You are on page 1of 134

THE EFFECTS OF SUBSTITUTING RETELLING FOR BASAL READER

COMPREHENSION INSTRUCTION ON STUDENTS IN A SIXTH GRADE

READING PROGRAM

by

Donna M. Johnson

B.A., Southwest Minnesota State University, 1975

M.S., Minnesota State University, Mankato, 1984

A Dissertation Submitted in Parti a! Fulfillment of


The Requirements for the Degree of
Doctor of Education

Division of Curriculum and Instruction


In the Graduate School
The University of South Dakota
December 2008
UMI Number: 3351192

Copyright 2008 by
Johnson, Donna M.

All rights reserved.

INFORMATION TO USERS

The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy
submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and
photographs, print bleed-through, substandard margins, and improper
alignment can adversely affect reproduction.
In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript
and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized
copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion.

UMI
UMI Microform 3351192
Copyright 2009 by ProQuest LLC.
All rights reserved. This microform edition is protected against
unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code.

ProQuest LLC
789 E. Eisenhower Parkway
PO Box 1346
Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346
©2008

Donna M. Johnson

ALL RIGHTS RESERVED


ABSTRACT

Donna M. Johnson, Ed.D., Curriculum and Instruction,


The University of South Dakota, 2008

The Effects of Substituting Retelling for Basal Reader Comprehension Instruction


on Students in a Sixth Grade Reading Program

Dissertation directed by Dr. Maurine V. Richardson

Teachers often assume that if a student is able to pronounce the words on a page,

comprehension of the text will follow automatically. This is not always the case, and

teachers struggle to find ways to effectively "teach" comprehension. Educational research

done in the 1980s and 1990s supported teaching strategies to help students actively

increase their own reading comprehension capabilities. Retelling was one of the

comprehension-fostering instructional strategies examined and recommended. Even

though the benefits of retelling were well-supported, its use in classrooms either did not

become established or has waned in recent years.

This experimental study was designed to examine the value and ease of use of

retelling in a contemporary classroom setting. Students in two intact sixth grade reading

groups, one made up of proficient readers and the other of less-proficient readers,

participated in the study.

The purpose of the study was twofold. First, it endeavored to determine if replacing

existing group comprehension instruction and individual comprehension workbook

assignments with retelling instruction and oral and written retelling practice would

significantly impact the reading comprehension achievement of middle school students,

and second, it sought to discover how retelling would affect the reading comprehension

in
achievement of students at different levels of reading capability (proficient and less-

proficient).

The retelling intervention lasted for five weeks with each group of students. Pre- and

post-test reading comprehension achievement data were collected using three

instruments: a standardized reading test, a curriculum-based measure, and a scale

evaluating the quality of written retellings. An analysis of the data revealed that retelling

was indeed an effective reading comprehension strategy. While statistical significance

was not reached on all measures used, effect sizes reflected the practical significance of

the retelling strategy. Both proficient and less-proficient readers benefited from the

retelling instruction. The effects observed are particularly important because the strategy

was implemented within an existing program without additional materials, costs, or

disruptions to the classroom schedule of either the students or the teachers.

This abstract of approximately 325 words is approved as to form and content. I

recommend its publication.

Signed /Vkj^^fuJ 7SjcAdJjUtM/t-J


Professor in Charge

iv
DOCTORAL COMMITTEE

The members of the committee appointed to examine the dissertation of Donna M.

Johnson find it satisfactory and recommend that it be approved.

Dr. Maurine V. RichaWson, Chair

Dr. Susan C. Gapp

rAris! M. Reed
Dr/tCristine

C,,.
i^x-^ t - V i / v ^ ^ ^^Mvk^y
Dr. Lisa A. Newland

v
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I thank my advisor, Dr. Maurine Richardson, for her assistance and encouragement

during the completion of this project. Throughout my program of study, she has always

done just the right thing at the right time to help me accomplish my goals.

The other members of my dissertation committee, Dr. Susan Gapp, Dr. Kristine

Reed, and Dr. Lisa Newland, have each mentored and supported me in class and during

my research and writing; I thank them.

My research could not have taken place without the permission and cooperation of

the school board, administration, teachers, students, and parents in the school district in

which I completed my study. I am grateful for their help.

My fellow students, Dr. Trudi Nelson, Dr. Penelope Smith, Mr. Bruce Brown, and

Mrs. Carla McMurry-Kozak, lent companionship, encouragement, and enjoyment to our

shared pursuit of advanced learning.

Dr. Linda Reetz and Mr. Gordon Reetz provided hospitality and friendship I greatly

appreciate.

My daughters, Mariel, Bethany, and Elsa, have encouraged me from their distant

locations at other schools of higher education. My husband, Chris, has been patiently

supporting me at home; he will welcome the repossession of his computer and office and

the increased levels of companionship and attention that will ensue with the completion

of this project.

VI
TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page

Abstract iii

Doctoral Committee v

Acknowledgements , vi

List of Tables xi

Chapter

1. Introduction 1

Background of the Problem 3

Statement of the Problem 6

Purpose of the Study 6

Significance of the Study 7

Definition of Terms 9

Limitations of the Study 10

Delimitations of the Study 11

Organization of the Remainder of the Study 11

2. Review of Related Literature and Research 12

Introduction 12

Foundational Studies 13

Cognitive Psychology and the Development of

Reading Comprehension Models 14

Reading Comprehension Research in Educational Settings 17

vii
Retelling in Educational Settings 22

Using Retelling with Struggling Readers 32

Retelling in a Contemporary Middle School Setting 35

Summary 38

Organization of the Remainder of the Study 39

3. Methodology 40

Review of Related Literature 41

Population and Setting 42

Instrumentation 43

Standardized Pre-Post Test 43

Instructional Materials 44

Curriculum-Based Assessment 45

Written Retelling Assessment 46

Pilot Study 48

Intervention and Data Collection 49

Data Analysis , 51

Summary 53

4. Results... , 54

Introduction ...., 54

Research Questions 54

Demographic Data 55

Findings 56

vni
Questions One and Two 56

Questions Three and Four „ 59

Question Five 61

Summary 62

5. Summary 63

Purpose of the Study 63

Research Questions 63

Review of Related Literature and Research 64

Methodology and Procedures 66

Research Findings 67

Conclusions 68

Discussion 69

Recommendations 73

Summary 75

References 76

Appendices

A. USD Office of Human Subjects Protection Exempt


Review and Approval 91
B. Communication with School District: Presentation to School Board &
Permission to Conduct Study 93

C. USD Office of Human Subjects Protection Documentation:


Request for Parental Consent (English & Spanish) 96

D. USD Office of Human Subjects Protection Documentation:


Request for Student Assent 101

IX
E. Pro-Ed Inc. Test Use Permission/Agreement 103

F. Scott Foresman Leveled Readers Unit 5 105

G. Scott Foresman Leveled Readers Unit 6 107

H. Trade Books Used ., 109

I. Scott Foresman Unit Skills Test Validity/Reliability Information 110

J. Irwin-Mitchell Richness in Retelling Scale:


Permission to Use, Criteria, & Checklist 114

K. Oral Retelling Procedures 118

L. Written Retelling Procedures 120

x
LIST OF TABLES

Table Number Page

1. Reading Comprehension Pre, Post, and Change Scores


of Two Groups of Readers 57

2. Comparison of Pre/Post Scores on Two Comprehension


Tests for Two Groups of Readers 58

3. Comparison of Comprehension Change Scores of Control


and Experimental Groups , 59

4. Reading Comprehension Change Scores of Proficient and Less-


Proficient Retellers 60

5. Comparison of Comprehension Change Scores of Proficient and Less-

Proficient Readers 60

6. Means and Standard Deviations of Written Retelling Change Scores 61

7. Change Scores on Written Retellings 62

XI
1

CHAPTER 1

Introduction to the Study

Every day, children talk with each other about friends and family, experiences at

home and school, games and sports, and other daily events (Brown & Cambourne, 1987;

Goodman, 1982; Van Dongen, 1987). They use their own words to relate what they have

seen, heard, and experienced; in other words, they retell (Kalmbach, 1980; Martin, 1972).

Children are skilled at understanding and producing complex narratives (Paris & Paris,

2003). Both children and adults who enjoy reading stories often retell these to each other,

sometimes casually and other times more carefully (Caldwell & Leslie, 2005). Without

taking particular notice, most people retell to themselves in their minds when they review

their schedule for the day or continue a project at work or pick up the mystery novel on

the bed stand (Caldwell & Leslie, 2005; Rosen, 1986). Retelling, according to Britton

(1971) both depends upon and enhances memory:

Going over past events in our minds must occupy us for a great deal of our spare

time 'Memory' as we usually think of it, takes a narrative form. It may well be

that the stage at which narrative speech becomes possible to a child is the point at

which memory in this sense begins, (p. 71)

The ability to narrate a story, which is essential to memory and cognitive

development, is also believed to be critical for early literacy (Paris & Paris, 2003).

Engagement by children in the story-telling process enhances interest in and attention to

reading and writing. A child's use of personal narrative language and thought leads to

competence in these literary tasks (Van Dongen, 1987).


2
When teachers and researchers ask students to retell, they are asking for "as much of

the information in the text as possible" (Moss, 2004, p. 711). In response, children do not

recite back word for word what they have read or heard, nor do they articulate a neat,

organized summary. Rather, they elaborate, expand, and emphasize the bits most

important to them; when they repeat a text, they also change it and make it personal and

original (Rosen, 1986). Students choose what they consider to be the main points and

generate a retelling, which is then itself a newly-created story (Kalmbach, 1986a; Kintsch

& van Dijk, 1983; Rumelhart, 1975b; Steinruck, 1978).

Spoken or written retellings have been used for more than 80 years to "gather data in

an amazing range of language based inquiries" (Kalmbach, 1986a, p. 327), much of it

related to memory and comprehension. In 1926, Jean Piaget used retellings to investigate

children's ideas about time. In his classic 1932 study on the formation of memory, Sir

Frederic Bartlett asked participants to study a picture or story and then either draw or tell

it later. Bartlett found that memory is constructive, interpretative, and dependent upon

what he termed schemata: personal, conceptual memory patterns based on past

experiences.

Schema theory and other cognitive psychological concepts and processes related to

reading were greatly neglected during a half century of interest in behaviorism (Wittrock,

1981; Pearson, 1985). In the late 1950s and early 1960s, interest in cognitive psychology

re-emerged (Anderson, Wang, & Gaffney, 2006; Wittrock, 1981), and research focusing

on reading comprehension, verbal processes, imagery, perception, and memory became

prominent once again.


3
In 1967, O'Donnell, Griffin, and Norris asked elementary school children to watch

two silent cartoons and then narrate or write their retellings. In the 1970s and 1980s,

retellings were used to study the influence of story grammar on comprehension (Mandler

& Johnson, 1977; Stein & Glenn, 1979) and investigate and understand reader miscues

(Goodman, 1982; Goodman & Burke, 1972). In 1980 (Chafe), people from cultures

across the globe viewed a film of an elderly man picking pears and then narrated orally

what they had seen and heard; these retellings were analyzed to determine relationships

among culture, cognition, and language. Kintsch and van Dijk (1983) based their model

of text comprehension on data collected from retellings.

Background of the Problem

Both in research and in the classroom, retelling has most often been used as an

assessment of reading comprehension (Gambrell, Koskinen, & Kapinus, 1991; Gambrell,

Pfeiffer, & Wilson, 1985; Golden & Pappas, 1978; Morrow, 1985a). As evidence for the

value of retelling increased in the 1980s, researchers urged that it be used as an

instructional strategy and suggested ideas for practical classroom applications (Koskinen,

Gambrell, Kapinus, & Heathington, 1988). Teachers, however, continued to "assume that

retellings belong in the laboratory and not in the classroom" (Kalmbach, 1986a, p. 327);

in 1991, Gambrell et al. noted that the number of studies investigating retelling as an

instructional strategy were quite limited. Five years later, in 1996, Morrow emphasized

the importance of practicing retelling and suggested a variety of formats for classroom

retellings.

Retelling strategies have been used to build the reading comprehension ability of

delayed and struggling readers (Gillam & Carlile, 1997; Hansen, 1978; Houge, 2000;
4
Kuldanek, 1998; Rhodes & Milby, 2007; Wood & Jones, 1998) and second language

learners (Hu, 1995). In 1997 and 2004, Moss recommended using retelling with

expository as well as narrative texts. Retelling has been an assessment and

comprehension strategy recommended by educational organizations in the states of

Louisiana, Oregon, and Hawaii (Benson & Cummins, 2000; Early Literacy and

Assessment for Learning Series, 2005; Oregon State Department of Education,

1998/1999). Read and Retell: A Strategy for the Whole Language/Natural Learning

Classroom, written by Brown and Cambourne (1987), was one of the first retelling

resources to be widely used by classroom teachers; Brown's students created many

written texts. Other teacher resources, e.g. The Power of Retelling (Benson & Cummins,

2000) and Retelling Strategies to Improve Comprehension (Shaw, 2005) have since

become available.

The National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) has been used to assess

the academic achievement of American students since 1969. Reading scores for students

in grades 4, 8, and 12 have shown slight gains and losses over the years, but for the most

part, have "yielded a flat trend" (Taylor, Pearson, Garcia, Stahl, & Bauer, 2006). During

these same years, and in spite of what has been learned about developing children's

reading comprehension, there has been a consistent lack of comprehension instruction in

elementary classrooms (Durkin, 1978/1979; Pressley, Wharton-McDonald, Mistretta-

Hampston, & Echevarria, 1998; Taylor et al., 2006). New models of reading

comprehension instruction (including retelling) built on schema theory and the findings

of cognitive psychologists have not become established, due in part to political pressures

(Sarroub & Pearson, 1998). Both conservative and liberal groups objected to new forms
5
of instruction and assessment for a variety of reasons, including "utility issues (the new

approaches took too long and were too expensive and difficult to score), and equity

concerns (will minority students do any better or will they, in fact, do worse . . .?)"

(Sarroub & Pearson, p. 102).

Teachers also found the new comprehension strategy instruction difficult to

understand and teach (Hilden & Pressley, 2007). Many viewed retelling as difficult,

cumbersome, and time-consuming (Benson & Cummins, 2000; Gambrell et al., 1985;

Morrow, 1986); others felt they did not have the training or experience to teach their

students to retell (Benson & Cummins, 2000; Shaw, 2005). Retelling, in spite of its

recognized worth, has been and continues to be an infrequently used strategy for

improving reading comprehension (Caldwell & Leslie, 2005; D'Allessio, 1996; Gambrell

et al., 1985; Gambrell et al, 1991; Morrow, 1986; Morrow, 1996; Morrow et al., 1986;

Morrow, Tracey, Wood, & Pressley, 1999; Thompson, 1990). As is sometimes the case,

there is a contrast between what reading research has verified and how this knowledge

has been applied in classrooms.

More than twenty years have elapsed since American literacy researchers first

recommended the use of retelling as an instructional strategy to enhance students' reading

comprehension, but it is not currently receiving the attention it should by researchers or

educators (J, N. Mitchell, personal communication, February 4, 2008; Tucker, 2001).

While research is ongoing and change is constant in the field of education, the value of

retelling has never been discounted or denied. This study, therefore, re-examined the

effectiveness, usefulness, and practicality of retelling in a contemporary, conventional

classroom setting.
6
Statement of the Problem

This study examined the effects of using oral and written retelling as a reading

comprehension instructional strategy on the reading achievement of two groups of

students in the sixth grade.

Purpose of the Study

The purpose of the study was twofold. First, it endeavored to determine if replacing

existing group comprehension instruction and individual comprehension workbook

assignments with retelling instruction and oral and written retelling practice would

significantly impact the reading comprehension achievement of middle school students,

and second, it sought to discover whether the reading comprehension achievement of

students at different levels of reading capability (proficient and less-proficient) would be

affected equally. Specifically, the study answered the following questions:

1. Is there a difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a

standardized reading test between students who engage in retelling and students who do

not?

2. Is there a difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a

curriculum-based unit test between students who engage in retelling and students who do

not?

3. Is there a difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a

standardized reading test between proficient and less-proficient readers who engage in

retelling?
7

4. Is there a difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a

curriculum-based unit test between proficient and less-proficient readers who engage in

retelling?

5. Is there a gain in reading achievement as measured by a comparison of the pre-

and post-assessments of written retellings of students who engage in retelling?

Significance of the Study

Retelling has been the subject of many studies. In the past, concerns have been raised

about the design and methodology of retelling research. Golden and Pappas (1990)

examined 81 retelling studies conducted between 1977 and 1986 and identified areas of

concern. They raised questions about the texts used in the studies, the varying

expectations of the researchers and the participants, and the procedures used to analyze

oral and written retellings.

This study included procedures that took into account the recommendations of

Golden and Pappas: all texts used were authentic and unaltered, participants most often

retold to naive listeners (other students who had not read the same text or portion of text),

and written retellings were analyzed by two raters using a valid, well-established retelling

scale. By addressing these issues, this study avoided some of the pitfalls of earlier

retelling research.

Most often, when retelling has been part of a study, it has been used to assess reading

comprehension (e.g. Goodman, 1982; John, Lui, & Tannock, 2003; O'Donnell, Griffin,

& Norris, 1967; Zimilies & Kuhns, 1976). Students with no experience retelling have

been asked to read and then tell or write everything they can recall. This is "not an easy

procedure for students, no matter what their ages" (Morrow, 1988, p. 129). It was while
8
using retelling to assess comprehension that researchers in the 1980s began to consider its

value as an instructional strategy (Morrow, 1988). In this study, retelling was a

comprehension-fostering strategy used every school day for a five-week intervention

period. Because retelling was compared directly to the use of workbook page completion

as comprehension instruction, it will interest reading teachers who wish to use strategies

that have been examined empirically.

Many previous examinations of retelling have taken place in lower and middle grade

elementary classrooms, or have examined retells of stories read to young pre-readers.

This study will add information about reading comprehension instruction that will be of

interest to teachers of middle school students, both students considered to be proficient

readers and those who struggle. The ability to comprehend what they read is of crucial

importance to students as they enter high school and take on the study of content area

subjects at advanced levels.

Finally, this study was designed to focus on practical as well as statistical

significance. "The practical significance of any intervention is dramatically improved if it

can be implemented under conditions approaching those of the normal classroom"

(Palincsar & Brown, 1984, p. 157). The study took place in a school setting with intact

groups of students, followed existing class schedules, and used regular curriculum

materials. The current study is unique in its emphasis on applying retelling to classroom

group instruction rather than in a more typical one-on-one researcher-to-student

arrangement. Reading teachers and curriculum directors will be interested in knowing

about the effectiveness of a strategy that can easily be incorporated into existing

classrooms and reading programs.


9
Definition of Terms

The following definitions will be used in the study. In cases in which the definition

does not contain a citation, the term was defined by the researcher.

Comprehension: Comprehension is the active construction of meaning derived from

oral or written language. It is a complex process of reasoning which involves evaluation,

problem solving, and the development, elaboration, and integration of prior knowledge

(Johnston, 1983).

Retelling: A retelling is a post reading or post listening recall in which the reader or

listener retells in their own words what they remember from reading or listening to a text.

Retelling can be oral or written, and it can be used to assess or develop comprehension

(Morrow, 1996 p. 267; Morrow, 2005, p. 396).

Written retelling: A strategy that reflects a holistic concept in which children are

actively involved as they reconstruct a written retelling of the story they have read or

heard (Brown and Cambourne, 1987).

