You are on page 1of 11

Why do consumers procrastinate and what

happens next?
Shabnam Azimi
Department of Marketing, Loyola University Chicago, Chicago, Illinois, USA, and
George R. Milne and Elizabeth G. Miller
Department of Marketing, University of Massachusetts Amherst, Amherst, Massachusetts, USA

Abstract
Purpose – This paper aims to examine the factors leading to and resulting from procrastination under high price uncertainty and provide
recommendations for how managers can reduce consumer procrastination, thus decreasing consumer regret, anger and retaliatory behaviors.
Design/methodology/approach – Hypothesized relationships were tested through two scenario-based experiments using student samples. Data
was analyzed using general linear model, path analysis and Wald chi-square test.
Findings – Long time limits, price uncertainty and price consciousness, all increase the likelihood of procrastination. Prestige seeking reduces
procrastination, but only when time limits are short. When one delays a purchase and later the price of the item gets increased or one makes a
purchase and later the price gets further reduced, procrastination and purchase decision both equally can lead to anger, which then increases the
probability of exit, voice or word of mouth (WOM); however, procrastination has a much stronger impact than deciding to purchase on self-
responsibility and regret, which in turn increases negative WOM.
Research limitations/implications – This paper provides a greater understanding of antecedents and consequences of procrastination as well as
the drivers of retaliatory behavior. Further, the findings highlight differential consequences of consumer regret and anger on consumption behaviors.
Practical implications – This paper provides practical suggestions for reducing consumers’ procrastination through leveraging the effects of
purchase time limit and price uncertainty in general, and more specifically, for prestige-seeker and price conscious consumers. The findings provide
evidence for a silent path from procrastination to retaliation and highlight the importance of possible remedies or interventions by the companies to
mitigate consumer emotions resulting from procrastination.
Originality/value – To the best of the authors’ knowledge, this research is the first to apply temporal motivation theory in the context of consumer
behavior under price uncertainty, and examine consequences of consumer procrastination in terms of thoughts, feelings and retaliatory behavior.
Keywords Procrastination, Delay decision-making, Price uncertainty, Consumer emotions, Consumer complaint
Paper type Research paper

Introduction understanding antecedents and consequences of consumer’s


intentional delay of a purchase which will be referred to as
Many consumers experience buying an item at a price and later
“consumer procrastination.”
observing others bought the same item from the same company
Procrastination behavior is often defined as a delay in
for a much cheaper price as a result of downward price
completing a task (Akerlof, 1991; Shu and Gneezy, 2010). The
adjustment. Research indicates that in such situations, firms’
term is not necessarily used in a negative sense as several
most valuable customers may feel antagonized and stop
researchers also define procrastination as a functional delay or
purchasing from the company resulting in significant loss of
revenue (Anderson and Duncan, 2008). While research has avoiding rushed decisions (Bernstein, 1998; Choi and Moran,
explored the implications of downward price adjustments, the 2009; Chu and Choi, 2005). Specifically, procrastination can
reverse situation, or the upward price adjustment, has received be a sound strategy when the outcome is uncertain and more
limited attention, even though it is also common. What information is needed (Bernstein, 1998). Along with this line of
happens if a consumer decides to delay a purchase when an research, we define consumer procrastination as an intentional
item is on sale and later sees the firm is charging a higher price delay of a purchase task under uncertain outcome conditions
for the same item? Would he/she feel any regret or anger? despite one’s intent and ability to buy.
Would he/she become hostile? Why did he/she decide to delay Consumer procrastination is a common behavior, especially,
the purchase in the first place? In this research, we focus on as in an intensely competitive business environment,
consumers are extensively challenged and confused when there
is a high level of price uncertainty. Using a detailed price data
The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available on Emerald set, Nakamura and Steinsson (2008) have estimated that the
Insight at: https://www.emerald.com/insight/0736-3761.htm median frequency of consumer price change is around 20% per

Journal of Consumer Marketing


Received 16 July 2019
37/7 (2020) 795–805 Revised 6 March 2020
© Emerald Publishing Limited [ISSN 0736-3761] 28 May 2020
[DOI 10.1108/JCM-07-2019-3329] Accepted 15 July 2020

795
Consumers procrastinate Journal of Consumer Marketing
Shabnam Azimi, George R. Milne and Elizabeth G. Miller Volume 37 · Number 7 · 2020 · 795–805

