You are on page 1of 6

Journal of Abnormal Psychology Copyright 1990 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1990, Vol. 99, No. 1,16-21 0021-843X/90/I00.75

Literalism as a Marker of Hypnotic "Trance": Discontinuing Evidence


Joseph P. Green, Steven Jay Lynn, John R. Weekes, Bruce W. Carlson,
John Brentar, Lance Latham, and Robert Kurzhals
Ohio University

The responses given by highly hypnotizable, hypnotic subjects and those of unhypnotizable subjects
who simulated hypnosis to questions of the type, "Do you mind telling me your name?" and "Do
you mind standing up?" were contrasted. The purpose was to examine Erickson's (1980) assertion
that literalism (answering "yes" or "no" verbally or nonverbally without any cognitive elaboration)
is a marker of hypnotic "trance." Simulators exhibited a greater rate of literalism than hypnotic
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

"virtuosos" (i.e., extreme scorers on both group and individual hypnotizability measures). Hypno-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

tized subjects and nonhypnotized subjects approached in the campus library responded comparably.
Because less than a third of hypnotic virtuosos responded literally, our results strongly refuted Erick-
son's assertion that literalism is a cognitive feature of hypnosis.

One feature commonly attributed to the hypnotic state has sponses of waking subjects over a period of more than 25 years.
been literalism of response. To illustrate, White (1941) stated Erickson asked questions such as, "Do you mind telling me
that the hypnotized subject's manner "differs from the ordi- your name?" or "Do you mind reading this?" (while handing
nary: he seems literal and humorless, he shows no surprise and subjects a card bearing a typed sentence, such as "This is a nice
makes no apology for bizarre behavior, he appears entirely un- day"). Erickson reported that 95% of the subjects in the waking
self-conscious, and very often he acts abstracted, inattentive, state—whether a friend, an acquaintance, or even a total
almost as if he were insulated against his surroundings" (p. stranger to the hypnotist—acquiesced to the implication of the
481). Pattie (1956) believed that characteristics of the hypnotic question—they would actually say their name or read the card,
state included "passivity. . . disinclination to talk. . . literal- for example. In contrast, 80% of the subjects in a "light trance"
mindedness, and a lack of spontaneity and initiative" (p. 208). uttered "no" or exhibited a negative shaking of the head; 90%
Weitzenhoffer (1957) maintained that, even when behaving in of subjects in a "medium trance" and 97% of subjects in a "deep
a most natural manner, hypnotized subjects "still show a con- trance" behaved in this manner. In short, although Erickson
striction of awareness, a characteristic literal-mindedness, some claimed that literalism of response was a classical indication of
psychomotor retardation, and possibly a degree of automatism" deep trance, the majority of subjects at any level of "trance"
(p. 212). More recently, Shor (1962, 1979) noted that the hyp- exhibited literalism.
notized subject "opens his eyes and interacts with reality objects Unfortunately, Erickson's experiment comprises a series of
but perceives them in a fresh, literal, concrete, here and now informal trials that are impossible to replicate exactly. The
way, isolated from their conventional meanings and abstract questions themselves and the order of questions were not stan-
evaluative interpretations" (p. 123). dardized; the situation in which questioning occurred was not
Despite being recognized as a hypnotic phenomenon, literal- held constant; the hypnotist/experimenter was apparently
ism was not hailed as an indicator of a trance state until Milton aware of the experimental hypotheses; and some subjects were
Erickson (Erickson, Hershman, & Secter, 1961) observed that asked questions in the "wake" state and in hypnosis, whereas
the presence of "trance" included literalness of response. Erick- others were only hypnotized or tested in the wake state. Further-
son (Erickson, Rossi, & Rossi, 1976) forcefully stated that "lit- more, "light," "medium," and "deep" trance were not denned
eralism . . . is a classical indication of deep trance" (p. 160). or operationalized. The fact that Erickson believed that literal-
According to Erickson, literalism involves an exact reception ism was a classical indicator of trance suggests that the presence
of ideas without an elaboration of them in terms of implied or of literalism and judgments about trance might have been con-
associated meanings. founded.
Erickson (1980) claimed to have experimentally assessed lit- Only two subsequent investigations (Lynn, Green, et al.,
eralness in 1,800 hypnotized subjects' responses and 3,000 re- 1989; McCue & McCue, 1988) have been undertaken to deter-
mine whether literalism is a valid marker of hypnosis. In an
experiment consisting of "three stages," McCue and McCue
(1988) found no evidence for literalness: Even when subjects
This research is based on Joseph P. Green's master's thesis, conducted were cued to respond in a literal manner, "only a minority of
under the supervision of Steven Jay Lynn. subjects displayed purely literal responses" (p. 197). Unfortu-
We would like to express our appreciation to John F. Kihlstrom, Ju-
nately, the following limitations render the interpretation of
dith R. Rhue, and anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on
an earlier version of this article. their findings questionable: Not all their subjects were prese-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Steven lected for high hypnotizability, nor was hypnotizability uni-
Jay Lynn, Psychology Department, Ohio University, Athens, Ohio formly assessed in a conventional, standardized manner, some
45701. subjects were tested twice, their performance confounded by
16
LITERALISM AS A MARKER OF HYPNOTIC TRANCE 17

