You are on page 1of 10

Clinical Psychology Review 30 (2010) 288–297

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Clinical Psychology Review

Autobiographical memory biases in social anxiety


Julie Morgan ⁎
Psychology Research Unit, Kingston University, Penrhyn Road, Kingston upon Thames, Surrey, KT1 2EE, UK

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Individuals with social anxiety preferentially attend to threatening social information during and following
Received 8 September 2009 social events. As such, cognitive models predict that social anxiety should be associated with biases in the
Received in revised form 1 December 2009 recall of social events. However, initial experimental studies examining this assumption either failed to find
Accepted 3 December 2009
such biases or found only weak evidence for an autobiographical memory bias. The current review examines
an emerging line of evidence offering support for the role of an autobiographical memory bias in the
Keywords:
Social anxiety
development and maintenance of social anxiety. The review begins by examining current theoretical
Social phobia approaches to autobiographical memory before looking at empirical studies that have examined differences
Autobiographical memory between socially anxious and non-anxious individuals in the recall of autobiographical memories. Specific
Cognitive biases memory biases include properties of social-threat memories, the imagery associated with these memories,
and the cognitive processing styles that have been found to either facilitate or inhibit the recall of emotional
memories. Limitations in methodologies used to study retrieval of memories and the implications of findings
for future research are discussed.
© 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 288
2. Theories of autobiographical memory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 289
2.1. Autobiographical memories as reconstructions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 289
2.2. The self-memory system . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 291
2.2.1. Memory and current self-perceptions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 291
2.2.2. Memory and self-regulation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 291
3. Autobiographical memory and anxiety . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 291
3.1. Autobiographical memory and social anxiety . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 291
3.2. Recall of social-threat memories in social anxiety . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 291
3.3. Properties of anxiety-related memories . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 292
3.4. Memory perspective biases and social anxiety . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 293
4. Early autobiographical memories and social anxiety . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 293
4.1. Recall of early adverse social experiences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 293
4.2. Imagery and early autobiographical memories . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 294
5. Cognitive processing styles and autobiographical memories in social anxiety . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 294
5.1. Effects of imagery on autobiographical memory recall . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 294
5.2. Effects of rumination on autobiographical memory recall . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 294
5.2.1. Anticipatory processing and memory recall . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 294
5.2.2. Post-event processing and memory recall . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 295
6. Directions for future research . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 295
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 296

1. Introduction

Social anxiety disorder is characterized by a marked and persistent


⁎ Tel.: +44 20 8547 2000; fax: +44 20 8547 7292. fear of social or performance situations that provoke an immediate
E-mail address: J.Morgan@kingston.ac.uk. anxiety response (DSM-IV, American Psychiatric Association, 1994).

0272-7358/$ – see front matter © 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.12.003
J. Morgan / Clinical Psychology Review 30 (2010) 288–297 289

Anxiety typically focuses on concerns about negative evaluation from systems. The former consists of meaningful facts about the world and
other people. Individuals with social anxiety disorder anticipate that contextual knowledge, while the latter consists of a record of specific
they will embarrass or humiliate themselves before entering a social experiences. Autobiographical memory may be viewed as a particular
event, shift their attention towards detailed monitoring and observation type of episodic memory concerned with “specific experiences and
of the self during the event, and engage in rumination following the memory for the personal facts of one's life” (Conway, 1990, p.4).
event (Clark & McManus, 2002). Social anxiety is characterized there- Autobiographical memories consist of complex events and contain a
fore by a number of biases in cognitive processing (see Heinrichs & high degree of self-reference and personal significance. In addition,
Hofmann, 2001) that are a direct result of the social context in which these memories typically feature sensory, perceptual, and reflective
anxiety is apprehended and experienced. Given that social anxiety is information, including details about the particular time and place in
associated with enhanced attention to perceived threat in social sit- which an event was experienced (Rubin, 1996). Theoretical accounts
uations, a basic assumption of cognitive models of social anxiety (Clark of autobiographical memory have focused on the way in which mem-
& Wells, 1995; Rapee & Heimberg, 1997) postulates that individuals will ories are structured and represented in long term memory.
display a tendency to recall negative experiences of social events. As
such, it is expected that social anxiety will be associated with biases in 2.1. Autobiographical memories as reconstructions
the recall of certain types of autobiographical experiences.
In comparison with the wealth of research that has focused on the Current approaches to autobiographical memory are based on
role of implicit and explicit memory biases in social anxiety (see Coles & earlier theories that proposed autobiographical memories are repre-
Heimberg, 2002; Mitte, 2008), the role of autobiographical memory sented within hierarchical knowledge structures. Neisser (1986)
biases in social anxiety has been relatively under researched. However, suggested that when individuals recall an experienced event, they do
there is good reason to believe that such biases play an integral role in not revive a single record of the event but revive a whole network of
the maintenance of this disorder. Clinical observation suggests that memories associated with this event. This view emphasizes that auto-
individuals with social anxiety disorder tend to be preoccupied with biographical memory is organized into multiple levels of descriptions of
emotionally upsetting social events from the past, particularly in rela- events (e.g., actors, meanings, and perceptual and temporal features).
tion to perceived social failures (Clark & Wells, 1995; Coles & Heimberg, Because these levels are nested within one another, recalling an event
2002). In addition, cognitive models of social anxiety have highlighted involves moving between these levels. Neisser proposed that at the base
the importance that memory processes may play in exacerbating anxiety. of this hierarchical structure are specific autobiographical memories. A
For example, Rapee and Heimberg's (1997) cognitive-behavioral model key assumption of this approach is that memories are reconstructed
proposes that socially anxious individuals' perception of their social based on what was perceived from an event rather than from what
performance is likely to be mediated by prior social experiences that are actually happened. For this reason, it has been argued that autobio-
represented either accurately or inaccurately in long term memory. graphical memories are typically inaccurate and are often vague, incom-
Furthermore, Clark and Wells' cognitive model argues that recently per- plete, or distorted (Conway, 1990; Neisser, 1982). Both the encoding and
ceived social failures may be added to an extensive list of past social retrieval of autobiographical memories may be influenced by a com-
failures that are stored in memory, together serving to strengthen neg- bination of bottom-up processes, in which incomplete or ambiguous
ative beliefs about social inadequacy and the threat of social situations. sensory information is received via the senses, and top-down processes, in
Whereas autobiographical memory biases have been consistently which information already stored in memory as prior knowledge in-
demonstrated in depression and post-traumatic stress disorder (Moore & fluences expectations and interpretations of sensory input. What is re-
Zoellner, 2007; Williams et al., 2007), research examining whether such membered therefore appears to be influenced by what is already known.
biases are apparent in other anxiety disorders has been more equivocal The reconstructivist account has been further emphasized in more
(Burke & Mathews, 1992; Levy & Mineka, 1998; Richards & Whittaker, recent approaches to autobiographical memory (Anderson & Conway,
1990). Indeed, preliminary research examining memory biases in social 1993; Conway, 1996; Conway & Pleydell-Pearce, 2000). Autobiograph-
anxiety initially failed to find differences between individuals with social ical memories are not viewed as discrete, holistic units in long term
anxiety disorder and non-anxious controls in the retrieval of threat- memory, but rather they are temporary mental representations that
related autobiographical memories (Rapee, McCallum, Melville, Raven- are reconstructed through levels of autobiographical knowledge stored
scroft, & Rodney, 1994), despite clinical observation and theoretical in an autobiographical knowledge base. According to Anderson and
models that would argue to the contrary. However, an emerging line of Conway (1993), these temporary or transitory mental representations
research in this area has begun to highlight new ways of examining are reconstructed in the context of a specific processing episode. They
autobiographical memory biases. For example, the link between early suggest that the autobiographical knowledge base facilitates the re-
autobiographical memories and current images of the self (Hackmann, trieval of these memories. This base contains at least three levels of
Clark, & McManus, 2000; Wild, Hackmann, & Clark, 2008), and the effect knowledge. At the highest level of the hierarchy, lifetime periods re-
of certain cognitive processing styles on the retrieval of autobiographical present the goals, plans, and themes of the self during particular periods.
memories (Morgan & Banerjee, 2008; Stopa & Jenkins, 2007) have tog- For example, knowledge of significant relationships with others and
ether highlighted the role of autobiographical memory in the develop- records of goal attainment that are characteristic of a particular period
ment and maintenance of social anxiety. are represented at this level. The next level is represented by general
The current review will begin by introducing the concept of events, which encompasses both repeated, extended events and single,
autobiographical memory, presenting the view that memories are specific events. At the lowest level, event-specific knowledge contains
reconstructed from self-knowledge, with particular reference to knowledge of the sensory-perceptual detail of specific events and highly
Conway and Pleydell-Pearce's (2000) self-memory system. Current specific facts.
ways of examining autobiographical memory in social anxiety will The process through which autobiographical memories, or the
then be reviewed (see Table 1), emphasizing the role that specific three layers of the knowledge base, are retrieved has been described
cognitive processing styles may play in enhancing the accessibility of in terms of cyclic retrieval patterns (Anderson & Conway, 1993;
emotionally adverse memories. Conway, 1996). In this way, when an internal or external cue is first
elaborated, activation travels in channels through the knowledge
2. Theories of autobiographical memory base such that if a search accesses a lifetime period, activation is
subsequently channelled by cues available at this level to associated
Tulving (1972) made an important distinction between two general events and event-specific knowledge. At this stage of the
systems in long term memory: the semantic and episodic memory cyclic retrieval process, knowledge accessed by the cue is evaluated
290 J. Morgan / Clinical Psychology Review 30 (2010) 288–297