month. This level of price fluctuation makes it very unlikely for E V


Utility ¼ (1)
consumers to predict outcomes of their purchase decisions, C D
increases the complexity of consumer decision-making and can
result in delayed decisions. Not only does this lack of Where, utility is defined as the desirability of a behavior; this
confidence in the price hinder purchase decisions, it can also formula has been used to explain the likelihood of
deteriorate shopping enjoyment and post-purchase satisfaction procrastination by assuming that tasks with higher utility are
for consumers. This paper studies consumer procrastination as more desirable and hence less likely to be procrastinated.
an outcome of this price change climate and as a delay strategy Expectancy (E) is the probability that an action leads to an
used by consumers in hope of maximizing the chance of buying outcome; value (V) is the attractiveness or the perceived
at the best possible price. Through two studies, we examine satisfaction from an outcome; delay (D) represents a task’s
both the variables leading to (Study 1) and the ones resulting deadline or time limit to complete a task; and sensitivity to delay
from (Study 2) consumer procrastination. (C) is related to the variability in individuals’ personality traits
Prior research has looked at causes of consumers’ decisions which influences the effect of delay on procrastination (Steel
to delay purchases (Greenleaf and Lehmann, 1995; Shu and and König, 2006). Based on this theory, people are less likely to
Gneezy, 2010) but have not organized those reasons into a procrastinate activities with higher expectancy and value, as
comprehensive theoretical framework that might guide greater such activities are more desirable and have higher utility.
understanding of consumer procrastination under certain Further, tasks for which the rewards are delayed or have long
boundary conditions. In addition, while the marketing deadlines are perceived to have less utility and lead to higher
literature has examined the outcomes of consumers’ actions likelihood of procrastination. Individuals with greater
(i.e. purchases), the consequences of consumers’ inactions (i.e. sensitivity to deadlines have greater likelihood of
procrastination) has not been studied. To address these gaps procrastination on tasks with long deadlines.
and extend the previous findings, in this paper, we aim to first Although TMT has been found to be useful in explaining
understand why consumers choose to procrastinate in the procrastination that is defined in a negative sense, the domain
context of price variability. To address this research question, of its applicability to other contexts is not yet clear (Steel,
we adapt an integrative theory of motivation to study the effects 2007). In this research, we apply TMT to the consumer
of several marketing-related factors on the decision to behavior context in which procrastination is not defined as a
procrastinate. Our second purpose is to examine whether
self-regulatory problem but is used as an intentional strategy in
consumer procrastination has any cognitive (i.e. self-blame),
hope of gaining a more positive outcome. In doing so, we
emotional (i.e. regret and anger) and behavioral (i.e. retaliation
explore the roles of expectancy (degree of uncertainty), delay
decisions such as negative word of mouth [WOM], exit and
(time limit) and sensitivity to delay (personal characteristics) on
complaining [voice]) outcomes. Hypothesized effects are
consumer procrastination. The attractiveness of the purchase
subsequently tested through scenario-based experiments.
decision which represents the impact of the value component in
Our findings contribute to marketing theory and practice by
providing a greater understanding of the antecedents and TMT is excluded for simplicity.
consequences of procrastination as well as the drivers of
retaliatory behavior. The following sections present the Role of price uncertainty
theoretical framework to address antecedents and The degree of uncertainty about a purchase decision in the
consequences of consumer procrastination followed by Studies consumer behavior context, which often occurs because of
1 and 2, their results and discussion of the implications. frequent price promotions (Mehta et al., 2003), represents the
role of expectancy factor in TMT. Past research indicates that
Antecedents of consumer procrastination consumers procrastinate on their decisions when they are
Past research identifies two sets of antecedent factors that lead uncertain about their decisions and when they perceive a
to consumer procrastination: situational (e.g. market-related) financial risk (Greenleaf and Lehmann, 1995). An underlying
and individual. Examples of situational causes are: lack of time, reason for this inaction can be related to anticipation of regret.
aversive shopping tasks, performance and financial risks of the In a scenario-based experiment, Simonson (1992) asked the
purchase, the need for advice, the need for further research and participants to choose between buying an available item or wait
fear of price changes (Greenleaf and Lehmann, 1995). for a better sale. In the treatment condition, people were
Individual causes of procrastination on shopping tasks may prompted to consider the regret they may feel if they wait for a
include indecision or lack of energy (Ferrari, 1993). This better sale and then realize their decision was wrong. A greater
research uses the temporal motivation theory (TMT) to number of people in this condition chose to buy the available
provide an overarching framework for categorizing situational item (rather than wait) compared to the control condition.
and individual variables that have been shown to explain Based on the above discussion, when people are uncertain
consumer procrastination and provides new insights into their about the price and anticipate a price fall, they tend to maintain
joint influence on procrastination. TMT is a theory of inaction to avoid possible future regret. Therefore, we
motivation in social psychology that has been found to be useful hypothesize:
in explaining procrastination (Steel, 2007). TMT proposes that
four factors, expectancy, value, delay and sensitivity to delay, H1. In purchase situations with higher price uncertainty,
predict the motivation or desirability of a task according to consumers are more likely to procrastinate rather than
equation (1): buying an available product at regular price.

796
Consumers procrastinate Journal of Consumer Marketing
Shabnam Azimi, George R. Milne and Elizabeth G. Miller Volume 37 · Number 7 · 2020 · 795–805

Role of time limit Consequences of consumer procrastination


According to TMT, the time between action and reward (i.e.
Research has found that people anticipate more regret for
task deadline) is an important factor in determining the level of
negative actions rather than negative inactions (Kahneman and
procrastination. While past research provides a good
Tversky, 1982). This tendency is named omission bias (Baron
understanding of the effect of deadlines on procrastination in
and Ritov, 1994). Baron and Ritov (1994) expanded this idea
completing aversive tasks (Ariely and Wertenbroch, 2002), the
to the conditions in which both actions and inactions entail
effect of time limits is rarely studied for positive enjoyable some positive and some negative outcomes. In these “tradeoff”
activities. One of the few examples is Shu and Gneezy’s (2010) situations, the authors found subjects prefer inaction
research, which showed that similar to the findings related to (omission) rather than action. The authors consequently
the aversive tasks, people with unlimited time windows or long predicted an interaction between action vs omission and better
deadlines were more likely to procrastinate engaging in pleasant vs worse outcomes and the greater effects for the actions was
tasks such as visiting a landmark. In this research, we extend named “amplification effect.”
these previous findings and test whether the same effect for In our context, purchase is the relevant action, while
time limit holds for the purchase context and shopping tasks. procrastination represents inaction. We compare consumers’
In-line with both lines of research on positive and negative emotions in two conditions:
tasks, we expect later purchase deadlines to increase the 1 one does not make a purchase when a product is on sale
likelihood of procrastination on shopping tasks: and then learns that there is no future sale (forgone gain);
and
H2. In purchase situations with longer time limits, 2 one makes a purchase in a sale and then learns that the
consumers are more likely to procrastinate rather than future sale is larger (perceived loss).
buying an available product at regular price.
This interaction can be illustrated in terms of the value function
in prospect theory (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979). Based on
Role of consumer characteristics prospect theory, the value function is S-shaped and losses loom
To capture the role of sensitivity to delay factor in TMT, we larger than gains. This study examines whether the value
focus on those who presumably are more sensitive vs less function looms larger for perceived loss (resulted from
sensitive to delay (i.e. purchase deadlines). To capture these purchase) compared to forgone gain (resulted from
two tendencies, we focus on price consciousness and prestige procrastination/not purchase) as suggested by Baron and
seeking. We expect that price conscious individuals are more Ritov’s (1994) amplification effect. That is, we predict that the
sensitive to purchase deadlines and prestige seekers are less negative emotional and behavioral outcomes associated with
sensitive to purchase deadlines, as we explain below. perceived loss are stronger than the ones associated with
Price consciousness: Some consumers are more concerned forgone gain. We discuss this position along with our
about paying low prices than others. Price consciousness has hypotheses in the following sections.
been defined as “the degree to which consumer focuses
exclusively on paying low prices” and it is positively associated Cognitive appraisal and emotional outcomes
with low price search outside the store and sale response Past research shows that people attribute the causes of their
behavior (Lichtenstein et al., 1993). Hence, definitionally, we success or failure to a variety of sources. The explanations that
expect price conscious consumers to be more likely to delay individuals provide for the causes of their success or failure can
purchases in hope of better prices when their purchase be categorized under three dimensions: causality, stability and
deadlines are longer: controllability (Russell, 1982). The causality dimension is
related to the perception of responsibility which indicates
H3a. In purchase situations with longer time limits, whether one perceives self or others responsible for the failure
consumers who are more price conscious are more or success. The stability dimension is the perception that the
likely to procrastinate rather than buying an available cause is permanent or temporary, and controllability is related
product at regular price. to perceiving the cause as being controllable by one-self or
others. The distinction between causal attributions is important
Prestige seeking is consumers’ tendency to respond more as it helps researchers understand the sources of emotions and
positively to products’ quality, high price and uniqueness as behaviors and make more accurate predictions regarding those
indicators of the purchasers’ social status and conspicuousness responses.
(Vigneron and Johnson, 1999). Based on this definition, in Smith and Ellsworth (1985) posit that similarly valenced
longer time limits, we expect prestige seekers to make their emotions such as anger and regret can be differentiated based
purchase decisions based on drivers such as sociability and self- on their underlying cognitive appraisal dimensions. Individuals
expression rather than delaying purchases in hope of lower experience anger when they blame others or the situation and
prices. Therefore, we expect prestige seekers to be less sensitive perceive high other-control over negative outcomes (Averill,
to longer time limits in making their purchase decisions: 1983). This experience often involves the perception that the
outcome was not fair (Smith and Ellsworth, 1985). In contrast,
H3b. In purchase situations with longer time limits, regret is an emotional response to comparing one’s outcome
consumers who are more prestige seeking are less likely with a better forgone alternative (Bell, 1985). According to
to procrastinate rather than buying an available product Loomes and Sugden (1982), regret theory serves as an
at regular price. explanation for decision under uncertainty and it is believed to