Guided retelling: Teacher use of interventions in the form of prompt questions.

These prompts are used when children have a difficult time moving through retellings on

their own (Glazer and Brown, 1993, p. 116).

Unguided retelling: Students retell without intervention from teachers, peers, or

other listeners (Glazer & Brown, 1993, p. 116).

Proficient reader: A proficient reader is able to comprehend the meaning of text at a

reading level equal to his age and/or grade.

Less-proficient reader: A less-proficient reader struggles to comprehend the meaning

of text at a reading level equal to his age and/or grade.


10
Story grammar: A description of the elements typically found in stories: setting,

characters, theme (problem), plot episodes (key events), and resolution (Marshall, 1983).

Limitations of the Study

This study has the following limitations:

1. The assignment (and the criteria used to determine the assignment) of the

participants to either the proficient or the less-proficient reading group was made by the

classroom teachers prior to the onset of the study. While the researcher did not participate

in this decision-making process, the assignment of students to groups appeared to be

appropriate.

2. Previously established schedules of participants in the experimental group

required their occasional absence from retelling instructional time (e.g. to attend band

lessons or to be kindergarten buddies during lunch time). When this happened, the

researcher attempted to meet with the student to make up the missed retelling activity so

that all students in the experimental group were exposed to the intervention in its entirety.

3. The classroom teachers planned all lessons and instructional activities for the

students in the control group throughout the duration of the study. The teachers were not

asked to make any changes in their usual literacy teaching procedures. To minimize

diffusion of treatment, the researcher did not consult with the classroom teachers about

either their lesson plans for the control group or her lesson plans for the experimental

group.
11

Delimitations of the Study

1. Because the participants in this study were a convenience sample of sixth-grade

students in a rural, Midwestern public school, any findings and results would most likely

apply to students of a similar age in a similar setting.

2. No students in special education were included in the study; these students

typically received their reading instruction in a resource room during the literacy

instruction hour each day. There was, however, a range of reading proficiency

represented by the study participants.

Organization of the Remainder of the Study

Chapter 1 outlined the background, purpose, and need for this study. A review of the

related literature will be provided in Chapter 2. The methods and procedures used to

implement the study and gather the data will be described in Chapter 3. The major

findings of the study will be reported in Chapter 4, along with data analyses for each of

the research questions. Chapter 5 will contain a summary of the study, major findings,

conclusions, discussion of the results, and recommendations for practice and further

study.
12
CHAPTER 2

Review of Related Literature and Research

Introduction

This study examined the effects of using oral and written retelling as a reading

comprehension instructional strategy on the reading achievement of two groups of

students in the sixth grade. The purpose of this study was, first of all, to determine if

replacing existing group comprehension instruction and individual comprehension

workbook assignments with retelling instruction and oral and written retelling practice

would significantly impact the reading comprehension achievement of middle school

students. Second, it sought to discover whether the reading comprehension achievement

of students at different levels of reading capability (proficient and less-proficient) would

be affected equally.

Five interrelated bodies of research informed and guided the study. The first of these

encompassed a small number of studies that took place in the early years of the 20th

century and established a foundation for later reading research. Three additional

complementary fields of reading research germane to the study developed concurrently in

the mid-to-latter years of the 20th century and were therefore reviewed. Finally, literature

relevant to the procedures and methodology of the study were examined.

This review of literature will therefore provide a discussion of: (1) early studies

foundational to research on memory and reading comprehension, (2) reading

comprehension models developed by cognitive psychologists, (3) reading comprehension

research in instructional settings, (4) retelling research in educational settings with both
13
proficient and struggling readers, and (5) issues related to investigating retelling in a

contemporary middle school setting.

Foundational Studies

The Psychology and Pedagogy of Reading, Huey's seminal work, was published in

1908; his insights into the reading process and the teaching of reading have stood the test

of time and are valued and respected a century later. In his studies, Huey examined the

physiological aspects of reading, particularly eye movements and eye fixations, but he

also studied the psychological processes involved in reading. Huey recognized that

comprehension is a holistic process that integrates information from the text with the

reader's previous experiences. He recommended that parents and teachers provide

children with excellent literature from the library rather than depend on readers published

as school textbooks. Huey observed children selecting favorite books and retelling the

stories to themselves.

Psychologist and educator E. L. Thorndike first studied animal behavior and then

became interested in human learning and psychology. In addition to developing a

behavioral theory which he called connectionism (learning defined as the formation of a

connection between stimulus and response), Thorndike was one of the first researchers to

understand the significance of the potential link between the fields of education and

cognitive psychology (Sears, 2007). In 1917, Thorndike wrote that reading depends on

three things: the meaning of the individual words, the meaning of the words in context,

and the reader's integration of the ideas of the text with prior knowledge and current

reading purpose. Reading is more than a "mechanical, passive, undiscriminating task, on

a totally different level from the task of evaluating or using what is read. . . . It is not a
14
small or unworthy task to learn 'what the book says'" (p. 332). Thorndike's definition of

"reading as reasoning" (p. 323) meant that reading could no longer be considered just the

pronunciation of letters and words; literacy education and the practices of literacy

instruction were irrevocably altered.

Bartlett was another of the forerunners in the field of cognitive psychology. The

conclusions he reached in his classic 1932 work on memory at the University of

Cambridge differed from accepted theories of the time (Stein & Glenn, 1979). Bartlett

asked his subjects to study a story or a picture and then later either reproduce or retell it.

Rather than recall a story exactly, participants blended, omitted, transformed, and

reconstructed the details within their impression of the story as a whole. Bartlett found

that memory is constructive, interpretative, and dependent upon what he termed

schemata: personal, conceptual memory patterns based on past experiences.

Cognitive Psychology and the Development of Reading Comprehension Models

For approximately 50 years, from about 1920 to 1970, educational psychology was

largely dominated by the behaviorist movement, which was more concerned with

external behavior than with what was happening in the mind to bring about the behavior

(Anderson, 1995; Sarroub & Pearson, 1998). Little research related to the cognitive

processes of reading was undertaken during this time (Wittrock, 1981). Modern cognitive

psychology emerged in the 1950s along with a renewed interest in linguistics and

research in computer science and artificial intelligence. By the mid-1950s, psychologists

were again studying and theorizing about reading and the reading comprehension process

(Pearson, 1985).
15
It is difficult to measure or define reading comprehension; the comprehension

process must always be mediated by a product (Johnston, 1983; Rhodes & Shanklin,

1993), but it is in fact a complex process that involves reasoning, problem solving, and

monitoring and correction strategies. "When we comprehend something, we know that

we have understood, but not how we managed to do so" (Mandler, 1984, p. 33). Several

cognitive psychologists have developed theories to explain this invisible process.

Rumelhart (1975a) revived the idea of schema that originated with Bartlett (1932)

and Piaget (1974) as the basis for his general theory of comprehension. Rumelhart (1984)

proposed "to develop the concept of a schema in its modern reincarnation to show why

this idea has seemed powerful, and to show how a schema theory can help make sense of

a number of cognitive processes" (p. 163) including comprehending, organizing, and

recalling story structure.

Other researchers followed Rumelhart and used his work as a base to further study

"story grammar," the term developed to refer to the key parts of a story. A story grammar

consists of the elements typically found in traditional folk stories and other stories:

setting (including characters), an initiating event, a series of episodes directed to

achieving a goal, the reactions of the characters to the events, and a final resolution

(Johnson & Mandler, 1980; Marshall, 1983). Because they possess a standard internal

structure, traditional stories were used for psychological study to evaluate the presence of

story schema in the minds of children (Mandler, 1984; Whaley, 1981). Story grammar

studies assumed that "comprehension is organized and that the closer the reader's

organization is to that of the text, the greater comprehension is likely to be" (Marshall, p.

517). Comprehension studies based on story grammar made universal use of retelling -
16
generally oral, but occasionally written - to collect data from participants (e.g.

Nezworski, Stein, & Trabasso, 1982; Pappas & Brown, 1987).

Another theory of the reading comprehension process, Wittrock's model of

generative learning, was proposed in 1974. According to this model, readers use complex

psychological processes to construct ("generate") meaning for printed texts. Three of the

several fundamental processes of reading comprehension are attention, encoding, and

memory. Good readers, in order to comprehend a text, attend to its meaning rather than to

surface characteristics; they also bring relevant background knowledge and past

experiences to the reading comprehension task. Underlining, note taking, adopting a

different perspective, drawing pictures while reading, and constructing mental images

while reading can all increase generative processing.

Wittrock and his colleagues conducted studies in schools to test their generative

learning theory. In each of two experiments conducted by Doctorow, Wittrock, & Marks

(1978) with nearly 500 children in grade six, comprehension and recall were

approximately doubled in the experimental conditions using generative learning tasks.

Later, Linden and Wittrock (1981) assigned students in a fifth grade class to one of four

groups. Following the treatment, comprehension tests were administered to all

participants; the two groups exposed to generative teaching procedures scored

significantly higher on reading comprehension.

The construction-integration (CI) model of text comprehension was first explained

by Kintsch and Van Dijk in 1983; over the years (Kintsch, 2005) they added to the CI

model to further describe the interaction between the top-down (memory and knowledge)

processes that direct comprehension and bottom-up (sensory input) processes that define
17
and limit it. "In the CI model, the words of the text are used to construct propositions;

these activate further propositions without top-down control and then ambiguities and

contradictions are resolved by an integration process" (Kintsch, 2005, p. 127). Retellings

were used as the principal data for Kintsch and van Dijk's model of discourse

comprehension because retellings revealed the story events readers selected during top-

down processing.

By the late 1970s and early 1980s, theoretical and experimental work on the

cognitive processes of comprehension supported a move away from the idea that

information is transferred directly during reading toward theories that recognize the

active, constructive role of the reader (Pearson, 1985: Rowe, 1986). The assumptions of

many educators that children would instinctively learn to comprehend as they learned to

decode (Snow, 2002) and that there was not much teachers could to do develop

comprehension skills (Pearson, 1985) had been called into question; it seemed well worth

the time and effort of teachers to focus their attention on reading comprehension

instruction. Researchers began to examine school settings to see if theory had begun to

inform and improve classroom practice.

Reading Comprehension Research in Educational Settings

In the classic study examining grade three-through-six reading and social studies

classrooms to determine how much time was spent teaching reading comprehension

strategies, Durkin (1978/1979) began with a search for the definition of "comprehension

instruction." Finding little help in existing literature, Durkin developed a list of categories

for teacher behavior that could be considered to be comprehension instruction. The major

finding of the study was that almost no comprehension instruction was taking place.
18
During almost 18,000 minutes of observation of social studies and reading lessons, only

45 minutes of time were spend on comprehension instruction. Instead, teachers "relied

heavily on workbooks and ditto sheets" (p. 525) and much attention was directed to

giving, completing, and checking the assignments in these materials. In a follow-up

study, Durkin (1981) examined the manuals of five basal reading programs; these teacher

editions also devoted generous amounts of space to literal-level questions and contained

only brief, infrequent instructions for teaching reading comprehension.

Durkin's (1978/1979, 1981) findings instigated further research into reading

comprehension (Pressley et al, 1998). In 1985, Shake and Allington observed reading

lessons in second grade classrooms to examine teachers' use of reading manuals. The

teachers in their study did not adhere to the comprehension questions in the manuals, but

rather used their own, which tended to be even simpler and less text-based than those

prescribed.

The common practice of assessing comprehension by asking a series of mainly literal

oral follow-up questions came under question by those researching reading

comprehension processes (Clark, 1982; Rhodes & Shanklin, 1993; Rosenshine &

Meister, 1994; Shake & Allington, 1985). A report sponsored by the National Institute of

Education (Anderson, Hiebert, Scott, & Wilkinson, 1985) and an article by Pappas and

Brown (1987) also warned against continued excessive use of workbooks and work

sheets during reading instruction time. New theories and conceptions of reading

comprehension had contributed to a "move away from information transfer theories of

reading comprehension and toward theories that acknowledge the active, constructive

role of the reader" (Rowe, 1986, p. 43, see also Meredith, Mitchell, & Hernandez-Miller,
19
1992). Children would benefit more from comprehension instructional tasks such as

asking purpose questions, using advance organizers, summarizing, dramatizing, and

retelling - strategies that had proven to be effective in improving reading comprehension

(Pearson, 1985; Rowe, 1986; Valencia & Pearson, 1987). Retellings, in particular,

demonstrated that readers expand what is written in the book and add from their own

schemas to "build a personal model of what the text means" (Pearson, 1985, p. 735).

As new reading comprehension instructional practices came into prominence,

reading comprehension assessment formats also changed (Sarroub and Pearson, 1998).

Formal systematic testing, which began around the time of World War I, had used short

answers, multiple choice, essays, and oral answers to discussion questions to determine

comprehension capabilities. Informal reading comprehension assessments began with

informal reading inventories in the 1940s and continued with reading miscue inventories

in the 1970s. During the 1980s and 1990s, the cognitive revolution in teaching reading

was closely followed by a sociocultural revolution brought on in part by the translation

and influence of the writings of Vygotsky (1986). One of the important advances in

comprehension during this period was the use of retelling to assess comprehension.

"Many scholars developed systems for evaluating the depth and breadth of students' text

understandings based on their attempts to retell or recall what they had read. . . . There

was a conscious attempt to take into account reader, text, and context factors" (Sarroub &

Pearson, 1998, p. 101).

Unfortunately, the advances made in reading comprehension theory, research, and

models of instructional practice during the 1980s did not become firmly established in

practical application. In the years just preceding the new millennium, "education
20
officials, in the name of holding schools and teachers responsible for student

performance,. .. created such a high-stakes environment that people end up 'teaching to

the test' in a way that narrows rather than expands curricular opportunities" (Sarroub &

Pearson, 1998, p. 97; see also Martinez & McGee, 2000). Some of the more promising

methods of fostering and monitoring reading comprehension had not been allowed to take

root, partially because of political constraints that led to a renewed emphasis on

beginning reading instruction (Paris & Paris, 2003).

Although there had been an opportunity to revolutionize comprehension instruction,

no such revolution had taken place (Pressley, 2001). Twenty years after Durkin's

(1978/1979) study alerted educators to the dire condition of reading comprehension

instruction, Pressley et al. (1998) studied literacy instruction in ten upper elementary

classrooms over the course of a year. Although the teachers reported that reading

comprehension strategies were important components of the curriculum, very little direct

comprehension strategy instruction occurred. One of the hypotheses developed from the

study was that "comprehension-strategies instruction appears to have made little progress

since Durkin (1978/1979) described it 20 years ago" (p. 186). Asking questions to assess

student comprehension continues to be a prevalent practice in classrooms (Caldwell &

Leslie, 2005). This lack of progress is detrimental to young readers facing an increasing

need to comprehend a variety of texts in the real world. Reading achievement scores of

young people (high school seniors) as reported by the National Assessment of

Educational Progress (NAEP) did not improve over a 30-year period, but rather revealed

a recent significant decline (Snow, 2002). According to Snow, "reading comprehension

instruction is often minimal or ineffective" (p. 5) and teachers were not well-prepared to
21
teach reading comprehension strategies or deal with different levels of reading

comprehension capability in their classrooms. One group of classroom teachers trying to

improve their reading strategy instruction (Villaume & Brabham, 2002) expressed

concern about student resistance and lack of motivation and also the possibility that

reading strategy instruction inhibits rather than enhances the reading of skillful readers.

In spite of the recent discouraging turn of events, the fact remains that cognitive

reading comprehension strategies developed and taught during the 1970s and 1980s were

credited with improving students' reading comprehension (Pressley, 2002; Rosenshine &

Meister, 1994; Taylor, Pearson et al., 2006). Much of the research on comprehension

strategies during those decades followed the pattern of assigning one group of students to

use a particular strategy while the control group did not. "In general, the students taught

to use the comprehension strategy outperformed the students not given the instructions"

(Pressley, 2002, p. 16). Some of the strategies which have been shown to be effective are

reciprocal teaching (Palincsar & Brown, 1984), relating text to prior knowledge, mental

imagery, questioning, and summarization (Pressley, 2002). As students practiced using

these strategies, they internalized them, and eventually began to use them on their own,

with the result that their memory for and comprehension of text improved (Pressley,

2001).

Researchers agreed that strategy instruction had been found to improve

comprehension; n o w they began to ask w h y this happened. It m a y have been that the

actual strategy was less important than the role the strategy played in engaging the reader

in interacting with the text (Rosenshine & Meister, 1994; Snow 2002). Shortly after the

National Reading Panel (2000) recommended using reading strategies, Taylor et al.
22
(2006) began an investigation to determine if the effectiveness of reading strategies was

due to the additional engagement with text the strategies required. "Somewhat ironically,

strategy instruction may not improve children's use of strategies but may encourage them

to look at text in a different manner, possibly increasing their cognitive engagement with

text, and through this increased engagement, become better at comprehending" (p. 307).

Retelling in Educational Settings

Retelling, which had long been used by cognitive researchers to study memory and

comprehension, became one of the strategies used for both reading comprehension

assessment and reading comprehension instruction beginning in the 1970s. At first, the

studies were few and far between; the bulk of retelling research was conducted in the

1980s and 1990s; this was followed by a decline in retelling research in the late 1990s

and the early years of the new century.

In an early study on retention and memory, Zimiles and Kuhns (1976) randomly

assigned six-to-eight-year-old children to one of three experimental groups and then

asked them to listen to a tape-recorded story. Immediately afterward, half the children

told the story back. Following an interval of either two days, two weeks, or two months,

each child was asked to retell the story and then prompted by a series of questions to

identify information remembered but not included in the initial spontaneous recall.

Zimiles and Kuhns noted that oral retellings do not include everything a child remembers.

The researchers found that retelling the story immediately after hearing it significantly

aided subsequent recall for all ages and all time intervals, and that the older children were

better at remembering and retelling. For ten years, Zimiles and Kuhn's study stood as the
23
only research that used retelling to improve, rather than to assess, comprehension

(Morrow, 1985b).

In 1980, Kalmbach asked sixth-grade students to read a story aloud and then retell

everything they could remember about it. The two stories used had been rewritten and

edited to be similar in readability, length, and narrative structure. The retellings were

analyzed by syntactical structure, narrative structure, and central events. Kalmbach

concluded that "a retelling . . . is not an attempt at verbatim recall, rather it is an attempt

to communicate an understanding of a story by selecting, organizing and emphasizing

certain events from the story" (p. 22). Retellings are transactions between the reader and

the text (Rosenblatt, 1978) rather than detail-for-detail repetitions of the original story.

In two subsequent articles, Kalmbach (1986a, 1986b) explained why and how

classroom teachers might use retelling. In the first article, Kalmbach (1986a) encouraged

teachers to use retelling for reading comprehension assessment. By analyzing student

retellings, teachers could evaluate the impact stories were having, as well as learn about

reader strengths and weaknesses. In the second article, Kalmbach (1986b) critiqued

methods of assessing retellings that were being used in classrooms and cautioned against

using complex analytic systems. Rather, Kalmbach suggested using an approach

combining a holistic score with an assessment of recall and an analysis of the student's

organizing strategy.

After having examined hundreds of student retellings for more than ten years,

Goodman (1982) concluded that while retellings cannot reveal everything a child

comprehends, they do provide valuable information about comprehension processing.