Table 1
Summary of studies examining autobiographical memory biases in social anxiety.

Study Sample Memory recall task Social induction Memory measure Main finding

Recall of social-threat memories


1. Rapee et al. (1994), Study 4 33 SAD Social and neutral cue words No Anxiety ratings No effect of SA on anxiety ratings of
21 NAC social memories
2. Wenzel et al. (2002) 16 SAD Social-threat and neutral cue words No IC affect ratings Weak effect of SA on negative affect ratings
17 NAC of social-threat memories
3. Wenzel, Werner et al. (2004) 15 SAD Positive, neutral, and social-threat No IC affect ratings No effect of SA on ratings of social-threat
17 NAC cue words memories when controlling for depression

Properties of anxious memories


1. Wenzel, Pinna et al. (2004) 99 Affective categories No IC affect ratings Panic/trauma memories recalled with more
students vivid and detailed properties than
worry/social anxiety memories
2. Erwin et al. (2006) 45 SAD Clinical interview No PDS-revised SAD group responded to stressful social
30 NAC memories with PTSD-type symptoms
3. D'Argembeau et al. (2006) 17 SAD Social and non-social events No MCQ SAD group recalled social events with
17 NAC greater self-referential information
4. McNally et al. (2001) 12 SAD Fearful and neutral memory No IC affect ratings No effect of diagnostic group on affective
32 PD content of recalled memories
13 MDD
24 NAC
5. Anderson et al. (2008) 42 SAD Adverse social memories No IC linguistic content Greater use of self-referential and anxiety
27 NAC words in memories recalled by SAD group

Memory perspective biases


1. Wells et al. (1998) 12 SAD Social and non-social event No Image perspective SAD group more likely to recall social
12 NAC events from an observer perspective
2. Wells and Papageorgiou (1999) 12 SAD Social and non-social event No Image perspective SAD and AP groups tend to recall social
12 AP events from an observer perspective
12 B/IP
12 NAC
3. Coles et al. (2001) 30 SAD Low, medium, high anxiety No MAQ, Image SAD group more likely to recall high anxiety
24 NAC social event perspective social events from an observer perspective

Recall of early adverse memories


1. Harvey et al. (2005) 55 SAD Childhood events No LHQ Greater recall of peer group difficulties from
30 PTSD childhood in SAD group
30 NAC
2. Stemberger et al. (1995) 68 SAD Clinical interview No SAHIQ-R SAD group more likely to recall a traumatic
25 NAC social conditioning event
3. Roth et al. (2002) 514 Childhood events No TQ Social anxiety and anxiety sensitivity
students associated with recall of teasing events
4. Hackmann et al. (2000) 22 SAD Memory based on self-imagery No Interview Event depicted in memory tended to
coincide with onset of disorder

Cognitive styles and memory


1. Magee and Zinbarg (2007) 35 hi-SA Negative social event Social N/A Focusing on negative memory led to larger
35 lo-SA anticipation increase in shyness than suppressing
memory in high SA group
2. Stopa and Jenkins (2007) 20 hi-SA Positive, negative, and neutral Self-imagery Retrieval latencies Participants took longer to retrieve positive
cue words memories when holding negative image in
mind during speech
3. Vassilopoulos (2008) 24 hi-SA Social event Social vignettes IC affect ratings High SA group recalled fewer positive social
22 lo-SA events than low SA group
4. Hinrichsen and Clark (2003), 20 hi-SA Social event No Interview, ASBQ High SA group reported more past
Study 1 20 lo-SA perceived social failures during social
anticipation
5. Mellings and Alden (2000) 58 hi-SA Memory of social interaction Social IC memory PEP associated with recall of negative
58 lo-SA interaction description self-related information following
interaction
6. Field and Morgan (2004) 33 hi-SA Free recall of events PEP Affective ratings High SA group recalled anxious and
33 lo-SA shameful memories following PEP
7. Morgan and Banerjee (2008), 29 hi-SA AMQ Response style Affective rating High SA group recalled memories with
Study 2 30 lo-SA highest anxiety ratings after ruminating
about a hypothetical social event

Note. SAD = social anxiety disorder; NAC = non-anxious controls; IC = independent coder; PDS-revised = post-traumatic diagnostic scale-revised; MCQ = memory characteristics
questionnaire; PD = panic disorder; MDD = major depressive disorder; AP = agoraphobia; B/IP = blood or injury phobia; MAQ = memory attributional questionnaire; LHQ =
learning history questionnaire; SAHIQ-R = social anxiety history and interview questionnaire-revised; TQ = teasing questionnaire; ASBQ = anticipatory social behaviors
questionnaire; PEP = post-event processing; AMQ = autobiographical memory questionnaire.

and is assessed by whether the knowledge is appropriate and required (1993) maintain that central control processes modulate the retrieval
or whether the retrieval process should be terminated. Consequently, cycle (e.g., the working self, Conway, Singer, & Tagini, 2004). This
an autobiographical memory is retrieved once a stable pattern of allows current themes and discrepancies of the self to influence
activation is established across the appropriate set of knowledge memory construction because current preoccupations of the self are
layers in the autobiographical knowledge base. Anderson and Conway believed to be part of these central control processes.
J. Morgan / Clinical Psychology Review 30 (2010) 288–297 291