797
Consumers procrastinate Journal of Consumer Marketing
Shabnam Azimi, George R. Milne and Elizabeth G. Miller Volume 37 · Number 7 · 2020 · 795–805

be a simpler alternative to Kahneman and Tversky’s (1979) Previous literature manifested complaint intention in terms
prospect theory. Regret theory posits that people experience of voice, exit (Hirschman, 1970) or negative WOM (Singh,
regret when they realize they could make a better decision; 1990). Exit is consumers’ decision to end the exchange
therefore, in their decisions, individuals try to anticipate and relationship; voice is consumers’ attempt to reflect their
account for this possible future regret (Loomes and Sugden, complaint to someone from the company that listens; and
1982). To avoid future regret, individuals may decide not to negative WOM is when consumers complain to friends and
take an action or delay it further. According to Bonifıeld and relatives. Consistent with the past research explained above, we
Cole (2007), regret is different from anger as it occurs as a expect those consumers who experience greater anger to be
result of higher perception of self-control over negative more prone to indicate greater complaint intention in terms of
outcome and blaming oneself. Based on the above discussion, it voice, exit and negative WOM:
is expected that in situations with negative outcomes (i.e.
perceived loss or foregone gain), consumers who decided to H8. Consumer who experience greater anger will show
purchase rather than procrastinate will perceive greater other- greater complaint intention in terms of exit, voice and
responsibility and those who decided to procrastinate will negative WOM.
perceive greater self-responsibility. In addition, regret is driven
by self-responsibility so those with higher self-responsibility Based on H7 and H8, those in perceived loss condition perceive
should have higher regret: greater anger than consumers in the forgone gain condition and
greater anger can result in greater complaint intention.
H4. Consumers in the forgone gain condition perceive Therefore, a natural extension of the above hypotheses is to
greater self-responsibility than consumers in the expect those individuals in perceived loss condition to show
perceived loss condition. greater complaint intention compared to the ones in forgone
gain condition:
H5. Consumers who perceive greater self-responsibility will
experience greater regret but not greater anger. H9. Consumers in the perceived loss condition show greater
complaint intention in terms of exit, voice and negative
Based on H4 and H5, those in forgone gain situation perceive
WOM than consumers in the forgone gain condition.
greater self-responsibility and those with greater self-
responsibility experience greater regret. Therefore, we would
expect those in forgone gain condition to experience greater
regret while those in perceived loss condition will experience Study 1
greater anger:
Experimental design and procedure
H6. Consumers in the foregone gain condition perceive A total of 411 (57.4% male) students at a large state university
greater regret than consumers in the perceived loss in the US participated in the survey. Participants were
condition. randomly assigned to the experimental conditions in a 2 (price
uncertainty: low vs high)  2 (purchase time limit: 2 weeks vs
H7. Consumers in the perceived loss condition perceive 2 months) between-subject factorial design. Uncertainty and
greater anger than consumers in the forgone gain time limit were manipulated through scenario-based method.
condition. Scenarios were adapted from work by Simonson (1992).
Participants were presented with a purchase decision situation
Behavioral responses (see Appendix 1 for the scenarios) and asked to rate how likely
Research in service failure domain indicates consumers’ it is that they wait for a better sale on a six-point Likert scale as a
appraisals of their experience predict their emotions which in measure of consumer procrastination.
turn affect their behavioral responses. The result of a study on After the question related to procrastination tendency,
passengers’ reactions to delayed flights showed that greater participants completed a filler task followed by manipulation
perception of controllability of the problem results in greater checks and measures of price consciousness and prestige
anger and greater complaint intention. Anger was the partial seeking. As intended, 92.9% of the people in the short time
mediator in this effect (Folkes et al., 1987). Similarly, Bonifıeld limit condition (2 weeks) and 93% of the people in long
and Cole (2007) found anger as a strong predictor of time limit condition (2 months) correctly recalled the time of
consumers’ retaliatory behavior such as aggressive complaining the wedding (X2= 302.57, df = 1, p < 0.001). Similarly, people
and negative word-of-mouth. Their result, however, showed in the conditions with low price uncertainty (M = 3.48) were
that the feeling of regret also affects conciliatory behavior such significantly more certain [F (1, 409) = 85.54, p < 0.001] about
as positive WOM and willingness to return to the service the price change than the people in conditions with high price
provider. Unlike anger, when examining the effect of regret on uncertainty (M = 2.69) when they were asked to rate their
complaint intention, Tsiros and Mittal (2000) did not find a certainty levels (1 = very uncertain and 5 = very certain). Thus,
significant effect. In the same context of service dissatisfaction, our manipulations were successful.
however, Zeelenberg and Pieters (1999) examined the Price consciousness (a = 0.717) and prestige seeking (a =
differential effects of regret vs disappointment and found that 0.847) were measured based on the scales validated by
regret can result in more switching behavior, but Lichtenstein et al. (1993). See Appendix 2 for a complete list of
disappointment can lead to more WOM. scales.

