You are on page 1of 26

12843.C04.

PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 125

4
Paired Comparisons as a Fine-Tuning Tool
in Hearing Aid Fittings

FRANCIS K. KUK

The Need to Go Beyond Prescriptive sure such indices (Cox and Bisset, 1982; Skin-
Formulas ner and Miller, 1983). Fluctuations in the lis-
tener’s internal criteria could also affect the
The use of prescriptive formulas to specify reliability of such indices. Additionally, the
frequency-gain characteristics of hearing presence of a fluctuating loss (e.g., Meniere’s
aids simplifies hearing aid selection. Satisfac- disease) or of an ear with atypical middle ear
tion is assumed when the measured gain of resonance and impedance characteristics
the hearing aids matches the gain prescribed may result in the prescribed frequency-gain
by the formulas. Knowledge of individual response as being inappropriate for the hear-
preference and psychophysical skills are not ing aid wearer (Gilman et al, 1981). These
required or needed when using this selection observations suggest that, unless one can
process. An implicit requirement for the use control the intra- and intersubject variability
of prescriptive formulas is that audiologic in- seen in audiologic measurements and ac-
dices [e.g., thresholds, most comfortable lis- count for the individual differences when
tening levels (MCLs), and loudness discom- specifying frequency-gain response, there is
fort levels (LDLs)] can be measured reliably the potential that the prescribed frequency-
and accurately. Furthermore, when predict- gain response may be inappropriate for the
ing 2-cc coupler frequency-gain characteris- wearer.
tics, the formulas require that the individual Assuming that audiologic indices can be
wearer have ear canal characteristics that re- defined accurately and that target gain may
semble those of the average ear. be achieved, individual frequency-gain re-
Unfortunately, neither of these require- sponses specified by group data (as in the
ments can be met all the time. For example, prescriptive approach) may not be satisfac-
several investigators have shown that audi- tory to all hearing aid wearers. For example,
tory thresholds could vary from 4 to 9 dB Neuman et al (1987) showed that seven
hearing level (HL) upon retest (Byrne and of eight hearing aid wearers preferred fre-
Dillon, 1981; Skinner and Miller, 1983). quency-gain responses that were different
Suprathreshold loudness judgments (i.e., from those prescribed when using an MCL
MCLs, LDLs) can be even more variable de- approach (Pascoe, 1978). Kuk and Pape
pending on instructions, stimulus type, and (1992) reported that 18 of 20 subjects se-
psychophysical method employed to mea- lected frequency-gain responses that devi-

125
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 126

126 STRATEGIES FOR SELECTING AND VERIFYING HEARING AID FITTINGS

ated from the National Acoustic Laborato- 2000). Although these newer compression
ries’ (NAL) prescription (Byrne and Dillon, hearing aids and digital signal processing
1986). On the other hand, Byrne and Cotton (DSP) hearing aids promise new and better
(1988) reported that only 10 to 20% of their ways to serve individuals with hearing im-
hearing aid wearers selected a frequency- pairment, prescriptive formulas that are
gain response that deviated significantly available were based on single-channel, lin-
from the NAL prescription. These observa- ear signal processing hearing aids. Thus,
tions suggest that additional measures are these formulas did not consider the effect of
needed to verify the appropriateness of the the number of processing channels and re-
prescribed frequency-gain response to en- lease times on the actual real-world output
sure maximum satisfaction in all wearers. of the hearing aids (Kuk and Ludvigsen,
Alternative methods to select frequency- 1999). Consequently, although the output of
gain response on hearing aids may be nec- multichannel nonlinear hearing aids may
essary. There is increasing evidence that match a prescriptive target, the wearer may
hearing aid wearers prefer a unique fre- be dissatisfied with the real-world output of
quency-gain response from their hearing the hearing aids. Unlike linear hearing aids
aids when they listen in various acoustic en- with which there are at least several gener-
vironments and/or when they use different ally accepted approaches for their fitting,
criteria to select preferred frequency-gain re- there are only a few manufacturers of non-
sponses. For example, Byrne (1986) found linear hearing aids that provide guidelines
that preferred frequency-gain response that to clinicians on fitting these devices. Unfor-
was selected with a criterion of “pleasant- tunately, methods to evaluate the adequacy
ness” was different from that selected based of the manufacturers’ guidelines or of the
on the judgment of “intelligibility.” Kuk performance of these devices are lacking. As
(1990) demonstrated that the preferred inser- in traditional linear hearing aids, verifica-
tion gain for listening to one’s own vocaliza- tion of optimal settings on these devices is
tion is different from listening to externally necessary to ensure success with the special
presented stimuli. Tecca and Goldstein (1984) circuits. One approach that can help to ver-
showed that hearing-impaired listeners pre- ify the appropriateness of the selected hear-
ferred less low-frequency gain from a hear- ing aid settings and can potentially be help-
ing aid as the stimulus level was increased. ful in hearing aid selection and fine-tuning
Kuk et al (1994) showed that the preferred is the paired comparison technique.
frequency responses for speech in noise are
different depending on the signal-to-noise
What Is Paired Comparison?
ratio (SNR) of the input signal. Presently, no
guidelines are available to specify how these The method of paired comparison was at-
frequency-gain responses should be pre- tributed to Fechner as a data collection tech-
scribed. The concerned clinician needs to en- nique for the study of sensory perception
sure that the measured frequency-gain re- (Thurstone, 1927). In this method, an experi-
sponse provides optimal performance under mental subject makes binary decisions on a
these special listening situations or that the number of stimuli that are presented in
frequency-gain response can be modified to pairs. The experimenter usually sets the cri-
accommodate the listener’s needs in these teria of judgment. The subject’s task is to in-
situations. dicate which one of the two comparison
Advances in technology improve the so- stimulus intervals meets the experimenter’s
phistication of hearing aids. Today, 70% of criterion. In hearing aid research, the results
hearing aids sold in the United States are ei- of the comparison may show the relative
ther nonlinear analog hearing aids (21%) or “perceptual” distances among a list of items
programmable and digital hearing aids (49%) being compared (Punch et al, 1980), or the
that use nonlinear signal processing (Skafte, comparison may show the relative ranking
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 127

CHAPTER 4 ■ PAIRED COMPARISONS AS A FINE-TUNING TOOL IN HEARING AID FITTINGS 127

of the comparison hearing aids (Punch, combinations of settings are stored in the
1978). Moreover, the comparison may result temporary memory of each programmable/
in a recommendation of the best hearing digital hearing aid. The clinician activates
aids (Studebaker et al, 1978). Finally, a com- the first setting of the hearing aid, alerts the
bination of settings on programmable or listener of hearing aid setting A, and plays
digital hearing aids may be recommended back the stimulus. Typically, short (between
based on the results of this process (Levitt et 10 and 20 seconds) discourse passages can
al, 1978; Neuman et al, 1987; Kuk and Pape, be used as stimuli for comparison. After-
1992). ward, the clinician activates the second set-
Figure 4–1 is a block diagram representa- ting of the hearing aid, announces hearing
tion of the sequence of events involved in aid setting B, and plays back the same pas-
a paired comparison trial. Two settings (e.g., sage for comparison. This process is re-
different low-frequency gains) on a pro- peated until the listener is ready to make a
grammable hearing aid are compared. There decision on the preferred hearing aid set-
are three important components in a single ting. Because of the way the hearing aid set-
paired comparison trial: the instructions, tings are labeled, paired comparison is also
the presentation, and the decision. In the in- commonly known as AB comparison.
structional stage, the clinician explains the The listener must make a preference judg-
task and specifies the criterion for judgment ment on the two hearing aid settings in the
to the listener. For example, one may use decision stage. He or she must decide if
the following instructions for selection of a hearing aid setting A or hearing aid setting B
hearing aid setting that maximizes speech meets the criterion of better speech intelligi-
intelligibility: bility regardless of the similarity or dissimi-
larity between the two comparison hearing
You will listen to a short passage played aids. Responses like “no difference” and
back twice, each time with a different
hearing aid setting. As the passage is
“they both sound good/bad” are not accept-
played back, I will indicate to you which able. If such is the response, the listener will
is hearing aid A and which is hearing aid be reinstructed, and the same stimulus will
B. After you have listened through both be presented again.
hearing aid settings, you must indicate Although the performance of hearing aids
which hearing aid (A or B) yields more
intelligible speech. By that I mean the
selected using paired comparison technique
hearing aid setting with which you can has been favorable, absolute performance
understand more of the spoken passage. is not measured, and thus its performance
You must indicate one preference even is not guaranteed. The hearing aid settings
though you may find these two hearing that are selected using paired comparisons
aid settings to sound very similar. I will
gladly repeat the presentation if you so
reflect the listener’s relative judgment for
desire. Do you have any questions? the settings available for comparison. Maxi-
mum satisfaction is guaranteed only if at
During the presentation stage, the clini- least one of the available combinations re-
cian adjusts the hearing aids to the appro- sults in maximum satisfaction. Direct mea-
priate settings for comparison. These two surement of listener satisfaction with the

