus of the present research because they
similar lines, Spanos (1982) has noted that are two dimensions postulated by attri-
"Interpreting behavior as an action in- bution theory (Weiner, 1972) to be re-
volves attributing causality to the self (e.g., lated to attributions of causality. It is
I did it), while interpreting it as a hap- hypothesized that subjects' reports of in-
pening requires that causality be attrib- voluntariness and hypnotic responding will
uted to sources other than the self (e.g., be positively associated with viewing the
it happened to me)." Given prevalent hypnotist as being responsible for their
misconceptions of hypnosis that center hypnotic responsiveness (i.e., external at-
around perceptions of the hypnotist as a tribution).
Svengali-like figure, along with the pop- This study is also designed to evaluate
ular belief that hypnotized subjects are the possibility that subjects' involvement
passive, hypnotizability is likely to be as- in fantasy and imaginative experiences in
sociated with subjects' attributing their their daily lives is related to their tend-
responsiveness and subjective experi- ency to attribute the locus of their hyp-
ences to the hypnotist, as well as to their notic responsiveness to their own abilities
own hypnotic ability. Further, it is likely and efforts. By requiring that subjects
that subjects' ratings of involuntariness complete an inventory of fantasy and im-
would be associated with attributions of aginative experiences (Inventory of
response causality to external factors such Childhood Memories and Imaginings;
as the hypnotist's ability and effort rather Wilson & Barber, 1982), it will be pos-
than to perceptions of the self as an active sible to address the question of whether
effortful agent and author of hypnotic ex- subjects who report a history of everyday
perience. Documentation of this relation- fantasy involvements will be more likely
ship would be consistent with research that to attribute their hypnotic responsiveness
has shown subjects' beliefs about hyp- and involuntariness to their own abilities
nosis are associated with reports of in- and efforts rather that those of the hyp-
voluntariness and hypnotic responding notist.
(Lynn, Nash, Rhue, Frauman, & Swee- Another focus of the present research
ney, 1984; Spanos, Cobb, & Gorassini, is on the relationship between subjects'
38 LYNN, SNODGRASS, RHUE, NASH AND FRAUMAN

rapport with the hypnotist and experi- item version of the Harvard Group Scale
enced involuntariness. A number of re- of Hypnotic Susceptibility, Form A
searchers (Lynn et aI., 1984; Dolby & (HGSHS:A) of Shor and E. Orne (1962)
Sheehan, 1977; Sheehan, 1971, 1980; which omitted items of arm rigidity and
Sheehan & Dolby, 1975) have shown that hands moving together.
some susceptible subjects display very
positive rapport with the hypnotist and
Procedure
appear to be specially motivated to re-
spond in accord with his or her intent. The present study was conducted in the
Subjects who exhibit positive rapport may context of an ongoing research project on
be more likely to view the hypnotist as a hypnotic experience and the develop-
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 11:30 04 December 2014

powerful figure and attribute their hyp- mental correlates of hypnotic susceptibil-
notic responsiveness to his or her abilities ity. The subjects were tested in four
and efforts. Although previous research evening sessions in groups of approxi-
has not assessed the relationship between mately 50 persons. The overall study was
rapport, involuntariness, and the external presented to subjects as an investigation
attribution of response causality, it seems of hypnosis, hypnotic experience, and early
reasonable to expect that rapport and lik- life experience. Informed consent was ob-
ing for the hypnotist will be positively tained for all subjects. Prior to the admin-
related to attributions of responsiveness to istration of the HGSHS:A and the
the hypnotist's ability and effort as well questionnaires of relevance to the present
as to involuntariness. Finally, high-sus- study, the hypnotist asked subjects to
ceptible subjects are predicted to express complete an inventory of early childhood
more positive rapport, liking for the hyp- experiences and the 52-item Inventory of
notist, involuntariness, and attributions of Childhood Memories and Imaginings
responsivenss to the efforts and ability of (ICMI) of Wilson and Barber (1982). This
the hypnotist than low-susceptible sub- inventory correlates highly with the Tel-
jects. Medium-susceptible subjects will be legen Absorption Scale (range-r= .61 to
included in the experimental design but .85) and has adequate validity and reli-
no predictions will be made for these sub- ability (Lynn & Rhue, 1986; Rhue &
jects. Lynn, in press). Next, the HGSHS:A was
administered. The standard preliminary
Method remarks about hypnosis also were in-
cluded. The subjects then completed a
Subjects
questionnaire entitled "Hypnotic Experi-
The subjects were 205 undergraduate ence Scale." The instructions, which were
psychology students who received extra read by the hypnotist and presented in
course credit for their participation. High- written form, emphasized reading and
(N=66, 12 male, 54 female; M=8.68, carefully responding to each question in
range= 8-10), medium- (N= 114, 48 an honest and straightforward manner.
male, 66 female; M=5.60, range = 4-7), Subjects were assured that the hypnotist
and low- (N = 25, 10 male, 15 female; would not have an opportunity to see their
M = 2.16, range = 0-3) susceptible sub- responses. Two male hypnotists partici-
jects were assigned to groups on the basis pated in the study; each was responsible
of their scores on a slightly shortened, 10 for conducting two of the group sessions.
ATIRIBUTIONS AND INVOLUNTARINESS 39