their earlier responses; the exact questions used to assess literal- other hand, asking the next question immediately following the
ism were neither standardized nor reported; "it was not always subject's saying "yes" or "no" could cue a literal, "yes" or "no"
clear whether a S had passed a particular scale item" (p. 193), response to subsequent questions. To minimize the cue proper-
nor were all subjects' verbal responses heard clearly, which fur- ties of the interquestion interval, subjects in the present study
ther complicated scoring; and, finally, it was unclear whether were provided with a rationale for this time period. We in-
the experimenter waited for some time after the subject indi- formed subjects in advance of hypnosis that they would be
cated "yes" or "no" or whether the next question in a series was asked a number of questions and that there would be a brief
asked immediately. The third stage of the research program also period between questions to allow the hypnotist to record their
apparently contained strong cues to respond behaviorally (non- responses. Finally, in addition to randomizing the order of pre-
literally). sentation of the tests of literalism, we inquired about subjects'
In the first controlled study of literalism, Lynn, Green, et al. expectancies and how active or passive subjects felt when asked
(1989) compared the performance of highly hypnotizable, hyp- each of the questions.
notic subjects with that of task-motivated (Barber, 1969) sub- To evaluate Erickson's claim that subjects in the wake state
jects. In contrast to Erickson's claims, Lynn, Green, et al. found almost invariably respond in a nonliteral manner, we tested
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