Figure 4–1. Sequence of events in


a single paired comparison trial.
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 128

128 STRATEGIES FOR SELECTING AND VERIFYING HEARING AID FITTINGS

selected hearing aid settings is needed to de- were recorded and played back in the earlier
termine absolute performance. paired comparison trials. This approach of
recording and then playing back the stimuli
was improved by subsequent investigators.
History of the Use of Paired
For example, a Knowles Electronic Manikin
Comparison in Hearing Aid Selection
for Acoustic Research (KEMAR) with a Zwis-
A requirement for using paired comparison locki coupler was used to record in the sound
to evaluate hearing aids in the clinic is the field to approximate the acoustic effects asso-
ability to switch rapidly among various elec- ciated with the head and body baffle and ear
troacoustic settings (or hearing aids) for canal response (Punch, 1978). Hearing aid re-
comparison. This was impossible using con- ceivers and/or earmolds were used as out-
ventional hearing aids without undue delay put transducers in later studies (Studebaker
during the switching process. Zerlin (1962) et al, 1980).
is credited with first proposing a manage- Although this approach allows rapid com-
able way of performing paired comparison. parison between speech processed by differ-
In Zerlin’s method, speech was recorded ent hearing aid settings, this is impractical
through two hearing aids that were coupled for clinical use because of the labor involved
to separate couplers. Output from the cou- in the recording process, the limitations in
plers was recorded on two separate tracks of the number and variations of hearing aid
a magnetic tape. Different pairs of hearing settings that can be compared, the time in-
aids were connected to the couplers, and volved in clinical comparison, and the diffi-
processed speech was recorded in the se- culty of transferring the results of the paired
quence in which it would be presented dur- comparison to commercial hearing aid use.
ing the evaluation. The output from the tape Paired comparison was primarily used as a
recordings was presented to the listeners research tool.
through earphones. Listeners switched be- Clinical pairwise quality judgments have
tween the two tracks of the tape and indi- been made with a master hearing aid (Wat-
cated preferences for one of the two taped- son and Knudsen, 1940; Pascoe, 1975). Using
speech segments. This was used to indicate this approach, the clinician adjusts the set-
their preference for the hearing aid that was tings on a master hearing aid (the size of a
used to process the speech signal. Figure 4–2 portable audiometer), and speech (taped or
shows a schematic diagram of how stimuli live) is delivered through headphones to the

Figure 4–2. Instrumentation in-


volved in original paired com-
parison trials.
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 129

CHAPTER 4 ■ PAIRED COMPARISONS AS A FINE-TUNING TOOL IN HEARING AID FITTINGS 129

listeners, who then indicate their preference conditions and for a wide range of hearing
as the settings are adjusted and presented in loss configurations.
pairs. The setting that is preferred will be Second, in many digital and programma-
recommended. Although the labor involved ble hearing aids, switching of settings can
in recording speech stimuli is eliminated, ac- be performed rapidly through the use of a
curate simulation of true hearing aid perfor- programming cord or via a remote control
mance in commercial hearing aids is still a device using frequency-modulated (radio-
major difficulty with the master hearing aid frequency and ultrasonic) signals. This al-
approach. Guidelines to vary the settings lows rapid switching between comparison
systematically were also lacking. settings.
Levitt et al (1978) were the first to vary the Third, the availability of multiple memo-
frequency-gain responses systematically on ries in some digital and programmable hear-
a wearable master hearing aid using an ing aids also facilitates comparison of fre-
adaptive procedure (the simplex procedure). quency-gain responses in the clinic and in
In this procedure, listeners identified non- everyday listening situations.
sense syllables while wearing a master hear- Fourth, several manufacturers of digital
ing aid that was adjusted to various settings. and programmable hearing aids can inter-
Through selective testing of choice electro- face their units directly to external comput-
acoustic settings, the simplex procedure con- ers so that paired comparison can be per-
verges at a combination of settings on the formed in an automatic manner. The computer
master hearing aid that yields a maximum controls for stimulus delivery, tracks re-
speech understanding score. Although the sponses, and adjusts settings on the hearing
method had significant appeal, it was not aid according to defined rules and algo-
adopted for clinical use because of its com- rithms. This could significantly reduce the
plexity, inefficiency, and dependence on a time involved in the comparison and im-
computer. Several hours were needed to se- prove the reliability in which comparisons
lect a frequency-gain response using the sim- are made. Indeed, Kuk (1992) demonstrated
plex procedure. the feasibility of adapting the use of the mod-
Neuman et al (1987) modified the simplex ified simplex procedure to select frequency-
procedure to incorporate the use of pairwise gain responses for a commercially available
comparison. Rather than comparing speech programmable multimemory hearing aid.
recognition scores among the selected set- Some recent digital hearing aids also have
tings, the modified simplex procedure used automated algorithms that allow for paired
subjective judgment of relative intelligibility comparisons.
as a criterion and allowed systematic pair-
wise comparison of settings. These modifi-
Paired Comparison Techniques
cations substantially reduced the time to se-
lect a frequency-gain response. Although paired comparison involves only
The advent of digital and programmable binary decisions on pairs of stimuli, stimu-
hearing aids facilitates the use of paired lus pairing (i.e., the manner in which the
comparisons in a clinical environment. Four different settings are paired and compared)
features of such hearing aids contributed affects the information available from the
significantly in this regard. The reader is comparison. The different strategies in which
warned that not all digital and all program- this technique has been used in hearing aid
mable hearing aids share all four features. research include the round-robin tournament,
First, the wide range of electroacoustic ad- single- and double-elimination tournaments,
justments on some digital and programma- simple up-down procedure, and the modified
ble hearing aids allows them to be viewed as simplex procedure. Although these strategies
stand-alone wearable master hearing aids can be performed either manually or through
and to be used for a wide range of listening the assistance of a PC-based software, the
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 130

130 STRATEGIES FOR SELECTING AND VERIFYING HEARING AID FITTINGS

use of a computer with custom software telligibility (or any other criteria) as they lis-
could greatly facilitate comparison. ten to discourse passages presented in a
noise background. Assuming that the win-
ners of these comparisons are B, A, A, B, B,
Round-Robin Tournament
and C, respectively (second row), the rela-
The object of a round-robin tournament is to tive ranks of these four hearing aids settings
rank order the available hearing aids or dif- will be B, A, C, and D because B has the most
ferent settings within hearing aids based on wins and D has no wins at all. As indicated
some defined criteria. In this approach, earlier, the result of a round-robin tourna-
every hearing aid (or combinations of set- ment is a rank order of hearing aid prescrip-
tings) is paired and compared with every tions based on subject preference.
other hearing aid (or settings). For N hearing The advantage of the round-robin tourna-
aids or combinations of settings, a round- ment is its ability to rank order hearing
robin tournament involves N(N  1)/2 pairs aids. In addition to selecting the best hearing
of comparisons. As a result, hearing aids are aid (or setting) among the comparisons, the
ranked according to the frequency in which round-robin tournament also allows the study
they are chosen. of relationships among electroacoustic para-
Figure 4–3 illustrates the manner in which meters and their relative contribution to the
hearing aids containing the necessary set- perceptual process. For example, Punch et al
tings appropriate to achieve one of four pre- (1980) used this approach to study factors
scriptive formulas (i.e., A, B, C, and D) are governing subjective preference for hearing
compared in a round-robin tournament (i.e., aid processed speech.
N = 4). For four hearing aid settings, the From a clinical standpoint, the round-
number of comparisons would be 4(4  1)/2 robin tournament may be practical only if
or 6. This includes the comparisons between (1) specific ranking information is needed
A and B, A and C, A and D, B and C, B and D, among the comparison hearing aids; and (2)
and C and D (bottom row). Subjects may be the number of comparison hearing aids (or
asked to judge which one of two hearing aid settings) is small (i.e., under four). Other
settings provides clearer speech or better in- methods may be more efficient to verify the