Dependent Measures performed on the seven dependent varia-


All of the items on the "Hypnotic Ex- bles. No overall effect for sex of subject
perience Scale" were presented in a 5- was found; additionally, no interaction ef-
point Liken-type format. The following fect between sex of subject and hypnotic
four questions assessed subjects' attribu- susceptibility was observed. A highly sig-
tions: (1) How much of your responsive- nificant effect for susceptibility was found
ness to the suggestions was based on your (F [14, 382] = 8.56, P <(01). There-
hypnotic ability? (2) How much of your fore, univariate F-tests were examined for
responsiveness to the suggestions was each of the seven dependent variables for
based on the ability of the hypnotist? (3) the susceptibility main effect. The results
How much of your responsiveness to the are presented in Table 1.
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 11:30 04 December 2014

suggestions was based on your efforts to An inspection of the data reveals sig-
go along with the suggestions? and (4) nificant main effects for all of the depen-
How much of your responsiveness to the dent variables. To interpret these findings,
suggestions was based on the efforts of Newman-Keuls posttests (criterion set at
the hypnotist? Each of the above ques- p < .05) were performed on the suscep-
tions was anchored 1 = none of it; 3 = tibility group means for each of the de-
some of it; 5 = all of it. To gauge sub- pendent variables presented in Table 1.
jects' experience of nonvolition, subjects Highs differed from low susceptibles in
were asked "To what extent did you ex- the predicted direction on the rapport and
perience your responses to the sugges- attraction measure. On the attributional
tions as happening automatically or measures, high-, medium-, and low-sus-
involuntarily vs. under your conscious, ceptible subjects were all significantly
deliberate, voluntary control?" (1 = to- different, in the predicted direction, on
tally voluntary; 5 = totally involuntary). the measures of attributions of respon-
The rapport with the hypnotist item asked siveness of ability and effort to the hyp-
subjects to rate "Rapport with the hyp- notist. Self-attributions were as follows:
notist (quality of your relationship with low susceptibles attributed less of their
the hypnotist)" (1 = very good; 3 = neu- responsiveness to their own ability than
tral; 5 = very poor). Liking for the hyp- either mediums or highs who did not dif-
notist was assessed by the item "I basically fer in their attributions. Low susceptibles
_ _ _ the hypnotist"(1 = dislike; 3 attributed more of their responsiveness to
feel neutral about; 5 = like). their own efforts that medium suscepti-
bles. High susceptibles did not differ from
Results either low or medium susceptibles on this
measure. On the measure of involuntari-
Preliminary analyses of variance were ness, highs experienced greater involun-
performed on the data to determine whether tariness than mediums who, in turn,
differential effects for the two hypnotists experienced more involuntariness than low
were evident. Because no such effects were susceptibles.P
obtained, the data were collapsed across
The zero order correlations between
hypnotists for the analyses that follow.
A 2 (sex of subject) X 3 (high, me-
dium, low susceptible) multivariate 2 The pooled total group standard deviation is
analysis of variance (MANOYA) was .94.
40 LYNN, SNODGRASS, RHUE, NASH AND FRAUMAN