2.2. The self-memory system attainment often results in intense affective experiences (Carver &
Scheier, 1998). Research suggests that self-defining memories play an
The relationship between autobiographical memory and current important role in regulating mood states. For example, non-depressed
self-perceptions has been emphasized in more detail in Conway and individuals tend to employ positive self-defining memories to repair
Pleydell-Pearce's (2000) self-memory system account of autobiograph- their negative mood, whereas mildly depressed individuals are less
ical memory. This model suggests that autobiographical memories are likely to employ positive memories when in a negative mood state
the result of two competing demands: adaptive correspondence and (Josephson, Singer, & Salovey, 1996). Certain types of autobiograph-
self-coherence. Conway et al. (2004) describe the former as the need ical memories may therefore play an important role in self-regulation.
to encode a record of ongoing goal activity and experiences (i.e., In terms of anxiety, it is likely that emotional memories (e.g., anxious
reality) while the latter reflects the need to maintain a coherent and memories) are more accessible to anxious individuals, and may
stable record of the self's interaction with the world (i.e., an integrated inhibit the effective regulation of anxious mood.
life story). This account still maintains that memories are transitory
mental constructions, but argues that a critical function of memory is 3. Autobiographical memory and anxiety
to track progress in the attainment of goals and therefore memories
should reflect reality to some extent. Autobiographical memory in anxiety disorders has typically been
investigated in two ways: 1) studies of overgeneral memory have
2.2.1. Memory and current self-perceptions found that the tendency to recall overgeneral memories coupled with
The self-memory system predicts that the competing demand of reduced ability to access specific memories is associated with PTSD
coherence enhances the availability of memories that support and (McNally, Litz, Prassas, Shin, & Weathers, 1994; McNally et al., 1995),
confirm current self-perceptions. This idea may help to explain why and 2) studies have examined whether anxiety is associated with a
individuals with social anxiety disorder are preoccupied with retrieval bias for anxiety-related personal memories. In support of
perceived social failures from the past. These memories should be this, Richards and Whittaker (1990) found that following a mood
highly salient to socially anxious individuals because they reconfirm induction task to increase anxiety, individuals with high trait anxiety
current self-views about the ability to interact effectively within were faster to recall specific personal memories in response to
interpersonal and social performance situations. In fact, recent anxiety-related cue words than happiness-related words. Memories
research has shown that individuals with social anxiety disorder recalled by individuals with low trait anxiety were unaffected by the
recall and rate autobiographical experiences of social situations with type of cue word. Similarly, Burke and Mathews (1992) found that in
greater self-conscious emotion (Anderson, Goldin, Kurita, & Gross, comparison to non-anxious controls, individuals with generalized
2008) and greater self-referential information (D'Argembeau, Van der anxiety recalled more autobiographical memories associated with
Linden, d'Acremont, & Mayers, 2006) than non-anxious controls. A nervousness and were quicker to recall memories associated with
similar pattern of findings has been shown in studies from other types anxiety in response to neutral cue words. Certain types of memories
of anxiety disorders. For example, individuals with post-traumatic therefore appear to be particularly salient to anxious individuals and
stress disorder (PTSD) report more trauma-related memories than may be more frequently rehearsed. Indeed, research has shown that
individuals without PTSD (Kangas, Henry, & Bryant, 2005; McNally, stressful memories are associated with more emotional intensity and
Lasko, Macklin, & Pitman, 1995). In addition, Sutherland and Bryant more frequent retrieval (Rubin, Boals, & Berntsen, 2008). Accessibility
(2008) found that perceiving a discrepancy between one's current self of anxious memories in individuals with high trait levels of anxiety
and ideal self is related to the recall of traumatic memories. Current may therefore be particularly enhanced, making them more readily
views of the self therefore appear to influence the type of memories available for retrieval from autobiographical memory.
recalled. The effect of current self-view on the recall of past events has
been further emphasized in studies examining self-appraisal theory 3.1. Autobiographical memory and social anxiety
(Ross & Wilson, 2002; Wilson & Ross, 2001). These studies
demonstrate a distancing bias in which individuals with high self- It has been well established that individuals with social anxiety
esteem report feeling subjectively further away from unfavorable past preferentially attend to threatening social information (Gilboa-
experiences than from favorable ones, whereas individuals with low Schechtman, Foa, & Amir, 1999; Musa, Lépine, Clark, Mansell &
self-esteem do not show any such bias. Ross and Wilson argue that Ehlers, 2003; Veljaca & Rapee, 1998). Rapee and Heimberg (1997)
such individuals are motivated to maintain high levels of self regard argue that socially anxious individuals examine their internal and
through subjectively distancing themselves from unflattering experi- external environment for signs of negative evaluation, detect such
ences, suggesting that autobiographical memory plays an important signs rapidly, and have difficulty disengaging attention from these
role in regulating self-views. signs. Given that socially anxious individuals tend to closely monitor
threat and focus their attentional resources on perceived threat
2.2.2. Memory and self-regulation during social experiences, several researchers have reasoned that
In further describing the types of memories which may be social anxiety should be associated with biases in the recall of social
retrieved through the balance of adaptive correspondence and self- events. Several lines of research have examined this view in terms of:
coherence, Conway et al. (2004) define a special class of autobio- 1) whether social anxiety is associated with greater recall of social-
graphical memories called self-defining memories which can be threat memories, 2) whether the properties of anxiety-related
distinguished on the basis of their distinct attributes. These memories memories differ from more neutrally valenced memories, and 3)
are often highly vivid and have intense affective qualities. They are whether the perspective from which social memories are recalled
subject to high levels of rehearsal and are connected often to enduring differs between socially anxious and non-anxious individuals.
concerns or ongoing conflicts (Singer & Blagov, 2004; Sutin & Robins,
2005). For example, Sutherland and Bryant (2005) found that 3.2. Recall of social-threat memories in social anxiety
individuals with PTSD are more likely to report their trauma as a
self-defining memory. It has been found that the affective quality of Several studies have used an autobiographical memory cueing
self-defining memories is a function of the relevance of these procedure to examine the recall of social-threat memories in social
memories to the attainment of an individual's most desired goals anxiety. These studies are based on the assumption that the emotion
(Moffitt & Singer, 1994). This finding is consistent with goal-based or situation specified by a cue word should activate an associated
models of self-regulation that suggest assessment of progress in goal memory.
292 J. Morgan / Clinical Psychology Review 30 (2010) 288–297