Figure 4–3. Example of a round-robin tournament. Letters A to D represent four different


hearing aids each fitted using specific prescriptive formulas.
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 131

CHAPTER 4 ■ PAIRED COMPARISONS AS A FINE-TUNING TOOL IN HEARING AID FITTINGS 131

appropriateness of a selected frequency-gain of the comparison, in this case A, C, E, and


response. For example, Neuman et al (1987) G, are compared in a second round where A
reported that it required an average of 83.8 is compared to C, and E is compared to G.
minutes to complete a round-robin tourna- The winners of the second round of compar-
ment, but only 8.0 minutes to complete a ison, A and E, are further compared in the
modified simplex procedure when the same third round to determine a winner (A). A
combinations of settings (a total of 25) are hearing aid is eliminated if it loses any com-
compared. parison. However, a hearing aid can only be-
come the winner when it has won M rounds
of comparison where 2M equals N, the total
Single-Elimination Tournament
number of hearing aids. For example, in Fig-
The object of an elimination tournament is to ure 4–4, three rounds of comparisons are
determine which one of several hearing aids needed to determine a winner when eight
(or combinations of settings within a hearing (23) hearing aids are compared. No ranking
aid) is more preferable than the others. In information is available from the results of
this approach, each hearing aid is first com- an elimination tournament.
pared with one other hearing aid. Winners The manner in which hearing aids are
of each pair of comparison are further com- paired in a single-elimination tournament is
pared several times to result in an overall important. Comparison hearing aids can be
winner. For N hearing aids where N is an in- paired either by random assignment or ac-
teger power of 2, there will be N  1 pairs of cording to some seeding rules. A good seed-
comparison hearing aids. ing rule is to pair hearing aids with greater
An example of a single-elimination tour- difference in their frequency-gain character-
nament is shown in Figure 4–4 where eight istics during the first round. Variability in
hearing aids are compared. Letters identify the selection will be increased if hearing aids
the hearing aids in the comparison (A–H). In with similar electroacoustic characteristics
the first round of comparison, A is compared are paired (White and Studebaker, 1978;
to B, C to D, E to F, and G to H. The winners Montgomery et al, 1982).

Figure 4–4. Example of a single-elimination tournament. Letters A to H represent eight


different hearing aids for comparison.
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 132

132 STRATEGIES FOR SELECTING AND VERIFYING HEARING AID FITTINGS

Double-Elimination Tournament bracket. On the other hand, losers in this


round are compared among themselves (B
When two hearing aids with relatively simi-
with D and F with H) to yield the winners in
lar characteristics are compared, it is possible
the losers’ bracket. During the second round
that the “better” hearing aid is eliminated
of comparison between A and C and E and
because of random error. To safeguard a
G, the losers of these comparisons in the win-
“good” hearing aid from being eliminated in
ners’ bracket (C and G) are compared with
the early rounds of comparison, Studebaker
the winners (B and F) in the losers’ bracket
and his colleagues (1979a, 1980) proposed
after the first round of comparison. In
the use of a double-elimination tournament
essence, the losers in the winners’ bracket are
as a means to compare hearing aids (or com-
given one more chance to compare with the
binations of settings within a single hearing
winners in the losers’ bracket. The winner in
aid). Instead of being eliminated after only
each bracket (A in winners’ bracket and F in
one loss, each hearing aid has to lose twice
losers’ bracket) is compared one last time to
before it is eliminated from the tournament.
determine the overall winner (i.e., hearing
Higher test-retest reliability was reported
aid A). There are 2(N  1) pairs of compar-
with the double-elimination tournament
isons for N hearing aids, where N is an inte-
than with the single-elimination tournament
ger power of 2. In this example, there are a
(Studebaker et al, 1979a).
total of 2(8  1) or 14 pairs of comparisons.
Figure 4–5 illustrates a double-elimination
tournament. Assume again that the same
Simple Up-Down Procedure
eight hearing aids (A–H) are compared. Dur-
ing the first round of comparison, hearing The “goodness” of the verified settings using
aid A is compared to hearing aid B, C to D, E a round-robin or an elimination tournament
to F, and G to H. Winners of this round depends on the “goodness” of all settings se-
of comparison (in this case A, C, E, and G) lected for comparison. One may need to
are further compared within the winners’ compare with a large number of settings to

Figure 4–5. Example of a double-elimination tournament. Letters A to H represent eight


different hearing aids for comparison.
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 133

CHAPTER 4 ■ PAIRED COMPARISONS AS A FINE-TUNING TOOL IN HEARING AID FITTINGS 133

ensure that a recommended setting is indeed that the listener may find most satisfactory.
the best setting available on the hearing aids. This initial setting is the initial estimate. This
Because of potentially large numbers of set- initial setting can be based on any prescrip-
tings that need to be compared and the time tive formula, on manufacturer recommenda-
constraints in a clinical setting, the use of tions, or on clinician intuition. When the
round-robin and elimination tournaments method of paired comparison is used, this
may not be practical. initial estimate is compared with another set-
An adaptive procedure is an estimation ting that differs from the initial estimate on
procedure in which a listener’s response on the same adjustable parameter of the hearing
a test trial dictates the direction of stimulus aid. A selected criterion (e.g., relative intelli-
change in the next trial. This helps to focus gibility) is used for comparison between two
comparisons on only those settings con- settings. Any acoustic stimuli can be used for
tributing to the estimation and allows one to judgment (e.g., discourse presented in noise).
fine-tune a selected setting without testing a Comparison continues with different set-
large number of settings. As in all estimation tings that are varied in a systematic manner
procedures, one assumes the existence of only until the criterion to terminate comparison
one combination of hearing aid settings that is met.
will optimize listener preference. The goal of Figure 4–6 is an illustration of a simple
the adaptive procedure is to estimate that up-down procedure to determine optimal
setting in the shortest amount of time while low-frequency setting on a hearing aid. As-
satisfying the clinician’s and/or the hearing sume that the initial estimate corresponds to
aid wearer’s criteria. a low-frequency setting at interval 3 (open
The simple up-down procedure is an rectangle). The listener compares this setting
adaptive procedure that allows verification with interval 4 (black rectangle). The direc-
(and fine-tuning) of settings in one dimen- tion of comparison during the first trial may
sion. In using this procedure, the clinician be arbitrary or purposeful. The change in
first estimates the initial hearing aid setting adjustment between these settings is termed

Figure 4–6. Example of a simple


up-down procedure to fine-tune op-
timal low-frequency setting on a
hearing aid.
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 134