that 87.5% of hypnotizable subjects' responses were nonliteral. nonhypnotized subjects for literalism in a naturalistic setting—
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Furthermore, hypnotic and task-motivated subjects did not the university library. This condition permitted us to examine
differ in their rate of literal responding. In summary, this re- the base rate of literal versus nonliteral responding in the college
search, taken together with McCue and McCue's (1988) study, student population and to compare these frequencies with hyp-
suggests that Erickson's (1980) claims are highly misleading. notic and simulating subjects' responses.
The present study was designed to provide a stronger test of
the hypothesis that literalism is a marker of hypnotizable sub- Method
jects' hypnotic responses. To do so, we examined literalism of
responding in subjects who scored at the top range of hypnotiz- Subjects
ability on two standardized measures of hypnotizability (so The 24 hypnotic and simulating subjects (16 women and 8 men) par-
called "hypnotic virtuosos") and compared their responses ticipated in order to receive extra course credit The 12 hypnotizable
with those of a group of unhypnotizable, simulating (Orne, subjects (6 women and 6 men) were required to meet two criteria: (1)
1971) subjects. To achieve a score of 11 or above on the 12-suggestion HGHSS:A admin-
Register and Kihlstrom (1986) have shown that only 36% of istered in a screening session (M = 11.33, SD = 0.49) and (2) to achieve
the subjects scoring in the high range (10-12) of the Harvard a score of 11 or above on the 12-suggestion SHSS:C (Weitzenhoffer &
Group Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale (HGHSS:A; Shor & Orne, Hilgard, 1962), as well as to pass the posthypnotic amnesia suggestion.
1962) continue to do so on the individually administered Stan- In order to secure the sample of 12 hypnotizable subjects (Stanford scale
ford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale, Form C (SHSS:C; Weitzen- M = 11.08, SD = 0.29), it was necessary to hypnotize 23 subjects who
scored in the highest range on the Harvard scale. Thus, 52% of the sub-
hoffer & Hilgard, 1962). The Stanford scale is thought to be a
jects who scored in the virtuoso range of the Harvard scale did so on
particularly demanding hypnotizability scale, because many of the Stanford scale. Twelve unhypnotizable subjects who scored three or
its suggestions call for perceptual and cognitive alterations. Reg- below on the HGHSSIA were selected for participation as simulators
ister and Kihlstrom (1986) have shown that only about 5% to (M = 1.92; SD = 0.92); these subjects scored 10.67 (SD = 0.78) on the
10% of the population score in the virtuoso range on the SHSS: SHSS:C. That unhypnotizable subjects successfully simulated hypnosis
c (11 -12). To secure a sample of subjects whose hypnotic ability is evident in the fact that the hypnotists were able to correctly identify
was truly unassailable, we required our subjects to pass at least only 25% of the simulators posthypnotically.
11 of the 12 Harvard scale suggestions and to pass at least 11 of The library sample consisted of 101 subjects (63 women and 38 men).
the 12 Stanford scale suggestions. Hypnotizability scores were unavailable for these subjects.
Hilgard (1988) has described Erickson as a master "play-
wright" and "director." If this characterization is apt, then his Test of Literalism
clients' behavior may be attributable to their willingness and
The following four questions constituted the test of literalism: (1) "Do
ability to enact and profit from the "roles" that were directly you mind telling me your name?" (2) "Do you mind standing up?" (3)
or indirectly suggested by Erickson. Thus, the high degree of "Are you willing to tell me where you are?" (4) "Say something." The
literalism of response exhibited by Erickson's subjects might first two questions were included in Erickson's research on literal re-
have been a function of their perception of the requirements of sponding (Erickson, 1980) and in our first study. The last two tests of
the hypnotic situation—part of the role they enacted in re- literalism were taken from measures of literalism in the Indirect Trance
sponse to perceived demands. For this reason, we thought it was Assessment Scale (ITAS of Rossi, 1986). The questions and statement
particularly important to examine the responses of unhypno- were chosen because they tap diverse dimensions of responding (i.e., a
tizable simulating subjects to evaluate the demand characteris- verbal expression of information; a behavioral, motor response; a verbal
tics of the experimental situation. Because the simulating model expression of spatial/reality orientation; and a verbal response to an
open-formatted, unstructured question).
(Orne, 1959, 1971) ensures that the hypnotist is blind to the
hypnotizability status of the subject, the possibility of inadver-
tent subject cuing is minimized. Procedure: Hypnotized and Simulating Subjects
In our first study (Lynn, Green, et al., 1989), there was a 1- In the initial screening session, four mate clinical psychology graduate
min interval between the questions that constituted the tests of students administered a live HGSHS:A to subjects in groups ranging in
literalism. It is possible that this interval cued subjects that a size from approximately 40 to 80 subjects. Subjects were informed that
more complete (i.e., nonliteral) response was expected. On the our research group was attempting to compile a profile of how Ohio
18 GREEN ET AL.

University students compare with other hypnosis subjects across the na- Table 1
tion. Only subjects motivated to take part in other studies were asked to Number of Literal Versus Nonliteral Responses of Hypnotized,
volunteer. Eligible subjects were contacted by telephone by the project Simulating, and Library Subjects
coordinator and invited to volunteer to participate in an individual hyp-
nosis session. Question Nonliteral Literal
Treatment of hypnotic and simulating subjects. For the actual experi-
mental phase, an experimental assistant read subjects simulating in- Tell name
structions (Orne, 1971) that informed simulators that if their pretense Hypnosis 10 2
were detected, the session and the experiment would be terminated. Simulating 5 7
The simulators were informed that hypnotizable subjects would also
Library 26 3
Stand up
participate in the research. Simulators were asked to continue their role Hypnosis 7 5
playing until the experimental assistant signaled them to stop, after a Simulating 3 9
postexperimental questionnaire was completed. The hypnotic subjects Library 19 8
had no knowledge that simulating subjects were participating in the Where you are
study. The hypnotists were unaware of the subjects' group status but Hypnosis 9 3
were required to guess whether subjects were hypnotized or simulating. Simulating 4 8
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Prior to the administration of the Stanford induction, subjects were Library 11 4