Table 1
Cell Means, F-Ratios of the Dependent Variables, and Correlations with Susceptibility
Susceptibility Univariate F
High Medium Low (df=2,199)
Rapport*** 1.91 2.43 3.20 22.03**
Liking 3.53 3.38 2.96 4.28*
Ability-Self 3.64 3.29 2.12 21.22**
Effort-Self 3.58 3.30 3.84 3.11*
Ability-Hypnotist 3.85 3.35 2.24 25.19**
Effort-Hypnotist 3.91 3.45 2.68 16.91**
Involuntariness 3.83 3.27 2.20 30.10**
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 11:30 04 December 2014

***Iower value represents greater rapport


**p<.OI
*p<.05

susceptibility and the dependent variables pressed greater positive rapport, liking for
presented in Table I are all in the pre- the hypnotist, and greater experienced in-
dicted direction. Partial correlational voluntariness than their low-susceptible
analyses were also conducted with hyp- counterparts; they also attributed more of
notic susceptibilityand involuntariness with their responsiveness to the hypnotist's
the variance attributable to other variables ability and efforts. Medium susceptibles
partialed out of the scores of imaginative were distinguishable from both highs and
involvement; internal (self-ability and ef- lows on all of these measures except lik-
fort), external attribution measures (hyp- ing for the hypnotist, in which case they
notist ability and effort), and the attraction were similar to highs. The correlation be-
ratings (rapport and liking) were corre- tween experienced involuntariness and
lated with involuntariness and suscepti- hypnotic susceptibility obtained in our
bility with each of the other sets of study is quite comparable to that secured
variables and ICMI scores partialed out. in previous research with the Harvard
These results are presented in Table 2. Group Scale (e.g., Farthing, Brown, &
Finally, a stepwise multiple regression Venturino, 1983); even with the variance
analysis was performed using involuntar- attributable to the other measures par-
iness scores as the criterion variable. Each tialed out, hypnotizability and involuntar-
of the dependent measures was included iness remained significantly correlated.
as a predictor variable; however, the only Further, involuntariness ratings were pre-
two significant predictors of involuntari- dicted by the measures of attribution of
ness were attributions to the hypnotist's responsiveness to the hypnotist's ability
ability (Step 1 multiplier = .33; F (1,195) and rapport with the hypnotist, and partial
= 23.85, P <.00 I) and rapport with the correlational analyses revealed that invo-
hypnotist (Step 2 multiplier = .36; change luntariness and an external attribution rat-
F (2, 194) = 5.35, P < .(02). ing (responsiveness to the hypnotist's
ability) correlated even when the variance
Discussion due to other variables was partialed out.
As predicted, high susceptibles ex- Taken together, these findings are con-
ATTRIBUTIONS AND INVOLUNTARINESS 41

Table 2
First Order Correlations of All Measures and Partial Correlations with Involuntariness and
Hypnotizability
Hyp ICMI Rap Like In Sel-Ab Sel-Ef Hy-Ab Hy-Ef
Hypnotizability .32** .46** .16* .47** .47** .03 .50** .41**
(.25**) (.25**) (.07) (.33**) (.21 **) (.12) (.21 **) (.23**)
ICMI .22** .10* .08 .12 .05 .16* .14*
(.02)
Rapport .31** .31** .32** .09 .40** .26*
(.13)
Liking .18** .21** .05 .29** .18*
(.05)
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 11:30 04 December 2014

Involuntariness .30** .01 .33** .24**


(.17*) (.04) (.14*) (.05)
Self-Ability .57** .45**
Self-Effort .05 .13
Hypnotist-Ability .71**
Note: Partial correlations are presented in parentheses.
**p<.OI; *p<.05 (two tailed)