134 STRATEGIES FOR SELECTING AND VERIFYING HEARING AID FITTINGS

a step, and the magnitude of such change is will be needed if these assumptions are not
termed the step size. Assume that the listener met.
prefers the setting with more low-frequency Because not all settings are evaluated in
gain (i.e., interval 4); this indicates that the the simple up-down procedure, an occa-
direction of the next comparison should be sional listener not showing strong preference
in the direction of more low-frequency gain for a particular setting may reveal random
(i.e., intervals 4 and 5 will be compared). A preference during the evaluation and lead
comparison in the same direction is termed a to errors in the estimation procedure. The
run (trials 1 and 2). If during the second trial round-robin and elimination tournaments,
the listener prefers less low-frequency gain because of their sampling of all comparison
(i.e., interval 4 to interval 5), a reversal (in the settings, will not be as affected by homoge-
direction of preference) has occurred. In Fig- neous preference.
ure 4–6, reversals occurred after trials 2, 3, There are several issues to consider when
and 4. Although one can continue the com- using a simple up-down procedure. These
parison indefinitely, comparison is termi- issues are fully discussed by Levitt (1970,
nated after the third reversal, with interval 4 1978, 1992) but will be briefly reviewed for
being the listener’s preferred low-frequency completeness.
setting on the hearing aid. This preferred
setting is termed the final estimate. In this
choice of initial estimate
case, the initial estimation of optimal low-
frequency gain differs from the listener’s In theory, the initial estimate should be as
preference by one interval. similar to the final estimate as possible to en-
The simple up-down procedure differs sure maximum efficiency. The disadvantage
from the round-robin and elimination tour- of a dissimilar initial estimate is that it will
naments in the manner in which settings are require more comparisons to reach the final
selected for comparison and the number of estimate. Kuk and Lau (1995a) compared the
comparisons. Assume that eight intervals on convergence time (time to reach the final esti-
the low-frequency dimension are compared. mate) of four initial estimates [NAL revised
The round-robin tournament requires all (NAL-R), low- and high-frequency varia-
eight intervals to be compared with each tions of NAL-R, and a flat response). They
other to result in 28 (or 8(8  1)/2) pairs of found that all four initial estimates resulted
comparisons. Seven (or 8  1) pairs of com- in the same final estimate, but the con-
parison are needed in the single-elimination vergence time differed. The NAL-R setting
tournament and 14 (or 2(8  1)) pairs are showed the least convergence time and was
needed in the double-elimination tourna- the closest to the final estimate. The advan-
ment. In the simple up-down procedure il- tage of an initial estimate that is very differ-
lustrated previously, only four comparisons ent from the final estimate is that these esti-
are needed to estimate the preferred low- mates may enable listeners to discriminate
frequency gain. better the differences between stimulus inter-
It is important to note that whereas all in- vals and to be better familiarized with the
tervals are compared in the round-robin and test routine. Levitt (1970, 1978, 1992) recom-
elimination tournaments, only the intervals mended that data obtained from the first run
near the final estimate are compared several should not be analyzed so as to minimize
times in the simple up-down procedure. In- bias resulting from an inappropriate initial
tervals 1, 2, 6, 7, and 8 are not compared. estimate.
This observation, however, is only true Different approaches have been attempted
when the initial estimate is close to the final to determine the best initial estimate. For ex-
estimate and when the listener is consistent ample, Neuman et al (1987) used a fre-
in his or her judgments. More comparisons quency-gain response that placed the aver-
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 135

CHAPTER 4 ■ PAIRED COMPARISONS AS A FINE-TUNING TOOL IN HEARING AID FITTINGS 135

age speech spectrum at the user’s MCL as the listener is until after the comparison has
initial estimate. Kuk and Pape (1992) used started. As a general rule, the step size used
the frequency-gain response recommended must be larger than the just noticeable differ-
by NAL-R as the initial estimate. Both stud- ence (JND) of the wearer. For the average
ies, however, showed that the final estimates hearing-impaired wearer, this is roughly 5 dB
deviated from the initial estimates for a ma- gain change in the low- or high-frequency
jority of subjects. Furthermore, the listening settings (Kuk, 1994). Because hearing-
conditions, types of stimuli, and criteria used impaired wearers differ in their psychophysi-
to make the comparison can alter the final es- cal abilities, the use of a fixed step size may
timate. In other words, there may not be one not be the most efficient.
fixed optimal initial estimate for all test Robbins and Monro (1951) recommended
conditions. Clinicians who use this technique starting with a large step size (arriving at the
for the first time may consider following vicinity of the final estimate sooner) and
the approach of Leijon et al (1991) and Kuk gradually reducing the step size as compari-
and Pape (1992) by using frequency-gain re- son continues. Mathematically, the step size
sponse recommended by NAL-R as the ini- on run N is d/N, where d is the step size
tial estimate. After trying this on a few listen- used on the first run. For example, the step
ers (perhaps 10 to 20), it should become size for the third run may be reduced to one
possible for clinicians to choose initial esti- interval if the step size for the first run was
mates based on their knowledge of the final set at three intervals. In practice, this rule
estimates obtained under the same listening may be difficult to implement on commer-
condition. Furthermore, the default settings cial analog hearing aids because most can
recommended by most manufacturers of vary only in limited intervals on each pa-
digital and programmable hearing aids are rameter. This may not be a problem with
also good initial estimates to use when per- digital and programmable hearing aids. A
forming paired comparisons with a commer- compromise is to start the first run with a
cial hearing aid. step size of two intervals and reduce it to
An alternative approach is to compare one interval after the first reversal.
several distinct settings (less than four) on
the hearing aid using a tournament strategy.
termination rule
The winner of the comparisons may be used
as the initial estimate for selecting the best Rules to terminate an adaptive procedure
setting available on the hearing aids. are necessary after the clinician is reasonably
certain that the final estimate reflects the lis-
tener’s preference. In the example provided
step size
earlier, three reversals were required to ter-
The ideal step size for statistical efficiency de- minate the comparison. In theory, one can
pends on the consistency of the listener’s re- terminate a comparison after the first rever-
sponse and the accuracy of the preceding esti- sal. The accuracy, however, of the estimation
mates. Theoretically, a small step size should may not be acceptable. Precision and relia-
be used if the listener is consistent in his or bility in estimation improve as the number
her response and if the estimate is close to the of reversals is increased. However, the time
final estimate. When using small step sizes, involved in the estimation will be increased
the precision of the estimation is increased, also. In practice, three reversals can be used
but at the expense of increasing evaluation with fair reliability (Neuman et al, 1987; Kuk
time. A large step size allows one to converge and Pape, 1992).
at the final estimate quickly, but at the ex- The simple up-down procedure, in its var-
pense of losing estimation precision. In prac- ious forms, has been used to estimate thresh-
tice, one would not know how consistent a olds, to estimate MCL (Wall and Gans, 1984),
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 136

136 STRATEGIES FOR SELECTING AND VERIFYING HEARING AID FITTINGS

and to select and verify low-frequency gain combination of settings that reflects the lis-
on a noise reduction hearing aid (Kuk et al, tener’s preference for the listening condition.
1992). Figure 4–7 shows the matrix representa-
tion of the frequency-gain response of a pro-
grammable hearing aid in the high- and
Modified Simplex Procedure
low-frequency dimensions. Each cell on the
The simplex procedure was originally pro- matrix represents a unique combination of
posed by Box (1957) as a means to optimize high- and low-frequency gain. For example,
productivity. It was adapted by Levitt et al in Figure 4–7, cell (2L, 2H) represents the fre-
(1978) and later modified by Neuman et al quency response with a low-frequency set-
(1987) as an alternative to select the optimal ting of 2 and a high-frequency setting of 2. A
settings on more than one parameter on a total of 5  5 or 25 combinations of electro-
wearable master hearing aid. Like the sim- acoustic settings are available for compari-
ple up-down procedure, the modified sim- son in this example.
plex procedure assumes the existence of one Assume that cell (2L, 2H) represents the
and only one combination of settings on a initial estimate (I). Comparisons will be per-
hearing aid that optimizes listening under a formed in the low- and high-frequency di-
specific condition. mensions with the initial estimate as the ver-
Like the simple up-down procedure, the tex of the comparison. The direction of the
process starts with an estimation of the lis- first comparison can be arbitrary (i.e., cells
tener’s preferred settings on the hearing aid with more or less gain can be chosen for com-
(prescriptive phase to determine initial esti- parison). In this example, we choose cells
mate). This combination is compared with with less gain. In the low-frequency region,
other combinations of settings in a system- one compares (2L, 2H) with (3L, 2H), and in
atic manner (adaptive phase to determine the high-frequency region one compares (2L,
final estimate) until the termination rules are 2H) with (2L, 3H). Assume that the listener
met. The goal of the comparison is to select a prefers less low-frequency gain and more

Figure 4–7. Matrix representation


of a modified simplex procedure to
fine-tune optimal low- and high-
frequency settings on a hearing aid.
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 137