798
Consumers procrastinate Journal of Consumer Marketing
Shabnam Azimi, George R. Milne and Elizabeth G. Miller Volume 37 · Number 7 · 2020 · 795–805

Results Our results showed no significant interaction between time limit


General linear model (GLM) examined the effects of and price consciousness (F = 1.70, p > 0.10) but a significant
independent variables (time limit, price uncertainty, price interaction between time limit and prestige seeking (F = 3.91, p <
consciousness and prestige seeking) and interactions with time 0.05) (Figure 2) The significantly positive effect of price
limit on consumer procrastination likelihood. Continuous consciousness on procrastination was no different in the long
predictor variables were mean centered before calculating the time limit (b = 0.81, p < 0.0001) compared to the short one
interaction terms to reduce multicollinearity. The GLM was (b = 0.60, p < 0.0001). However, the effect of prestige-seeking
significant [F (7, 403) = 20.04, p < 0.0001] with adjusted R2 of tendencies on procrastination was only positive and significant in
0.24 (see Figure 1 for means and Table 1 for the GLM results). the short time limit condition (b = 0.32, p < 0.01) and was non-
Consistent with H1 and H2, people in conditions with higher significant in the long time limit condition (b = 0.07, p > 0.10).
uncertainty level (M = 3.39) reported greater likelihood of These results are contrary to what we expected in H3a and H3b.
procrastination (F = 13.08, p < 0.001) than those in low
uncertain conditions (M = 2.89); and subjects with a longer Discussion
time limit (M = 3.59) reported greater likelihood of Study 1 indicated that longer time limits increase consumers’
procrastination (F = 47.93, p < 0.001) than the ones who had a procrastination likelihood regardless of whether price
shorter time limit (M = 2.68). As Figure 1 shows, the uncertainty is high or not. However, the impact of price
interaction between time limit and price uncertainty was uncertainty is contingent upon time limit. In short time slots,
significant (F = 5.38, p < 0.05). Price uncertainty influenced consumers make much faster decisions if the uncertainty is low
procrastination when the time limit was short [Mlow uncertainty = compared to high. In long time slots, however, low uncertainty
2.28; Mhigh uncertainty = 3.08, F (1, 222) = 21.25, p < 0.001], (i.e. greater certainty) does not accelerate consumers’ decision-
but not when the time limit was long [Mlow-uncertainty = 3.49; making. Our findings did not provide any evidence for higher
Mhigh-uncertainty = 3.70, F (1, 185) = 1.06, p > 0.10]. In sensitivity to deadlines for price conscious consumers. Results
contrast, the difference in procrastination means were related to prestige seekers provided a complex picture
significant in both low [M2-weeks = 2.28; M2-months = 3.49, F (1, indicating that in longer time limits, consumers who score
198) = 41.09, p < 0.001] and high [M2-weeks = 3.08; higher or lower on prestige seeking may equally procrastinate
on purchasing an item on a regular price. However, for
M2-months = 3.70, F (1, 209) = 10.53, p < 0.001] uncertainty
purchase tasks with shorter deadlines, those consumers who
levels.
score lower on prestige seeking chose to procrastinate less and

Figure 1 Interaction between time limit and price uncertainty on


Figure 2 Interaction between time limit and consumer characteristics
procrastination likelihood (Study 1)
on procrastination likelihood (Study 1)

Table 1 Antecedents of consumer procrastination (Study 1)


Independent variables F (df = 1) B SE
Uncertainty 13.085 0.750 0.17
Time limit 47.930 1.165 0.18
Price consciousness 19.321 0.562 0.13
Prestige-seeking 7.934 0.334 0.12
Time limit 3 Uncertainty 5.385 0.586 0.25
Time limit 3 Price consciousness 1.700 0.245 0.19
Time limit 3 Prestige-seeking 3.914 0.349 0.18
Adjusted R2 = 0.245

Notes: p < 0.001;  p < 0.01;  p < 0.05

799
Consumers procrastinate Journal of Consumer Marketing
Shabnam Azimi, George R. Milne and Elizabeth G. Miller Volume 37 · Number 7 · 2020 · 795–805