sonant with the hypothesis that external suggested, that "effort" is an ambiguous
attributions of response causality are as- term; that is, "effort" could be inter-
sociated with hypnotic responding and in- preted by subjects as either reflecting the
voluntariness ratings. Hypnotizable application of their abilities to the hyp-
subjects tend to rate their hypnotic expe- notic setting (becoming involved in sug-
riences as involuntary and view the hyp- gestion-related imaginings, etc.) or as
notist both in positive terms and as a reflecting the quality of their hypnotic ex-
relatively powerful figure through which periences. Given the likelihood that high-
their responses and experiences are man- and low-susceptible subjects experience
ifested. hypnosis very differently, high suscepti-
Whereas high susceptibles attributed bles may interpret self-effort attributions
more of their responsiveness to their own in terms of ability employment; whereas
ability than low susceptibles, medium- and low susceptibles may interpret self-effort
high-susceptible subjects were indistin- attributions in terms of their largely un-
guishable. This finding may well reflect successful attempts to experience hyp-
subjects' tendency to make hypnotic abil- notic suggestions. It is unclear whether
ity attributions on the basis of observa- low-susceptible subjects apportioned re-
tions of their own behavior (see Coe, sponsibility for what little success they
1983). had or for their more substantial failures.
Low-susceptible subjects attributed more Future research can make a contribution
of their responsiveness to their own ef- by clarifying aspects of the attribution
forts than medium susceptibles. Interest- process and providing subjects with an
ingly, low susceptibles did not differ from opportunity to make attributions regard-
high susceptibles on this measure. It may ing other potential causes noted by attri-
be, as McConkey and Sheehan (1982) have bution theory (Weiner, 1972) such as task
42 LYNN, SNODGRASS, RHUE, NASH AND FRAU MAN

difficulty and chance factors. It would also cognitive dimension of the experience of
be of interest to investigate subjects' at- hypnosis.
tributions in an experiment in which there
is greater hypnotist-subject involvement
in order to enhance the meaningfulness of References
subjects' attributions to the hypnotist's Arnold, M. D. (1946). On the mechanism of
suggestion and hypnosis. Journal of Abnor-
abilities and efforts.
mal and Social Psychology, 41, 107-128.
Differential experiences of hypnosis may
Bowers, K. (1976). Hypnosisfor the seriously
also account for unhypnotizable subjects
curious. New York: W. W. Norton.
rating the hypnotist as less positive and Bowers, K. (1981). Do the Stanford Scales
according him less responsibility for their tap the "classic suggestion effect"? Inter-
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 11:30 04 December 2014

responsiveness than high susceptibles who national Journal of Clinical and Experi-
have a more positive and less frustrating mental Hypnosis, 29,42-53.
experience of hypnosis. Further, since Bowers, P. (1982). The classic suggestion ef-
susceptible subjects are more responsive fect: Relationships with scales of hypnotiz-
to suggestions than medium and low sus- ability, effortless experiencing, and imagery
ceptibles, more extreme ability and hyp- vividness. International Journal of Clinical
notist-relatedattributions may be a function and Experimental Hypnosis. 30, 270-279.
of their having greater responsiveness to, Coe, W. C. (1978). The credibility of post-
in effect, "parcel out" to various sources. hypnotic amnesia: A contextualist's view.
However, recent research in our labora- International Journal of Clinical and Ex-
perimental, Hypnosis, 26218-245.
tory (Lynn, Snodgrass, Hardaway, &
Coe, W. C. (1983, August) Measuring non-
Lenz, 1984) suggests that high- and low-
volitional experiences of hypnotic subjects
susceptible subjects differ in their judg-
with state and control reports. Paper pre-
ments about the degree to which their re- sented at the annual convention of the
sponses are a function of their own versus American Psychological Association, Ana-
the hypnotist's abilities and efforts even heim, CA.
before they are hypnotized. Subjects' re- Dolby, R. & Sheehan, P. (1977) Cognitive
sponse predictions regarding the hypno- processing and expectancy behavior in hyp-
tist's ability and effort correlated positively nosis. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology. 86,
with subjects' hypnotic responsiveness and 334--345.
their ratings of involuntariness assessed Farthing, G., Brown, S., & Venturino, M.
after hypnosis. (1983). Involuntariness of response on the
The current study did not find support Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic Suscep-
for the possibility that subjects' everyday tibility. International Journal of Clinical and
Experimental Hypnosis, 31,170-180.
involvement in fantasy and imaginative
Hilgard, E. (1977). Divided consciousness:
experiences is related to their tendency to
Multiple controls in human thought and ac-
attribute response causality to their own tion. New York: Wiley.
abilities and efforts. In fact, low but sig- Hilgard, E. (1981). Hypnotic susceptibility
nificant correlations were obtained be- scales under attack: An examination of
tween the imagination measure and Weitzenhoffer's criticisms. International
external attributions of causality. In clos- Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hyp-
ing, examining the variables and processes nosis. 29, 24--41.
associated with subjects' causal attribu- Lynn, S. J., Nash. M. R. J., Frauman, D., &
tions can play a role in elaborating the Sweeney, C. (1984) Nonvolition, expectan-
ATTRIBUTIONS AND INVOLUNTARINESS 43