CHAPTER 4 ■ PAIRED COMPARISONS AS A FINE-TUNING TOOL IN HEARING AID FITTINGS 137

high-frequency gain. That is, cell (3L, 2H) is of parameters to include in the comparison.
preferred over cell (2L, 2H), and cell (2L, 2H) Increasing the number of parameters to
is preferred over cell (2L, 3H). The winning compare will increase the number of unique
cells are indicated by a number sign (#). combinations of settings dramatically. In
The vertex of the next comparison will be general, the number of combinations is given
formed by coordinates of the winning cells. by Nm, where N is the number of stimulus
Because “3L” and “2H” are the winning in- intervals within a dimension and m is the
tervals in the low- and high-frequency re- number of dimensions. For example, if three
gions, respectively, cell (3L, 2H) becomes the dimensions each with five intervals are com-
new vertex (II). The direction of the new pared, a total of 53 (125) settings will be avail-
comparison is with cells with even less low- able for comparison. Although not all combi-
frequency gain (i.e., 4L) and greater high- nations are compared, it is inevitable that
frequency gain (i.e., 1H). The comparison more comparisons are necessary as the num-
cells will be (4L, 2H) and (3L, 1H). Assuming ber of dimensions is increased. To keep the
that the listener selects cell (3L, 2H) over cell number of comparisons manageable, a rule
(4L, 2H) and cell (3L, 2H) over cell (3L, 1H), of thumb is to compare only those parame-
the new vertex is again formed at cell (3L, ters whose optimal settings one cannot easily
2H). However, the direction of the next com- predict.
parison is reversed to cells with more low- Similar to the simple up-down procedure,
frequency gain and less high-frequency gain. listeners who do not show a strong prefer-
This is recorded as the first reversal. ence for only one combination of settings
As illustrated in Figure 4–7, the second re- (i.e., multiple preferences or same preference
versal is encountered after the third compar- for all settings) may exhibit random prefer-
ison when cell (3L, 2H) is compared with cell ence judgments during paired comparisons
(2L, 2H) in the low-frequency region and cell and lead to errors in the estimation process.
(3L, 3H) in the high-frequency region. The A sampling of the listener’s preference prior
third reversal is encountered after the fourth to using the modified simplex procedure
comparison when cell (3L, 2H) is compared may be helpful. Despite this potential limita-
with cell (4L, 2H) in the low-frequency re- tion, Neuman et al (1987) and Kuk and Pape
gion and cell (3L, 1H) in the high-frequency (1992) failed to find any of their subjects (N =
region. If the termination rule is set at three 8 and N = 20, respectively) who showed mul-
reversals, the new vertex formed at cell (3L, tiple preferences in their studies. In a subse-
2H) will become the final estimate or pre- quent study, Kuk and Lau (1996a) reported
ferred frequency-gain response for the lis- that six of their seven subjects showed a
tener. strong preference during their paired com-
This example shows a case in which all the parison judgments when the stimulus was
comparison settings are within the range of presented at a positive SNR. However, only
values available on the specific instrument. four subjects showed a strong preference
For listeners who may prefer a parametric when a negative SNR was used. This sug-
value that is beyond the range available on gests the possibility that the stimulus condi-
the instrument, Levitt (1978) recommended tions (and possibly criteria also) may affect
that testing proceed with a value within the the strength of the preference.
available range as if a reversal had occurred. The modified simplex procedure yields a
Issues that are important to consider in final estimate that agrees remarkably well
the simple up-down procedure are also im- with those selected with round-robin and
portant to consider in the modified simplex double-elimination tournaments. Neuman et
procedure. These issues include initial esti- al (1987) showed that four of eight subjects
mate, step size, and termination rules. An participating in their study chose the same
additional consideration in the modified setting as the final estimate using the three
simplex procedure is the optimal number procedures. The remaining subjects chose an
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 138

138 STRATEGIES FOR SELECTING AND VERIFYING HEARING AID FITTINGS

adjacent cell as the final setting. Further- ing loss required an 8% difference in word
more, an average of 8.0 minutes was re- recognition scores between two comparison
quired to complete a modified simplex pro- hearing aids to correctly select the one with
cedure, but 36.3 minutes was necessary to better intelligibility in 75% of the compar-
complete a double-elimination tournament, isons. Normal-hearing subjects require only
and 83.8 minutes was necessary to complete a 3% difference in word recognition scores to
a round-robin tournament. Based on the re- achieve the same level of selection.
sults of these studies, the modified simplex Byrne and colleagues (Byrne, 1986; Mur-
procedure may be the most efficient of the ray and Byrne, 1986; Byrne et al, 1990) evalu-
three procedures. ated the sensitivity of paired comparison to
differentiate frequency-gain responses that
are more homogeneous in electroacoustic
Advantages of the Paired characteristics than those reported in earlier
Comparison Technique studies. Despite the homogeneity, these au-
The use of paired comparison as a clinical thors reported that 70 to 80% of the compar-
tool to select hearing aid settings has been isons showed a significant preference for
suggested since the late 1970s (Punch, 1978; one frequency-gain response, whereas few
Punch and Howard, 1978; Punch et al, 1980; significant differences were observed when
Studebaker et al, 1978, 1979a,b, 1980; Punch using speech recognition testing.
et al, 1991). It is generally agreed that this Purdy and Pavlovic (1992) compared the
method provides a viable alternative with frequency responses selected using paired
which to verify and select frequency-gain comparison, magnitude estimation, and
characteristics of hearing aids. Some of the category rating tasks in elderly listeners.
advantages of the paired comparison tech- Their results showed that all three proce-
nique in relation to conventional methods dures are equally sensitive to differentiate
are discussed in the following sections. small changes in frequency-response charac-
teristics.
Preminger et al (2000) compared speech
Greater Sensitivity
recognition scores and speech intelligibility
The most frequently cited advantage of ratings with frequency responses selected
paired comparison is its sensitivity over with a modified simplex approach and that
speech recognition tests in differentiating the selected by NAL-R. Two of the seven sub-
improvements provided by settings of dif- jects reported significantly better speech un-
ferent amplification systems even when such derstanding in the real world, whereas the
differences are not apparent when using rest of the subjects reported similar speech
speech recognition tests (Zerlin, 1962; Witter understanding with both methods of fre-
and Goldstein, 1971; Punch, 1978; Punch and quency response selection.
Howard, 1978; Studebaker et al, 1978, 1982;
Tecca and Goldstein, 1984; Studebaker and
Equal or Greater Reliability
Sherbecoe, 1988).
Studebaker et al (1980) compared the The reliability of the method of paired com-
number of times a hearing aid was correctly parison, or the consistency with which sub-
selected in a paired comparison task with jects select a preferred hearing aid, is report-
the absolute difference in word recognition edly high. Zerlin (1962) showed that 7 of 11
scores between two comparison hearing aids. subjects ranked the same hearing aid first in
They reported that correct identification in- both test and retest trials when using a paired
creased exponentially as the difference in comparison procedure. Studebaker et al (1978)
mean word recognition scores between the revealed correlations (R) in excess of 0.70 for
two comparison hearing aids increased. For paired comparison data obtained across dif-
example, at a 0-dB SNR, subjects with a hear- ferent subject populations and in different lab-
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 139

CHAPTER 4 ■ PAIRED COMPARISONS AS A FINE-TUNING TOOL IN HEARING AID FITTINGS 139

oratory settings. For the same subjects, signifi- discourse passages as the criterion. An aver-
cantly lower correlations were obtained (be- age of 80% of all subjects showed less than
tween 0.40 and 0.70) on word recognition a 5-dB variation in their frequency-gain re-
tests. This illustrates the relative consistency sponse selection upon retest. A similar con-
with which reliability data on paired compari- sistency was seen when between-session and
son were obtained in different laboratories. within-session data were examined. Stelma-
Punch and Beck (1980) and Punch and Parker chowicz et al (1994), Eisenberg and Dirks
(1981) also showed higher reliability when (1995), and Eisenberg et al (1997) also showed
using pairwise quality judgments in compari- similar reliability. This reflects minimal learn-
son with word recognition testing. ing effects and suggests that this method can
The reliability of paired comparison judg- be expected to yield reliable results when
ment may be affected by test conditions and clear instructions are provided to elicit judg-
choice of judgment criteria. For example, ments. On the other hand, Purdy and Pavlovic
Punch (1978) found that subjects were most (1992) reported poorer reliability with paired
reliable when male speech was used as the comparison technique than the use of cate-
stimulus, whereas music yielded the least gory rating.
reliable selection. Punch and Howard (1978)
showed higher reliability when using clarity
Valid Predictor of Hearing Aid
judgments of connected discourse presented
Performance
in quiet than with intelligibility judgments
of discourse presented in noise. Studebaker Two approaches to validate the results of
et al (1979b) demonstrated higher reliability paired comparison include correlation with
in intelligibility rankings at a 0-dB SNR than speech recognition scores and real-world
at an SNR of +7 dB. Kuk and Pape (1992) re- evaluation of the selected settings. Results of
ported similar findings. correlation between paired comparison judg-
Individuals with hearing loss may not be ments and speech recognition scores varied
as reliable during paired comparison testing according to the criteria and test conditions
as normal hearing listeners. Studebaker et al used in the paired judgment. Punch and
(1980) compared the reliability of pairwise Howard (1978) reported low correlation (R =
judgment of relative intelligibility and word 0.46, to 0.34) between results of paired
recognition score [Northwestern University comparison of relative intelligibility and sen-
(NU)-6] between normal hearing and sub- tence scores on the Central Institute for the
jects with hearing loss. When paired com- Deaf (CID) Sentence Test. On the other hand,
parison data were analyzed, 83% of normal Studebaker et al (1978) using the same crite-
hearing subjects and 54% of subjects with a rion, found excellent correlation (R = 0.98)
hearing loss ranked the same hearing aid between results of paired comparison and
first in both test and retest sessions. When scores on the speech perception in noise
data from word recognition tests were ana- (SPIN) test.
lyzed, 42% of normal hearing subjects and Studebaker et al (1980) demonstrated the
49% of subjects with a hearing loss ranked validity of paired comparison judgment of
the same hearing aid first in both sessions. intelligibility by reporting that almost 73%
Schwartz et al (1979) also obtained similar of hearing aids chosen as the best during
findings. These data suggest that the relia- paired comparison judgment also received
bility of paired comparison is as good as, if the highest mean speech recognition score.
not better than, speech recognition. Neuman et al (1987) also showed that six of
Kuk and Pape (1992) evaluated the within- eight subjects obtained equal or higher indi-
session and between-session reliability in vidual speech recognition scores with hear-
which elderly hearing aid wearers (N = 20) ing aids selected using paired comparison
selected their preferred frequency-gain re- than those selected using an MCL approach
sponse using pairwise clarity judgment of (Pascoe, 1978).
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 140