In one of the first studies to examine autobiographical recall biases has therefore begun to examine properties of these memories, par-
for threatening social events, Rapee et al. (1994) instructed individuals ticularly those concerned with anxiety-related experiences. This re-
with social anxiety disorder and non-anxious controls to describe the search has questioned whether anxious memories differ qualitatively
first memory which came to mind following the presentation of words from other, more emotionally neutral events and experiences.
relating to social situations (e.g., interview, party) and neutral words Wenzel, Pinna and Rubin (2004) have argued that autobiographical
(e.g., river, dog). They assigned participants to either a “self” condition, memories of anxiety-related experiences and the verbal descriptions,
in which memories from their own lives were described, or an “other” mental imagery, and emotional content associated with these memories
condition, in which memories involving another person (e.g., a sibling or may help to explain why they could be more accessible to anxious
close friend) were described. The groups then rated their memories on a individuals. In a study that looked at the properties of different types
measure of how much anxiety was associated with each memory. of anxiety-related autobiographical memories, Wenzel et al. asked un-
Although memories recalled from the social words were rated with dergraduate students to retrieve three specific memories to each
greater anxiety than memories recalled from neutral words, the results of five categories reflecting panic, trauma, worry, social anxiety, and
indicated no differences between the socially anxious and non-anxious contentment. They were then asked questions about the properties of
groups in the retrieval of these memories. these memories (e.g., vividness, accuracy, and sensory and emotional
Further studies examining whether individuals with social anxiety experiences). Wenzel et al. found that the panic- and trauma-related
recall a greater proportion of social-threat memories found mixed memories retrieved by students tended to be more vivid, were ac-
results. Wenzel, Jackson, and Holt (2002) asked individuals with social companied by intense negative emotions, and were more likely to be
anxiety disorder and non-anxious controls to record specific personal perceived as reflecting an accurate portrayal of the event than the other
memories in response to social-threat and neutral cue words. memory categories. This suggests that panic or trauma autobiographical
Independent coders rated the memories for positive or negative memories may have more vivid and detailed properties than memories
affective tone. There were no differences between the groups in the associated with worry or social anxiety. However, there may be im-
percentage of specific memories retrieved overall. However, the socially portant differences between anxious memories recalled by normal
anxious group did recall a greater percentage of memories subsequently populations and those recalled by clinical groups. Indeed, recent re-
rated as reflecting negative affect than the non-anxious group when search shows that memories of stressful social events recalled by
cued by social-threat words. It should be noted that the memories individuals with social anxiety disorder are in fact re-experienced with
reflecting negative affect cued by social-threat words were made by the type of trauma symptoms that characterize PTSD. Erwin, Heimberg,
only 8% of the socially anxious group. Thus, the findings from the study Marx, and Franklin (2006) found that unlike non-anxious controls,
provide only weak evidence for an autobiographical memory bias in individuals with social anxiety disorder reacted to their memories
social anxiety. A similar procedure using positive, social-threat, and of past socially stressful events with PTSD-type symptoms such as
neutral cue words was used to measure retrieval latencies and affective hyperarousal and avoidance.
tone in autobiographical memories recalled by individuals with and Properties of memories for social events recalled by socially anxious
without social anxiety disorder (Wenzel, Werner, Cochran & Holt, individuals have been further examined by D'Argembeau et al. (2006).
2004). The study found that social anxiety did not influence either the They asked individuals with social anxiety disorder and non-anxious
time it took to recall threat-related memories or the affective content of controls to recall recent social and non-social events associated with
these memories when accounting for levels of depression. positive and negative emotion. Ratings of phenomenal characteristics
Evidence from studies using memory cueing procedures suggest that associated with these memories revealed a bias towards recalling social
at best there is only weak evidence to support the view that social anxiety events with greater self-referential information (i.e., memory for one's
is characterized by biases in autobiographical memory. These findings own behavior and what one thought and said) but less sensory
contradict cognitive models of social anxiety (e.g., Clark & Wells, 1995; information (i.e., memory for visual and auditory details) in the socially
Rapee & Heimberg, 1997) that would predict greater recall of social-threat anxious group. There were no differences between the groups in
memories in socially anxious individuals. However, there are several phenomenal characteristics for non-social events.
limitations in using cue words to elicit social-threat memories in socially Using ratings made by independent coders, McNally, Otto, and
anxious individuals. For example, Holmes, Mathews, Mackintosh, and Hornig (2001) found no evidence to distinguish the properties of fear-
Dalgleish (2008) have shown that mental imagery is more effective in related memories recalled by anxious individuals from those recalled by
eliciting emotion than verbal processing of the same material. Further- non-anxious controls. They asked individuals with panic disorder, social
more, Holmes et al. found that imagery descriptions are more likely to anxiety disorder, major depressive disorder, and non-anxious controls
resemble personal memories than sentence descriptions. Imagery (e.g., to describe their most frightening experience (i.e., a fearful memory)
use of pictures) may therefore provide a more effective route to accessing and what they had for breakfast that morning (i.e., a neutral memory).
emotional autobiographical memories than cue words. In addition, Clark Responses were audiotaped and the speech was content-filtered to
and Wells' model suggests that individuals with social anxiety will tend to eliminate high frequencies while leaving paralinguistic features such
dwell on memories of social-threat when either anticipating a social event as pitch and loudness intact. This technique removes semantic content
(i.e., worrying about how they will come across to others) or when in the speech but the resulting sound can be analysed for emotional
ruminating about a recent social interaction (i.e., worrying about how content. Independent coders rated the content-filtered speech on
they did come across). As such, it could be argued that experiments several emotional dimensions to assess whether the fear memories
exploring retrieval biases for negative information should include a social- reported by the anxiety and depression groups were emotionally
threat manipulation to activate the dysfunctional assumptions or self- distinguishable from neutral memories and the memories reported by
schemas that characterize social anxiety disorder (Hirsch & Clark, 2004). the control group. However, they did not find evidence to support this
Activating a state level of social anxiety (e.g., by requiring participants to hypothesis. Regardless of the diagnostic status of the individuals, the
engage in an anxiety-provoking social task) before presenting cue words content-filtered fear memories were rated as more anxious, arousing,
would ensure that memory biases are examined under conditions that re- and dominating than the neutral memories.
semble the way information is processed in threatening social situations. In a further study in which independent coders analysed the lin-
guistic content of autobiographical narratives, Anderson et al. (2008)
3.3. Properties of anxiety-related memories did find differences in the properties of social memories recalled by
individuals with social anxiety disorder and non-anxious controls.
Autobiographical memories incorporate detailed descriptions of Participants were asked to recall autobiographical situations charac-
memorable events and experiences. Recent research in social anxiety terized by vivid social humiliation, embarrassment, or shame. In line
J. Morgan / Clinical Psychology Review 30 (2010) 288–297 293

with D'Argembeau et al.'s (2006) findings, the narratives written by Coles, Turk, Heimberg, and Fresco (2001) found that individuals with
the socially anxious group contained greater self-referential informa- social anxiety disorder tended to recall social events, which they had
tion and greater use of words reflecting anxiety symptoms than the rated as highly anxiety-provoking, from an observer perspective. In
narratives written by non-anxious controls. Importantly, Anderson et comparison, events rated as low in anxiety were more likely to be
al. further found that the socially anxious group reported greater recalled from a field perspective. This suggests that socially anxious
current self-conscious emotions when recalling social situations. individuals do not simply recall all social situations from an observer
Taken together, the findings from Anderson et al. and Erwin et al. perspective but that a memory perspective bias may play a role in
(2006) suggest that the recall of past social encounters in social social events that are perceived as greatly threatening.
anxiety can impact on emotions currently being experienced and may Research into memory perspective biases suggests that the way a
disrupt the processing of past social events. social event is recalled is important for understanding how memory
Overall, studies examining properties of anxiety-provoking mem- biases maintain social anxiety. The research findings are consistent
ories that found support for an autobiographical memory bias in social with the view that during social situations, individuals with social
anxiety tended to examine characteristics of memory other than, or in anxiety have heightened self-focused attention (see Spurr & Stopa,
addition to, affective valence. Although models of anxiety (e.g., Beck, 2002). It seems reasonable to assume that excessive self-monitoring
Emery, & Greenberg, 1985) would predict that anxiety-provoking during a social event leads to the encoding and retrieval of an observer
memories should be associated with specific types of emotion, it is perspective memory. However, it should be noted that a discrepancy
important to note that the emotions associated with autobiographical exists in the social anxiety literature: Clark and Wells' (1995) model
memories are likely to be multilayered across specific events and argues that socially anxious individuals monitor internal (i.e.,
lifetime periods and may not always fall into discrete emotional negative cognitions) threat cues during social events, while Rapee
categories. As shown in Table 1, studies examining autobiographical and Heimberg's (1997) model also emphasizes the role of external
memory biases in social anxiety commonly measure the affective threat cues such as the social-evaluative behaviors of interaction
content of memories. Although this provides a useful measure of the partners (see Schultz & Heimberg, 2008). As such, further research is
emotion associated with the memory, it limits the conclusions that needed to examine more directly how and whether focus of attention
can be drawn regarding further characteristics of the memory (e.g., during a social event affects the way that event is later recalled by
vividness, sensory detail, visual perspective, age of memory). As socially anxious individuals.
studies such as Anderson et al.'s (2008) show, using a qualitative
approach such as content analysis to systematically categorise and
4. Early autobiographical memories and social anxiety
classify properties of social memories is a potentially important and
interesting methodological procedure for future work in this area.
Development of social anxiety disorder has been associated with a
broad range of environmental influences (Bögels, van Oosten, Muris,
3.4. Memory perspective biases and social anxiety
& Smulders, 2001), individual response factors (Harvey, Ehlers, &
Clark, 2005), and genetic factors (Kendler, Karkowski, & Prescott,
There is evidence to suggest that social anxiety may be associated
1999). In particular, problems with peer group relations have been
with a memory perspective bias. It has been proposed that individuals
shown to coincide with the onset of social fears (La Greca & Lopez,
with social anxiety disorder should demonstrate a marked tendency
1998). Harvey et al. (2005) found that when recalling social situations
to recall social situations from an observer perspective due to detailed
prior to the onset of the disorder, not fitting in with a peer group was
observation and monitoring of the self during anxiety-provoking
indicated as one of the most common events to concur with the
social situations (Wells, Clark, & Ahmad, 1998). In an observer
development of social anxiety. In addition, retrospective studies have
perspective memory, individuals concentrate on viewing themselves
shown that socially anxious adults recall more childhood inhibition,
as if they were observing the situation. Hence, a socially anxious
particularly in childhood social and school situations, than adults with
individual may concentrate on how he/she appears to others in the
generalized anxiety (Mick & Telch, 1998; Neal, Edelmann, & Glachan,
memory of an anxious social event. This type of perspective is
2002). Memory for these adverse social experiences suggests that
typically contrasted with a field perspective image in which the
such experiences may play an important role in the initial develop-
individual looks out at the situation from their own eyes. Several
ment of the disorder. Indeed, Clark and Wells' (1995) model predicts
studies have shown that socially anxious individuals have a greater
that the cognitive processing biases that characterize social anxiety
tendency to report social situations from an observer perspective
develop as a consequence of early adverse social experiences. In
(Wells et al., 1998). For example, Wells and Papageorgiou (1999)
addition, the recall of such experiences may be implicated in the
asked participants to recall an anxiety-provoking social event and a
maintenance of the disorder. Approaches to studying the impact of
non-anxiety-provoking, non-social event. Using a bipolar rating,
these early memories in social anxiety have focused on whether these
participants indicated the extent to which the image in each memory
memories indicate a role for traumatic conditioning experiences and
was viewed from an observer or field perspective. They found that
whether there is a link between early memories and current self-
individuals with a diagnosis of social phobia or agoraphobia were
imagery in social anxiety.
more likely to report viewing the image in the social event from an
observer perspective than individuals with blood/injury phobia or
non-patients. This suggests that recalling anxious social memories 4.1. Recall of early adverse social experiences
from an observer perspective may be specific to individuals with
greater social-evaluative concerns. Further research has shown that The recall of adverse social memories from earlier in the
the use of observer perspective imagery is associated with more individual's life may indicate that social fears in social anxiety
frequent negative thoughts, more safety behaviors, and worse self- disorder develop in response to a specific conditioning episode of a
evaluation of social performance (Spurr & Stopa, 2003). In addition, traumatic social experience. Although negative life-events generally
D'Argembeau et al. (2006) found an observer perspective bias for precipitate phobia onset (Magee, 1999; Öst & Hugdahl, 1981),
social events recalled by individuals with social anxiety disorder, but Rachman (2002) states that social fears develop gradually and cannot
not for non-anxious controls. be linked to specific conditioning experiences. Contrary to this,
These findings are supported by research which has shown that Stemberger, Turner, Beidel, and Calhoun (1995) found that 56% of a
the level of anxiety associated with a social encounter influences sample with specific social phobia and 40% of a sample with
whether the event is recalled from an observer or field perspective. generalized social phobia were able to recall a traumatic conditioning
294 J. Morgan / Clinical Psychology Review 30 (2010) 288–297