those who scored higher on prestige seeking chose to included a single item for exit, two items for WOM (r = 0.439;
procrastinate more. These finding suggest higher sensitivity to p < 0.001) and five items underlying voice (a = 0.869)
“shorter deadlines” for prestige seeker consumers. Findings of dimension.
Study 1, all together provided a greater understanding about Manipulation checks for purchase decision and outcome
market and individual variables leading to consumer knowledge were successful. Of the respondents, 92.6% of those
procrastination. Our hypotheses related to the cognitive, in the “purchased” condition and 88.2% of those in the “not
emotional and behavioral variables resulting from consumer purchased” condition, correctly recalled whether the scenario
procrastination will be tested and discussed in Study 2. indicated they had purchased the item on their first trip or not
(X2 = 269.57, df = 1, p < 0.001). Similarly, 74.1% of those in
Study 2 the “no future sale” condition and 85.0% of those in the “60%
off future sale” condition correctly identified whether the item
Experimental design and procedure was priced higher or lower on their first trip vs now (X2 =
The same group of subjects who completed Study 1 were 145.28, df = 1, p < 0.001).
prompted to participate in Study 2. We placed a distractor task
between the two studies to minimize any potential carry-over Results
effects. Study 2 was a 2 (decision: purchase vs not purchase The means related to the cognitive, emotional and behavioral
when the item is on sale)  2 (future price: larger future sale vs outcomes in the two negative outcome conditions are shown in
no future sale) between-subjects factorial design. Hypothetical Table 2. Note that consistent with the originally adopted scales,
scenarios (see Appendix 1) were used to manipulate the self-responsibility was measured using a nine-point semantic
independent variables. Participants were presented with a pre- differential scale, while the other measures used five-point
purchase decision situation following by a post-decision Likert scales. In-line with H4, self-responsibility means were
situation. To control for potential effects of price and type of significantly different [F (1, 219) = 26.09, p < 0.001] between
product, a hedonic low-price product (i.e. a shirt) and a the forgone gain (M = 5.11) and perceived loss conditions
utilitarian high-price product (i.e. a laptop) were used as an (M = 3.85). Similarly, regret was significantly different [F (1,
additional between-subjects factor. The type of product did not 219) = 9.72, p < 0.01] between the forgone gain (M = 3.88)
have any significant impact on the outcome variables of interest and perceived loss conditions (M = 3.51), supporting H6. The
and consequently we combined the results across the two differences in means for anger [F (1, 219) = 0.859, p > 0.10]
product classes. After reading the scenarios, participants were non-significant between the forgone gain and perceived
completed ratings of their emotions and complaint intentions loss conditions. Thus, H7 was not supported. The results
along with manipulation check measures and demographic further indicate that the means for two out of three behavioral
questions. intentions (i.e. WOM and voice) are significantly greater for
The interaction between the two experimental conditions of perceived loss than forgone gain, providing partial support for
purchase decision (purchased, not purchased) and future price H9. When a negative outcome is experienced (perceived loss or
(better future sale, no future sale) created situations foregone gain), respondents in action condition (perceived
representing foregone gain (not purchased, no future sale) and loss) indicated greater intention for voice [Mperceived-loss = 2.04,
perceived loss (purchased, larger future sale), as well as two Mforgone-gain = 1.76; F (1, 219) = 7.04, p < 0.01] and spread of
additional conditions: foregone loss (no purchase, larger future negative WOM [Mperceived-loss = 2.71, Mforgone-gain = 2.41; F (1,
sale) and perceived gain (purchased, no future sale). Because of 219) = 10.96, p < 0.001]. However, the difference between the
the nature of the dependent variables of interest, our analysis perceived loss and foregone gain conditions were not significant
will focus on differences between foregone gain and perceived in terms of exit [M perceived-loss = 2.41, M forgone-gain = 2.16; F
loss to capture the possible differences between negative (1, 219) = 3.68, p > 0.05] (Table 3).
outcome conditions (Table 2). The above analysis suggests differences between
Self-responsibility (a = 0.747) was measured using locus of consequences of action vs inaction; however, it does not
causality subsection of Russell’s (1982) consumer appraisal provide a clear picture regarding the processes through which
of the experience scale. We also used a modified version of procrastination or purchase may lead to cognitive, emotional
Bonifıeld and Cole’s (2007) regret (a = 0.934) and anger (a = and behavioral outcomes. Path analysis was used to examine
0.922) scales and Singh’s (1988) complaint intention scale to these processes and the results are shown in Table 4. Overall
measure behavioral intentions. The behavioral intention scale model fit was good with comparative fit index (CFI) of 1.00
and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) of
Table 2 Interaction between purchase decision and future price/outcome 0.000. Relationships of interest are shaded in the table. The
knowledge (Study 2) parameter estimates indicate that consistent with H5, self-
responsibility positively impacted regret ( b = 0.051, p < 0.05)
Future price but had no effect on anger ( b = 0.039, p > 0.10). Regret, in
Purchase decision 70% future sale No future sale turn, had a marginally positive effect on WOM ( b = 0.080, p =
Purchased at 30% off Perceived loss Perceived gain 0.06) and no significant effect on exit ( b = 0.084, p > 0.10) or
Delayed purchase at 40% off Forgone loss Forgone gain voice ( b = 0.031, p > 0.10). Lastly, consistent with the
prediction in H8, effect of anger was positive and significant on
Notes:  Because of our focus on the negative-outcome conditions, only all three behavioral intentions ( b = 0.461, p < 0.001 on exit;
the two highlighted conditions will be used in this study. In both b = 0.183, p < 0.001 on WOM; and b = 0.376, p < 0.001on
conditions, there was a net loss of 40%
voice). Figure 3 illustrates these effects.

800
Consumers procrastinate Journal of Consumer Marketing
Shabnam Azimi, George R. Milne and Elizabeth G. Miller Volume 37 · Number 7 · 2020 · 795–805

Table 3 Means of consequences of consumer procrastination in the two negative outcome conditions (Study 2)
Forgone gain Perceived loss
(Not purchased at 40% off, future price: original price) (Purchased at 30% off, future price: 70% off) df = 1, 219
Means F
Self-responsibility (1–9) 5.11 3.85 26.089
Regret (1–5) 3.88 3.51 9.721
Anger (1–5) 2.90 3.01 0.859
WOM (1–5) 2.41 2.71 10.963
Exit (1–5) 2.16 2.41 3.678
Voice (1–5) 1.76 2.04 7.043

Notes: p < 0.001;  p < 0.01

Table 4 Results of path analysis (Study 2)


Direct effects Coefficient SE

Outcome knowledge fi Self-responsibility 1.118 0.24
Purchase decision fi Self-responsibility 0.888 0.23
Outcome 3 Purchase fi Self-responsibility 0.749 0.33
Outcome knowledge fi Anger 0.807 0.13
Purchase decision fi Anger 1.107 0.13
Outcome 3 Purchase fi Anger 2.080 0.18
Self-responsibility fi Anger 0.039 0.03
Outcome knowledge fi Regret 1.791 0.12
Purchase decision fi Regret 2.106 0.12
Outcome 3 Purchase fi Regret 3.595 0.17
Self-responsibility fi Regret 0.051 0.02
Outcome knowledge fi Exit 0.552 0.14
Purchase decision fi Exit 0.365 0.14
Outcome 3 Purchase fi Exit 0.697 0.22
Anger fi Exit 0.461 0.05
Regret fi Exit 0.084 0.05
Outcome knowledge fi WOM 0.590 0.11
Purchase decision fi WOM 0.362 0.12
Outcome 3 Purchase fi WOM 0.651 0.18
Anger fi WOM 0.183 0.04
Regret fi WOM 0.080^ 0.04
Outcome knowledge fi Voice 0.579 0.12
Purchase decision fi Voice 0.391 0.12
Outcome 3 Purchase fi Voice 0.728 0.19
Anger fi Voice 0.376 0.04
Regret fi Voice 0.031 0.05
Model fit: CFI = 1.00, SRMR = 0.008, RMSEA = 0.000, chi-square = 1.664 and df = 3
Notes:  p < 0.001;  p < 0.01;  p < 0.05; and ^ p < 0.10. Purchase decision: no = 0, yes = 1; outcome knowledge: no future sale = 0; future sale = 1;
SRMR = standardized root mean square residual