Stories that are relevant and predictable make comprehension easier. A child who reads
24
haltingly and with many miscues, may never-the-less have a good conceptual

understanding of what he has read and be able to provide an adequate retelling. No two

retellings will ever be the same because each child has a different set of experiences and

knowledge to bring to his reading. "Retelling a story is an opportunity for a reader to

present his or her ideas to the world and to . .. rehearse the story . . . and to integrate it,

modify it, and add to its comprehension" (p. 305). In 1987, Goodman, Watson, and

Burke recommended that a retelling, usually oral, be part of every reading miscue

analysis. A good retelling procedure included waiting patiently for the reader to retell,

asking open-ended questions without giving the reader information from the text, and not

taking "I don't know" for an answer. In 2000, Wilde recommended continued use of

miscue analysis and retelling procedures.

Irwin and Mitchell (1983) investigated the question: "Which retellings are superior -

those in which the reader restates the passage content in accurate, precise detail or those

in which the reader makes in-depth generalizations about life as s/he summarizes the text

content?" (p. 391). While evaluating the retellings of high school students, the authors

perceived a need to design a holistic rating system. Irwin and Mitchell's Richness in

Retelling Scale and its accompanying matrix-style checklist provided a new way to

evaluate reading comprehension by assessing retellings. The researchers concluded that

retellings of both narrative and expository texts could be scored holistically.

In 1985, Gambrell, Pfeiffer, and Wilson noted that while retelling was well-

established as an assessment tool in reading research, it was not being used in classrooms

as a strategy to improve reading comprehension. The researchers designed a study to

"investigate retelling as a learning strategy using procedures which would be acceptable


25
for use in the typical classroom setting" (p. 217). The participants were fourth-grade

students randomly assigned to one of two generative learning activity treatments, either

retelling or drawing. The retellers practiced by silently reading four passages, writing

down what they thought were the most important ideas, and then retelling them orally to

a partner. The children in the drawing group read the passages and drew pictures of what

they considered to be the important ideas in the stories.

For the testing session, the students met individually with a researcher, read a story

silently, and then immediately retold important information. Two days later, all

participants retold the story again and then answered 20 recall questions. The data

analysis showed that the reading comprehension of the children in the retelling group was

superior to the children in the drawing treatment group on all measures tested.

Further studies by Gambrell and her associates followed throughout the 1980s. In

1987, Kapinus, Gambrell, and Koskinen examined the effects of practice in retelling on

the reading comprehension of proficient and less-proficient readers in a fourth grade

class. Students in both groups demonstrated improved reading comprehension

performance. In 1988, Koskinen et al. used their previous work as a basis to help teachers

develop a methodology for using retelling to instruct less-proficient readers. Finally, in

1989, Koskinen, Gambrell, and Kapinus examined the effectiveness of combining

rereading with retelling under three experimental conditions with students in fourth

grade. Students in the read condition read a story silently and then retold it orally.

Students in the read/reread condition read the story twice and retold it. Students in the

read/retell/reread condition read the story silently and retold it orally and then repeated

the read/retell procedure a second time. The data analysis revealed that there were no
26
differences between the read and read/reread groups, but the read/retell/reread group

performed significantly better than the other two.

In the mid-1980s, Morrow carried out a series of studies to evaluate the efficacy of

using a retelling strategy with young children; comprehension, concept of story structure,

and oral language development were the dependent variables. Morrow (1985b) was

motivated in part by opinions about retelling expressed by teachers in response to a

survey; the teachers considered retelling to be "time consuming, difficult for children,

and without documented educational value" (p. 648). The participants in Morrow's

studies (1985a, 1985b, 1986) were children in kindergarten classrooms. Stories were read

to the children. Those in the experimental groups did oral retellings; in each study, the

retellers demonstrated significant gains in comprehension and oral language skills.

Two years later, Morrow (1988) reviewed the rapidly-accumulating literature

pertaining to retelling and described multiple uses of retelling for reading comprehension

assessment. Because retelling is a challenging task, children need time to practice before

they are asked to retell for assessment purposes. Students should also be told they will be

asked to retell, the purpose for the retell should be established, and in some cases, props

should be provided. Morrow also recommended retelling as an instructional strategy to

teach comprehension of both narrative and informational materials and lamented the

infrequent occurrence of its use.

B y 1996, M o r r o w had been researching and writing about retelling for over ten

years. Morrow observed:

Retelling indicates a reader's or listener's assimilation and reconstruction of text

information, and it reflects comprehension. Retelling allows a reader or listener to


27
structure responses according to personal and individual interpretations of text. It is

an active procedure. . . . Retelling encourages both integration and personalization of

content, helping children see how parts of the text interrelate and how they mesh

with their own experiences, (pp. 267-268)

In 2005, Morrow reiterated the benefits of retelling, e.g. the value of retelling for the

development of children's concepts of narrative and expository structures, for reading

comprehension, and for including past experience, original thinking, and organization of

thought with a literary experience. Morrow also emphasized the need for setting a

purpose for retelling and for providing opportunities to practice.

A retelling procedure developed by Brown and Cambourne (1987) was tested in

schools in New Zealand and endorsed as a comprehension strategy by the New Zealand

Department of Education in the late 1980s. For a number of days or weeks, children were

immersed in a topic and genre. The children then shared predictions before reading,

studied a text, and retold or rewrote it without referring to the original work. Cambourne

said that retelling demands that children focus on the meaning of a text, reflect on it, and

recreate it.

In 1990, Pappas and Golden undertook a meta-analysis of studies based on retelling.

For more than a decade, retelling had been used in research to examine cognitive

processes, including reading comprehension. "This retelling procedure was appealing to

researchers because it resulted in a text itself. . . [which] could then be studied

presumably to reveal insights into how subjects understand and remember various types

of written language" (p. 21). Golden and Pappas critically examined 81 oral retelling

research studies that had been conducted with preschool and elementary grade children
28
between 1977 and 1986. They looked at four areas: speaker-listener characteristics,

instructions given to the children, type of texts used, and ways in which the retellings

were analyzed. Concerns were identified in each of the areas examined. With regard to

speaker-listener characteristics, children were often asked to retell to examiners who had

just heard or read the passage. A child may not be inclined to provide a complete retelling

to a non-naive listener. In regard to instructions, the authors considered it possible that

instructions to "recall" or "retell" may mean different things to different people. In regard

to texts, most used in the studies were constructed or altered by the researchers so

specific factors could be controlled or isolated. In regard to text type, whether a text is

narrative or expository is likely to influence predictions, expectations, memory, and

recall. The fourth area of concern examined by Golden and Pappas were the analysis

schemes used to analyze the retellings. Oral and written recalls received very different

scores when they were evaluated using different systems. Some retellings were evaluated

holistically, some by propositions or structural elements, and some by a combination of

systems.

Golden and Pappas (1990) concluded by making suggestions for the designs of

future studies. Among other recommendations, the authors suggested that researchers use

"more complex, naturally occurring texts" (p. 37), that underlying theories of reader-text

interaction be reported in studies, that systems of analysis be considered in light of

assumptions about the reading process, and that the social, as well as the cognitive

context of retelling be considered. "There is no such thing as 'free recall'. . . since all

recall or retelling tasks are elicited through some social filter or context of situation" (p.

38).
29
Glazer and Brown (1993) described protocols and applications for two kinds of

retelling. For an unguided retelling, students received no prompts. Glazer and Brown

agreed with Morrow (1985b) and stated emphatically that "any prompts are instruction"

(p. 116). They also suggested that the best time to retell is immediately after the reading

of a text, and that it is best for children "not to look back to the book for information to

complete their retellings" (p. 124). Glazer and Brown used Morrow's (1988) scoring

system to assess retellings. They suggested that students learn to self-monitor written and

oral retellings and use retelling checklists as guides for written compositions.

In 1993, Rhodes and Shanklin discussed assessing reading comprehension as a

process rather than as a product. "We need to assess a student's process if we are to teach

the student to process text more effectively and thus improve the product" (p. 211). The

authors noted that comprehension cannot be directly observed and that it is personally

and socially constructed. Children make their own decisions about the parts of the text

they find to be important and the organization of their retellings. Retellings are superior

to more commonplace questioning methods of assessing comprehension, which provide

students clues about what the teacher hopes to discover.

Moss (1993) used data from an in-progress descriptive study to address the subject of

using retellings to assess children's comprehension of expository text. The children in the

study were in grades kindergarten through five. Three students each of high, average, and

low ability from each of the six grade levels participated. Each child met individually

with a researcher, listened to a read-aloud from an informational trade book, drew a

picture about the book, and then retold the book "as if telling it to a friend who had never

heard it before" (p. 4). Irwin and Mitchell's (1983) five-point scale was used to evaluate
30
the retellings. Results of the study suggested that children in the two higher ability groups

were well able to retell the content of the nonfiction trade books, while just half of the

low ability readers were able to do so. Most children in ail three groups were able to

identify main ideas and details and sequence and summarize the text; making inferences

and relating text to experience were more prevalent in the higher ability groups. Moss

suggested further research into the effect of background knowledge, the impact of the

structure of expository books, and the impact of practice on retelling of expository text.

In 1997, Moss undertook a qualitative study that continued her work with children's

retelling of expository text. Moss theorized that more could be learned about children's

understanding of informational text through retelling than by asking questions. To test the

theory, Moss had 20 research assistants individually read aloud a book to 20 children in a

first grade classroom. Following the read aloud, the children retold the story and then

answered four questions asked by the researcher. Half the children received a score of

three on their retellings and eight received a score of four or better (on a five-point scale).

An analysis of the follow-up questions revealed that young children are able to

summarize, identify important information, make inferences, and express opinions and

rationale to support their opinions.

Moss wrote about retelling in 2004: "Involving students in retelling information

trade books represents a promising means not only for engaging students with

outstanding literature but also for improving their understanding of expository text" (p.

710). Retellings are not short, succinct summaries, but holistic representations of a

student's understanding. Teachers can teach the retelling process by modeling, followed

by many opportunities for student practice.


31
When statewide assessments began to use retelling to measure comprehension,

Benson and Cummins (2000) found that many classroom teachers struggled to teach their

students to retell. The retelling model devised by the authors was developmental, with

four hierarchical levels: pretelling, guided retelling, story map retelling, and written

retelling. "Pretelling" means thinking forward; Benson and Cummins stated that until

children have the cognitive ability to reason sequentially, they will find it difficult to

think back to retell a story. After they learn to pretell, children learn story structure

(guided retelling), how to arrange and represent a story using a graphic organizer (story

map retelling), and finally, how to produce written retellings.

Noting that not many studies have "investigated retellings as an instructional strategy

for writing development" (p. 109), Geist and Boydston (2002) also noted that the studies

that had been done yielded encouraging results. Geist and Boydston undertook a study to

examine the effect of using written retelling on student test performance on the Test of

Written Language (TOWL-2). In addition, interaction effects of teaching style and the

use of written retelling to teach grammar were measured.

The study participants were from eight second grade classrooms, four of which were

traditional and four of which used a writing workshop approach. The retelling

intervention was implemented in two traditional and two writing workshop classrooms;

the two remaining traditional classes and the two remaining workshop classes served as

controls. Twelve folk and fairy tales were selected for the written retellings. The data

analysis showed that the students in the traditional classrooms that used written retelling

did not score significantly better than the students in the traditional control group

classrooms. Children in the whole language classrooms that used written retelling did
32
show significant improvement over students in the control group writing workshop

classrooms.

Using Retelling with Struggling Learners

One of the first investigations into the story retells of students with learning

disabilities was conducted by Hansen (1978). The study compared average fifth- and

sixth-grade students with students in self-contained learning disabilities classrooms. All

the students practiced by reading a first-grade passage orally from a standardized

informal reading test and retelling the passage in their own words. They then read and

retold an additional passage and answered the comprehension questions provided with

the test. The analysis of the retellings revealed significant differences between the

comprehension scores and the retellings of the average students and those students with

learning disabilities. For both groups, there was a significant correlation between fluent

retelling and comprehension.

An investigation of the effects of retelling and visual imagery on the reading

comprehension of children with learning disabilities was conducted by Rose, Cundick,

and Higbee in 1983. Thirty elementary students identified as learning disabled were

assigned to one of three groups: a visual imagery treatment group, a retelling treatment

group, or an unaided instruction control group. The students in the retelling group paused

after reading a few sentences and retold aloud what they had read. The comprehension of

students in both treatment groups was significantly better than that of children in a

control group, with retelling proving to be more effective than visual imagery.

Griffith, Ripich, and Dastoli (1986) compared the story retellings of students with

learning disabilities to those of students who were not learning disabled across three age
33
levels (7 through 12 years) and three levels of story difficulty. All the participants

listened to, rather than read, the stories. Generally, the children with learning disabilities

recalled information and retold the stories as well as their peers without learning

disabilities. The authors speculated that "LD children may recall more information when

they do not have to read the material being presented" (p. 552). There were perceptible

developmental differences; older students in both groups recalled more story events.

Children with learning disabilities struggle to verbally organize and reconstruct

stories. Sisco (1992) examined the effects of retelling on story structure, listening

comprehension, and oral language with a group of young children identified as learning

disabled. Participants in the experimental group retold in an interactive, conversational

context with a supportive adult. These children made significant improvements in

listening comprehension and understanding and using story structure when compared to

the participants in the control group.

In a study designed to compare the oral reading and retelling of children with

specific language impairment (SLI) to that of children without SLI, Gillam and Carlile

(1997) examined four research questions: Do students with SLI and those without have

similar miscues when reading aloud? Are there group differences in the amount of

information retained from retellings? Are there group differences in the overall quality of

retellings? What is the relationship between the reading miscues and the retelling of the

students with SLI? Twelve students with SLI were matched with twelve students without

SLI who had similar single-word reading ability. Goodman, Watson, and Burke's (1987)

Reading Miscue Inventory procedures were used to collect and assess oral reading

samples from the participants. The analysis of the data showed that while the students
34
with SLI made significantly more miscues in their oral reading, they retained as much

information in their retellings as their reading-matched peers.

Ten students with learning disabilities in a self-contained classroom participated in a

study reported by Kuldanek (1998). The students ranged in age from six to seven years

and in reading ability from beginning first to beginning second grade level. A pre-test

consisting of a read-aloud story followed by a series of comprehension questions and an

oral retelling was given to all the participants. The treatment lasted for five weeks during

which all the students participated in a language arts unit built around a theme as

suggested by Brown and Cambourne (1987). The unit comprised ten stories about bears,

as well as bear poetry and bear projects. Story grammar and a retelling strategy were

directly taught to all the participants. The analysis of the pre- and post-test scores

revealed significant gains in both comprehension and awareness of the elements of story

structure. Kuldanek concluded that the Brown and Cambourne retelling procedure was

effective with the students in the study.

Flurkey and Goodman (2004) used a case study to illustrate how a struggling reader

managed the retelling of a complex text. A fourth-grade student read and retold one of the

Magic School Bus books. The books in this series use a format that combines a story and

pictures with substantial amounts of informational text related to a science topic. Often, a

struggling reader would not be encouraged to read such a text, even if he expressed an

interest in doing so. The researchers analyzed the student's reading miscues and his

retellings. They recommended that students' reading not be limited to leveled books that

"have no reference to the reader's knowledge, culture, or interest" (p. 243). Students are
35
willing and motivated to struggle with difficult texts that are of interest to them. In this

case, the child's retelling revealed that he understood the book's content.

After reviewing the value of retelling, Rhodes and Milby (2007) described a method

of creating e-books (using presentation software) to enhance the retellings of readers with

disabilities. The teacher began by reading a book to the class; after discussing the story,

the class retold it with the help of a graphic organizer. Text based on the student retelling

was incorporated into a slide show with digital photos and clip art. Finally, the students

recorded their narration into the e-book and each received a download on their desktop

computers. The authors found this to be "an effective instructional strategy for students

with disabilities" (p. 258).

Retelling in a Contemporary Middle School Setting

At first glance, retelling seems to be a simple procedure. In a school setting, a

student is asked to read or listen to a text and then tell or write everything he or she

remembers. Although retelling may be easy for a teacher to request, it is not necessarily

easy for children to produce (Morrow, 2005). Many children familiar with answering

questions are not familiar with the more holistic, complex task of retelling. Furthermore,

some teachers have found it difficult to teach their students to retell (Benson & Cummins,

2000).

Much of the research on retelling has taken place in schools, but not under typical

classroom conditions. Participants in retelling studies have often worked one-to-one with

a researcher in a setting outside the classroom for a limited number of sessions. As the

body of work on retelling began to accumulate and researchers recognized the value of

retelling as an instructional strategy, they made recommendations for using retelling in


36
classrooms. The read-and-retell procedure advocated by Brown and Cambourne (1986)

was based on several years of trials in classroom settings. Koskinen et al. (1988)

suggested a sequence which included teacher sharing of a rationale for retelling, teacher

modeling, and guided practice before students were asked to retell in a large group or

with a partner. Palincsar and Brown (1984) recognized the importance of evaluating

reading comprehension strategies with "naturally occurring groups within school

settings" (p. 157); the practical implications of this type of research are significantly

increased.

Research has suggested additional implications regarding the use of retelling in

classroom settings. Reading comprehension researchers in both theoretical and

educational settings have asked whether retelling capabilities improve with practice

(Moss, 1993; Shannon, Kameenui, & Baumann, 1988; Tucker, 2001). Repeatedly and

resoundingly, the answer has been "yes" (Caldwell & Leslie, 2005; Gambrell et al., 1985;

Morrow, 1985b, 1996). While no optimal time for a retelling intervention has been

suggested or recommended, the general consensus seems to be the longer the better.

Another consideration in retelling research is age difference as it relates to narrative

abilities. At what age do children understand story structure? Should classroom time be

devoted to instruction in story grammar, "the set of rules which classify the components

of a story" (Dreher & Singer, 1980, p. 262)? To find out, Dreher and Singer designed a

study to determine whether teaching story structure would help students in a fifth grade

class to recall a story. They concluded that middle level students do not need to be

formally taught the structures of a story grammar; other researchers have reached the
37
same conclusion (Goldman & Rakestraw, 2000; Griffith, Ripich, & Dastoli, 1986; John,

Lui, & Tannock, 2003; Marshall, 1983).

Many literacy researchers believe children do not need to adhere to the structure of a

story grammar when they retell (Britton, 1970). On the contrary, retellers construct their

own meaning using the text as a blueprint, but making decisions about the most important

aspects and how to organize them (Kalmbach, 1980; Kintsch &Van Dijk, 1983; Rhodes

& Shanklin, 1993; Wilson & Cleland 1985). Retellings have two components: the

information recalled from the original story and the unique, personal narrative created by

the reteller (Goodman, 1982; Koskinen et al., 1988; Tucker 2001). A retelling is not a

summary (Moss, 2004), but rather a natural, holistic view of a child's concept of a story.

According to Rosen (1988) children change stories, not because they forget, but

because they retell by taking into account their own experiences. Story telling is a

cognitive, social, emotional, and moral process. Telling and retelling require that children

pay close attention to the world, to the structures of genre, and to the way that a story's

meaning interconnects with their lives.

One issue Golden and Pappas (1990) raised in relation to retelling research was the

type of texts used: was the content from textbooks, trade books, or constructed by

researchers? Golden and Pappas found that many texts had been constructed by the

experimenters and were relatively short in length. They proposed that future research

make use of more complex, authentic texts. The recommendation of Golden and Pappas

concurs with the emphasis of psycholinguist theory on the value of using authentic texts.

Martinez and McGee (2000) pointed out "that teachers who work with older students will
38
increasingly choose to use complete works of literature rather than excerpts that are

currently found in basal readers" (p. 167).