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

informed that the hypnotist would ask the subject a number of ques- Say something
tions during the induction. They were told that a brief time period Hypnosis 8 4
Simulating 8 4
would follow each question to allow the hypnotist to record their an- Library 23 7
swers. To make this salient, subjects were asked whether they had any
questions about how the experiment would be conducted. The hypno-
tist then administered the Stanford scale. After the 12 suggestions were
administered, the experimenter asked subjects the four questions that
constituted the tests of literalism, with a 1 S-s interquestion interval. The nonverbal responses. The raters were not associated with any other
questions were presented in a random order, and the experimenter re- phase of the research; they were unaware of subjects' condition and the
corded the response latencies to each of the four questions. research questions under consideration.
Postexperimental questionnaire. For each of the questions, subjects Following Rossi (1986), we adopted literalism scoring criteria of re-
were asked how active or passive they felt when the question was asked. sponding "yes" or "no," verbally or nonverbally, to the first three ques-
For example, the question regarding standing up was worded as follows: tions without enacting a behavioral response. None of the subjects re-
"When the hypnotist asked the question, 'Do you mind standing up?', sponded nonverbally in the absence of a behavioral response or a verbal
how did you feel?" (1 = passive, did not feel like moving; 3 = neither "yes" or "no" acknowledgment. The request "Say something" was
passive nor active; 5 = active, felt like moving). scored as literal if subjects responded with the reply "something." A
Subjects also received scales designed to examine their beliefs about composite literalism score was derived by summing subjects' responses
how the hypnotist expected them to respond. For each test of literalism, across the four questions. The raters agreed on 100% of the hypnotized
they received a question of the following form: "When the hypnotist and simulating subjects' responses and disagreed on only 2 of the 101
asked the question, 'Do you mind telling me your name?', the hypnotist responses of the subjects tested in the library sample. Differences were
expected me to respond b y . . . a. answering "Yes" or "No" b. stating resolved through discussion.
my name."
Treatment of the library sample. Six undergraduate research assis- Results
tants (3 men and 3 women) served as experimenters. Each experimenter
asked between 14 and 19 subjects one of the questions that constituted Measures of literalism of responding. Table 1 presents the
a test of literalism. The experimenters were instructed not to select sub- number of literal versus nonliteral responses of hypnotized,
jects on the basis of dress, race, or physical appearance. Personal ac- simulating, and library subjects across the four questions. An
quaintance was a basis for exclusion. To approximate a more random analysis of variance (ANOVA) of the composite literalism mea-
basis of selection, the research assistants were instructed to ask ques- sure indicated that simulating subjects (M = 2.33, SD = 1.07)
tions of subjects who were walking in or out of the library, looking for
exhibited more literalism of responding than hypnotizable sub-
books, or sitting in the library cafeteria, in order of appearance or seat-
ing arrangement. Prior to reading the question, the experimenters were
jects (M = 1.17, SD = 1.03) across the four questions, F(l,
told to say, "Excuse me." They then asked the question, recorded the 22) = 7.38,p=.01.'
answer and the response latencies, and said, "Thank you" at the end of To analyze the frequency of literal versus nonliteral responses
the interaction. If the subject responded with "What?" or "Why?", the across each of the four questions, for hypnotized versus simulat-
experimenter restated the question as often as needed. The experiment- ing subjects, separate chi-square analyses were performed. Sim-
ers did not inform the subjects that it was an experiment unless the ulators exhibited a greater frequency of literal responses than
subject asked them about the purpose of the question or after the subject did hypnotized subjects to the questions that asked subjects
had responded. Experimenters were told nothing about the purpose of
the research. However, they were told to anticipate a variety of re-
sponses. All of the subjects' responses were scored unless they clearly ' Erickson clearly stated that subjects who exhibited literalism simply
refused to respond to the question (e.g., walked away or stated they were answered "yes" or "no" without making any behavioral response (Er-
too busy) or simply stated, "Why" repeatedly and never answered di- ickson, 1980, p. 9). However, to provide a liberal or nonstringent test of
rectly. Six responses were not scored for these reasons. literalism, we performed an analysis in which we scored responses as
Scoring of literalism. Independent raters, who scored subjects' re- literal when the subject simply answered "yes" or "no" or when the
sponses for literalism, examined transcripts of subjects' responses re- subject first responded "yes" or "no" and then enacted a behavioral
corded by the hypnotist. Inasmuch as pilot testing revealed perfect response. According to this index of literalism, hypnotized subjects
agreement between the judges in rating the presence or absence of non- failed to differ in their responses from both simulating and "library"
verbal responses, the hypnotist was responsible for recording subjects' subjects.
LITERALISM AS A MARKER OF HYPNOTIC TRANCE 19