Effects of anger vs regret First, the path from anger to WOM and exit were constrained
These results indicate that anger and regret lead to different to be equal. The chi-square difference between this nested
behavioral outcomes. Anger may lead to exit, voice and model and unconstrained model was significant (X2 = 24.13,
negative WOM, while regret may lead to negative WOM. But p < 0.001) and therefore the null hypothesis related to the
does anger equally result in all three types of complaint equality of the two paths was rejected. This result showed the
intentions or are the strength of these effects different? path from anger to exit is statistically stronger than the path
Similarly, do anger and regret equally lead to negative WOM or from anger to WOM. Second, we constrained the path from
is the effect of one of these emotional states on negative WOM anger to exit to be equal to the path from anger to voice and
stronger? To answer these questions, we used the Wald chi- found the path from anger to exit was stronger than the path
square test, a test of chi-square difference, and results are from anger to voice (X2 = 3.46, p < 0.05). Third, we tested the
shown in Table 5. equality of the paths from anger to WOM and anger to voice.

801
Consumers procrastinate Journal of Consumer Marketing
Shabnam Azimi, George R. Milne and Elizabeth G. Miller Volume 37 · Number 7 · 2020 · 795–805

Figure 3 Process by which procrastination leads to behavioral outcomes (Study 2)

Table 5 Behavioral outcomes of regret vs anger (Study 2) voice, and lastly on negative WOM. The feeling of regret leads
2 to complaint intention in the form of negative WOM.
Null hypothesis X df p-value
Nevertheless, WOM can equally result from anger or regret.
A=B 24.132 1 0.000
B=C 3.456 1 0.063 General discussion and implications
A=C 19.237 1 0.000
A=D 1.908 1 0.167 Every day, consumers deal with a variety of choices
involving uncertainties and continuously consider trade-offs
B (SE) B (SE) to decide to purchase or delay a purchase (procrastinate).
A: Anger fiWOM 0.183 (0.04) D: Regret ! WOM 0.080^ (0.05) This paper examined the factors leading to and resulting
B: Anger fi Exit 0.461 (0.05) E: Regret ! Exit 0.084 (0.05) from consumer procrastination under high price
C: Anger fi Voice 0.376 (0.04) F: Regret ! Voice 0.031 (0.05) uncertainty. The results of this research have several
Conclusion implications for theory and practice.
Anger fi Exit > Anger fi Voice > Anger fi WOM TMT (Steel and König, 2006) as an established theory of
Anger fi WOM = Regret fi WOM motivation has been previously tested for explaining
 procrastination as a self-regulatory failure (Steel, 2007). Our
Notes: p < 0.001;  p < 0.01;  p < 0.05; ^p < 0.10
research extends the domain of applicability of TMT to the
consumer behavior context to explain consumers’ intentional
The result of the test showed the path from anger to voice is purchase delay. We applied the key elements of TMT to
stronger than the path from anger to WOM (X2 = 19.24, p < integrate established (e.g. time limit) and new (e.g. price
0.05). Lastly, as both anger and regret impacted WOM, we consciousness, prestige seeking and interactions with price
examined whether anger and regret have different impacts on uncertainty) antecedents of procrastination into a cohesive
WOM. Our results showed that WOM can equally result from framework which was tested in an experimental setting. Our
anger or regret (X2 = 1.91, p > 0.10). analysis of main and interaction effects indicated several
interesting and inter-twined paths through which situational
Discussion and personality-related factors impact consumer
Findings in Study 2 indicated that respondents who decided to procrastination. These findings have multiple novel managerial
wait for a better sale and did not get one (forgone gain) implications for planning effective procrastination termination
experience greater self-responsibility and regret but have lower strategies for different types of consumers.
intention for exit, voice or WOM compared to the people who First, our findings suggest that shortening the purchase time
had purchased the product missing out on a better future sale limit is the most powerful tool for decreasing consumers’
(perceived loss). There was no significant difference in anger procrastination as it works under different levels of price
between those in the foregone gain and perceived loss uncertainty for different types of consumers (i.e. price
conditions. Individuals who attribute the cause of the conscious or not price conscious) and products (high or low
unpleasant outcome to themselves rather than others priced). Although marketers may have no control over
experience greater regret but no greater anger. Further, anger’s consumers’ deadlines in completion of certain purchase tasks
positive effect on complaint intention is strongest on exit, then, (e.g. an upcoming birthday), they can still shorten purchase

802
Consumers procrastinate Journal of Consumer Marketing
Shabnam Azimi, George R. Milne and Elizabeth G. Miller Volume 37 · Number 7 · 2020 · 795–805