Research has determined that there are many benefits to reading and retelling

engaging, authentic texts, both narrative and expository. Retelling supports the

development of language structures, comprehension, and organization of thought

(Morrow, 2005). When children read with a view to retell, their focus and attention

improve (Kuldanek, 1998). Retelling requires active involvement as a student listens to

the author's voice, as well as his own (Cooper, 2004) and provides a means of self-

checking for a child who has not read with understanding (Bromley, 1998). Retelling

brings about social interaction. When children read and interact with literature, they

connect with their own emotions, and with people and ideas from all the world's times

and places (Cooper, 2004; Martinez & McGee, 2000).

Summary

This chapter first reviewed literature related to the history, psychology, and theories

of reading comprehension and reading comprehension instruction. Views about reading

and reading comprehension shifted dramatically in the closing decades of the 20th century

and brought about many changes in educational research and practice. Strategies, such as

retelling, were developed and used, first to assess the process of reading and then as an

instructional tool to enhance and improve reading comprehension. Literature related to

retelling in educational settings with both proficient and struggling readers was reviewed,

as well as literature addressing specific issues related to retelling in a contemporary

middle school setting.


39
Taken together, these studies provided a basis for the current study of the effects of

retelling as an instructional strategy on the reading comprehension capabilities of two

groups of middle school students.

Organization of the Remainder of the Study

Chapter 2 provided a review of the literature related to the study. The methods and

procedures used to conduct the research will be described in Chapter 3, followed by the

data analyses for each of the research questions in Chapter 4. The summary, major

findings, conclusions, discussion, and recommendations for practice and further study

will be presented in Chapter 5.


40
Chapter 3

Methodology

This research was an experimental study designed to investigate the impact of

retelling as an instructional strategy on reading comprehension. The purpose of the study

was twofold. First, it endeavored to determine if replacing existing group comprehension

instruction and individual comprehension workbook assignments with retelling

instruction and oral and written retelling practice would significantly impact the reading

comprehension achievement of middle school students, and second, it sought to discover

whether the reading comprehension achievement of students at different levels of reading

capability (proficient and less-proficient) would be affected equally. Specifically, the

study answered the following questions:

1. Is there a difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a

standardized reading test between students who engage in retelling and students who do

not?

2. Is there a difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a

curriculum-based unit test between students who engage in retelling and students who do

not?

3. Is there a difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a

standardized reading test between proficient and less-proficient readers who engage in

retelling?

4. Is there a difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a

curriculum-based unit test between proficient and less-proficient readers who engage in

retelling?
41
5. Is there a gain in reading achievement as measured by a comparison of the pre-

and post-assessments of written retellings of students who engage in retelling?

The methodology and research procedures used to investigate these research

questions are described in Chapter 3. The chapter describes the process used to access

and review relevant research, the population and setting of the study, the tests and

instruments used to collect data, the pilot testing procedure, the intervention and data

collection process, and the data analysis procedures. The fifth edition of the Publication

Manual of the American Psychological Association (2001) was used as the style manual

for writing the dissertation.

Review of Related Research

Literature relevant to the relationship between retelling and reading comprehension

was located using the resources found at the I. D. Weeks Library on the campus of The

University of South Dakota in Vermillion, South Dakota. Additional topics researched

included: early studies of memory and reading comprehension; reading comprehension

models developed by cognitive psychologists; and issues, components, and processes of

retelling in a contemporary middle school setting. Databases searched included:

Educational Resources Information Center (ERIC), Dissertation Abstracts International

(DAI), EBSCO Megafile, ProQuest, Academic Search Premier, Education Research

Complete, EJS E-Journals, PsycARTICLES, Psychology and Behavioral Sciences

Collection, PsycINFO, and others. When necessary and possible, researchers and authors

were contacted via electronic mail to answer questions or assist with retrieval of articles

or publications not available through searches of the databases listed.


42
Population and Setting

The participants in this study were a convenience sample of students in two intact

sixth-grade classes in a public school in a small Midwestern city school district. The

families in the community represented a range of cultural groups and socioeconomic

levels; 52% of the children in the elementary school qualified to receive free or reduced-

price lunches (P. L. Anderson, personal communication, June 25, 2008).

Approval to conduct the study was granted by The University of South Dakota

Office of Human Subjects Protection (Appendix A). Permission to carry out the study

was given by the school district following a meeting with the principal and a presentation

to the school board (Appendix B). Letters were mailed by the researcher to the parents or

legal guardians of 38 students who received their primary reading instruction in the two

classrooms (Appendix C). After one week, the researcher telephoned families who had

not returned the letters to see if they had questions about the study and reminded them to

return the consent form. After two weeks, 33 parents or legal guardians had returned the

consent forms; 32 gave permission for their children to participate in the study and 1

parent declined. The students who received parental consent to be in the study received

an assent form and verbal description of the research and were invited to ask questions

(Appendix D). All 32 of the students agreed to participate.

The students in the study had previously been assigned by their teachers to one of

two groups for their literacy instruction each day. Twenty-three of the participating sixth-

graders, identified as proficient readers, were assigned to one of the sixth-grade teachers;

the remaining 9 participants, identified as less-proficient readers, were assigned to the


43
other sixth-grade teacher. All students in both groups covered the same material in their

basal reading series during each week's five one-hour-long reading lessons.

In each of the two reading groups (proficient and less-proficient), the participating

students were randomly assigned to either the experimental (retelling) or control

(workbook) group by matching an alphabetical list of their last names to a list of

computer-generated random numbers.

Instrumentation

Standardized Pre-Post Test

All students participating in the study were pre- and post-tested using the Test of

Reading Comprehension, third edition (TORC-3), published by Brown, Hammill, and

Wiederholt (1995) and used with permission (Appendix E). The four general

comprehension core subtests (general vocabulary, syntactic similarities, paragraph

reading, and sentence sequencing) of the TORC-3 were administered to all participants.

A standard score, grade and age equivalents, and a reading comprehension quotient

(RCQ) were calculated for each participant.

The TORC-3 is an untimed silent reading test for students from 7 to 18 years of age.

The TORC-3 manual states that the test is based on "a constructivist orientation that

focuses on holistic, cognitive and linguistic aspects of reading" (Brown, Hammill, and

Wiederholt, 1995, p. v). The normative data for the TORC-3 were collected in 1993-94; it

was normed on a sample of 1,962 children and young people from 19 states representing

all regions of the United States, both genders, urban and rural students, students with

disabilities, and children from diverse ethnic backgrounds.


44
Test-retest reliability for the TORC-3 ranges from .89 to .97, and internal

consistency reliabilities are generally over .90. Interrater reliabilities ranges from .87 to

.98. The publisher cites six studies as evidence of criterion validity and median point-

biserial correlations for each subtest at each age for content validity. A correlation of .81

between the TORC-3's Reading Comprehension Quotient and the Wechsler Intelligence

Scale for Children - Revised and mean scores from ten samples of students are given to

substantiate construct validity. Both reviewers in Buros Mental Measurements Yearbook

(Impara & Plake, 1998) suggested that the TORC-3 was appropriate for students of

middle school age, but may not be suitable for primary students because of the small size

of the norming population for children from ages seven to eight years and the floor

effects for scores for the younger age group.

Instructional Materials

Throughout the study, all students were instructed by their classroom reading

teachers or the researcher using reading curriculum materials published by Scott

Foresman (Afflerbach et al., 2004). Each student's materials comprised a hard-bound

reader and a soft cover Practice Book. The reader was divided into six five-selection

units. A week was devoted to each selection; a unit, therefore, was completed in a five-

week period, followed by a week between units for special projects, poetry study, and

unit testing. Each week, students completed 9 pages in their Practice Book: 1 vocabulary

page, 1 phonics/word study page, 2 research/study skills pages, 3 comprehension pages,

and a 2-page selection test.

During the study, students in the control (workbook) group continued to answer the

questions on all the workbook pages each week. Students in the experimental (retelling)
45
group completed the workbook pages designated for vocabulary, phonics/word study, and

research/study skills, as well as the selection test. During the intervention, the retelling

students were not required to complete the three weekly comprehension workbook pages;

rather, they did oral and written retelling.

Additional resources provided by the Scott Foresman curriculum (Afflerbach et al.,

2004) or suggested for supplemental use were available to the teachers and the

researcher. The classrooms contained 10-booklet sets of 16-page guided readers at three

levels for each weekly unit (Appendices F & G); the teacher's manual also listed a

suggested selection of easy, on-level, and challenge trade books to supplement each

week's lesson. The leveled readers were used selectively by both the classroom teachers

and the researcher. The researcher used eight of the suggested trade books during the

study (Appendix H). The classroom teachers did not make use of any of the

recommended trade books during the same interval.

Curriculum-Based Assessment

A Unit Skills Test, part of the Scott Foresman Reading Program for grade six, was

normally administered to all students in both reading groups at the end of each five-week

unit. The Unit Skills Test evaluated comprehension through 24 cued questions based on

one-page narrative and expository selections, and a study skills section of either five or

ten questions. Each of the Skills Tests also included six word analysis items and five

grammar items. The usual post-unit assessment procedure was continued for all

participants (retelling and workbook) in the study. Scores of the Unit Skills Test from the

previously-completed unit were used as a pre-test and compared to scores obtained at the
46
close of the intervention period to determine the change in reading achievement for each

student over the period of the study.

Pearson Scott Foresman was contacted early in the study via email and telephone to

ascertain information related to the reliability and validity of the Unit Skills Tests. When

no response was forthcoming, a letter sent to Pearson Scott Foresman's editorial offices

garnered a reply (Appendix I) stating that the tests "have not been subjected to statistical

studies, had reliability and validity measures run on them, or been normed using large

student populations" (R. Swensson, personal communication, July 15, 2008). Pearson

Scott Foresman considers their Unit Skills Tests to be valid because each skill taught

during a unit of study is linked to a question on the test. Because there is only one form of

each test, test/retest reliability has not been determined.

The Scott Foresman Unit Skills Tests were used in the study as a curriculum-based

assessment despite concerns about their reliability, validity, and content (assessment of

skills other than comprehension). One of the research questions called for an examination

of the effect of the intervention on a curriculum-based reading comprehension

assessment; the Scott Foresman Unit Skills Test was the best available instrument.

Written Retelling Assessment

All students assigned to the experimental group completed a written retelling of a

story before the intervention began and at the close of the unit. These retellings were

evaluated using the holistic, five-level Richness in Retelling Scale (RIR) developed by

Irwin and Mitchell (1983) and used with permission (Appendix J). There are two parts to

the RIR Scale: criteria describing each of the five levels and a checklist forjudging the

retellings (Appendix J). Written retellings at a Level 1 include details only (no
47
summarizing statement or major point) and are incomplete, lacking in coherence, and

difficult to comprehend. A written retelling judged to be at Level 5 includes a

summarizing statement and all major points as well as supporting details, and is

complete, comprehensible, and coherent. Levels 2 through 4 describe gradations in the

quality of retellings between the lowest and highest levels.

Irwin and Mitchell (1983) initially used their scale to rate student retellings of both

narrative and expository texts. The researchers validated their ratings by asking a group

of experienced teachers to rate the same retellings. After two sessions of instruction on

the use of the scale and exposure to rated samples, the agreement of the teachers with the

designers of the scale was 87.5%. The RIR Scale has since been used by teachers and

researchers for more than 20 years (Kalmbach, 1986b; Morrow, 1988; Morrow, et al.,

1986; Moss, 1993, 1997, 2004; Robnolt, 2004). Scores of oral retellings evaluated by the

RIR Scale have been shown by Robnolt to be significantly inter-correlated with scores of

retellings assessed by Morrow's (1988) story retelling scale, another measure with a

longstanding history of use as a retelling assessment (Sisco, 1992). The Richness in

Retelling Scale evaluates elements of story grammar, but also gives consideration to the

reteller's contribution. It is consistent with the purpose of a retell, which is not to recall a

story exactly, but to select, organize, and personalize the story content (Tucker, 2001).

To establish the validity of written retelling assessment scores in the present study,

an independent rater (an English teacher at the school in which the study took place) was

familiarized with the use of Irwin and Mitchell's scale. The pre and post written retellings

of each participant were scored independently by the researcher and the independent

rater. The interrater reliability, which was calculated by dividing agreements by


48
agreements plus disagreements, was .85. After determining the correlation, the raters met

and reached a consensus on the written retellings that had been scored differently

(Pearman, 2008).

Pilot Study

Prior to the study, a small-scale pilot test was conducted to evaluate the efficacy of

the proposed oral and written retelling procedures (Appendices K and L). Three middle-

school students (two female and one male) from a neighboring school district were

selected to participate; two of these students were familiar with the Scott Foresman

curriculum, which was used in their reading class. The participants read selections from

the Scott Foresman Unit 4 Leveled Readers (162A, 164A, 163B, or 165B) and retold

what they had read. The oral retelling procedures evaluated were: Team Retelling (Hoyt,

1999), Paired Retelling (Koskinen et al., 1988), and Consecutive Retelling (Appendix K).

Each of these procedures proved to be satisfactory and no modifications were made.

Each of the three participants in the pilot study also attempted one of the written

retelling procedures (Appendix L): Plot Summary Maps (Hansen, 2004), Retelling

Journal Entries (Brown & Cambourne, 1987), or Retelling to an E-Pal (Shaw, 2005). The

first written retelling, using the Plot Summary Maps procedure, was handwritten; the

procedure worked well. The students attempting the second and third procedures used

word processors to record their retellings. Their retellings were satisfactory, but the pilot

work revealed that the students' poor keyboarding skills did not allow them to complete

their retellings in a timely manner. Therefore, it was determined that all written retellings

in the study would be handwritten.


49
Intervention and Data Collection

Data were collected over two six-week periods during the spring semester of the

2007-2008 school year. Each student in both groups of sixth graders was randomly

assigned to one of two groups: the retelling (experimental) group or the workbook

(control) group. Prior to the start of the treatment, each student's current reading

comprehension achievement was assessed using the Test of Reading Comprehension,

third edition. In addition, each student assigned to the first retelling group read an

assigned selection and then composed a written retelling based on the reading. The

students were instructed to write everything they could remember about the story; they

were also told that spelling, punctuation, grammar, and other conventions would not be

evaluated. A minimum of 15 minutes and a maximum of 25 minutes was allowed for

completion of the written retellings.

For the first five weeks of the study, the retelling intervention was used with the

reading group made up of students identified as proficient readers. The 11 students in the

randomly-determined workbook (control) group continued to receive reading instruction

from their regular reading teacher as usual. They also completed assignments and

activities according to the plans of their teacher.

The 12 readers randomly assigned to the retelling (experimental) group participated

in their regular reading class, taught by their regular teacher, for instruction related to

building background, vocabulary, word study, and study skills. The students in the

retelling group were exempt from activities and workbook pages specifically identified as

comprehension instruction by the publisher of the materials or by the classroom teacher.

For example, generalizing, fact and opinion, paraphrasing, and drawing conclusions were
50
designated comprehension topics. Rather than complete comprehension workbook pages

or participate in classroom comprehension instructional activities, students in the retelling

group met with the researcher for oral and written retelling instruction and practice.

Approximately 40% of weekly reading instructional time was customarily devoted to

reading comprehension instruction and assignment completion. Therefore, a total of 120

minutes of the weekly 5 hours of reading time was designated for the retelling

intervention: 20 minutes on Monday and Thursday, and 40 minutes on Tuesday and

Wednesday. The schedule was adjusted weekly as needed to accommodate changes in the

school calendar such as faculty in-service meetings, special events, field trips, vacation

days, and snow days; the proposed amount of retelling time per week was maintained

throughout the intervention period.

The first two retelling sessions each week were devoted to reading and orally

retelling the main selection for the week. Oral retelling procedures included: Team

Retelling (Hoyt, 1999), Paired Retelling (Koskinen et al., 1988), or Consecutive Retelling

(Appendix K). During the third retelling session of the week, work on the main selection

was completed and a new text from a leveled reader or a trade book was introduced.

During the final meeting of the week, each student composed a written retelling based on

the supplemental text. Weekly written retellings (Appendix L) were completed using one

of the written retelling procedures: Plot Summary Maps (Hansen, 2004), Retelling

Journal Entries (Brown & Cambourne, 1987), or SPOT the Story (Bos & Vaughn, 1994).

Retelling to an E-Pal (Shaw, 2005) was eliminated from use after the pilot study revealed

that students in sixth grade may not be proficient at word processing and when it was
51
determined that the students did not have access to email while at school. Oral and

written retellings generally took place after one reading of a text.

At the close of the five-week intervention period, the students in both the retelling

(experimental) and the workbook (control) groups were assessed using the Scott

Foresman Reading 6 Unit Skills Test. The students in the retelling (experimental) group

completed a post-test written retelling following the protocol used for the pre-test written

retelling collected prior to the intervention.

During the next five-week unit, the researcher worked with 5 students from the

group of less-proficient readers while the 4 students in the workbook group were

instructed by their reading teacher. The procedures used with the retelling (experimental)

group replicated those which had been used during the five-week intervention with

students from the proficient reading group. One male student in the workbook (control)

group was withdrawn from the study thereby reducing the number of students in the

workbook group to 3. At the close of the second unit of study, the 8 remaining students in

both groups of less-proficient readers were tested using the Scott Foresman Reading 6

Unit Skills Test. The students in the less-proficient retelling group followed the earlier

protocol for reading a story and generating a written retelling of the text.

At the close of both intervention periods, all participants in both the control and

experimental proficient and less-proficient reading groups were post-tested using the

TORC-3.

Data Analysis

The data collected in this study were analyzed descriptively and inferentially to

answer the research questions and test for differences in the means of the two groups or
52
the two tests being examined (control/experimental, experimental proficient/experimental

less-proficient, pre/post written retelling).

For questions one and two, a pre-test/post-test control group experimental design

was used to examine the effect of the independent variable (retelling as an instructional

strategy) on the dependent variable (reading comprehension achievement). The means

and standard deviations for the workbook (control) and retelling (experimental) groups

on the formal and informal measures of reading comprehension achievement were

computed, followed by a Student's paired samples t test for each of the measures for both

groups. The effect size for the difference between the mean test scores of the

experimental and control groups as measured on the TORC-3 was also calculated.

Means, standard deviations, and t tests for equality of means were used to analyze

the data for the comparison of the reading comprehension achievement of the proficient

and less-proficient readers in the retelling group (questions three and four).

Question five was addressed using a one group pre-test/post-test design to examine

the effect of the retelling intervention on reading comprehension as measured by an

assessment of the written retellings of the students in the experimental group. Data were

analyzed by computing means, standard deviations, and Student's t tests to determine if

there was a significant difference in the mean scores between the pre- and post-

assessments of the written retellings. The effect size for the difference between the mean

scores of the pre- and post-test scores was also calculated.

All statistical analyses were carried out using the statistical program SPSS 16.0

(Statistical Package for the Social Sciences).


53

Summary

In Chapter 3, the process used to access and review relevant research related to

retelling has been presented, as well as information about the population and setting of

the study, the tests and instruments used to collect data, the data collection process, and

the data analysis procedures. Chapter 4 reports the findings of the study. The summary,

major findings, conclusions, discussion, and recommendations for practice and further

study are presented in Chapter 5.