whether they would mind telling the hypnotist their name and question to obtain a measure of agreement between type of re-
stating where they were, \2(l,N= 24) = 4.44 and 4.20, respec- sponse and expectation. Agreement between type of response
tively, p < .05. Odds ratios computed between hypnotized and and expectation was above chance on each question (z name =
simulating subjects on the type of response showed that the 2.11, p < .05; z stand = 2.78, p < .05; z where = 4.07, p < .05;
probability for simulators to respond literally to the question, z something = 4.06, p < .05). Kappa values ranged from .42
"Do you mind telling me your name?" was approximately ("Do . . . standing up?") to .83 ("Do . . . where you are?").
seven times greater than the probability for hypnotized subjects Kappas conditional on type of response, computed for each
to respond literally to that question (O = 6.99). The probability question, showed that the agreement between responding liter-
for simulators to respond literally to the question, "Are you ally and reporting an expectation to respond literally was
willing to tell me where you are?" was approximately six times greater than or equal to the amount of agreement between re-
greater for simulators than for hypnotized subjects (O = 5.99). sponding nonliterally and expectation to respond nonliterally.
Simulators and hypnotized subjects did not differ in the fre- Conditional kappas ranged from .56 (name) to .83 (where) for
quency of literal responses to the questions that involved their literal responses and from .33 (name) to .83 (where) for nonlit-
responding to "Say something" and being asked whether they eral responses.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

minded standing up, x20> N = 24) < 1 and 2.74, respectively,


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

both ns. Discussion


Separate chi-square analyses across each of the four questions
were conducted for subjects in the library group versus hypno- This study constitutes the most definitive disconfirmation of
tized subjects. None of the chi-squares achieved significance (all Erickson's literalism hypothesis to date. Our design examined
X2 values < 1). the responses of the most hypnotizable individuals in the college
An examination of the percentage of literal versus nonliteral population. If literalism is a characteristic of hypnotized sub-
responses for subjects in each of the three groups is informative. jects' responses, as Erickson has claimed, we would expect the
The percentage of literal responses of the hypnotized and the greatest frequency of literalism to be exhibited by these talented
library subjects were quite comparable (29.17% vs. 21.78%, re- hypnotic virtuosos. Despite the fact that we provided perhaps
spectively). In contrast, the simulating subjects responded liter- the most propitious conditions for literalism to emerge, less
ally to 58.33% of the questions. than a third of the hypnotized subjects' responses were literal
Latencies. A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) in nature. This stands in sharp contrast to Erickson's claim that
comparing real and simulators' latencies in response to each of 97% of subjects in a deep trance respond literally. The failure
the questions separately failed to reach significance, F(4, of highly hypnotizable subjects to exhibit literalism cannot
19) = 1.26. readily be attributed to experimenter cuing or to the ambiguity
Active/passive measure. To explore real-simulating differ- of the interquestion interval. Experimenter blindness, the inclu-
ences on the measure of active/passive feelings for the four tests sion of simulating subjects in the experimental design, and the
of literalism, MANOVA was performed. This analysis failed to fact that subjects were prehypnotically informed about the
reveal a multivariate effect, F(4, 19) < 1. For each of the ques- meaning of the interquestion interval minimized these possibil-
tions, we performed an ANOVA to determine whether hypnotiz- ities. Our findings are entirely consistent with our first study
able and simulating subjects who responded literally felt more and with McCue and McCue's (1988) findings that literal re-
passive than did subjects who responded nonliterally. We found sponses to questions are infrequent and are by no means mark-
support for this hypothesis in relation to three of the four ques- ers of hypnotic responding.
tions: tell name, f\l, 22) = 10.24, p = .004 (Mliteral respon- Not only did hypnotizable subjects generally not respond in
ses = 3.11, SD = 0.61; M nonliteral = 4.13); stand up, P(l, a literal manner, but unhypnotizable subjects, asked to role play
22)= 18.25, p < .0001 (M literal = 2.14, SD = 1.02; M hypnosis, were twice as likely to respond literally. Hypnotizable
nonliteral = 3.80, SD = 0.79); tell where, P(l, 22) = 5.16, p = and simulating subjects were not found to differ in their latency
.033 (M literal = 3.46, SD = 0.82; M nonliteral = 4.15, SD = of responding to the questions. However, the fact that simulators
0.69). No differences were evident with respect to the statement were more responsive on the literalism index is particularly in-
"Say something," F( 1,22) < 1. teresting: Very few studies have found that hypnotizable and
Expectancies. An ANOVA was conducted on subjects' expec- simulating subjects can be distinguished in terms of their behav-
tations by summing the frequency of endorsed literal expectan- iors and cognitive processes.
cies (e.g., the hypnotist expected a literal response) on the ex- Literalism of response can now be added to a handful of mea-
pectancy questionnaire across each of the four tests of literal- sures that differentiate hypnotizable and simulating subjects.
ism. A tendency was observed for simulating (M = 2.42, SD = These include measures of trance logic (see Peters, 1973;
1.08) subjects' expectancies for literal responding to exceed that Spanos, 1986 for a review), "involuntary" response to counter-
of hypnotizable (M = 1.42, SD = 1.56) subjects, P(l, 22) = suggestions (see Lynn, Nash, Rhue, Frauman, & Sweeney,
3.31, p = .082. Chi-square analyses, performed on individual 1984), posthypnotic behavior outside the experimental setting
questions, revealed that simulators more frequently believed (Orne, Sheehan, & Evans, 1968), and source amnesia (Evans,
that the hypnotist expected a literal response than did hypnotic 1979), although the interpretation of real-simulator differences
subjects, in response to two questions: "Do you mind standing in each instance has been questioned (e.g., Lynn, Rhue, &
up," X2( 1, N = 24) = 4.44, p < .05 (Hypnotized = 41.66%; Sim- Weekes, 1989; Spanos, 1986; Spanos, Gwynn, Delia Malva, &
ulators = 83.33%) and "Do you mind telling me where you Bertrand, 1988; St. Jean, 1978).
are?", X2( 1, N = 24) = 4.20, p < .05 (Hypnotized = 25%, Simu- Simulators were found to be more likely than hypnotizable
lators = 66.66%). Cohen's kappa (1960) was computed for each subjects to respond in a manner consistent with theoretical pre-
20 GREEN ET AL.