140 STRATEGIES FOR SELECTING AND VERIFYING HEARING AID FITTINGS

On the other hand, pairwise judgment noise (SNR = +5 dB). Listeners completed a
of speech quality may not yield a high cor- questionnaire to indicate their satisfaction
relation when evaluating intelligibility. For with the hearing aid in 22 listening situa-
example, Punch and Parker (1981) reported tions. Subjects with a sloping hearing loss
negligible correlations between subjective (N = 10) showed similar preference for hear-
judgments of speech quality and speech ing aids fit with either approach. Of the nine
recognition scores despite significant correla- subjects who have a relatively flat hearing
tion (R = 0.70) between relative intelligibility loss, seven showed significantly higher satis-
judgment and speech recognition scores. De- faction for hearing aids selected with the
spite the low correlation between speech pairwise approach, and only one showed
quality judgment and measured speech in- higher satisfaction for a hearing aid selected
telligibility, Punch and Parker did not ob- with the NAL-R formula. Four of the seven
serve poorer measured speech intelligibility subjects selected more low-frequency gain
for hearing aids selected on the basis of qual- than NAL-R recommendation, whereas the
ity. A later study (Punch and Beck, 1986) con- remaining three subjects selected less low-
firmed the low correlation between speech frequency gain than NAL-R specification.
quality judgments and speech intelligibility In a subsequent study, Kuk (1994) pro-
scores. On the other hand, Studebaker and posed a screening procedure for the use of
Sherbecoe (1988) found that hearing aids se- the modified simplex. Subjects who were
lected on the basis of pairwise quality judg- screened to prefer an alternate frequency re-
ment provided better measured speech intelli- sponse from the NAL-R were further en-
gibility than hearing aids selected with gaged in the modified simplex to select their
magnitude estimation of speech intelligibility. preferred frequency response in different
This points to the potential difference between noise backgrounds. When asked to complete
hearing aids selected with paired comparison a questionnaire on their real-world satisfac-
and those with magnitude estimation. tion of the selected frequency response, the
Kuk and Lau (1996a) compared the pre- majority of subjects rated the frequency re-
ferred frequency gain response obtained sponse selected with the simplex procedure
through paired comparison and category higher than that prescribed by the NAL-R.
rating. Although the preferred frequency re- Preminger et al (2000) had a similar finding
sponse selected with paired comparison was in some of their subjects.
always unique (i.e., one setting only) and The results of these studies show that
was always highly rated, several subjects hearing aids selected with the paired com-
also showed similarly high ratings for adja- parison technique are just as effective as, if
cent frequency responses. This suggests that not more effective than, those selected with
the results of paired comparison are similar a prescriptive method. This suggests that the
to, and possibly more sensitive than, cate- use of paired comparison to verify hearing
gory rating. Eisenberg and her colleagues aid fitting may further enhance the fitting of
(1991, 1995, 1997) also showed high validity hearing aids for some wearers.
of the paired comparison technique com-
pared to category rating.
Ability to Judge Several
Hearing aid settings that are selected with
Subjective Attributes
paired comparison may result in increased
user satisfaction than those selected using a The method of paired comparison has been
prescriptive method. Kuk and Pape (1993) used with different criteria. This includes
evaluated listeners’ everyday satisfaction “overall quality” (Jeffers, 1960; Witter and
with hearing aids selected with the NAL-R Goldstein, 1971; Punch, 1978; Harris and
formula and those selected with pairwise Goldstein, 1979; Punch and Beck, 1980; Sulli-
clarity judgment of discourse passages read van et al, 1988; Leijon et al, 1991; Kuk et al,
by a male speaker and mixed in a babble 1992), “intelligibility” (Zerlin, 1962; Stude-
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 141

CHAPTER 4 ■ PAIRED COMPARISONS AS A FINE-TUNING TOOL IN HEARING AID FITTINGS 141

baker et al, 1982; Sullivan et al, 1988; Byrne et scores. Consequently, the time required to
al, 1990), and “pleasantness” or “natural- obtain a reliable result may be substantial.
ness” (Byrne, 1986; Murray and Byrne, 1986; Paired comparison procedures can be
Byrne and Cotton, 1988; Leijon et al, 1991). completed in substantially reduced time. Yet
Other scales, for example “hollowness” (Kuk the results can be equally satisfactory, if not
et al, 1992), “noise interference” (Kuk et al, providing greater satisfaction than the set-
1990), and “amount of distortion,” although tings that are selected using speech recogni-
not used with paired comparison, could tion tests. For example, an average of only 8
potentially be useful as criteria for paired minutes is required to select the low- and
comparison judgment. Kuk and Tyler (1990) high-frequency gain settings on a master
demonstrated that listeners with hearing im- hearing aid (Neuman et al, 1987). This
pairment could differentiate among various makes paired comparison technique an ideal
subjective criteria. These criteria could be clinical tool with which to examine a large
useful in evaluating nonlinear and newer number of hearing aid settings under differ-
types of signal processing hearing aids. ent listening conditions.

Ability to be Performed Under More Minimal Involvement of


Listening Conditions Auditory Memory
Byrne (1991) indicated that speech testing per- In the method of paired comparison, acous-
formed in quiet or in conditions of poor SNR tic stimuli processed by two hearing aid set-
(e.g., <10 dB) does not help to differentiate tings are presented sequentially with mini-
amplification systems and restricts the test mal time delay between presentations. This
conditions for which speech recognition tests reduces any memory factor that may affect
can be performed. He further indicated that the sensitivity of the judgment. Studebaker
paired comparison judgments are less suscep- (1982) suggested that judgment of small dif-
tible to the ceiling effect than are speech recog- ferences between stimuli is easier when per-
nition tests. Consequently, paired comparison formed in a comparative mode of minimal
may be used in more test conditions (e.g., dif- delay than in an isolated mode.
ferent SNR, different types of noise back-
grounds) than speech recognition tests to ver- Simple Instructions and Easy Task
ify the appropriateness of settings selected for
a programmable or digital hearing aid. Kuk et Listeners are instructed to choose from one
al (1994) used paired comparison to measure of two stimulus intervals that meets the set
the preferred frequency response at different criterion (e.g., better sound quality). A verbal
SNRs as well as different overall levels to esti- or manual response (i.e., press one of two re-
mate the preferred long-term frequency re- sponse buttons) is usually accepted. The task
sponse characteristics of hearing aids that can be performed by listeners of all ages. For
allow automatic gain regulation. example, Eisenberg and Levitt (1991), and
Eisenberg and Dirks (1995) reported that al-
Reduced Testing Time most all hearing-impaired children are capa-
ble of performing paired comparison tasks
The reliability of clinical speech recognition by 6.5 years of age. The oldest subject (88
test is related to the number of items on the years old) in a study by Kuk and Pape (1992)
test. For example, Studebaker (1982) calcu- reported no difficulty completing the paired
lated that a 10% difference between two comparison task.
speech scores is considered significant at the
5% probability level if 135 test items are pre-
Wide Applications
sented. At least 3381 test items must be pre-
sented if the same level of confidence is de- The method of paired comparison has been
sired for only a 2% difference between test used to study sound quality perception of
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 142