54
CHAPTER 4

Results

Introduction

This study examined the effects of using oral and written retelling as a reading

comprehension instructional strategy with middle school students in a sixth grade

classroom. Both a standardized test {Test of Reading Comprehension, third edition) and a

curriculum-based measure (Scott Foresman Reading 6 Unit Skills Tests) were used to

compare change in the reading comprehension achievement of participants in the

workbook (control) group with change in the reading comprehension achievement of

participants in the retelling (experimental) group. The same measures were used to

compare the change in reading comprehension achievement of two subgroups within the

retelling (experimental) group: proficient readers and less-proficient readers. Finally,

change in the reading comprehension capability of students exposed to the intervention

was measured by assessing pre and post written retellings using the Richness in Retelling

Scale.

In this chapter, the research questions are stated, demographic information

describing the participants is reported, and findings obtained from the statistical analyses

of the data are presented.

Research Questions

1. Is there a difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a

standardized reading test between students who engage in retelling and students who do

not?
55

2. Is there a difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a

curriculum-based unit test between students who engage in retelling and students who do

not?

3. Is there a difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a

standardized reading test between proficient and less-proficient readers who engage in

retelling?

4. Is there a difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a

curriculum-based unit test between proficient and less-proficient readers who engage in

retelling?

5. Is there a gain in reading achievement as measured by a comparison of the pre-

and post-assessments of written retellings of students who engage in retelling?

Demographic Data

The participants in this study were a convenience sample of students in two intact

sixth-grade classes in a small, Midwestern public school. Fifteen of the 40 students (35%)

enrolled in the two classes at the time of the study were female and 25 of them (65%)

were male. Two of the forty students in the sixth grade received their reading instruction

in a special education resource room setting and were therefore not available to

participate in the study. Of the remaining 38 sixth-grade students, 24 were part of a

reading group for proficient readers instructed by the female sixth grade teacher.

Fourteen less-proficient readers received reading instruction in the second sixth grade

classroom and were taught by the male sixth grade teacher.

The parents/legal guardians of the 38 eligible students received a letter inviting them

to give consent for their children to participate in the study. Of these, 33 parents or legal
56
guardians (87%) returned the consent forms; 32 gave permission for their children to

participate in the study and 1 declined. All 32 of these students agreed to participate in

the study. One of the student's participation was discontinued when it was discovered he

had signed his mother's name on the consent form.

Of the 31 students completing the study, twelve (39%) were female and nineteen

(61%) were male. Twenty-three of the 24 students from the group for proficient readers

(96%) participated in the study; twelve of these were randomly assigned to the retelling

(experimental) group and 11 were randomly assigned to the workbook (control) group.

Consent letters for 10 of the 14 students (71%) in the group for less-proficient readers

were returned by their parents or guardians. One of these parents refused consent.

Initially, 5 of the students from the group for less-proficient readers had been randomly

assigned to the retelling (experimental) group, while 4 became part of the workbook

(control) group. The number of students in the less-proficient workbook group decreased

to 3 when one of the participants was dismissed.

When the study began, the students ranged in age from 11 years 10 months to 13

years 1 month with a mean age of 12 years 6 months. All participants were verbally

proficient English speakers, though some were from homes in which English was not the

primary spoken language. None of the students participating in the study were currently

enrolled in the school's program for English language learners.

Research Findings

Research Questions One and Two

Is there a difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a standardized

reading test between students who engage in retelling and students who do not? Is there a
57

difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a curriculum-based unit test

between students who engage in retelling and students who do not?

The Reading Comprehension Quotient (RCQ) of the Test of Reading

Comprehension, third edition (TORC-3), was used as a standardized measure of reading

comprehension. The Unit Skills Tests administered following the completion of each

five-week unit of the Scott Foresman Reading 6 (SF-6) program were used to obtain an

informal curriculum-based measure of comprehension; percentage of correct answers

achieved on the test given before and after the unit of study were compared. Table 1

presents the means and standard deviations for the pre- and post-tests, as well as the

change scores (post-test minus pre-test), for both treatment groups on the two measures

used to assess reading comprehension.

Table 1

Reading Comprehension Pre, Post, and Change Scores of Two Groups of Readers

Treatment Group

Retellers n = 17 Workbook n=\A

Test/Score Mean SD Mean SD


TORC-3 RCQ Pre-test 89.18 19.47 92.86 12.86

TORC-3 RCQ Post-test 96.76 15.18 94.43 10.40

TORC-3 RCQ Change 7.59 10.33 1.57 8.61

SF-6 Unit Skills Pre-test .68 .22 .72 .17

SF-6 Unit Skills Post-test .67 .18 .69 .16

SF-6 Unit Skills Test Change .01 .10 .03 .08


58
Comparison of the means of the pre- and post-tests for both comprehension measures

for both groups is presented in Table 2. The post-test scores for the TORC-3 of the

retelling (experimental) group differ significantly from the pre-test scores, t = 3.03,p =

.008. No other differences were statistically significant at the .05 level.

Table 2

Comparison of Pre/Post Scores on Two Comprehension Tests for Two Groups of Readers

Paired Sampl*JS T-tests

95% Confidence
Comprehension Test Mean Interval of the Sig.
& Group Difference SD Difference t value (2-tailed)
TORC Pre/Post Retellers 7.59 10.33 -2.28 12.90 3.03 .008*

TORC Pre/Post Workbook 1.57 8.61 -3.40 6.54 .68 .507

SF-6 Pre/Post Retellers .01 .10 -.04 .06 .44 .668

SF-6 Pre/Post Workbook .03 .08 -.18 .07 1.34 .203

*/?<.05,N= 17

Table 3 depicts the results of an independent samples t test to compare the difference

in means of the change scores of the TORC-3 between the retelling (experimental) group

(M= 7.59) and the workbook (control) group (M = 1.57). Although the difference in the

group means was not statistically significant at/? = .05 (t (29) = 1.74,/? = .093), the

calculated effect size (7.59 - 1.57)/9.96 (pooled SD of both groups) indicates a strong

practical significance (Bartz, 1998); the effect size was calculated to equal .60, which is

in the large range. The comprehension change score of the average student in the retelling

(experimental) group is therefore .60 standard deviations above the comprehension


59
change score of the average student in the workbook (control) group and exceeds the

change scores of 60% of the control group.

The difference between the means of the change scores of the Scott Foresman Unit

Skills Tests for the experimental (M= - 0.01) and control (M= - 0.03) groups was slight;

there was no statistically significant difference between the pre-test and the post-test

means, t (29) = .55,p = .590.

Table 3
Comparison of Comprehension Change Scores of Control and Experimental Groups

T-tests for Equality of Means

95% Confidence
Mean Interval of Sig.
Comprehension Test Difference the Difference t value (2-tailed)

TORC-3 RCQ Change 6.02 -1.07 13.10 1.74 .093

SF-6 Unit Test % Change .02 -.05 .09 .55 .590

Research Questions Three and Four

Is there a difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a standardized

reading test between proficient and less-proficient readers who engage in retelling? Is

there a difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a curriculum-based

unit test between proficient and less-proficient readers who engage in retelling?

Table 4 presents the means and standard deviations for the change scores (post-test

minus pre-test) on both measures used to assess reading comprehension for the two

subgroups of students that received the intervention.


60
Table 4

Reading Comprehension Change Scores of Proficient and Less-Proficient Retellers

Experimental (Retelling) Subgroup

Proficient N = 12 Less-proficient N = 5

Comprehension Test Mean SD Mean SD


TORC-3 RCQ Change 7.75 10.90 7.20 10.01

SF-6 Unit Skills Test Change -.05 .09 .08 .06

As shown on Table 5, an independent samples t test indicated that the difference in

means of the change scores of the TORC-3 between the retelling (experimental) students

in the proficient reading group (M= 7.75) and the less-proficient group (M = 7.20) was

not significant, t (15) = 0A0,p = .955. The means of the change scores of the Scott

Foresman Unit Skills Tests for the proficient readers (M= - 0.05) and the less-proficient

readers (M= 0.08) groups were significantly different, /(15)= 3.04,p = .008.

Table 5

Comparison of Comprehension Change Scores of Proficient and Less-Proficient Readers

T-tests for Equality of Means

95% Confidence
Mean Interval Sig.
Comprehension Test Difference of the Difference lvalue (2-tailed)
TORC-3 RCQ Change .55 -11.55 12.65 .10 .955

SF-6 Unit Skills Test Change .13 .04 .22 3.04 .008*

*p<.05,N=l7
61
Research Question Five

Is there a gain in reading achievement as measured by a comparison of the pre- and

post- assessments of written retellings of students who engage in retelling?

All students in the retelling (control) group were pre- and post-tested using the

Richness in Retelling Scale, a holistic 5-point measure with values ranging from 1 to 5

(low to high). The means and standards deviations for the change scores (post-test minus

pre-test) of the students in the retelling (control) group are presented on Table 6. On

average, the students improved the scores on their final written retellings by an average

of almost one point on the 5-point scale (M = .94).

Table 6

Means and Standard Deviations of Written Retelling Change Scores

Participants N Mean SD
All Retellers 17 0.94 1.14

Table 7 presents the results of a paired samples t test comparing the pre- and post-test

scores on the Richness in Retelling Scale for all students in the experimental group. The

pre-post reading comprehension change was statistically significant, t (16) - 3.39,

p - .004, and practically significant: the effect size (2.76-1.82)/.93 (pooled SD of both

scores) for the improvement on the written retelling measure was calculated to equal

1.01. The average comprehension score on the written retelling measure increased one

standard deviation.
62
Table 7

Change Scores on Written Retellings

Paired Samples T-tests ___^


95% Confidence
Mean Interval of the Sig.
Comparison Difference Difference /value (2-tailed)

Pre-and Post-Tests of All Retellers 0.19 0.07 0.31 3.39 .004*

*/?<.05,A r =17

Summary

Chapter 4 began with demographic data describing the participants in the study. This

information was followed with the data and statistical analyses used to answer each of the

research questions. A summary of the study, the findings, a discussion of the findings,

and recommendations for practice and future study will be presented in Chapter 5.
63
CHAPTER 5

Summary

This chapter provides a summary of the study, a report of the findings based on the

analysis of the data, the conclusions drawn from the findings, a discussion of the results

and conclusions, and recommendations for practice and future study.

Purpose of the Study

This study examined the effects of using oral and written retelling as a reading

comprehension instructional strategy on the reading achievement of two groups of

students in the sixth grade. Its purpose was twofold: first, to determine whether replacing

existing group comprehension instruction and individual workbook assignments with

retelling instruction and oral and written retelling practice would significantly impact the

reading comprehension achievement of middle school students, and second, to discover

whether the reading comprehension achievement of students at different levels of reading

capability (proficient and less-proficient) would be affected equally.

Research Questions

1. Is there a difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a

standardized reading test between students who engage in retelling and students who do

not?

2. Is there a difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a

curriculum-based unit test between students who engage in retelling and students who do

not?
64

3. Is there a difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a

standardized reading test between proficient and less-proficient readers who engage in

retelling?

4. Is there a difference in gains in reading achievement as measured by a

curriculum-based unit test between proficient and less-proficient readers who engage in

retelling?

5. Is there a gain in reading achievement as measured by a comparison of the pre-

and post-assessments of written retellings of students who engage in retelling?

Review of Related Literature and Research

Early in the 20th century, Huey (1908), Thorndike (1917), and Bartlett (1932)

conducted studies foundational to research and practice in American education; in part,

their work examined the psychology of reading, memory, and reading comprehension.

Many of their ideas lay dormant for almost 50 years, during which time educational

psychology and classroom practice was dominated by behaviorism (Anderson, 1995;

Sarroub & Pearson, 1998).

In the 1950s and 1960s, modern cognitive psychology emerged with a renewed

interest in the study of reading. Rumelhart (1975a), Wittrock (1974), and Kintsch and

Van Dijk (1983) theorized models of text comprehension to explain what might be

happening in the mind of a reader. Data collected from story retellings informed their

work. Mandler (1984) and other researchers built on Rumelhart's work to further study

story structure and its relationship to comprehension. Studies of story grammar, as it

came to be called, also relied heavily on retelling to gather data.


65
Eventually, interest in theories of reading comprehension translated into questions

about reading comprehension instruction in American classrooms. Durkin (1978/1979)

observed 300 hours of reading and social studies lessons in classroom settings and found

that almost no time was devoted to reading comprehension instruction. Many teachers

used workbooks and worksheets to "teach" comprehension and assumed that

comprehension and understanding of text automatically followed a child's acquisition of

beginning reading skills (Snow, 2002).

Durkin's study (1978/1979) prompted additional research into teaching and learning

about reading comprehension. Researchers recommended that teachers spend less time on

workbooks and literal-level questioning and more time on teaching comprehension

fostering strategies such as summarizing, dramatizing, and using advance organizers

(Pearson, 1985).

As researchers delved more deeply into the ways in which children make sense of

printed text, they often asked children to retell what they had read. Goodman (1982), for

example, examined children's retellings for many years beginning in the mid-1970s.

Frequent use of retelling as an assessment tool led to the realization that retelling might

also foster reading comprehension. Kalmbach (1980) studied retells he had elicited from

a group of students in sixth grade and began to encourage teachers to use retellings in

their classrooms. In the mid-1980s, Morrow (1982, 1985a, 1985b, 1986, 1988) completed

a series of retelling studies with young children and found that retelling helped them gain

oral language and comprehension skills. Gambrell and associates (1985) examined the

effectiveness of retelling as a reading comprehension strategy with children in the upper

elementary grades.
66
During the 1980s and 1990s, retelling became a commonly recommended reading

instructional strategy in the United States and in other countries (e.g. Brown and

Cambourne, 1987, in New Zealand). Moss (1993) recommended that retelling be used to

foster the comprehension of expository as well as narrative texts. Researchers found that

retelling was an effective strategy with struggling readers (e.g. Gillam & Carlile, 1997).

Unfortunately, in spite of the historic, scientific, and educational evidence supporting

the use of retelling as a comprehension instructional strategy, its use declined, and it is

not commonly used in present-day classrooms (Caldwell & Leslie, 2005). In an era of

accountability and high-stakes testing, many schools and teachers emphasize a basic

reading skills approach to the instruction of reading comprehension. New evidence is

needed to demonstrate the efficacy and practicality of retelling.

Methodology and Procedures

Reading comprehension achievement data were collected from 31 students in two

sixth-grade classrooms to evaluate the efficacy of an instructional intervention called

retelling. The students, stratified according to level of reading proficiency, were

randomly assigned to either the control (workbook) or the experimental (retelling) group.

Pre-test data were collected from all students using both a standardized {Test of Reading

Comprehension, third edition) and a curriculum-based measure (Scott Foresman Unit

Skills Test). Written retellings of the students in the experimental group were pre-

assessed using an additional measure of reading comprehension capability (Irwin and

Mitchell's Richness in Retelling Scale).

During the five-week intervention period, the control group (workbook) students'

reading instruction continued as usual. The students in the experimental (retelling) group
67
also carried on as usual except for the substitution of oral and written retelling instruction

and practice which replaced the customary comprehension instruction and workbook

assignments. Forty per cent of the weekly five hours of classroom reading instructional

time was allocated to the retelling intervention for students in the experimental group.

At the close of the intervention period, all students were post-tested using the TORC-

3 and the Scott Foresman Unit Test; the students in the experimental group also

completed a post-test written retelling which was evaluated using the Richness in

Retelling Scale.

T tests were used to determine whether there were statistically significant differences

between the reading comprehension achievement of the students in the control and

experimental groups following the intervention, whether there were statistically

significant differences between the achievement of the students in the proficient and less-

proficient reading groups, and whether the reading comprehension achievement of the

retelling students changed significantly as measured by their pre and post written

retellings. Findings were based on analyses of the data using a statistical significance

level of/? < .05. Effect sizes were calculated to determine the practical significance of the

retelling intervention.

Research Findings

The primary purpose of the study was to examine the efficacy of retelling as a

reading instructional strategy. Formal and informal reading and writing assessments were

used to measure the reading achievement of proficient and less-proficient readers, some

of whom used the retelling strategy and others of whom did not. Five research questions

guided the study. The following findings emerged from the analyses of the data collected:
68

1. On a standardized measure of reading comprehension, the difference in the mean

change score of the experimental group over the control group represented a medium

high effect size, and approached, but did not reach, statistical significance.

2. The difference in the mean change scores of the experimental and control groups

as measured by the curriculum-based unit test was not significant.

3. There were no statistically significant differences in gains in reading achievement

as measured on the standardized reading test between the proficient and less-proficient

retellers; the reading comprehension of both groups improved.

4. On the curriculum-based unit test, the less-proficient students in the experimental

group demonstrated a statistically significant gain in reading achievement over the

proficient readers in the experimental group.

5. Reading comprehension gains of the students in the experimental (retelling) group

as measured by a comparison of the pre- and post-assessments of their written retellings

showed statistical significance and a very large effect size.

Conclusions

The findings of the study led to a number of conclusions about the usefulness and

effectiveness of retelling as a strategy for teaching reading comprehension.

Generally, results from the Test of Reading Comprehension, third edition (TORC-3),

the standard measure of reading comprehension, indicate that retelling is an effective

reading comprehension strategy. When pre- and post-test scores are compared, retellers

show significant improvement while students in the workbook group do not. When the

mean change scores of the two groups are compared, the difference approaches, but does

not reach statistical significance. This difference does, however, reflect a large effect size.
69
The students in the retelling group gained, on average, approximately six-tenths of a

standard deviation more than their counterparts in the workbook group.

Retelling instruction did not improve the reading comprehension capabilities of the

retellers as measured on the Scott Foresman Reading Skills Test, the curriculum-based

assessment. On average, scores for both the retelling and the workbook groups decreased

on the Unit Skills post-test. The decrease was not statistically significant for either group,

nor was there a statistically significant difference when the change scores of the two

groups were compared.

Retelling is a beneficial reading instructional strategy for less-proficient readers.

There was no significant difference, as measured on the TORC-3, between the reading

achievement of the proficient and less-proficient retellers. The reading achievement of

both groups improved: M = 7.20 for the less-proficient readers and M= 7.75 for the

readers in the proficient group. Interestingly, the less-proficient readers in the retelling

group were the only students in the study whose scores improved on the Scott Foresman

Reading Skills Unit Test; following the intervention, the change in the scores differed

significantly (p = .008) from the proficient retellers.

Finally, the improvement in the scores of the students in the retelling group as

measured on the Richness in Retelling Scale indicate strongly that retelling is an effective

reading comprehension strategy. The mean change in the scores of the written retellings

were both statistically (p = .004) and practically (effect size = 1.01) significant.

Discussion

Retelling is a reading strategy that benefits children by improving their ability to

comprehend what they read. Students may be able to complete some workbook pages
70
purported to teach comprehension even if they have not read the associated text. They

cannot, however, retell what they do not know. In a few short, busy weeks during a

spring semester in a sixth grade classroom, students who learned about and practiced oral

and written retelling made significant and remarkable gains in reading comprehension.

Although it tells part of the story, a dispassionate analysis of the data does not reveal

the enthusiasm and eagerness the students demonstrated during the retelling intervention.

A student who missed a retelling session to go to a band lesson voluntarily returned later

to find out what she had missed; a boy who rarely borrows books from the school library

asked if The Phantom Tollbooth (Juster, 2005) might be available to be checked out.

After reading and retelling the first two chapters in class, he wanted to read the rest of the

story. Two weeks later, this same student recommended the book to a friend. The

students in the less-proficient retelling group loved the plot of the Mozart opera The

Magic Flute retold in a beautifully-illustrated picture book (Gatti, 2005). While writing

their retellings on a Friday morning, they listened to a sound recording of the opera and

repeatedly asked that the volume be increased.