time limits and reduce consumer procrastination by putting an Limitations and future research
expiration date on a promotion or on the amount of time an
Consistent with past work (Simonson, 1992), we used a
item is available. Second, we found that lowering price
scenario-based approach over field experiments to study
uncertainty can be another effective tool in reducing consumer
procrastination because it enabled us to have more control
procrastination but only when the time limit is shorter (no
over the conditions and measure the behavioral intention
effect in longer time limits). Marketers can reduce consumers’
more accurately. Despite these merits, future research is
perceived price uncertainty through more transparent
needed to test and validate these models in natural field
communication of their price changes, use of price alerts or
settings using nonstudents. In addition, we acknowledge
providing purchase advice (e.g. “75% Confidence Buy now”).
the limitation of using a one-item scale to measure
Examples of these techniques that are already in-use in the
consumer procrastination and suggest future research
market are Kayak’s price forecast in travel booking category or
focus on developing and validating multi-item scales to
buy/sell advice by Robinhood mobile application in stock
exchange. Third, our findings related to consumer measure this construct. Further, future research should
characteristics suggest that both price conscious and prestige- also seek to replicate our results regarding sensitivity to
seeker consumers have greater procrastination tendencies and delay using more direct measures, rather than indicators
above strategies can be targeted more strongly toward them. such as price consciousness and prestige seeking,
Our finding related to the sensitivity of prestige seekers to particularly as some of our findings regarding these
deadlines suggests a more specific strategy. For luxury items constructs were unexpected.
that are available for a limited amount of time, highlighting Past research indicates that those individuals who have a
product differentiation and providing assurance about prices greater tendency to delay their decisions are more likely to
can be effective tactics in lowering prestige-seekers’ decision- waste time online which in turn results in making greater
making time. purchase decisions (Zanjani et al., 2016). Examining the
Study 2 magnifies the importance of procrastination role of this decisional procrastination in explaining
termination strategies in protecting the company from procrastination behaviors using TMT, especially for the
consumer loss. Although past research has found that negative online context, would be another area for future research.
outcomes resulting from actions are stronger than the Finally, even though it was not hypothesized, this paper
outcomes resulting from inactions (Kahneman and Tversky, found a significant interaction between uncertainty and time
1982), our research found no significant difference in anger limit which does not fit the TMT model in its current form.
between action and inaction conditions but found evidence for Scrutinizing this interaction under different boundary
greater regret in inaction condition. Our findings further conditions for the possibility of expanding TMT or
suggest that individuals who experience negative outcome advancing a new procrastination model could be an
conditions resulting from procrastination may perceive greater interesting avenue to pursue.
self-responsibility which in turn results in greater regret. Regret
itself is positively related to spread of negative WOM and its
References
impact on WOM is no different from the impact of anger
resulting from actions. Akerlof, G. (1991), “Procrastination and obedience”, American
Impact of WOM resulting from procrastination and regret Economic Review, Vol. 98 No. 2, pp. 1-19.
can be more harmful than impact of voice resulting from Anderson, E. and Duncan, I.S. (2008), “Price stickiness and
purchase and anger for two reasons. First, consumer voice is a customer antagonism”, Quarterly Journal of Economics,
useful source of information that helps marketers identify the Vol. 125 No. 2, pp. 729-765.
reasons for dissatisfaction (Plymire, 1991), which mostly Ariely, D. and Wertenbroch, K. (2002), “Procrastination,
happens in more private contexts compared to WOM. Second, deadline, and performance: self-control by
past research indicates that those consumers who have a chance precommitment”, Psychological Science, Vol. 13 No. 3,
to complain to marketers will release negative emotions, will pp. 219-224.
ultimately be less dissatisfied and will be less likely to engage in Averill, J.R. (1983), “Studies on anger and aggression:
subsequent spread of negative WOM (Nyer, 2000). Previous implications for theories of emotion”, American Psychologist,
research shows that people experience regret when they feel Vol. 38 No. 11, pp. 1145-1160.
they could make a better decision and they take this experience Baron, J. and Ritov, I. (1994), “Reference points and omission
into account in their future choices (Loomes and Sugden, bias”, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes,
1982). Consistent with this position, regret resulting from Vol. 59 No. 3, pp. 475-498.
consumer procrastination can be a source of future change in Bell, D.E. (1985), “Disappointment in decision making under
behavior, and in the long run, can be self-correcting. uncertainty”, Operations Research, Vol. 33 No. 1, pp. 1-27.
Nevertheless, we found that consumer procrastination can Bernstein, P. (1998), Against the Gods: The Remarkable Story of
directly lead to anger, bypassing the perception of self- Risk, Wiley, New York, NY.
responsibility, which then may result in retaliatory behaviors Bonifıeld, C. and Cole, C. (2007), “Affective responses to
toward the company such as voice and exit. This unexpected service failure: anger, regret, and retaliatory versus
finding sheds light on the silent path from procrastination to conciliatory responses”, Marketing Letters, Vol. 18 Nos 1/2,
retaliation and highlights the importance of possible remedies pp. 85-99. Nos
or interventions by the companies to mitigate consumer Choi, J.N. and Moran, S.V. (2009), “Why not procrastinate?
emotions under procrastination conditions. Development and validation of a new active procrastination

803
Consumers procrastinate Journal of Consumer Marketing
Shabnam Azimi, George R. Milne and Elizabeth G. Miller Volume 37 · Number 7 · 2020 · 795–805