dictions regarding trance behavior. This finding is compatible On each of the four tests of literalism, moderate-to-high
with research (e.g., Hilgard, 1977; Spanos, deGroot, & Gwynn, agreement was found between literal responses and subjects'
1987; Williamsen, Johnson, & Erikson, 1965) showing that postsession belief that the hypnotist expected a literal response.
simulators tend to respond in an "ideal" fashion, to the point of Conversely, moderate-to-high agreement was found between
outperforming hypnotized subjects. The fact that simulators— nonliteral responses and subjects' reports that the hypnotist ex-
whose sole task is to discern the task demands and to respond pected them to respond in a nonliteral manner. These findings
accordingly—were more literal than hypnotized subjects sug- suggest that literal responses are expectancy-based. However, it
gests an explanation of the relatively high levels of literalism is unclear whether subjects responded in accordance with how
of response reported by Erickson. It is possible that Erickson's they perceived they were expected to respond or whether their
subjects' literal responses were not associated with "hypnosis," attribution of how the hypnotist expected them to respond was
or hypnotizability, but instead were the product of the demands a post hoc attribution based on how subjects actually re-
of therapy or the test context, which encouraged literal respond- sponded. To begin to tease out these possibilities, it is necessary
ing. Erickson's belief that literalism was a classic marker of hyp- to manipulate subjects' prehypnotic expectancies about literal-
nosis, combined with his subjects' belief that literalism was an ism as a marker of hypnotizable subjects' performance.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

accompaniment of hypnosis, could have resulted in a high level


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of literal responding. Concluding Comments