episode involving an uncomfortable social situation that marked the 5. Cognitive processing styles and autobiographical memories in
start of their social fears. social anxiety
Distinguishing between specific types of life experiences that are
more likely to be drawn upon by socially anxious individuals may be The research reviewed thus far suggests that certain types of
an important direction for future research. Initial research in this area emotional autobiographical memories may be more accessible to
suggests that the recall of memories related to teasing during socially anxious individuals. However, it is likely that there are certain
childhood is associated with social anxiety and anxiety sensitivity in circumstances under which these memories are more likely to be
adulthood (Roth, Coles, & Heimberg, 2002). The retrospective nature recalled and under which such memories exert stronger emotional
of these studies limits the conclusions that can be drawn. In particular, responses. For example, Magee and Zinbarg (2007) examined how the
it is difficult to ascertain whether Roth et al.'s study indicates that type of cognitive processing style used in response to emotional social
teasing actually led to the development of high trait levels of social memories could affect levels of state anxiety in socially anxious
anxiety, or whether the individual's current self-view intensifies or individuals. They found that when high socially anxious individuals
distorts the adversity of the event recalled. However, the studies recalled and focused on a negative memory of a social interaction,
suggest that socially anxious individuals may be more likely to draw they experienced larger increases in self-reported shyness than when
upon these adverse childhood experiences in adulthood. This may instructed to suppress the memory. The same pattern was not evident
have important implications for the maintenance of the disorder; in low socially anxious individuals. Examining the relationship
particularly given that such memories may prevent an individual from between information processing styles and autobiographical memo-
updating their current self-view. ries may therefore provide further insight into how such cognitive
processes interact and how certain cognitive styles may influence the
4.2. Imagery and early autobiographical memories types of memories recalled in social anxiety.

During social situations individuals with social anxiety direct their 5.1. Effects of imagery on autobiographical memory recall
attention inwardly towards monitoring internal bodily sensations,
negative images of the self, and negative thoughts concerning the Self-imagery may exert an important influence on memory
social situation (see Spurr & Stopa, 2002). This internal self- accessibility in social anxiety. As described above, current distorted
monitoring is often referred to as self-focused attention and has been images of the self appear to be based on memories of negative social
theorized to play an important role in heightening the anxious experiences (Hackmann et al., 2000). A recent study has shown
response during social events. Cognitive models of social anxiety additionally that self-imagery may actually affect the type of
predict that this increase in self-focused attention should lead autobiographical memories recalled by socially anxious individuals.
individuals to make erroneous inferences regarding how they believe Stopa and Jenkins (2007) investigated whether the valence of an
they appear to others during a social situation (Clark & Wells, 1995). image held in mind by high socially anxious individuals during a
In line with this, recent studies in social anxiety have shown that such speech would affect the types of autobiographical memories retrieved
erroneous inferences are a result of negative self-imagery during in response to positive, negative, and neutral cue words. When
social interactions (Hirsch, Clark, Mathews, & Williams, 2003; Hirsch, holding a negative image in mind, participants spent longer retrieving
Meynen, & Clark, 2004). positive than negative memories. However, there was no difference in
Distorted images of the self appear to support negative beliefs retrieval latencies between negative and neutral memories, suggest-
about the way the individual sees themselves in social situations. ing that negative imagery suppresses the retrieval of more positively
However, an emerging line of research further suggests that these valenced memories. Although the study did not employ a comparison
negative self-images may actually be based upon adverse social group of low socially anxious individuals, these initial findings suggest
experiences from earlier in the individual's life. Using an interview that negative self-imagery may have inhibitory effects on the retrieval
procedure, Hackmann et al. (2000) asked 22 patients with social of autobiographical memories in social anxiety.
anxiety disorder to bring to mind the recurrent images they Further research has shown that imagery for social events may affect
frequently experience during anxiety-provoking social situations. the recall of social memories. Vassilopoulos (2008) asked high and low
The patients were subsequently asked whether they could recall a socially anxious individuals to imagine future anxiety-provoking events
particular memory which was closely linked to the recurrent image. using vignettes of social scenarios. Participants then recorded their
The majority of patients (96%) reported a specific memory of an thoughts about the scenarios and were asked to describe a memory of a
adverse social event that was closely linked to the recurrent image in similar social event that came to mind after reading each vignette.
terms of both sensory detail (e.g., bodily sensations and perceptions) Although there was no difference between the high and low social
and interpersonal content. For 41% of the patients, the event recalled anxiety groups in the number of negative and neutral memories
had happened in the same year as the onset of the disorder. For 81% of recalled, the high social anxiety group did recall less positive events
the patients, the recalled event had happened no more than a year from the past. Interestingly, Vassilopoulos suggests this is due to a self-
following onset of the disorder. It therefore appears that the self- enhancing process in low socially anxious individuals, which is not
images drawn upon by socially anxious individuals contain essences engaged in by the high socially anxious individuals.
of adverse social events from the past. This proposal is further
supported by recent studies that highlight the benefits of adapting 5.2. Effects of rumination on autobiographical memory recall
treatment techniques such as imagery rescripting (see Holmes, Arntz,
& Smucker, 2007) to directly modify early memories that are linked to Clark and Wells' (1995) cognitive model suggests two stages in the
these images. Through identifying memories associated with recur- anxiety response when socially anxious individuals may be particu-
rent images and then using cognitive restructuring to update the larly vulnerable to dwelling on adverse memories: before entering a
meaning of these memories, Wild, Hackmann, and Clark (2007, 2008) social interaction and after leaving a social event. Clark and Wells refer
have shown that imagery techniques can be used by patients with to these stages as anticipatory processing and post-event processing,
social anxiety disorder to link new meanings to distressing social respectively.
memories. Wild et al. (2008) found that updating memories in this
way significantly reduced the distress and vividness associated with 5.2.1. Anticipatory processing and memory recall
the memories and led to improvements in more general maladaptive During anticipatory processing, individuals engage in a pre-
thoughts and beliefs associated with social anxiety. mortem of the social event in which they may review the possible
J. Morgan / Clinical Psychology Review 30 (2010) 288–297 295