scale”, The Journal of Social Psychology, Vol. 149 No. 2, Singh, J. (1990), “Voice, exit, and negative word-of-mouth
pp. 195-211. behaviors: an investigation across three service categories”,
Chu, A.H.C. and Choi, J.N. (2005), “Rethinking Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Vol. 18 No. 1,
procrastination: positive effects of active procrastination pp. 1-15.
behavior on attitudes and performance”, The Journal of Social Smith, C.A. and Ellsworth, P.C. (1985), “Patterns of cognitive
Psychology, Vol. 145 No. 3, pp. 245-264. appraisal in emotion”, Journal of Personality and Social
Ferrari, J.R. (1993), “Christmas and procrastination: Psychology, Vol. 48 No. 4, pp. 813-838.
explaining lack of diligence at a ‘real-world’ task deadline”, Steel, P. (2007), “The nature of procrastination: a meta-
Personality and Individual Differences, Vol. 14 No. 1, analytic and theoretical review of quintessential self-
pp. 25-33. regulatory failure”, Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 133 No. 1,
Folkes, V.S., Koletsky, S. and Graham, J.L. (1987), “A field pp. 65-94.
study of causal inferences and consumer reaction: the view Steel, P. and König, C.J. (2006), “Integrating theories of
from the airport”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 13 motivation”, Academy of Management Review, Vol. 31 No. 4,
No. 4, pp. 534-539. pp. 889-913.
Greenleaf, E.A. and Lehmann, D.R. (1995), “Reasons for Tsiros, M. and Mittal, V. (2000), “Regret: a model of its
substantial delay in consumer decision making”, Journal of antecedents and consequences in consumer decision
Consumer Research, Vol. 22 No. 2, pp. 186-199. making”, Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 26 No. 4,
Hirschman, A.O. (1970), Exit, Voice, and Loyalty; Responses to pp. 401-417.
Vigneron, F. and Johnson, L. (1999), “A review and a
Decline in Firms, Organizations, and States, Cambridge,
conceptual framework of prestige-seeking consumer
Mass.: Harvard University Press.
behavior”, Academy of Marketing Science Review, Vol. 2
Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A. (1979), “Prospect theory: an
No. 1, pp. 1-15.
analysis of decision under risk”, Econometrica, Vol. 47 No. 2,
Zanjani, S.H.A., Milne, G.R. and Miller, E.G. (2016),
pp. 263-292.
“Procrastinators’ online experience and purchase behavior”,
Kahneman, D. and Tversky, A. (1982), “The psychology of
Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Vol. 44 No. 5,
preferences”, Scientific American, Vol. 246 No. 1,
pp. 568-585.
pp. 160-173. Zeelenberg, M. and Pieters, R. (1999), “Comparing service
Lichtenstein, D.R., Ridgway, N.M. and Netemeyer, R.G. delivery to what might have been: behavioral responses to
(1993), “Price perceptions and consumer shopping regret and disappointment”, Journal of Service Research,
behavior: a field study”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 2 No. 1, pp. 86-97.
Vol. 30 No. 2, pp. 234-245.
Loomes, G. and Sugden, R. (1982), “Regret theory: an
Appendix 1
alternative theory of rational choice under uncertainty”, The
Economic Journal, Vol. 92 No. 368, pp. 805-824. Study 1 scenarios
Mehta, N., Rajiv, S. and Srinivasan, K. (2003), “Price Imagine that it is your friend’s wedding in two weeks
uncertainty and consumer search: a structural model of (two months) and you need to buy a new outfit/suit for
consideration set formation”, Marketing Science, Vol. 22 yourself to wear. You particularly like Store B. There is a Store
No. 1, pp. 58-84. B about 10 min drive from the place you live and you can
Nakamura, E. and Steinsson, J. (2008), “Five facts about easily get there.
prices: a reevaluation of menu cost models”, Quarterly Imagine you have gone to the mall today to check out a
Journal of Economics, Vol. 123 No. 4, pp. 1415-1464, doi: Store B that just opened there. Before going to the store, you
10.1162/qjec.2008.123.4.1415. have made a plan of the type of outfit/suit you want to
Nyer, P. (2000), “An investigation into whether complaining purchase and you exactly know what you are looking for.
can cause increased consumer satisfaction”, Journal of Arriving at the Store B, you find an outfit/a suit that is
Consumer Marketing, Vol. 17 No. 1, pp. 9-19. appropriate for the wedding. The outfit/suit is on regular price
Plymire, J. (1991), “Complaints as opportunities”, Journal of (is NOT on sale).
Consumer Marketing, Vol. 8 No. 2, pp. 79-81. You wonder whether to make your purchase now or wait to
Russell, D. (1982), “The causal dimension scale: a see if this item goes on sale. Remember that the wedding is in
measure of how individuals perceive causes”, Journal of two weeks (two months) and there is a branch of Store B about
Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 42 No. 6, 10 min from the place you live such that you can easily get
there whenever you want.
pp. 1137-1145.
The salesperson in the store tells you the outfit/suit you
Shu, S.B. and Gneezy, A. (2010), “Procrastination of enjoyable
liked will NOT go on sale in future. (The salesperson in the
experiences”, Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 47 No. 5,
store tells you she does not know whether the outfit/suit you
pp. 933-944.
liked will go on sale in future.)
Simonson, I. (1992), “The influence of anticipating regret and
responsibility on purchase decisions”, Journal of Consumer
Research, Vol. 19 No. 1, pp. 105-518. Study 2 scenarios
Singh, J. (1988), “Consumer complaint intentions and Imagine that two weeks ago you visited Store A and found a
behavior: definitional and taxonomical issues”, Journal of shirt/laptop you liked on sale for 40% (30%) off the original
Marketing, Vol. 52 No. 1, pp. 93-107. price. It was very close to what you planned to purchase.

804
Consumers procrastinate Journal of Consumer Marketing
Shabnam Azimi, George R. Milne and Elizabeth G. Miller Volume 37 · Number 7 · 2020 · 795–805

However, you were not sure whether to purchase it or wait for  The cause is outside of me 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 The cause is
a better sale, especially as the store does not allow returns. inside of me
Ultimately, you made your decision and did NOT purchased  The cause is something about others 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 The
(purchased) the shirt/laptop. cause is something about me
Now two weeks later, you visit the same store and realize Regret (Bonifıeld and Cole, 2007):
that the same shirt/laptop is no longer on sale (the price of the (1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree)
same shirt/laptop got further reduced as it is now 70% off the  I would regret (not) purchasing the product two weeks
original price). before when it was 30% off.
 I would feel bad about (not) purchasing the product two
Appendix 2 weeks before when it was 30% off.
Measures  In retrospect, I would feel that I could (not) have found a
Price consciousness (Lichtenstein et al., 1993): better deal. (This item was not used in the analysis)
(1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree)  I would feel sorry for (not) purchasing the product two
 I am not willing to go to extra effort to find lower prices. weeks before when it was 30% off.
 I will grocery shop at more than one store to take  I would feel that if I could do it all over, I would not
advantage of low prices. (would) purchase the product when it was 30% off.
 The money saved by finding low prices is usually not Anger (Bonifıeld and Cole, 2007):
worth the time and effort. (1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree)
 I would never shop at more than one store to find low  I would feel angry about my experience at this store.
prices.  I would feel very displeased with my experience at this
 The time it takes to find low prices is usually not worth the store.
effort.  The more I think about it, the more hostile I would feel
 toward the store.
Reverse coded
Prestige seeking (Lichtenstein et al., 1993): Complaint intention (Singh, 1990):
(1 = strongly disagree and 5 = strongly agree) (1 = very unlikely and 5 = very likely)
 People notice when you buy the most expensive brand of a  I forget about the incident and do nothing?
product.  Definitely complain to the store manager on your next
 Buying a high-priced brand makes me feel good about trip? (Voice)
myself.  Go back or call the Store A immediately and ask them to
 Buying the most expensive brand of a product makes me take care of your problem? (Voice)
feel classy.  Complain to a consumer agency and ask them to make the
 I enjoy the prestige of buying a high-priced brand. Store A take care of your problem? (Voice)
 It says something to people when you buy the high-priced  Report to the consumer agency so that they can warn
version of a product. other consumers? (Voice)
 Your friends will think you are cheap if you consistently  Take some legal action against the Store A. (Voice)
buy the lowest priced version of a product.  Decide not to purchase from Store A again? (Exit)
 I have purchased the most expensive brand of a product  Speak to your friends and relatives about your experience?
just because I knew other people would notice. (WOM)
 I think others make judgments about me by the kinds of  Convince your friends and relatives not to purchase from
products and brands I buy. Store A? (WOM)
 Even for a relatively inexpensive product, I think that  Write an online comment in social networking sites about
buying a costly brand is impressive. your experience? (WOM)
Locus of causality (Russell, 1982):
 The cause reflects an aspect of situation 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Corresponding author
The cause reflects an aspect of myself Shabnam Azimi can be contacted at: sazimi@luc.edu

For instructions on how to order reprints of this article, please visit our website:
www.emeraldgrouppublishing.com/licensing/reprints.htm
Or contact us for further details: permissions@emeraldinsight.com

805

You might also like