The value of retelling as a classroom instructional strategy has been well

documented. Retelling has proven to be helpful to children in kindergarten (Morrow,

1985b), students in special education (Gillam & Carlile, 1997), and college

undergraduates (Hu, 1995). This study supports previous research that recommends

retelling as a comprehension instructional strategy. Teachers, however, sometimes find

retelling difficult and time-consuming to implement (Benson & Cummins, 2000;

Morrow, 1986). This study was specifically designed to examine whether retelling could

be applied in an existing classroom without the use of additional specialized materials or


71
techniques. This proved to be the case. The intervention did not disturb the ongoing

classroom routine, increase the cost of education, or require extra effort or time on the

part of the students. In many past studies, retelling as assessment or instruction has

utilized a one-to-one teacher-student arrangement (e.g. Gambrell, Pfeiffer, & Wilson,

1985; Moss, 1993), a structure which is not often available in classroom settings. In this

study, the retelling was done with a group of students who especially enjoyed the social

aspects of the intervention.

The study yielded many positive results and some that were unexpected. It was

encouraging to find that even though the improvement in test scores of the retellers did

not differ significantly from the workbook group on the standardized test, the effect size

was strong. In education, an inexpensive, easily-implemented intervention that raises

achievement by even a small effect size is considered to be of value (Coe, 2002). In this

case, the effect was large, and the improvement benefited students at different levels of

reading proficiency.

The retelling students' reading comprehension improvement, especially as assessed

by their written retellings, was noteworthy. It might therefore be expected that a similar

improvement would be evidenced by scores on the curriculum-based assessment. The

study did not show, however, that the increase in reading achievement generalized to the

classroom setting as measured by the Scott Foresman Unit Skills Test used to assess the

students at the close of the intervention period. The mean scores of students in both the

retelling and the workbook group decreased on the Unit Skills Test given after the

completion of the five-week unit.


72
It can be noted that students in the control group who participated in all classroom

reading comprehension instruction and completed all reading comprehension workbook

pages did no better on the unit test than their peers in the retelling group who were

exempt from the regular comprehension instruction and activities. While missing out on

the regular classroom comprehension instruction did not adversely affect the scores of the

retellers on the curriculum-based measure when compared to the control (workbook)

group, being part of the retelling group did not seem to help them either. A reading

strategy is of little value if it does not transfer to other applications or assessments of

reading comprehension (Anderson, 1992; Pressley et al., 1992).

The use of the Scott Foresman Unit Skills Tests was problematic. Validity and

reliability data are not available for the assessments. In addition, the tests, though made

up largely of comprehension questions, also assessed word attack and grammar skills. It

should be noted that the less-proficient retellers did improve on the Unit Test and their

improvement was significant when compared to the proficient retellers. This result is

encouraging, but in light of the problems associated with the curriculum-based

assessment, the result is also suspect. While it would be nice to think that the

improvement in the scores of the less-proficient retellers was due to the retelling

intervention, that conclusion cannot be stated with any certainty.

Whatever the shortcomings of the assessment tool, however, it is important for

students to transfer what they have learned to other settings. The lack of transfer and poor

performance on the Scott Foresman Test may perhaps be explained. On the assent form

presented to them before the study began, the students were told that their Unit Test

Scores would be part of the data collected during the study. There was little done during
73
the five-week intervention period, however, to promote the transfer of the retelling

strategy to other settings, in particular to the Unit Test, and the students received no

feedback from the researcher in regard to their test performance. Direct instruction may

be needed to help students learn to apply what they have learned about retelling to other

settings.

Reading comprehension and the teaching of strategies to increase comprehension are

important and enduring issues in literacy education. In 1941, Paul McKee recommended

that schools spend more time on the "thinking side of reading,. . . helping pupils . . .

understand clearly and correctly what they attempt to read" (p. 224). The current study

supports the use of retelling, an instructional strategy that does just that.

Recommendations

Given the limitations of the study, a number of implications can reasonably be

proposed.

Recommendations for Practice

1. Teachers of middle school students may begin to incorporate oral retelling into

their current reading instruction. Previous retelling experience on the part of the students

is not necessary, nor is it necessary to use specific materials, methods, or procedures.

2. Teachers of middle school students may also wish to implement written retellings.

In the current study, just one weekly written retelling over a period of five-weeks

significantly increased students' comprehension of story content and their written

reproduction of ideas and details from the stories they read.

3. Struggling readers should be included in oral and written retelling instruction and

assignments. There were no significant differences between the proficient and less-
74
proficient readers on the standardized comprehension test or the written retelling

assessment; on the curriculum-based evaluation, the less-proficient readers' scores were

significantly higher than their peers in the proficient reading group.

Recommendations for Further Study

1. The written retelling procedure used in the study was effective for both the

proficient and less-proficient readers in the retelling group. In the design of the current

study, there was no control group with which to compare the performance of the students

completing written retellings. A study with that additional element is recommended.

2. In future studies of retelling with middle school students, an effort should be

made to instruct participants to apply the strategy to other settings. The success of the

instruction should then be evaluated.

3. The study could be replicated with additional attention to the quality, validity, and

reliability of the curriculum-based comprehension measure. The validity of the current

study would have been improved if just the items directly measuring reading

comprehension had been considered in the data analysis.

4. A current topic in reading comprehension research is the importance of

motivation and attitude to success in reading (Block, Gambrell, & Pressley, 2002; Block

& Parris, 2008). Given the interesting observations of the motivation and enthusiastic

participation of individual students in the retelling group, it is recommended that case

studies of the effects of retelling on individual student's attitudes and reading

comprehension achievement be undertaken.

5. Nearly all texts used for oral and written retelling in the current study were

narrative. While there are articles recommending retelling as a strategy with


75
informational texts (Moss, Leone & Dipillo, 1997) and some research in this area has

been conducted, future additional studies should focus on expository texts and the

application of retelling procedures in content-area classes.

6. Pressley (2001) noted that reading comprehension strategies have "consistent and

striking benefits" when they are taught and practiced for a semester or an entire school

year. It is therefore recommended that additional retelling research using longer

intervention periods be undertaken. In addition, studies with larger groups of students in

diverse settings would add information about the efficacy of retelling as an instructional

strategy.

7. While retelling research has addressed students at various levels of age and

ability, there is little research using retelling as an instructional strategy with English

language learners. Such studies are needed and encouraged.

Summary

This chapter reviewed the purposes of the study, the related literature, the

methodology and procedures, and the major findings. It also presented conclusions, a

discussion of the findings and conclusions, and recommendations for practice and further

study.

"Reading is comprehension" (Cambourne, 1988, p. 158). Reading comprehension

strategies, including retelling, are important instructional elements in literacy teaching

and learning. Retelling is an effective way to help children increase their reading

comprehension capability.
76
References

Afflerbach, P., Beers, J. W., Blachowicz, C. L., Boyd, C. D., Cheyney, W., Diffily, D., et

al. (2004). Great expectations: Scott Foresman reading, grade 6. Glenview, IL:

Pearson Education, Inc.

American Psychological Association. (2001). Publication manual of the American

psychological association, (5l ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychological

Association.

Anderson, J. R. (1995). Cognitive psychology. New York: W. H. Freeman.

Anderson, R. C , Hiebert, E. H., Scott, J. A., & Wilkinson. (1985). Becoming a nation of

readers. Washington, DC: National Institute of Education.

Anderson, R. C , Wang, Q., & Gaffney, J. S. (2006). Comprehension research over the

past three decades. In K. A. Dougherty Stahl & M. C. McKenna (Eds.), Reading

research at work (pp. 275-315). New York: Guilford Press.

Anderson, V. (1992). A teacher development project in Transactional Strategy Instruction

for teachers of severely reading-disabled adolescents. Teaching and Teacher

Education, 5(4), 391-403.

Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study of experimental and social psychology.

Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Benson, V., & Cummins, C. (2000). The power of retelling: Developmental steps for

building comprehension. Chicago, IL: The Wright Group.

Block, C. C , Gambrell, L. B., & Presley, M. (Eds.). (2002). Improving comprehension

instruction: Rethinking research, theory, and classroom practice. San Francisco,

CA: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.


77
Block, C. C , & Parris, S. R. (Eds.). (2008). Comprehension instruction: Research-based

best practices (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.

Bos, C. S. & Vaughn, S. (1994). Strategies for teaching students with learning and

behavior problems. Needham Heights, MA: Paramount.

Britton, J. (1970). Language and learning. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann/Boynton.

Bromley, K. D. (1998). Language arts: Exploring connections (3rd ed.). Boston: Allyn &

Bacon.

Brown, H., & Cambourne, B. (1987). Read and retell. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

Brown, V. L., Hammill, D. D., & Wiederholt, J. L. (1995). Test of reading

comprehension: A methodfor assessing the understanding of written language (3r

ed). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

Caldwell, J. S., & Leslie, L. (2005). Intervention strategies to follow informal reading

inventory assessment: So what do I do now? Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson

Education.

Cambourne, B. (1988). The whole story: Natural learning and the acquisition of

literacy in the classroom. Auckland, NZ: Ashton Scholastic Limited.

Chafe, W. L. (Ed.). (1980). The pear stories: Cognitive, cultural, and linguistic aspects

of narrative production. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Clark, C. H. (1982). Assessing free recall. Reading Teacher, 35(4), 434-439.

Coe, R. (2002, September). It's the effect size stupid: What effect size is and why it is

important. Paper presented at the British Educational Research Association

annual conference, Exeter, England.

Cooper, E. (Ed.). (2004). When children love to learn: A practical application of


78
Charlotte Mason's philosophy for today. Wheaton, IL: Crossway Books.

D'Allessio, J. A. (1996). Retelling in the improvement of reading comprehension scores

of urban, lower socio-economic fourth graders. Unpublished manuscript, Kean

College of New Jersey. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED394135)

Doctorow, M., Wittrock, M. C , & Marks, C. (1978). Generative processes in reading

comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 70(2), 109-118.

Dreher, M. J., & Singer, H. (1980). Story grammar instruction unnecessary for

intermediate grade students. The Reading Teacher, 34(3), 261-268.

Durkin, D. (1978/1979). What classroom observations reveal about reading

comprehension instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 14(A), 481-533.

Durkin, D. (1981). Reading comprehension instruction in five basal reading series.

Reading Research Quarterly, 16(4), 515-544.

Early Literacy and Assessment for Learning (K-3) Series. (2005). Exploring

comprehension through retelling: A teacher's story. Honolulu, HI: Pacific

resources for Education and Learning (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No.

ED490189)

Flurkey, A. D., & Goodman, Y. M. (2004). The role of genre in a text: Reading through

the waterworks. Language Arts, 81(3), 233-244.

Gambrell, L. B., Pfeiffer, W. R., & Wilson, R. M. (1985). The effects of retelling upon

reading comprehension and recall of text information. Journal of Educational

Research, 78(4), 216-220.

Gambrell, L, B., Koskinen, P. S., & Kapinus, B. A. (1991). Retelling and the reading

comprehension of proficient and less-proficient readers. Journal of Educational


79
Research, 84(6), 356-362.

Gatti, A. (2005). The magic flute. San Francisco: Chronicle Books.

Geist, E. A., & Boydston, R. C. (2002). The effect of using written retelling as a teaching

strategy on students' performance on the TOWL-2. Journal of Instructional

Psychology 29(2), 108-117.

Gillam, R. B., & Carlile, R. M. (1997). Oral reading and story retelling of students with

specific language impairment. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools,

28(1), 30-42.

Glazer, S. M , & Brown, C. S. (1993). Portfolios and beyond: Collaborative assessment

in reading and writing. Norwood, MA: Christopher-Gordon Publishers, Inc.

Golden, J. M., & Pappas, C. C. (1990). A sociolinguistic perspective on retelling

procedures in research on children's cognitive processing of written text.

Linguistics and Education, 2(21), pp. 21-41.

Goldman, S. R., & Rakestraw, J. A. (2000). Structural aspects of constructing meaning

from text. In M. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook

of reading research (Vol. 3, pp. 311-336). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Goodman, Y. M. (1982). Retellings of literature and the comprehension process. Theory

Into Practice, 21(A), 301-307.

Goodman, Y. M., & Burke, C. L. (1972). Reading miscue inventory: Manual and

procedures for diagnosis and evaluation. New York: MacMillan.

Goodman, Y. M., Watson, D. J., & Burke, C. L. (1987). Reading miscue inventory:

Alternative procedures. New York: Richard C. Owen Publishers, Inc.

Griffith, P. L., Ripich, D. N., & Dastoli, S. L. (1986). Story structure, cohesion, and
80
propositions in story recalls by learning-disabled and nondisabled children.

Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 15(6), 539-555.

Hansen, C. L. (1978). Story retelling used with average and learning disabled readers as

a measure of reading comprehension. Learning Disability Quarterly, 7(3), 62-29.

Hansen, J. (2004). "7e// me a story:" Developmentally appropriate retelling strategies.

Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

Hilden, K. R., & Pressley, M. (2007). Self-regulation through transactional strategies

instruction. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 23(1), 51-75.

Houge, T. T. (2000). Written retellings of narrative and expository texts: A case study of

elementary primary grade delayed male readers. Ed.D. dissertation, University of

Nevada, Las Vegas, United States - Nevada. Retrieved January 30, 2008, from

ProQuest Digital Dissertations database. (Publication No. AAT 9973958)

Hu, J. (1995). Bringing written retelling into an ESL writing class. Journal of

Developmental Education, 79(1), 12-15.

Huey, E. B. (1968). The psychology and pedagogy of reading. Cambridge, MA:

The M.I.T. Press. (Original work published in 1908)

Hoyt, L. (1999). Revisit, reflect, retell: Strategies for improving reading comprehension.

Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

Impara, J. C , & Plake, B. S. (Eds.). (1998). The thirteenth mental measurements

yearbook. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

Irwin, P. A., & Mitchell, J. N. (1983). A procedure for assessing the richness of

retellings. Journal of Reading, 26(5), 391-396.

John, S. F., Lui, M., & Tannock., R. (2003). Children's story retelling and comprehension
81
using a new narrative resource. Canadian Journal of School Psychology, 18 (1/2),

91-113.

Johnson, N. S., & Mandler, J. M. (1980). A tale of two structures: underlying and surface

forms in stories. Poetics, 9(1), 51-96.

Johnston, P. H. (1983). Reading comprehension assessment: A cognitive basis. Newark,

DE: International Reading Association.

Juster, N. (2005). The phantom tollbooth. New York: Yearling.

Kalmbach, J. R. (1980). The structure of narrative retelling. Houghton, MI: Michigan

Technological University. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED216317)

Kalmbach, J. R. (1986a). Getting at the point of retellings. Journal of Reading, 29(4),

326-333.

Kalmbach, J. R. (1986b). Evaluating informal methods for the assessment of retellings.

Journal of Reading, 30(2), 119-127.

Kapinus, B. A., Gambrell, L., & Koskinen, P. S. (1987). Effects of practice in retelling

upon the reading comprehension of proficient and less-proficient readers. In J.

Readance & R. S. Baldwin (Eds.), Research in Literacy: Merging Perspectives (pp.

135-141). Rochester, NY: National Reading Conference.

Kintsch, W. (2005). An overview of top-down and bottom-up effects in comprehension:

The CI perspective. Discourse Processes, 39(2/3), 125-128.

Kintsch, W., & van Dijk, T.A. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension. New

York: Academic Press.

Koskinen, P. S., Gambrell, L. B., & Kapinus, B. A. (1989). The effects of repeated
82
reading and retelling on young children's' reading comprehension. In S. McCormick

& J. Zutell (Eds.), Cognitive and social perspectives for literacy research and

instruction, (pp. 233-240). Chicago, IL: National Reading Conference.

Koskinen, P. S., Gambrell, L. B., Kapinus, B. A., & Heathington, B. S. (1988).

Retelling: A strategy for enhancing students' reading comprehension. The

Reading Teacher, 41(9), 892-896.

Kuldanek, K. (1998). The effects of using a combination of story frames and retelling

strategies with learning disabled students to build their comprehension ability.

Unpublished master's thesis, Kean University, Union, New Jersey. (ERIC Document

Reproduction Service No. ED416469)

Linden, M., & Wittrock, M. C. (1981). The teaching of reading comprehension according

to the model of generative learning. Reading Research Quarterly, 17(1), 44-57.

Mandler, J. M. (1984). Stories, scripts, and scenes: Aspects of schema theory. Hillsdale,

NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Mandler, J. M., & Johnson, M. J. (1977). Remembrance of things parsed: Story

structure and recall. Cognitive Psychology, 9(1), 111-151.

Martin, N. (1972). Children and stories: Their own and other people's. English in

Education, 6(2), 43-54.

Martinez, M. G., & McGee, L. M. (2000). Children's literature and reading instruction:

Past, present, and future. Reading Research Quarterly, 35(1), 154-169.

Marshall, N. (1983). Using story grammar to assess reading comprehension. The Reading

Teacher, 36(7), 16-20.

McKee, P. (1941). The problem of meaning in reading. English Journal, 30(3), 219-224.
83
Meredith, K. E., Mitchell, J. N., & Hernandez-Miller, M. E. (1992). Psychometric

properties of the reader retelling profile: A case study. In C. K. Kinzer & D. J.

Leu (Eds.). Literacy Research, Theory, and Practice: Views from Many

Perspectives (pp. 129-136). Chicago: National Reading Conference, Inc.

Morrow, L. M. (1985a). Reading and retelling stories: Strategies for emergent readers.

The Reading Teacher, 38(9), 870-875.

Morrow, L. M. (1985b). Retelling stories: A strategy for improving young children's

comprehension, concept of story structure, and oral language complexity. The

Elementary School Journal, 85(5), 647-661.

Morrow, L. M. (1986). Effects of structural guidance in story retelling on children's

dictation of original stories. Journal of Reading Behavior, 18(2), 135-152.

Morrow, L. M. (1988). Retelling stories as a diagnostic tool. In S. M. Glazer, L. W.

Searfoss, & L. M. Gentile (Eds.). Reexamining reading diagnosis: New trends and

procedures. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

Morrow, L. M. (1996). Story retelling: A discussion strategy to develop and assess

comprehension. In L. B. Gambrell & J. F. Almasi (Eds.), Lively discussions!

Fostering engaged reading. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

Morrow, L. M. (2005). Literacy development in the early years: Helping children read

and write (5th ed.). Boston: Pearson Education.

Morrow, L. M., Gambrell, L., Kapinus, B., Koskinen, P. S., Marshall, N., & Mitchell, J.

N. (1986). Retelling: A strategy for reading instruction and assessment. In J. A.


84
Niles and R. V. Lalik (Eds.), Solving problems in literacy: Learners, teachers,

and researchers. Thirty-fifth yearbook of the National Reading Conference (pp.

73-80). Rochester, NY: National Reading Conference.

Morrow, L. M , Tracey, D. H., Wood, D. G., & Pressley, M. (1999). Characteristics of

exemplary first-grade literacy instruction. The Reading Teacher, 52(5), 462-476.

Moss, B. (1993, December). Using retellings to assess children's comprehension of

expository text. Paper presented at the meeting of the National Reading Conference,

Charleston, SC.

Moss, B. (1997). A qualitative assessment of first graders' retelling of expository text.

Reading Research and Instruction, 37(1), 1-13.