A rather different account of the real-simulator differences
emphasizes differences in the task demands and goals of hypno- Although investigators (e.g., Erickson, 1980; Pattie, 1956;
tized and simulating subjects. Because simulators' primary task Shor, 1962, 1979; Weitzenhoffer, 1957; White, 1941) have
is to role play hypnotic behavior and to avoid detection as a maintained that literalism is a marker of hypnosis, the available
faker, in situations in which the appropriate response is ambigu- research clearly contradicts this assertion. Across multiple tests
ous, as is perhaps the case in the present study, the safest re- of literalism, hypnotizable subjects' rate of literal responding
sponse may be to respond in a minimal or constricted (Sheehan, varied from slightly more than 10%, when nonstringently
1971) manner, by responding only "yes" or "no" (i.e., literally) screened subjects were tested (Lynn, Green, et al., 1989), to less
rather than engaging in a behavioral response. Furthermore, be- than 30%, when the most talented subjects in the college popu-
cause hypnotized subjects may adopt a more cooperative set lation were tested in the present study. These rates are dramati-
than simulating subjects, who assume a virtual adversarial posi- cally lower than those reported by Erickson. Furthermore, in
tion with the hypnotist, hypnotized subjects may be more likely our previous research (Lynn, Green, et al., 1989), nonhypnotic,
to behave in accordance with the implicit demand of the hypno- task-motivated subjects equalled the performance of hypnotiz-
tist's request (e.g., to stand up) and to thereby exhibit a nonlit- able subjects, and, in the present study, hypnotizable subjects'
eral response. performance was equalled by nonhypnotic subjects tested in a
Whereas Erickson asserted that literalism was a ubiquitous naturalistic setting and was surpassed by nonhypnotizable sim-
characteristic of hypnotizable subjects, he stated with equal ulating subjects. Thus, our research indicates that literalism of
force that nonliteral responding characterized nonhypnotic re- response is neither unique to hypnosis nor virtually absent in
sponses: Erickson reported that 95% of the subjects in the wak- unhypnotized subjects.
ing state, even in the presence of a total stranger, responded in a Erickson failed to specify exactly how he tested for literalism.
nonliteral manner. When we directly tested this claim, we found Nonetheless, three studies that used somewhat different ways of
that it was not valid. About a fifth of the subjects, tested infor- assessing literalism of response by cuing versus noncuing sub-
mally in a naturalistic setting, responded in a literal manner to jects for literalism (McCue & McCue, 1988), by testing literal-
questions they were asked. Indeed, an important finding of our ism formally (Lynn, Green, et al., 1989, and the present study)
research was that the frequency of literal responses of awake versus informally (McCue & McCue, 1988), and by adopting
subjects tested for literalism in a naturalistic setting was indis- brief (as in the present study) versus long (Lynn, Green, et al.,
tinguishable from that of our hypnotic virtuosos. 1989) interquestion intervals, have universally failed to support
As Barber (1969) has pointed out, many of the supposed Erickson's claims. Regardless of the method used to test literal-
trancelike characteristics that have been ascribed to hypnotized ism, the great majority of hypnotized subjects fail to exhibit
subjects, such as drowsiness, sleepiness, and relaxation, are literalism of response. Our research underlines the need for the
themselves suggested. Answering a question "yes" or "no" dur- controlled investigation of hypnotic phenomena and supports
ing hypnosis has been conceptualized as a "literal" response. our contention that literalism arises as a function of hypnotiz-
However, if we think of a simple "yes" or "no" as a minimal able subjects' sensitivity to the broad demands of the testing
response to a question, it is possible that subjects who feel context rather than as a function of a purported trance state.
drowsy, sleepy, passive, and relaxed would be particularly in-
clined to respond this way, especially if they believed that it was References
consistent with the hypnotist's intentions and the experimental
Barber, T. X. (1969). Hypnosis: A scientific approach. New Jersey: Power
demands. A literal response might, therefore, be viewed by
Publishers.
some subjects as proof, or as a way of demonstrating, that they Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educa-
are deeply hypnotized. Our findings provide support for this tional and Psychological Measurement, 20, 37-46.
possibility. Although hypnotizable and simulating subjects did Erickson, M. H. (1980). Literalness: An experimental study. In E. Rossi
not differ in their feelings of passivity in their responses to the (Ed.), The collected papers of Milton H. Erickson on hypnosis: Vol. 3.
questions, passive feelings were generally associated with more Hypnotic investigation of psychodynamic processes. New \brk: Ir-
literal responses. vington.
LITERALISM AS A MARKER OF HYPNOTIC TRANCE 21