outcomes of the social event in detail. Clark and Wells (1995) propose engaging in a reflective response style. Engaging in a ruminative
that this pre-mortem may be dominated by recollections of past response style therefore appears to enhance the accessibility of
failures, negative images of the self, and predictions of poor certain types of emotional memories for high but not low socially
performance and rejection in future social events. In line with this, anxious individuals. It could be argued that this type of elaborative
Hinrichsen and Clark (2003) have shown that in anticipation of an thinking reconfirms and strengthens existing dysfunctional assump-
anxiety-provoking social event, high socially anxious individuals tions people with social anxiety hold about themselves (e.g.,
report experiencing more negative bodily sensations, are more likely assumptions about the ability to make a positive impression on
to catastrophize on what might happen during the event, dwell on others), therefore increasing the likelihood of retrieving memories
ways of avoiding or leaving the social event, and are more likely to associated with anxious emotion.
recall past perceived social failures than low socially anxious
individuals. However, this research was based on self-report and the 6. Directions for future research
recall of specific events was not further assessed (e.g., for specific
properties of the memories). Further research is therefore needed to Research findings reviewed above suggest that high socially
specify the processes involved in the recall of past events during anxious individuals do experience enhanced memory for threatening
anticipation. Interestingly, Mellings and Alden (2000) found no and highly emotional autobiographical material. Furthermore, it
support for the notion that social anticipation enhances access to appears that memories which are highly representative of current
negative information about previous social events. However, they did self-perceptions tend to be more accessible to individuals with social
find that ruminative thinking following a social interaction predicted anxiety. Several lines of enquiry are important for future research
recall of negative self-related information. This tentatively suggests studies in this area. First, researchers should consider the value of
that recalling negative social cues and past perceived social failures further examining whether the negative impact of certain types of
may be associated with cognitive processes which follow a social memories maintains and exacerbates social anxiety. Second, given
event rather than those which characterize the anticipation of the that certain cognitive processing styles may be particularly influential
event. in maintaining social anxiety, future research examining the role of
these response styles in memory processes should be considered.
5.2.2. Post-event processing and memory recall Current research emphasizes the importance of examining memory
After leaving an anxiety-provoking social event, socially anxious for life-events in individuals with high social anxiety. While it is unclear
individuals engage in ruminative thought about the preceding social whether certain life-events have a greater negative impact for high
event (Mellings & Alden, 2000; Rachman, Grüter-Andrew, & Shafran, socially anxious individuals, research suggests that memory for events
2000) and show a bias for the recall of anxiety-related behavior occurring around the onset of social anxiety may be particularly in-
(Edwards, Rapee, & Franklin, 2003). It has been further suggested that fluential in the formation of current images of the self (Hackmann et al.,
such tendency to engage in rumination may occur in interaction with 2000; Wild et al., 2008). Accumulation of socially stressful life-events
memories of past social performance (Abbott & Rapee, 2004; Clark & may therefore contribute to the emergence and maintenance of social
Wells, 1995). Although the influence of rumination on memory recall anxiety. Future research should focus on examining whether high
has been more readily associated with mood-congruency studies socially anxious individuals display a bias for recalling memories which
(McFarland & Buehler, 1998) and research in depression (Lyubo- reflect enduring concerns and ongoing conflicts (e.g., self-defining
mirsky, Caldwell, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 1998), recent research suggests memories, Conway et al., 2004) and the role such memories play in
that ruminative responses, particularly following an ambiguous social maintaining dysfunctional beliefs about the self. Following research in
event, may lead to a bias in memory recall in social anxiety. Field and self-appraisal theory (Ross & Wilson, 2002), it is possible that in-
Morgan (2004) asked high and low socially anxious individuals to dividuals with social anxiety will perceive certain social memories as
describe a recent personal social interaction which they felt had been subjectively closer in time because they repeatedly draw upon these
ambiguous (i.e., uncertain about whether the social interaction had memories when forming negative images of the self.
gone well). Participants then either focused on negative aspects of the Research reviewed suggests that biases in autobiographical
interaction (i.e., negative post-event processing), positive aspects of memory are a central feature of social anxiety, however, it has not
the interaction (i.e., positive post-event processing), or engaged in a always been clear how to study the way in which adverse memories
distraction task. Following this, participants engaged in free recall of are actively involved in maintaining social anxiety. Initial research
memories of personal events and experiences. These memories were exploring the recall of social-threat memories in individuals with
subsequently rated along three dimensions of negativity, anxiety, and social anxiety – memories which are theoretically assumed to be
shame. Field and Morgan found that regardless of the type of post- associated with intense emotion for these individuals – generally
event processing engaged in, high socially anxious individuals either failed to find differences between high and low social anxiety
recalled memories rated as more negative and shameful than low groups or found only weak evidence for the presence of memory
socially anxious individuals. This study suggests that post-event biases (Rapee et al., 1994; Wenzel et al., 2002; Wenzel, Pinna et al.,
processing may therefore lead to a bias in the retrieval of past events 2004; Wenzel, Werner et al., 2004). It is likely that the way a socially-
and experiences in social anxiety. threatening event is encoded into memory will affect the strength of
Morgan and Banerjee (2008) have further demonstrated that the recollection for that event. For example, Rubin (2005) states that
style of post-event processing engaged in by socially anxious while in clinical disorders such as PTSD and panic disorder a highly
individuals can affect subsequent recall of memories. Based on negative event has actually occurred and the event itself is therefore
previous theoretical work (Treynor, Gonzalez, & Nolen-Hoeksema, strongly encoded in memory, in social anxiety disorder a highly
2003), Morgan and Banerjee distinguished between two types of negative event tends to be anticipated but the event itself may not
response style: a ruminative response style characterized by a always be socially stressful and therefore a threatening memory is not
tendency to dwell passively and repetitively on the causes and encoded. The research reviewed shows that there are a range of
consequences of negative feelings, and a reflective response style factors affecting the recall of anxiety-provoking memories in social
characterized by an openness to acknowledging and accepting anxiety, and it is likely that certain stages in the anxiety response may
negative thoughts and feelings. They found that compared to low differentially affect the encoding of these memories. Future research
socially anxious individuals, high socially anxious individuals who should focus on comparing memory for the anticipation of a social
engaged in a ruminative response style following an imagined social event, memory for the event itself, and memory for post-event pro-
event were more likely to recall anxious memories than after cesses to examine how vivid, detailed, and emotional these memories
296 J. Morgan / Clinical Psychology Review 30 (2010) 288–297