142 STRATEGIES FOR SELECTING AND VERIFYING HEARING AID FITTINGS

hearing aids (Punch et al, 1980), selection of potential may not be realized unless it is im-
MCL (Wall and Gans, 1984), adjustment of plemented properly. This section discusses
noise reduction hearing aids (Kuk et al, 1992), the considerations in implementing this tech-
comparison of frequency-gain responses for nique in the clinical setting.
different prescriptive formulas (Sullivan et al,
1988), selection of frequency-gain responses
Timing for Paired Comparison
for different listening situations (Kuk, 1990;
Kuk and Pape, 1992, Kuk et al, 1994), selec- An important issue to consider is when and
tion of gain for monaural and binaural hear- for whom paired comparison should be used.
ing aid fittings (Levitt et al, 1987; Punch et al, Although age of the subjects is not a major
1991; Balfour and Hawkins, 1992), and selec- factor (Eisenberg and colleagues, 1991, 1995,
tion of output-limiting preference in linear 1997; Kuk and Pape, 1992), it is unclear if
hearing aids (Hawkins and Naidoo, 1993). the amount of hearing aid experience of the
Neuman et al (1994, 1995) had also applied subjects would affect the reliability of the
the technique to select preferred compression judgments, especially if the judgments are
ratio and optimal release time on a compres- made during the initial visit. Kuk et al (2002)
sion hearing aid. As nonlinear hearing aids showed that experienced linear hearing aid
gain wider acceptance, it is conceivable that wearers with a severe-to-profound hearing
this technique will find even greater applica- loss did not experience the full benefit of a
tions with digital hearing aids in the future. digital nonlinear hearing aid especially for
low-level input sounds until after 1 month of
Individualized Fitting use of the device. This raises the question of
possible acclimatization effect as a confound-
The method of paired comparison not only ing variable in the observed results. One may
verifies if the selected settings are appropri- question if the results of paired comparison
ate for the individual listener but also speci- reflect the wearer’s experience with his or her
fies new settings if alternative settings are previous hearing aid settings (for experienced
preferred. The result of paired comparison wearers; unaided hearing for new wearers)
procedures can lead to a more appropriate and not a quest for the optimal setting?
combination of settings tailored to the indi- To examine this issue, Kuk and Lau (1996b)
vidual’s preference and psychophysical limi- correlated the preferred insertion gain on a
tations. Furthermore, there is the psychologi- programmable hearing aid obtained under
cal advantage to the listeners that they are six listening conditions (speech/noise levels
actually involved in the hearing aid evalua- of 55/50, 65/60, 75/70, 50/55, 60/65, 70/75
tion process. Unlike information provided by dBA) and one vocalization condition with the
prescriptive formulas, final settings recom- wearers’ used gain. The results showed that
mended by paired comparison procedures the amount of preferred insertion gain for lis-
are not restricted to frequency-gain response tening was not correlated with the wearers’
only. Other electroacoustic parameters, such used gain at any frequencies in any condition.
as compression settings (e.g., release time, On the other hand, the preferred insertion
compression ratios) or different types of sig- gain obtained during vocalization correlated
nal-processing techniques may also be exam- significantly (p < 0.05) with the wearers’ used
ined using this technique. gain. The authors suggested that wearer in-
sertion gain could affect preferred insertion
gain only when the test conditions are identi-
Integrating Paired Comparison in
cal to those that the hearing aid wearers expe-
Clinical Hearing Aid Fitting
rience in everyday life (i.e., own voice). In
Although previous research has demon- typical clinical situations, the preferred inser-
strated that the technique of paired compari- tion gain for listening is likely determined by
son is a powerful research tool, its clinical the stimulus characteristics and subjective
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 143

CHAPTER 4 ■ PAIRED COMPARISONS AS A FINE-TUNING TOOL IN HEARING AID FITTINGS 143

preference. The observation of acclimatiza- have been fitted with their hearing aids. Fre-
tion in Kuk et al (2002) originated from the quency-gain responses that are selected with
extra gain for low-input sounds [50-dB sound a prescriptive formula or recommended by
pressure level (SPL)] available on the digital the manufacturers should be used during
nonlinear hearing aids that were used during the interim. This 2- to 3-week delay gives the
the study. Many of the soft sounds that were wearers an opportunity to become ac-
inaudible to the wearers became audible, and quainted with the amplified sound. In addi-
over time, became meaningful. The preferred tion, it gives them an opportunity to identify
frequency responses were determined at a problem areas with the use of the hearing
suprathreshold level with a linear hearing aid aids, as well as the listening situations in
(Kuk and Lau, 1996b). Stimulus audibility which use of the selected frequency-gain re-
was never an issue, and thus hearing aid ex- sponse are less than satisfactory. Such infor-
perience should not confound the results of mation will form the basis for selecting stim-
paired comparisons. Berger and Hagberg ulus materials to use during subsequent
(1982) and Leijon et al (1990) reported similar paired comparisons.
impressions. Although paired comparison can and
Although wearer experience may not af- should be delayed for subsequent visits,
fect the result of paired comparison greatly the recommended frequency-gain response
(at least for frequency response adjustment), should be verified as optimal for the wearer
there are practical concerns to suggest delay- at least objectively during the initial fitting.
ing its use until later times. First, its high This means that one must ensure that the
sensitivity to the stimulus condition sug- chosen settings provide adequate gain for
gests that the choice of the stimulus condi- audibility of the softest speech sounds
tions would limit the outcome of the com- (around 20-dB HL across audiogram) and
parison. It will be impossible to select the comfort for loud sounds (above 100-dB SPL).
optimal stimulus condition for every hearing If real-ear target gain match is desired, make
aid wearer during the initial visit without sure that the target gain formula has consid-
knowing what listening conditions are en- ered the effects of nonlinear signal process-
countered by the wearers, and which condi- ing, effect of multiple channels, release time,
tions are difficult for them. Second, although etc. (Kuk and Ludvigsen, 1999) in the gain
the use of prescriptive formulas has its limi- formulation and that a composite signal be
tations, if used properly such formulas can used as the stimulus. Furthermore, because
yield adequate fitting for a good portion of the aided threshold represents the softest
potential hearing aid wearers. Furthermore, sound that a wearer can hear with his or her
even though the results of paired compari- nonlinear hearing aid, such an index should
son may yield a different set of frequency re- be measured to determine if the goal of audi-
sponse characteristics from the prescriptive bility for meaningful soft sounds is met (Kuk,
targets, the difference in responses may not 2001).
lead to functional difference in real life be- Thus the paired comparison technique
cause of the wearers’ psychophysical abili- should be used in the clinical setting during
ties or listening environments. Thus, the the follow-up visit as a fine-tuning (or trou-
time spent in paired comparison may not bleshooting) tool when the wearer has been
improve the fit of the hearing aid. Third, the given the opportunity to experience the rec-
time needed to carry out the procedure will ommended hearing aid settings for 2 to 3
necessarily limit the time that can be avail- weeks. Clinicians should start with the man-
able for other activities during the initial ufacturer’s recommended initial settings (be-
visit, for example counseling, verification, cause many are adjusted for device-specific
and so on. For these reasons, it may be strate- modification to the prescriptive target) and
gically desirable to delay the use of paired only fine-tune (via paired comparison) the
comparison to 2 to 3 weeks after the wearers recommended settings when the wearer re-
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 144