Moss, B. (2004). Teaching expository text structures through information trade book

retellings. The Reading Teacher, 57(8), 710-718.

Moss, B., Leone, S., & DiPillo, M. L. (1997). Exploring the literature of fact: Linking

reading and writing through information trade books. Language Arts, 74(6), 418-

419.

National Reading Panel. (2000). Report of the subgroups: National Reading Panel.

Washington, DC: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.

Nezworski, T., Stein, N. L., & Trabasso, T. (1982). Story structure versus content in

children's recall. Journal of verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21(2), 196-206.

O'Donnell, R., Griffin, W. J., & Norris, R. C. (1967). Syntax of Elementary School

Children: A Transformational Analysis (NCTE Research Report No. 8). Urbana, IL:

National Council of Teachers of English.

Oregon State Department of Education. (1998-1999, 1999-2000). Reading official


85
scoring guide, oral reading fluency: Third grade benchmark. (Office of

Assessment and Evaluation). Salem, OR.

Palincsar, A. S., & Brown, A. L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension-fostering

and monitoring activities. Cognition and Instruction, 7(2), 117-175.

Pappas, C. C. & Brown, E. (1987). Young children learning story discourse: Three case

studies. The Elementary SchoolJournal, 57(4), 455-466.

Paris, A. H., & Paris, S. G. (2003). Assessing narrative comprehension in young children.

Reading Research Quarterly, 38(1), 36-76.

Pearman, C. J. (2008). Independent reading of CD-ROM storybooks: Measuring

comprehension with oral retellings. The Reading Teacher, 61(8), 594-602.

Pearson, D. P. (1985). Changing the face of reading comprehension instruction. The

Reading Teacher, 38(3), 724-238.

Piaget, J. (1974). The language and thought of the child. New York: Meridian Book.

(Original work published in 1926)

Pressley, M. (2001). Comprehension instruction: What makes sense now, what might

make sense soon. Reading Online, 5(2). Retrieved June 1, 2008, from

http://www.readingonline.org/articles/handbook/pressley/index.html

Pressley, M. (2002). Comprehension strategies instruction: A turn-of-the century status

Report. In C. C. Block & M. Pressley (Eds.), Comprehension instruction: Research-

based best practices (pp. 11-27). New York: Guilford Press.

Pressley, M., El-Dinary, P. B., Gaskins, I., Schuder, T., Bergman, J., Almasi, L., et al.

(1992). Beyond direct explanation: Transactional instruction of reading

comprehension strategies. Elementary SchoolJournal, 92(5), 511-554.


86
Pressley, M., Wharton-McDonald, R., Mistretta-Hampston, J., & Echevarria, M. (1998).

Literacy instruction in 10 fourth-and fifth-grade classrooms in upstate New York.

Scientific Studies of Reading, 2(2), 159-194.

Robnolt, V. J. (2004) Predicting reading comprehension through macro and micro

analyses of oral story retellings. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Virginia, United

States - Virginia. Retrieved July 2, 2008, from Dissertations & Theses: Full Text

database. (Publication No. AAT 3131436)

Rhodes, J. A., & Milby, T. M. (2007). Teacher-created electronic books: Integrating

technology to support readers with disabilities. The Reading Teacher, 61(3), 255-

259.

Rhodes, L. K., & Shanklin, N. L. (1993). Windows into literacy: Assessing learners, K-8.

Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

Rose, M. C , Cundick, B. P., & Higbee, K. L. (1983). Verbal rehearsal and visual

imagery: Mnemonic aids for learning-disabled children. Journal of Learning

Disabilities, 16(6), 352-354.

Rosen, H. (1986). The importance of story. Language Arts, 63(3), 226-237.

Rosen, H. (1988). Stories of stories: Footnotes on sly, gossipy practices. In M. Lightfoot

& N. Martin (Eds.). The word for teaching is learning (pp. 195-205). Portsmouth,

NH: Boynton/Cook Publishers.

Rosenblatt, L. (1978). The reader, the text, and the poem: The transactional theory of

the literary work. Carbondale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.

Rosenshine, B., & Meister, C. (1994). Reciprocal teaching: A review of the research.

Review of Educational Research, 64(4), 479-530.


87
Rowe, D. W. (1986). Does research support the use of "purpose questions" on reading

comprehension tests? Journal of Educational Measurement, 23(1), 43-55.

Rumelhart, D. (1975a). Notes on a schema for stories. In D. Bobrow & A. Collins

(Eds.). Representation and understanding; Studies in cognitive science. New

York: Academic Press.

Rumelhart, D. (1975b). Understanding and summarizing brief stories. In D. L. LaBerge

& S. J. Samuels (Eds.). Basic process in reading: perception and comprehension.

Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

Rumelhart, D. (1984). Schemata and the cognitive system. In Handbook of Social

Cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates, 161-188.

Sarroub, L., & Pearson, P. D. (1998). Two steps forward, three steps back: The stormy

history of reading comprehension assessment. Clearing House, 72(2), 97-105.

Sears, L. (2007). Edward Lee Thorndike (1874-1949): A look at his contributions to

learning and reading. In S. E. Israel & E. J. Monaghan (Eds.). Shaping the

reading field: The impact of early reading pioneers, scientific research, and

progressive ideas (pp. 119-139). Newark, DE: International Reading Association,

Inc.

Shake, M. C , & Allington, R. L. (1985). Where do teachers' questions come from? The

Reading Teacher, 38(4), 432-437.

Shannon, P., Kameenui, E. J., & Baumann, J. F. (1988). An investigation of children's

ability to comprehend character motives. American Educational Research

Journal, 25(3), 441-462.

Shaw, D. (2005). Retelling strategies to improve comprehension. New York: Scholastic,


88
Inc.

Sisco, L. J. (1992). The effect of mediated story retelling on the development of listening

comprehension, story structure and oral language in young children with learning

disabilities. Ed.D. dissertation, Rutgers The State University of New Jersey - New

Brunswick, United States - N e w Jersey. Retrieved March 14, 2007, from ProQuest

Digital Dissertations database. (Publication No. AAT 9221259)

Snow, C. (2002). Reading for understanding: Toward an R&D program in reading

comprehension. Santa Monica, CA: Rand Corporation.

Stein, N. L., & Glenn, C. J. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension in elementary

school children. In R. Freedle (Ed.), New directions in discourse processes (Vol.

2). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Steinruck, Y. S. (1978, March). The retelling technique: An avenue for investigating

and enhancing comprehension. Paper presented at the 9l annual meeting of the

Illinois Reading Council of the International Reading Association, Charleston, IL.

(ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. EDI51775).

Taylor, B. M., Pearson, P. D., Garcia, G. E., Dougherty Stahl, K. A., & Bauer, E. B.

(2006). Improving students' reading comprehension. In K.A. Dougherty Stahl & M.

C. McKenna (Eds.), Reading research at work: Foundations of effective practice

(pp. 303-315). New York: The Guilford Press.

Thompson, J. L. (1990). The effects of free recall and cued recall on sixth-grade readers'

comprehension of expository text. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Maryland

College Park, United States - Maryland. Retrieved February 27, 2008, from

ProQuest Digital Dissertations database. (Publication No. AAT 9121442).


89
Thorndike, E. L. (1917). Reading as reasoning: A study of mistakes in paragraph

reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 8, 323-332.

Tucker, R. (2001). Story retelling: The effect of guided practice on kindergarten

children's ability to retell stories. Ed.D. dissertation, University of Pittsburgh,

United States - Pennsylvania. Retrieved March 11, 2007, from ProQuest Digital

Dissertations database. (Publication No. AAT 3013352).

Valencia, S. & Pearson, P. D. (1987). Reading assessment: Time for a change. Reading

Teacher, 20(8), 726-732.

Van Dongen, R. (1987). Children's narrative thought, at home and at school. Language

Arts, 64(1), 79-87.

Villaume, S. K., & Brabham, E. G. (2003). Comprehension instruction: Beyond

strategies. The Reading Teacher, 55(7), 672-675.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and Language (A, Kozulin, Trans.). Cambridge, MA:

The MIT Press. (Original work published 1934)

Whaley, J. (1981). Story grammars and reading instruction. The Reading Teacher, 34(1),

762-771.

Wilson, R., & Cleland, C. (1985). Diagnostic and remedial reading (3 ed.). Columbus:

Charles E. Merrill Publishing Co.

Wilde, S. (2000). Miscue analysis made easy. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

Wittrock, M. C. (1974). Learning as a generative process. Educational Psychologist,

11(2), 87-95.

Wittrock, M. C. (1981). Reading comprehension. In F. J. Pirozzolo & M. C. Wittrock


90
(Eds.), Neuropsychological and cognitive processes in reading. New York:

Academic Press, 1981.

Wood, K. D., & Jones, J. (1998). Flexible grouping and group retellings include

struggling learners in classroom communities. Preventing School Failure, 43(1), 37-

38.

Zimiles, H. L., & Kuhns, M. (1976). A developmental study of the retention of narrative

material, final report (Research Report 134, Washington, DC, National Institute of

Education), New York: Bank Street College of Education.


91

Appendix A

USD Office of Human Subjects Protection Exempt Review and Approval


92

From • "Langstraat. Deb L" <Deb.Langstraat@usd.edii>


Sent Friday, February 22, 2008 4:14 pm
To Donna.Johnson(a),usd.edu , "Richardson. Maurine" <Maurine.Richaidson(a>usd.edu>
Cc
Bcc
Subject IRB Letter Richardson-Johnson

Project Title: 2008.018-The Effects of Substituting Retelling for the Basal


Reader Comprehension Instruction on Students in a Sixth Grade
Reading Program
PI: Maurine Richardson Student PI: Donna Johnson
Level of Review: Exempt 1&2 Risk: No More than Minimal
Date Approved: 2/22/2008

The proposal referenced above has received an Exempt review and approval via the
procedures of the University of South Dakota Institutional Review Board 01.

Annual Continuing Review is not required for the above Exempt study. However, when
this study is completed you must submit a Closure Form to the IRB. You may close your
study when you no longer have contact with the subject.

Prior to initiation, promptly report to the IRB, any proposed changes or additions (e.g.,
protocol amendments/revised informed consents/ site changes, etc.) in previously
approved human subject research activities.

The forms to assist you in filing your: project closure, continuation,


adverse/unanticipated event, project updates /amendments, etc. can be accessed at
http://www.usd.edu/oorsch/compliance/applicationforms.cfm.

This e-mail serves as your official IRB response. If you have any questions, please
contact: deb.lanqstraat@usd.edu or lisa.korcuska@usd.edu or (605) 677-6184.

Sincerely,

Deb Langstraat, IRB Coordinator


The University of South Dakota
Office of Human Subjects Protection
AAHRPP Full Accreditation since 2005
http://www.usd.edu/oorsch/compliance/
Ph: 605.677.6184
Fax: 605.677.3134
93

Appendix B

Communication with School District:

Presentation to School Board

& Permission to Conduct Study


94

January 29, 2008

Board of Education
Xx Xxxx Public School
XXX XXth St., P.O. Box XXX
Xx Xxxx, XX XXXXX

RE: READING RESEARCH PROJECT

Dear Member of the Board of Education:

I am pleased to present information about my reading research project. I


have enjoyed working with your faculty, students, and staff in the past and
look forward to interacting with the sixth grade students and their teachers in
the upcoming weeks.

In every school I visit, I see great concern about effective reading


instruction. Much time and effort is given to ensure that each child learns to
read. My research project is directed at answering questions I have about the
best ways to help children comprehend what they read.

Teaching reading and encouraging children to become life-long readers is my


passion as an educator. To that end, I am continuing my education at The
University of South Dakota. I have recently completed the coursework
needed to become a reading specialist. The study of reading comprehension I
complete with the help of your sixth grade teachers and students will be the
basis for my doctoral dissertation.

If you require further information or have questions concerning this study,


you may contact my advisor, Dr. Maurine Richardson (605-677-6221), or the
Institutional Review Board (605-677-6184) at The University of South
Dakota.

Sincerely,

Donna M.Johnson
806 Highland Road
Windom, MN 56101
95

Xxxxxxxx Xxxx Public Schools


• Superintendent /7-12 Principal - Xxxx Xxxxx
• E-6 Principal - Xxxxxx Xxxxxxxx
• 7-12 Dean of Students - Xxxx Xxxxxxxxxxx
• K-12 Counselor - Xxxx Xxxx

Phone: (xxx) xxx-xxxx


Fax: (xxx) xxx-xxxx Elementary
Fax: (xxx) xxx-xxxxx High School

XXX XXth Street, P. O. Box XXX


Xxxxxxxx Xxxxx, XX XXXXX

February 4, 2008

Donna M. Johnson
806 Highland Road
Windom,MN 56101

Dear Ms. Johnson:

This letter is in reference to your request to conduct a research study at the Xxxxxxxx
Xxxx Public Elementary School.

After discussing it with the Superintendent, Xxxx Xxxxx, and informing the School
Board members at the January 2008 meeting, it is with great pleasure that the Xxxxxxxx
Xxxx Public Elementary School accepts your request to pursue your research within our
sixth grade classes.

We look forward to an informative and enjoyable experience.

Sincerely,

Xxxxxx Xxxxxxxx
Principal

XXX:xx
96

Appendix C

USD Office of Human Subjects Protection Documentation:

Request for Parental Consent (English & Spanish)


97
Informed Parental Consent for Child's Participation in Research
The University of South Dakota
Title: The Effects of Substituting Retelling for the Basal Reader Comprehension
Instruction on Students in a Sixth Grade Reading Program

Project Directors: Dr. Maurine Richardson, Mrs. Donna Johnson


School of Education: Curriculum & Instruction
Telephone: 605-677-6221

Dear Parent/Legal Guardian,

Your child is invited to be part of a research study comparing ways of teaching reading
comprehension. He/she was selected as a possible participant because she/he is in one
of the sixth grade reading classes at the Mt. Lake Public School.

WHAT IS THE PURPOSE OF THIS STUDY?


The purpose of this study is to see how using a reading comprehension strategy called
retelling compares to the current comprehension instruction being used by the sixth
grade reading groups.

HOW LONG WILL MY CHILD BE IN THIS STUDY?


Your child's participation in the study will last for five weeks. All the students in both sixth
grade reading groups (approximately 40 students) are being invited to participate in the
study which will take place for ten weeks (five weeks for each reading group) during the
2007-2008 spring semester.

WHAT WILL HAPPEN DURING THIS STUDY?


The students in your child's reading group will be randomly divided into two groups. If
your child is in the regular reading group, there will be no change in his/her reading
instruction. If your child is in the retelling group, he/she will receive his/her regular reading
instruction from the classroom teacher except for reading comprehension activities. For
approximately 20 minutes four days a week, the retelling students will participate in
retelling activities in the computer room adjacent to the sixth grade classrooms. Mrs.
Johnson will be the teacher for the retelling activities.

Most of the retelling activities will be oral. For example, the students may be asked to
retell the story from their reading book to a partner, describe the main problem in the
story and say how it was solved, or tell how the story reminds them of another story.
Some retellings will be written. For example, the students may be asked to write a letter
from one character in the story to another character, or write an email to a friend telling
them about the story,
The scores from the unit reading tests as well as a group pre- and post-test will be used in
the study.

There are no foreseeable risks to the participants of the study. Your child will not
necessarily benefit from being in this study, but the information gathered may benefit
teachers and students in the future. There will be no cost to the participants. The
University of South Dakota and the research team are receiving no payments from other
agencies, organizations, or companies to conduct this research study.

If you and your child choose not to participate in the study, your child's reading
instruction will continue as usual throughout the spring semester.
98
WILL INFORMATION ABOUT MY CHILD BE KEPT CONFIDENTIAL?
The records of this study will be kept private to the extent permitted by law. If a report
about this study is published, no participants will be identified. Your child's study record
may be reviewed by government agencies, USD's Research Compliance Office, and
The University of South Dakota Institutional Review Board (IRB). An IRB is a group of people
who review the research to protect the rights and welfare of you and your child.

Any information that is obtained in connection with this study and that can be identified
with your child will remain confidential and will be disclosed only with your permission or
as required by law. Confidentiality will be maintained by means of random numbers
assigned to e a c h participating student; names will not be used. The numbers and
associated student names will be kept in a secured location separate from the data that
is collected.

Your child's participation is voluntary. Your child may choose not to participate or may
discontinue participation at any time without penalty. The decision whether or not to
participate will not affect you or your child's current or future relations with the Mt. Lake
Public School or The University of South Dakota.

WHAT IF I HAVE QUESTIONS?


You may ask any questions you have now or later. The researchers conducting this study
are: Dr. Maurine Richardson (605-677-6221) and Mrs. Donna Johnson (507-831-2157). You
may call these numbers if you have questions, concerns, or complaints about the
research.

If you have questions regarding your rights as a research subject, you may contact The
University of South Dakota Institutional Review Board (IRB) at (605) 677-6184. You may also
call this number about any problems, complaints, or concerns you have about this
research study, if you cannot reach research staff, or if you wish to talk with someone
who is independent of the research team. General information about being a research
subject c a n be found by clicking "Information for Research Participants" on the web site:
http://www.usd.edu/oorsch/compliance/participants.cfm.

Please initial, sign, and return this letter to the school in the enclosed envelope:
Yes, I give permission for my child to participate in the reading comprehension
study.
No, I do not give permission for my child to participate in the reading
comprehension
study.

Your signature indicates that this research study has been explained to you, that your
questions have been answered, and that you voluntarily agree to permit your child to
take part in this study. You will receive a copy of this form.