are. This would further test the view that there are certain stages in Anderson, S. J., & Conway, M. A. (1993). Investigating the structure of autobiographical
memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 19,
the social anxiety response when autobiographical memory processes 1178−1196.
are more influential in maintaining dysfunctional beliefs about the Anderson, B., Goldin, P. R., Kurita, K., & Gross, J. J. (2008). Self-representation in social
self. anxiety disorder: Linguistic analysis of autobiographical narratives. Behaviour
Research and Therapy, 46, 1119−1125.
Cognitive models (Clark & Wells, 1995; Rapee & Heimberg, 1997) Beck, A. T., Emery, G., & Greenberg, R. (1985). Anxiety disorders and phobias: A cognitive
state that the experience of social anxiety is dependent on the activation perspective. New York: Basic Books.
of specific information processing biases and a negative self-schema. Bögels, S. M., van Oosten, A., Muris, P., & Smulders, D. (2001). Familial correlates of social
anxiety in children and adolescents. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 273−287.
Following from this, individuals with social anxiety should be more Burke, M., & Mathews, A. (1992). Autobiographical memory and clinical anxiety. Cog-
likely to demonstrate memory biases when such schemas are activated nition and Emotion, 6, 23−35.
and when engaged in specific types of cognitive processing. On this Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of behavior. Cambridge:
Cambridge University Press.
basis, it seems important that studies should employ a social-evaluative
Clark, D. M., & McManus, F. (2002). Information processing in social phobia. Biological
task either before or during the retrieval of autobiographical memories. Psychiatry, 51, 92−100.
Research that has examined the effect of cognitive processing styles on Clark, D. M., & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In R. G. Heimberg, M.
autobiographical memory (e.g., imagery and rumination) has shown Liebowitz, D. A. Hope, & F. R. Schneier (Eds.), Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment,
and treatment (pp. 69−92). New York: Guilford Press.
that these processing styles can either facilitate the retrieval of Coles, M. E., & Heimberg, R. G. (2002). Memory biases in the anxiety disorders. Clinical
memories with certain affective qualities (Morgan & Banerjee, 2008) Psychology Review, 22, 587−627.
or inhibit the retrieval of these memories (Stopa & Jenkins, 2007). Coles, M. E., Turk, C. L., Heimberg, R. G., & Fresco, D. M. (2001). Effects of varying levels
of anxiety within social situations: Relationship to memory perspective and
Further research into the facilitation and inhibition of autobiographical attributions in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 651−665.
memories in social anxiety will be important for understanding the role Conway, M. A. (1990). Autobiographical memory: An introduction. Milton Keynes: Open
of these memory processes in the maintenance of this disorder. University Press.
Conway, M. A. (1996). Autobiographical knowledge and autobiographical memories.
Research that has examined the effect of cognitive processing In D. C. Rubin (Ed.), Remembering our past: Studies in autobiographical memory
styles on memory recall has to date been carried out on analogue (pp. 67−93). New York: Cambridge University Press.
samples of high and low socially anxious individuals as determined by Conway, M. A., & Pleydell-Pearce, C. W. (2000). The construction of autobiographical
memories in the self-memory system. Psychological Review, 107, 261−288.
scores on various social anxiety measures. It is generally argued that Conway, M. A., Singer, J. A., & Tagini, A. (2004). The self and autobiographical memory:
anxiety disorders can be conceptualized on a continuum between low Correspondence and coherence. Social Cognition, 22, 491−529.
and high levels of anxiety rather than as discrete “all or none” cate- D'Argembeau, A., Van der Linden, M., d'Acremont, M., & Mayers, I. (2006). Phenomenal
characteristics of autobiographical memories for social and non-social events in
gories (Vriends, Becker, Meyer, Michael, & Margraf, 2007). Therefore,
social phobia. Memory, 14, 637−647.
research carried out with high socially anxious samples of non-clinical Edwards, S. L., Rapee, R. M., & Franklin, J. (2003). Post-event rumination and recall bias
groups provides a good exploratory framework for determining for a social performance event in high and low socially anxious individuals. Cog-
whether certain information processing biases are more likely to be nitive Therapy and Research, 27, 603−617.
Erwin, B. A., Heimberg, R. G., Marx, B. P., & Franklin, M. E. (2006). Traumatic and socially
evident in individuals with clinical disorders. However, until research stressful life events among persons with social anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety
is carried out to confirm the presence of these memory biases in Disorders, 20, 896−914.
clinical groups, the generalization of the results from these studies to Field, A. P., & Morgan, J. (2004). Post-event processing and the retrieval of
autobiographical memories in socially anxious individuals. Journal of Anxiety
clinically referred individuals with social anxiety disorder remains Disorders, 18, 647−663.
open to question. Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Foa, E. B., & Amir, N. (1999). Attentional biases for facial
In conclusion, several lines of evidence suggest that social anxiety expressions in social phobia: The effects of target and distractor in “face-in-the-
crowd” task. Cognition & Emotion, 13, 305−318.
may be characterized by biases in autobiographical memory recall. Hackmann, A., Clark, D. M., & McManus, F. (2000). Recurrent images and early
Such biases include: 1) recall of social memories with properties memories in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 601−610.
relating to self-referential information, 2) an imagery bias such that Harvey, A. G., Ehlers, A., & Clark, D. M. (2005). Learning history in social phobia.
Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 33, 257−271.
socially anxious individuals recall memories of anxiety-provoking Heinrichs, N., & Hofmann, S. G. (2001). Information processing in social phobia: A
social events from an observer perspective and base current images of critical review. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 741−770.
the self on memories of early adverse social experiences, and 3) a bias Hinrichsen, H., & Clark, D. M. (2003). Anticipatory processing in social anxiety: Two
pilot studies. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 34, 205−218.
in the types of memories recalled when engaged in specific styles of
Hirsch, C. R., & Clark, D. M. (2004). Information-processing bias in social phobia. Clinical
cognitive processing. The research findings reviewed have important Psychology Review, 24, 799−825.
implications for the treatment of social anxiety disorder. As discussed Hirsch, C. R., Clark, D. M., Mathews, A., & Williams, R. (2003). Self-images play a causal
above, rescripting distressing social memories can improve the role in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 909−921.
Hirsch, C. R., Meynen, T., & Clark, D. M. (2004). Negative self-imagery in social anxiety
symptoms of social anxiety and can lead to significant changes in contaminates social situations. Memory, 12, 496−506.
social cognitions (Wild et al., 2008). Further research in social anxiety Holmes, E. A., Arntz, A., & Smucker, M. R. (2007). Imagery rescripting in cognitive
should clarify which particular properties of social memories are behaviour therapy: Images, treatment techniques and outcomes. Journal of
Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38, 297−305.
important to restructure in memory rescripting to lessen the distress Holmes, E. A., Mathews, A., Mackintosh, B., & Dalgleish, T. (2008). The causal effect of
associated with these memories. In addition, recent research has mental imagery on emotion assessed using picture-word cues. Emotion, 8, 395−409.
highlighted the benefits of Mindfulness Based Cognitive Therapy for Josephson, B., Singer, J. A., & Salovey, P. (1996). Mood regulation and memory:
Repairing sad moods with happy memories. Cognition and Emotion, 10, 437−444.
individuals with social anxiety disorder (Kocovski, Fleming, & Rector, Kangas, M., Henry, J. L., & Bryant, R. A. (2005). A prospective study of autobiographical
2009) and for reducing ruminative tendencies (Teasdale et al., 2000). memory and posttraumatic stress disorder following cancer. Journal of Consulting
It is possible that this treatment approach could lessen the impact of and Clinical Psychology, 73, 293−299.
Kendler, K. S., Karkowski, L. M., & Prescott, C. A. (1999). Fears and phobias: Reliability
certain cognitive processing styles such as rumination on the recall of
and heritability. Psychological Medicine, 29, 539−553.
adverse autobiographical memories. Further research should there- Kocovski, N. L., Fleming, J. E., & Rector, N. A. (2009). Mindfulness and acceptance-based group
fore consider whether such interventions can be used to regulate the therapy for social anxiety disorder: An open trial. Cognitive Behavioral Practice, 16,
276−289.
retrieval of adverse social memories in social anxiety.
La Greca, A. M., & Lopez, N. (1998). Social anxiety among adolescents: Linkages with
peer relations and friendships. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 83−94.
References Levy, E. A., & Mineka, S. (1998). Anxiety and mood-congruent autobiographical
memory: A conceptual failure to replicate. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 625−634.
Abbott, M. J., & Rapee, R. M. (2004). Post-event rumination and negative self-appraisal Lyubomirsky, S., Caldwell, N. D., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Effects of ruminative
in social phobia before and after treatment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, and distracting responses to depressed mood on retrieval of autobiographical
136−144. memories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 166−177.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental Magee, W. J. (1999). Effects of negative life experiences on phobia onset. Social
disorders, 4th edition Washington DC: APA. Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 34, 343−351.
J. Morgan / Clinical Psychology Review 30 (2010) 288–297 297