cies, and hypnotic rapport. Journal of Ab- A. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists
normal Psychology, 93,295-303. Press.
Lynn, S. J. & Rhue, J. W. (1986). The fan- Spanos, N. (1982) Hypnotic behavior: A cog-
tasy prone person: Hypnosis, imagination, nitive, social psychological perspective.
and creativity. Journal of Personality and Research Communications in Psychology,
Social Psychology, 51, 447-452. Psychiatry, and Behavior, 7, 199-213.
Lynn, S. J., Snodgrass, M., Hardaway, R., Spanos, N., Cobb, P. C., & Gorassini, D. R.
& Lenz, J. (1984) Hypnotic susceptibility: (1983). Failing to resist hypnotic test sug-
Predictons and evaluations of performance gestions: A strategy for self-presenting as
and experience. Paper presented at the deeply hypnotized. Unpublished manu-
Meeting of the American Psychological As- script, Carleton University.
sociation. Spanos, N., Rivers" S., & Ross" S. (1977).
Downloaded by [University of Toronto Libraries] at 11:30 04 December 2014

McConkey, K. & Sheehan, P. (1982) Effort Experienced involuntariness and response


and experience on the Creative Imagination to hypnotic suggestions. In W. Edmonston,
Scale. International Journal ofClinical and Jr. (Ed.), Conceptual and investigative ap-
Experimental Hypnosis, 30, 280-288. proaches to hypnosis and hypnotic phenom-
Rhue, J. W. & Lynn, S. J. (in press). Fantasy ena. Annals of the New York Academy of
proneness: Developmental antecedents. Sciences, 296, 208-221.
Journal of Personality. Spanos, N., Weekes, J. R., & de Groh, M.
Sarbin, T. R. & cee, W. C. (1972) A social (1984). The "involuntary" countering of
psychological analysis of influence com- suggested requests: A test of the ideomotor
munication. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & hypothesis of hypnotic responsiveness.
Winston. British Journal of Experimental and Clini-
Sarbin, T. R. & Coe, W. C. (1979). Hypnosis cal Hypnosis, 1, 3-11.
and psychopathology: On the futility of Weiner, B. (1972). Theories of motivation:
mental state concepts. Journal ofAbnormal From mechanism to cognition. Chicago:
Psychology, 88, 506-526. Rand McNally.
Sheehan, P. (1971). Countering preconcep- Weitzenhoffer, A. (1974). When is an "in-
tions about hypnosis: An objective index of struction an instruction?" International
involvement with the hypnotist. Journal of Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hyp-
Abnormal Psychology Monograph, 78,299- nosis, 22, 258-269.
322. White, R. (1941). A preface toward a theory
Sheehan, P. (1980). Factors influencing rap- of hypnotism. Journal ofAbnormal and So-
port in hypnosis. Journal of Abnormal Psy- cial Psychology, 36, 477-505.
chology, 89, 263-281. Wilson, S. & Barber, T. (1982) The Inventory
Sheehan, P. & Dolby, R. (1975). Hypnosis of Childhood Memories and Imaginings.
and the influence of most recently perceived Unpublished manuscript. Cushing Hospital.
events. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Wilson, L., Greene, E., & Loftus, E. F. (1986).
84331-345. Beliefs about forensic hypnosis. Interna-
Shor, R., & Orne, E. (1962). The Harvard tional Journal of Clinical and Experimental
Group Scale ofHypnotic Susceptibility, Form Hypnosis, 34, 110-121.

You might also like