Child's Name:

Signature of Person Authorized to Provide Permission for the Child Date


99

Consentimiento informado paternal para la participacion del nino


en la investigacion La Universidad d e Dakota del Sur

Tftulo: Los Efectos de Sustituir Contar Otra Vez por la Instruction de la Comprension del
Lector Basal en los Estudiantes en un Programa de la Lectura del Nivel de Sexto Grado
Directoras del proyecto: La Dra. Maurine Richardson, La Sra. Donna Jonson
Departemento de la Educacion: Curfculo e Instruccion Telefono: 605-677-6221

Querido Padre/Guardian Legal,


Se invita a su niho/a que este en un estudio de la investigacion comparando los
metodos de la ensenanza de la comprension de la lectura. Seleccionaron a su nino/a
como participante posiPle porque su nino/a es alumno/a en uno de las closes de la
lectura del sexto grado en la escuela Mt. Lake Public School.
eCUAL ES EL PROPOSITO DE ESTA INVESTIGACION?
El proposito de esta investigacion es para comparar la estrategia de la comprension de
la lectura que se llama "contar otra vez" con Id instruccion de la comprension de la
lectura que actualmente se realiza en los grupos de la lectura del sexto grado.
j-CUANTO TIEMPO ESTARA Ml NINO EN LA INVESTIGACION?
La participacion de su niho/a sera cinco semanas. Se invitan a todos los alumnos en los
dos grupos de la lectura del sexto grado (aproximadamente cuarenta alumnos) para
participar en la investigacion que durard por diez semanas (cinco semanas para c a d a
grupo de la lectura) durante el semestre de la primavera en el ano escolar 2007 a 2008.
2QUE PASARA DURANTE LA INVESTIGACION?
Se dividirdn en dos grupos al azar a los alumnos en el grupo de la lectura de su niho/a.
Si se selecciona a su niho/a estar en el grupo normal de la lectura, no habra cambio de
ia instruccion de la lectura para el/ella. Si se selecciona a su niho/a estar en el grupo de
la lectura de contar otra vez, el/ella recibird sus instrucciones de la lectura como
siempre del maestro de la clase a menos que para las actividades de la comprension
de la lectura. Por aproximadamente veinte minutos por cuatro dfas a la semana, los
alumnos seleccionados para contar otra vez participardn en las actividades de contar
otra vez en el aula designada para computes, la cual que queda al lado de las aulas
del sexto grado. La Sehora Johnson sera la maestra para las actividades de contar otra
vez.
La mayoria de las actividades de contar otra vez serdn oral. Por ejemplo, se piden a los
alumnos de contar otra vez el cuento de su libro de la lectura a su compahero,
describiendo el problema principal del cuento y diciendo c o m o se resolvio, o decir
como el cuento se le hace recordar otro cuento. Algunas veces contaran otra vez en
una forma escrita. Por ejemplo, daran instrucciones a los alumnos para escribir una
carta de uno de los personajes del cuento al otro personaje, o quizds escribir una carta
electronica a un amigo contdndole cosas del cuento. Las notas de los exdmenes de la
unidad de la lectura tal como los exdmenes antes y despues de la unidad se usardn
para calcular las notas en la investigacion.
No se ve ningun riesgo a los participantes de esta investigacion. Su niho/a no
necesariamente beneficiard de estar en esta investigacion, pero la informacion y los
datos acumulados beneficiaran a los maestros y a los alumnos del future No habrd
costo ninguno a los participantes. La Universidad de Dakota del Sur y el equipo de la
investigacion no recibira ningun beneficio economico sea de otras agendas, otras
organizaciones, ni de las compahfas para realizar esta investigacion.
100
Si listed y/o su nino/a elige no participar en esta investigacion, la instruccidn de la
lectura de su nino/a continuard como normal durante el semestre de la primavera.
gSE MANTIENE PRIVADO LA INFORMACION DE Ml NINO/A?
Los expedientes de este estudio serdn conservados privados hasta lo permitida por la
ley. En ningun informe sobre este estudio que pueda ser publicado, a su nino/a no
identificardn. El expediente del estudio de su nino/a se puede repasar por las agencias
del gobiemo, los USD de la investigacion de la oficina de la conformidad, y la
universidad de los comites examinadores Dakota-lnstitucionales de Sur. El comite
examinador es un grupo de personas quienes revisan las investigaciones para asegurar
la proteccion de las derechas y el bienestar de usted y de su niho/a.
Cualquier informacion que se obtenga con respeto a este estudio y que se pueda
identificar con su nino/a seguira siendo confidencial y sera divulgada solamente con su
permiso o segun los requisitos de la ley. La confidencialidad sera conservada por
medio de un numero escogido al azar asignado a c a d a alumno que participa. Usar
este metodo significa que no hay nombres implicados. Los nombres y los numeros del
estudiante serdn bloqueados guardado adentro asegurado en localizaciones
separadas.
La participacion de su nino/a es voluntaria. Su nino puede elegir no participar o puede
elegir descontinuar participar en cualquier momento sin penalidad. La decision si o no
participar no le afectara relaciones actuales ni futuras con la Mt. Lake Public School ni la
Universidad de Dakota del Sur.
gQUE HAGO SI TENGO PREGUNTAS?
Usted puede hacer cualquier pregunta que usted tenga ahora o mas adelante. Los
investigadoras que conducen esta investigacion son: La Dra. Maurine Richardson (605-
677-6221) y la Sra. Donna Jonson (507-831-2157). Usted puede llamar estos numeros si
usted tiene preguntas, preocupaciones, o quejas sobre la investigacion.
Si usted tiene preguntas con respeto a sus derechas como personas que participan en
el estudio de la investigacion, usted puede entrar en contacto con la universidad del
comite examinador Dakota-lnstitucional del Sur (IRB) en (605) 677-6184. Usted puede
tambien llamar este numero para decirles sobre cualquier problema, quejas, o
preocupacion que usted tenga sobre este proyecto de la investigacion, si usted no
puede comunicar con nadie del equipo de la investigacion, o si usted quiere hablar con
alguien que sea independiente del equipo de la investigacion. Informacion general
sobre ser participante en una investigacion se puede encontrar al seleccionar
"Information for Research Participants" (La informacion para los participantes de la
investigacion) en la direccion de la red:
http://www.usd.edu/oorsch/compliance/participants.cfm.
Por favor firme y devuelva esta carta a la escuela en el sobre proveido:
Sf, Yo doy permiso para que mi nino/a participe en las investigaciones de la
comprension de la lectura.
No, Yo no doy permiso para que mi nino/a no participe en las investigaciones de
la comprension de la lectura.
Su firma indica que este estudio de la investigacion se ha explicado a usted, que se nan
contestado sus preguntas, y que usted acuerda voluntariamente permitir que su nino/a
participe en este estudio. Usted recibira una fotocopia de esta forma.
Nombre del Niho/a

firma de padre/madre/curaduria fecha


101

Appendix D

USD Office of Human Subjects Protection Documentation:

Request for Student Assent


102
Informed Assent for Student's Participation in Research
The University of South Dakota

Title: The Effects of Substituting Retelling for the Basal Reader Comprehension Instruction
on Students in a Sixth Grade Reading Program

Project Directors: Dr. Maurine Richardson, Mrs. Donna Johnson


School of Education: Curriculum & Instruction
Telephone: 605-677-6221

Dear Student,

We are doing a research study. A research study is a special way to find out about something.
We are trying to find out if retelling what they read helps middle school students with their reading
comprehension.

If you want to be in this study, we will ask you to do several things:

1. Be assigned to one of two groups for reading comprehension lessons during reading
class. If you are randomly assigned to be in Group 1, your reading lessons for Unit 5 will
be the same as they always are. If you are randomly, assigned to Group 2, your lessons
will be the same, except you won't do any of the regular reading comprehensions
workbook pages or worksheets. Instead, you and others in your group will do oral and
written retellings of your stories with Mrs. Johnson.
2. Your test scores on the Unit Test you usually take will be used for the study.
3. Everyone in the study will take a pre and post-test called the Tesf of Reading
Comprehension-3 (TORC-3) and the scores will be used in the study. The TORC-3 is a
group test and takes about an hour.
4. If you are in Group 2, scores from two of your written retellings will be used in the study.

We hope that by doing this study, teachers and students in the future will know more about
reading comprehension and how to teach it. You may or may not benefit from being in the study.

When we are done with the study, we will write a report about what we learned. We will not use
your name in the report.

You do not have to be in this study. It is up to you. If you decide to be in the study, but change
your mind, you can stop being in the study.

If you have any questions about this, you may ask them now or at any other time.

If you want to be in this study, please sign your name. If you do not nwant to be in the study, don't
sign your name. Just hand the sheet back.

Your name (printing is OK) Date

I certify that this study and the procedures involved have been explained in terms the child could
understand and that he/she freely assented to participate in this study.
103

Appendix E

Pro-Ed Inc. Test Use Permission/Agreement


8700 Shoal Creek Blvd.
Austin, TX 78757-6897
(800)897-3202
(800) 397-7633 FAX
www.proedtnc.com

PRO-ED Inc.
An Internationa! Publisher
Attn: Darci Mundfrom (darci@proedinc.com) Date 0 i'/«> &/ JOGf^
Data and Materials Manager
Test Development

Free Tea* K3t Request Form


Eligible for Use in Completion of a Master's Thesis or Doctoral Dissertation (*)

Thank you for your recent request to use the ' / ORG' O for your
master's thesis or dissertation research. Please complete the following information, tax (or mail) it to us,
and we will be happy to supply you with one free copy of the test. By completing and submitting this form,
this becomes a binding agreement that you will use this instrument for your research project, and in return
you will supply us with a copy of your paper upon project completion. Please allow at least 3 weeks for the
shipment of your tree test kit We will contact you within one month of your completion date to make
mailing arrangements of your paper.

Test Requested (Name and Acronym included):

Student's Full Name _£h,r\f}a M - ^ l k n f / r n

Estimated Completion Date (Month/Year) oft / ^aofj

Address 8ob •tii'akta#iA Qo&A


316
C% _ V _ _ ± _ _ _ _ _ _ _ St frl/V Zip Code ^ t p j Country g<A
( ]
Phone .557 £3/ ~-^57 Fax < 6~t'7 > 83/-5,65/
Email Don.no., TT^ivvx&^rtig. u(,A. eAu
Student's Signature AJcrvr^-jQl
wOrvr^&JFY) >JHn!y-j»rv
S^JHXKJ'ZMJ
(7 1
Advisor's Full Name ,V)r. Maorine. "Kic k a r r i S*m

School/Dept^%f. llni\"e>*>ihi , 4 - _ Y ^ 4 f e ! ) A k a ^ . / b l u ^
Address fy\^^'skC$sxA<Sbtji:ibeheil^!MciiC*\ feft-fer, •ko^yn^fTk'
c
City \fprrtui{wr\ State £?b Zip Code 57(>b t Country j(S,4-
)
Phone '- b6S f-,11' ClJ-1 Fax J ]
,
Email hAaarme.. R<cKar<g/^gy>fl, UsA.eAtj
Advisor's Signature Ot, lT)MimjJ(3^MkJdfi^
(*) Ffee Test Kit Offer applicable to PRO-ED, Inc. published tests only, and must be in-print at the date of request.
105

Appendix F

Scott Foresman Leveled Readers Unit 5


Scott Foresman Grade Six Leveled Readers

Unit 5 Into the Unknown

Easy

171A The Reindeer People

172A Hedwig 's Journey to America

173 A Junior Space Patrol

174A The Main Street of America: Route 66

175 A The Last Mile

On-Level

171B The Race to the South Pole

172B Baby Windy

173B The Story of the London Bridge

174B Mountain Climb

175B Volcanoes and Giant Tarantulas

Challenge

5C Into the Unknown


107

Appendix G

Scott Foresman Leveled Readers Unit 6


108

Scott Foresman Grade Six Leveled Readers

Unit 6 I've Got It!

Easy

176A Happy Lu

Ml A Too Close for Comfort

178A The Mystery of the Gold Pen

179A The Boat That Went on Both Land and Water

180 A Towers of Stone

On-Level

176B Paper Projects

177B Hot Gobs! The Art of Glass Blowing

178B MargaretBourke-White, Photographer

179B Aliens in Ancient Egypt

180B Eye to Eye with an Artist

Challenge

6C I've Got It!


109

Appendix H

Trade Books Used


110

Unit 5

Trapped by the Ice: Shackleton 's Amazing Arctic Adventure

by Michael McCurdy

Grandfather's Journey by Allen Say

Starry Messenger by Peter Sis

The Phantom Tollbooth by Norman Juster

Unit 6

Mailing May by Michael O. Tunnell

A Picture Book of Louis Braille by David A. Adler

One Grain of Rice by Demi

The Magic Flute by Anne Gatti


Ill

Appendix I

Scott Foresman Unit Skills Test Validity/Reliability Information


112

DONNA M. JOHNSON
806 Highland Road 507-831-2157
Windom, MN 56101 Donna.Johnson@usd.edu

Pearson Scott Foresman


Editorial Offices
1900 East Lake Avenue
Glenview, IL 60025-2055

To whom it may concern:

I am a graduate student at The University of South Dakota working on the research for
my doctoral dissertation. I am using the 2004 Scott Foresman Reading Grade 6
curriculum in my study of reading comprehension in two sixth grade classrooms. Scores
from the Unit Skills Tests (ISBN 0-673-62457-9) will be part of the data I collect.

Would you please send me information about the reliability of the Scott Foresman
Reading Grade 6 Skills Tests and also information about the content, criterion-related,
and construct validity of the test results? The reliability and validity information is not
included in the Teacher's Manual for the Unit and End-of-Year Skills Tests booklet.

If you need additional information, please contact me. I thank you for your help.

Sincerely,

Donna M. Johnson
113

From • "Swensson, Rochelle" <rochelle.swenssonrgjPearson.com>


Sent Tuesday, July 15, 2008 2:38 pm
To Donna. Johnson(g) usd.edu
Cc
Bcc
Subject Scott Foresman Reading

Donna:

Our Editorial Director for Elementary Basal Reading has provided the following response
to the questions posed in your recent letter:

The Scott Foresman Reading grade 6 Unit Skills Tests have not been subjected to
statistical studies, had reliability and validity measures run on them, or been normed
using large student populations. That is not their intent. Their purpose is to give a
snapshot of how well a student is reading overall in relation to what has been taught in a
unit in Scott Foresman Reading. In that sense, they are valid-they do test what Scott
Foresman Reading has taught. We can link a skill and/or strategy with each question,
and these are all skills the student has been taught during the course of the unit. Further,
we could say that the Unit Skills Tests have been user-validated because teachers who
have given these grade 6 Unit Skills Tests over the years have not reported to us
problems involving inaccurate or odd results. As for reliability, the tests are in one form
only; they do not come in different forms, and so there can be no test/retest reliability
studies done on them.

Rochelle

Rochelle Swensson
Pearson Elementary Product Specialist
Reading & Language Arts
rocheUe.swensson@pearson.com
800-848-9500 x 835

jjjFj Please consider the environment before printing this e-mail


114

Appendix J

Irwin-Mitchell Richness in Retelling Scale:

Permission to Use, Criteria, & Checklist


115
From • "Mitchell. Judy Nichols" <judym@wsu.edu>
Sent Wednesday, January 30, 2008 6:30 pm
To Donna M Johnson <Donna.Johnson(g),usd.edu>
Cc
Bcc
Subject RE: Request to use Retelling Evaluation Scale

Thanks Donna. Pi Irwin and I would be delighted to have you use the Richness in Retellings
scale. Let us know how your research goes - thanks

judymitchell

From: Donna M Johnson [mailto:DonnaJohnson@usd.edu]


Sent: Wednesday, January 30, 2008 1:57 PM
To: Mitchell, Judy Nichols
Subject: Request to use Retelling Evaluation Scale

Dear Dr. Mitchell,

I am a graduate student at the University of South Dakota working on the research for my
doctoral dissertation.

With your permission, I would like to use the 5-point scale you developed with Dr. Pi A.
Irwin to assess the written retellings of the sixth grade students who will be participating
in my study.

Thank you for considering my request.

Donna M.
Johnson
605-321-0741
116

Richness in Retellings Scale


Level Criteria for Establishing Level
Student generalizes beyond text: includes thesis (summarizing statement), all
major points, and appropriate supporting details; includes relevant
5 supplementations; shows high degree of coherence, completeness,
comprehensibility.
Student includes thesis (summarizing statement), all major points, and
4 appropriate supporting details; includes relevant supplementations; shows
high degree of coherence, completeness, comprehensibility.
Student relates major ideas; includes appropriate supporting details and
3 relevant supplementations; shows adequate coherence, completeness,
comprehensibility.
Student relates a few major ideas and some supporting details; includes
2 irrelevant supplementations; shows some degree of coherence, some
completeness; the whole is somewhat comprehensible.
Student relates details only; irrelevant supplementations or none; low degree
1
of coherence, incomplete; incomprehensible.
*From Irwin, P. A. & Mitchell, J.N. (1983). A procedure for assessing the richness of retellings. Journal of
Reading, 26(5), 391-396.
117

Richness in Retellings Scale

Checklist forjudging richness of retelling

5 4 3 2 1
Generalizes
beyond text X

Thesis
(summarizing) X X
statement

Major points X X X ? ?

Supporting details X X X X ?

Supplementations Relevant Relevant Relevant Irrelevant Irrelevant

Coherence High Good Adequate Some Poor

Completeness High Good Adequate Some Poor

Comprehensibility High Good Adequate Some Poor

*From Irwin, P.A. & Mitchell, J.N. (1983). A procedure for assessing the richness of retellings. Journal of
Reading, 26(5), 391-396.
118

Appendix K

Oral Retelling Procedures


119
Oral Retelling Procedures

1. Team Retelling (Hoyt, 1999)

Following the reading of an assigned narrative or expository text, groups of students

will orally retell in their own words. A purpose or focus for the retelling may be

determined beforehand; for example, the students may be asked to retell a part that seems

to them to be most important, interesting, funny, or sad. The group might be arranged so

that team members focus on specific story elements (e.g. problem, solution, and

resolution) or expository text structures (e.g. description, items in a sequence, comparison

and contrast). The researcher may use open-ended prompts to encourage and aid the

students. In some cases, students may be asked to connect their retellings to personal

experiences, prior knowledge, or other texts.

2. Consecutive Retelling (Developed by the researcher)

Following the reading of an assigned narrative or informational text, the first student

will retell the first episode or point in his own words with as much detail as possible. The

second student will summarize the first point and add the next main point, including as

much detail as possible. The third reader will summarize the retellings of the first two

students and add a third episode and so on until the entire story or article is retold.

3. Paired Retelling (Koskinen, Gambrell, Kapinus, & Heathington, 1988)

Students silently read a story or portion of a text and then team up with a partner for

retelling. "One partner becomes the 'storyteller' who tries to retell everything that is

important about the story, while the other partner is the 'listener' " (p. 893). Then the

partners reverse roles and read and retell another part of the text.
120

Appendix L

Written Retelling Procedures


121
Written Retelling Procedures

1. Retelling Journal Entries (Brown & Cambourne, 1987)

Entries written in retelling journals will follow a teacher read aloud or a student

silent reading. In either case, the each student will have his own copy of the text so that it

can be read as many times as the student wishes. The following points will be

emphasized:

a. The text should be read as many times as needed so the student feels "confident

and comfortable with the meanings in it" (Brown & Cambourne, 1987, p. 33).

b. Rereadings are done to gain understanding, not to achieve rote memory.

c. Any specific purpose for the retelling should be shared beforehand. For

example, the written retelling may be for the purpose of sharing a story with

someone who has never read it before, or to explain and summarize the main

points.

Instructions for the written retellings will be as follows:

a. Close the book you have been reading and don't look back at the story.

b. Neatness and spelling don't count. Just be sure you can read what you write.

c. Remember and write as quickly as you can.

In some instances, the written retellings may be shared with a partner to discuss how

they differ or are similar, to determine whether meaning is preserved, and to discuss word

choice.

2. Plot Summary Maps (Hansen, 2004)

After hearing or reading a story, students fill out a graphic organizer called a Plot

Summary Map. The organizer has spaces to record the main character's goal, the obstacle
faced by the character, the attempts made to overcome the obstacle, the outcomes of the

attempts, the resolution of the problem, and the ending. Using the completed Plot

Summary Map as a model, students write a detailed written retelling of the story using

complete sentences.

3. Electronic Retelling. (Shaw, 2005)

Students use computer applications to enhance or communicate their written

retellings. They will be expected to follow school policies regarding computer and

Internet use.

a. Students write a brief email message to a friend telling the setting, character,

and initial event from a story they have read. They tell the story so that their friend will

be interested in finding out more about it.

b. Students type (or scan) one of their retellings, attach it to an email, send it to a

partner who has read the same text, and ask for comments and reactions.

c. Students use presentation software to add clip art or photos to a retelling of a

narrative or expository text and share it with a friend or a group.

4. SPOT the Story (Bos & Vaughn, 1994).

After hearing or reading a story, students fill in four sections of a graphic organizer:

Setting - Who, what, when, and were

Problem - What is the problem to be solved or the goal to be reached?

Order of Action - What events happen to solve the problem or attain the goal?

Tail End - What happened at the end of the story? How is the problem resolved?

Students then use the information on the SPOT graphic organizer and write

everything they can about the story that was heard or read.

You might also like