Magee, J. C., & Zinbarg, R. E. (2007). Suppressing and focusing on a negative memory in graphical memories in people with and without posttraumatic stress disorder
social anxiety: Effects on unwanted thoughts and mood. Behaviour Research and symptoms. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 591−614.
Therapy, 45, 2836−2849. Schultz, L. T., & Heimberg, R. G. (2008). Attentional focus in social anxiety disorder:
McFarland, C., & Buehler, R. (1998). The impact of negative affect on autobiographical Potential for interactive processes. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 1206−1221.
memory: The role of self-focused attention to moods. Journal of Personality and Singer, J. A., & Blagov, P. S. (2004). The integrative function of narrative processing:
Social Psychology, 75, 1424−1440. Autobiographical memory, self-defining memories, and the life story of identity. In
McNally, R. J., Lasko, N. B., Macklin, M. L., & Pitman, R. K. (1995). Autobiographical D. Beike, J. Lampinen, & D. Behrend (Eds.), The self and memory (pp. 117−138).
memory disturbance in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour New York: The Psychology Press.
Research and Therapy, 33, 619−630. Spurr, J. M., & Stopa, L. (2002). Self-focused attention in social phobia and social anxiety.
McNally, R. J., Litz, B. T., Prassas, A., Shin, L. M., & Weathers, F. W. (1994). Emotional Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 947−975.
priming of autobiographical memory in post-traumatic stress disorder. Cognition & Spurr, J. M., & Stopa, L. (2003). The observer perspective: Effects on social anxiety and
Emotion, 8, 351−367. performance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1009−1028.
McNally, R. J., Otto, M. W., & Hornig, C. D. (2001). The voice of emotional memory: Stemberger, R. T., Turner, S. M., Beidel, D. C., & Calhoun, K. S. (1995). Social phobia: An
Content-filtered speech in panic disorder, social phobia, and major depressive analysis of possible developmental factors. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104,
disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 39, 1329−1337. 526−531.
Mellings, T. M. B., & Alden, L. E. (2000). Cognitive processes in social anxiety: The effects Stopa, L., & Jenkins, A. (2007). Images of the self in social anxiety: Effects on the retrieval of
of self-focus, rumination and anticipatory processing. Behaviour Research and autobiographical memories. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry,
Therapy, 38, 243−257. 38, 459−473.
Mick, M. A., & Telch, M. J. (1998). Social anxiety and history of behavioural inhibition in Sutherland, K., & Bryant, R. A. (2005). Self-defining memories in posttraumatic stress
young adults. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 12, 1−20. disorder. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 591−598.
Mitte, K. (2008). Memory bias for threatening information in anxiety and anxiety Sutherland, K., & Bryant, R. A. (2008). Autobiographical memory and the self-memory
disorders: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 886−911. system in post-traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 555−560.
Moffitt, K. H., & Singer, J. A. (1994). Continuity in the life story: Self-defining memories, Sutin, A. R., & Robins, R. W. (2005). Continuity and correlates of emotions and motives
affect, and approach/avoidance personal strivings. Journal of Personality, 62, 21−43. in self-defining memories. Journal of Personality, 73, 793−824.
Moore, S. A., & Zoellner, L. A. (2007). Overgeneral autobiographical memory and Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., Ridgeway, V. A., Soulsby, J. M., & Lau, M.
traumatic events: An evaluative review. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 419−437. (2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-
Morgan, J., & Banerjee, R. (2008). Post-event processing and autobiographical memory based cognitive therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 615−623.
in social anxiety: The influence of negative feedback and rumination. Journal of Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A
Anxiety Disorders, 22, 1190−1204. psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247−259.
Musa, C., Lépine, J. P., Clark, D. M., Mansell, W., & Ehlers, A. (2003). Selective attention in Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving, & W. Donaldson (Eds.),
social phobia and the moderating effect of a concurrent depressive disorder. Be- Organisation of memory (pp. 381−403). London: Academic Press.
haviour Research and Therapy, 41, 1043−1054. Vassilopoulos, S. P. (2008). Coping strategies and anticipatory processing in high and
Neal, J. A., Edelmann, R. J., & Glachan, M. (2002). Behavioural inhibition and symptoms low socially anxious individuals. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 98−107.
of anxiety and depression: Is there a specific relationship with social phobia? Veljaca, K. A., & Rapee, R. M. (1998). Detection of negative and positive audience
British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41, 361−374. behaviours by socially anxious subjects. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36,
Neisser, U. (1982). Snapshots or benchmarks? In U. Neisser (Ed.), Memory observed: 311−321.
Remembering in natural contexts (pp. 43−48). San Francisco: Freeman. Vriends, N., Becker, E. S., Meyer, A., Michael, T., & Margraf, J. (2007). Subtypes of social
Neisser, U. (1986). Nested structure in autobiographical memory. In D. C. Rubin (Ed.), phobia: Are they of any use? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 59−75.
Autobiographical memory (pp. 71−81). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Wells, A., Clark, D. M., & Ahmad, S. (1998). How do I look with my mind's eye?
Öst, L. G., & Hugdahl, K. (1981). Acquisition of phobias and anxiety response patterns in Perspective taking in social phobic imagery. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36,
clinical patients. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 19, 439−447. 631−634.
Rachman, S. (2002). Fears born and bred: Non-associative fear acquisition? Behaviour Wells, A., & Papageorgiou, C. (1999). The observer perspective: Biased imagery in social
Research and Therapy, 40, 121−126. phobia, agoraphobia, and blood/injury phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37,
Rachman, S., Grüter-Andrew, J., & Shafran, R. (2000). Post-event processing in social 653−658.
anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 611−617. Wenzel, A., Jackson, L. C., & Holt, C. S. (2002). Social phobia and the recall of
Rapee, R. M., & Heimberg, R. G. (1997). A cognitive-behavioural model of anxiety in autobiographical memories. Depression and Anxiety, 15, 186−189.
social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 35, 741−756. Wenzel, A., Pinna, K., & Rubin, D. C. (2004). Autobiographical memories of anxiety-
Rapee, R. M., McCallum, S. L., Melville, L. F., Ravenscroft, H., & Rodney, J. M. (1994). related experiences. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 329−341.
Memory bias in social phobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 32, 89−99. Wenzel, A., Werner, M. M., Cochran, C. K., & Holt, C. S. (2004). A differential pattern of
Richards, A., & Whittaker, T. M. (1990). Effects of anxiety and mood manipulation in autobiographical memory retrieval in social phobic and nonanxious individuals.
autobiographical memory. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 29, 145−153. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 32, 1−13.
Ross, M., & Wilson, A. E. (2002). It feels like yesterday: Self-esteem, valence of personal Wild, J., Hackmann, A., & Clark, D. M. (2007). When the present visits the past: Updating
past experiences, and judgments of subjective distance. Journal of Personality and traumatic memories in social phobia. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental
Social Psychology, 82, 792−803. Psychiatry, 38, 386−401.
Roth, D. A., Coles, M. E., & Heimberg, R. G. (2002). The relationship between memories Wild, J., Hackmann, A., & Clark, D. M. (2008). Rescripting early memories linked to
for childhood teasing and anxiety and depression in adulthood. Journal of Anxiety negative images in social phobia: A pilot study. Behavior Therapy, 39, 47−56.
Disorders, 16, 149−164. Williams, J. M. G., Barnhofer, T., Crane, C., Hermans, D., Raes, F., Watkins, E., & Dalgleish,
Rubin, D. C. (1996). Remembering our past: Studies in autobiographical memory. New T. (2007). Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psycholo-
York: Cambridge University Press. gical Bulletin, 133, 122−148.
Rubin, D. C. (2005). A basic-systems approach to autobiographical memory. Current Wilson, A. E., & Ross, M. (2001). From chump to champ: People's appraisals of their earlier
Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 79−83. and present selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 572−584.
Rubin, D. C., Boals, A., & Berntsen, D. (2008). Memory in posttraumatic stress disorder:
Properties of voluntary and involuntary, traumatic and nontraumatic autobio-

You might also like