Erickson, M. H., Hershman, S., & Secter, I. I. (1961). The practical International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 34,
applications of medical dental hypnosis. New York: Julian. 84-97.
Erickson, M. H, Rossi, E. L., & Rossi, S. I. (1976). Hypnotic realities. Rossi, E. L. (1986). The Indirect Trance Assessment Scale (ITAS): A
New York: Irvington. preliminary outline and learning tool. In M. Yapko (Ed.), Hypnotic
Erickson, M. H, & Rossi, E. L. (1979). Hypnotherapy: An exploratory and strategic interventions: Principles and practice. New York: Irving-
casebook. New York: Irvington. ton.
Sheehan, P. W. (1971). A methodological analysis of the simulating
Evans, F. J. (1979). Contextual forgetting: Posthypnotic source amne-
technique. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hyp-
sia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88,556-563.
nosis, 19,83-99.
Hilgard, J. R. (1977). Divided consciousness: Multiple controls in hu-
Shor, R. E. (1962). Three dimensions of hypnotic depth. International
man thought and action. New \brk: John Wiley.
Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 10,23-28.
Hilgard, J. R. (1988). Milton Erickson as playwright and director. Inter- Shor, R. E. (1979). A phenomenological method for the measurement
national Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 36, 128- of variables important to an understanding of the nature of hypnosis.
140. In E. Fromm & R. Shor (Eds.), Hypnosis: Developments in research
Lynn, S. J., Green, J. P., Weekes, J. R., Carlson, B. W., Brentar, J., La- and new perspectives. New \brk: Aldine.
tham, L., & Kurzhals, R. (1989). Literalism in hypnotized and task Shor, R. E., & Orne, E. C. (1962). Harvard Group Scale of Hypnotic
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

motivated subjects. Manuscript submitted for publication. Susceptibility, Form A. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Lynn, S. J., Nash, M. R., Rhue, J. W., Frauman, D. C, & Sweeney, C. Spanos, N. P. (1986). Hypnotic behavior A social-psychological inter-
(1984). Nonvolition, expectancies, and hypnotic rapport. Journal of pretation of amnesia, analgesia, and "trance logic." The Behavioral
Abnormal Psychology, 93,295-303. and Brain Sciences, 9,449-503.
Lynn, S. J., Rhue, J. R., & Weekes, J. R. (1989). Hypnosis and experi- Spanos, N. P., deGroot, H., & Gwynn, M. (1987). Hypnosis as incom-
enced nonvdition: An integrative social-cognitive model. In N. P. plete responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53,
Spanos & 1. Chaves (Eds.), Hypnosis: A cognitive-behavioral perspec- 911-921.
tive(pp. 78-109). New York: Prometheus Books. Spanos, N. P., Gwynn, M. I., Delia Malva, C. L., & Bertrand, L. (1988).
McCue, P. A., &. McCue, E. C. (1988). Literalness: An unsuggested Social psychological factors in the genesis of posthypnotic source am-
(spontaneous) item of hypnotic behavior? International Journal of nesia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 322-329.
Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 36,192-197. St. Jean, R. (1978). Posthypnotic behavior as a function of experimental
Orne, M. T. (1959). The nature of hypnosis: Artifact and essence. Jour- surveillance. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 20,250-255.
nal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, 277-299. Weitzenhoffer, A. M. (1957). General techniques of hypnotism. New
Orne, M. T. (1971). The simulation of hypnosis: Why, how, and what it York: Grune and Stratton.
means. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, Weitzenhoffer, A. M., & Hilgard, E. R. (1962). Stanford Hypnotic Sus-
19, 183-210. ceptibility Scale, Form C. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists
Press.
Orne, M. T, Sheehan, P. W., & Evans, F. J. (1968). Occurrence of post-
White, R. W. (1941). A preface to the theory of hypnotism. Journal of
hypnotic behavior outside of the experimental setting. Journal of Per-
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 36,447-505.
sonality and Social Psychology, 9, 189-196.
Williamsen, J. A., Johnson, H. J., & Eriksen, C. W. (1965). Some char-
Pattie, F. A. (1956). Methods of induction, susceptibility of subjects, acteristics of posthypnotic amnesia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
and criteria of hypnosis. In R. M. Dorcus (Ed.), Hypnosis and its 70, 123-131.
therapeutic applications. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Peters, J. E. (1973). Trance logic: Artifact or essence in hypnosis. Un- Received January 12,1989
published doctoral dissertation. Pennsylvania State University. Revision received July 14,1989
Register, P. A., & Kihlstrom, J. F. (1986). Finding the hypnotic virtuoso. Accepted July 21,1989 •

You might also like