144 STRATEGIES FOR SELECTING AND VERIFYING HEARING AID FITTINGS

turns after the trial period with specific com- choose the right stimulus/test
plaints on the sound quality/performance of conditions
the hearing aid. To provide the best solution
An advantage of paired comparison is the
to the wearer, the following procedural steps
potentially infinite number of test condi-
are important to consider.
tions in which one can evaluate the selected
hearing aid settings. In theory, one should
understand the wearers’ complaints
compare hearing aid settings in listening
Many clinicians take the wearers’ com- conditions that are identified as difficult by
plaints at face value when trying to solve the hearing aid wearers. The task for the
them. For example, a wearer may say, “I can clinician is to have replications (i.e., on cas-
hear far but I cannot hear close.” Some clini- sette tape or compact disc) of these listening
cians will increase the gain parameter of the conditions and to evaluate the wearers using
hearing aid that controls gain for low input appropriate stimulus materials. This, how-
(below 60-dB SPL), assuming sounds that ever, may be clinically impractical because
are presented at close proximity will be at a no two wearers’ listening environments are
low-input level. On the other hand, many identical. Furthermore, there is the difficulty
wearers use the same phrase to describe of physically re-creating the various listen-
their experience in a noisy restaurant where ing environments in a clinical setting. A
they can hear people from tables away, but compromise may be to use standardized lis-
not understand the person sitting across the tening conditions for general purposes, but
table from them. In essence, this is a speech- to have available a few representative sound
in-noise problem and not an audibility prob- effects that would represent the listening sit-
lem. Adjusting the gain parameter for low uations frequently encountered by the popu-
input (below 60-dB SPL) would probably lation of hearing aid wearers who are served
not solve the wearer’s complaint. in the community.
Thus, it is critically important to under- It is important to realize that the level of
stand the wearer’s complaint thoroughly. the stimulus used and the SNRs employed
Ask questions that may help clarify the in the comparison could affect the appropri-
wearer’s complaint. One needs to know ateness of the selected frequency-gain re-
“what” is wrong, “when” the problem oc- sponse. Specifically, our experience indicates
curs, “who” is involved, “where” the prob- that frequency-gain responses selected with
lem occurs, and “how” the problem occurs. female discourse passages presented at 62-
Knowing the answers to these questions al- dB SPL [root mean square (RMS), A-scale] in
lows the clinician to determine if electro- the presence of babble noise and at a favor-
acoustic adjustment of the hearing aid set- able SNR (>+10 dB) are appropriate for typi-
tings is even necessary. Many times, the cal listening situations. The same speech
wearers’ complaints may have to do with re- stimulus presented in quiet at a low input
strictive situations, for example, “buzz” in- level of 50-dB SPL is appropriate for select-
terference with security alarms, talking to an ing optimal settings for soft sounds, whereas
old patient in a noisy hospital, etc., where the same stimulus presented at 72-dB SPL
counseling and setting realistic expectations and at a negative SNR may simulate loud,
for the hearing aid would be more appropri- noisy situations. These test conditions can be
ate instead. If there is indeed a problem, marked for general use with paired compar-
knowing the answers to these questions will ison. Cox and Alexander (1991) and Pear-
help the clinician understand the environ- sons et al (1977) also offered some speech
ments so that the appropriate test conditions and noise levels that can be used in different
for paired comparison can be set up and the listening conditions. Sound effects that may
appropriate electroacoustic parameters ad- be useful for selection of frequency-gain
justed during fine-tuning. responses in specific listening situations in-
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 145

CHAPTER 4 ■ PAIRED COMPARISONS AS A FINE-TUNING TOOL IN HEARING AID FITTINGS 145

clude wind noise, music (orchestral and in- loudness (UCL), or saturation SPL (SSPL)
dividual instruments), office noise (type- depending on the manufacturer. Conse-
writer noise, computer fan noise), restaurant quently, if one knows that the complaint is
noise, and traffic noise. Additionally, one primarily occurring at a medium input level,
may perform paired comparison in an one may focus on the Gainmedium parameter
empty office or a large hall to verify or select in the correct frequency band during the
frequency-gain response for listening in re- fine-tuning. It is not uncommon to have
verberant environments. The levels at which wearer complaints that may involve more
these sound effects may be presented need than one electroacoustic parameter. The clin-
further investigation. ician must be thorough to include all the in-
The criterion that one uses to verify or se- volved parameters.
lect a frequency-gain response is dependent
on the needs of the hearing aid wearer for determine the right form of
the frequency-gain response. If the intent is paired comparison
to maximize speech understanding ability,
such criterion must be specified. If the intent The type of information that one is interested
is to search for a “natural”-sounding hearing to know influences the form of paired com-
aid, such criterion must be indicated. An parison task, that is, whether one should per-
earlier section summarizes some of the crite- form a single comparison, round-robin, elimi-
ria that were used (and may be used) during nation tournaments, or adaptive strategies. As
paired comparison. It is important to re- indicated previously, if one is interested to
member that the criterion used will affect know if one of several alternatives would pro-
the outcome of the comparison (Byrne, 1986; vide better satisfaction to the wearer, one may
Kuk, 1990). A criterion of “clarity” is recom- perform a single comparison, round-robin, or
mended for general use because it is the cri- elimination tournament. It is not necessary
terion by which most hearing aid wearers that these frequency-gain responses be sys-
judge their hearing aids (Hagerman and tematically related to each other. Indeed, it
Gabrielsson, 1985). may be worthwhile to choose alternative set-
tings that are sufficiently different from the
determine the appropriate prescribed frequency-gain response to ensure
parameters to adjust maximum notable differences between com-
parison frequency-gain responses. Although
The reason for asking wearers specific ques- these tasks could indicate if any of the alterna-
tions on their complaints is that one may tives is more preferred than the original set-
gain insights on which electroacoustic para- ting, none of these could ensure that the cho-
meters to adjust. Unlike linear hearing aids, sen settings are the most optimal for the
which use the same gain for all input levels, wearer, which would be dependent on the ap-
nonlinear hearing aids employ different propriate choice of comparison alternatives.
electroacoustic parameters to control gain at On the other hand, if one were interested in
different input levels. For example, Kuk the optimal settings for the wearer, an adap-
(2000) indicated that the compression thresh- tive strategy, either a simple up-down proce-
old of wide dynamic range compression dure for unidimensional comparison or a
(WDRC) hearing aids influences gain for modified simplex for multidimensional com-
low-input sounds (below 60-dB SPL). parison, would be preferable.
Medium input sounds (around 60- to 75-dB
SPL) are controlled by gain parameter called
implementation on commercial
Gain50, Gainmedium, or hearing threshold level
hearing aids
(HTL). High-level inputs (above 85-dB SPL)
are controlled by a parameter that may be Although the majority of current digital and
labeled as Gain90, Gainloud, uncomfortable programmable hearing aids have the capa-
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 146

146 STRATEGIES FOR SELECTING AND VERIFYING HEARING AID FITTINGS

bility to allow implementation of paired tential run sequence may be done was de-
comparison tasks, many do not have struc- scribed previously.
tured algorithms that allow automatic
stimulus presentation and response track-
number of comparisons for
ing. Instead, a second memory or multiple
statistical significance
memories are usually available that the clin-
ician can utilize to store the alternative set- I have been describing paired comparison as
tings. The clinician, however, has to manu- a single comparison event; that is, A and B
ally track the responses and update the are compared once to reach a decision. The
comparison settings. This limits the type of assumption is that the decision is true and
paired comparison procedures that can be error-free. In reality, human responses are
performed in the clinic. It is foreseeable that seldom error-free. Variability in response be-
more manufacturers of digital hearing aids haviors becomes more noticeable when the
will implement some of the mentioned pro- two comparison settings are very similar. In-
cedures in their software in the near future. deed, when the two settings are indiscrim-
Meanwhile, we illustrate how a hearing inable to the wearer, one would expect that
aid with two memories (permanent or tem- both settings would have the same chance of
porary) can be used for paired comparison being selected (i.e., 50%) given the nature of
purposes. For example, the wearer com- paired comparison tasks. This means that the
plains that the original setting of the hearing selected setting would have a 50% chance of
aid results in other people’s voices being too being the wrong choice!
“boomy.” This would suggest excessive gain Increasing the number of comparisons can
for conversational input (60- to 70-dB SPL) improve the accuracy of the selection. By
in the low frequency. Only the Gainmedium pa- modeling the paired comparison task with
rameter in the low frequency would need to the binomial theorem, Kuk and Lau (1995b)
be adjusted. Male speech produced at a con- were able to predict the minimum number
versational input level (around 62-dB SPL) of comparisons and the number of times a
should be chosen as the stimulus. Because particular setting must be chosen to reach a
only one parameter is involved, a simple up- given level of statistical significance. Figure
down procedure (Fig. 4–6) would be a good 4–8 summarizes this information. The y-axis
procedure to zero in on the optimal amount reports on the level of significance and the x-
of low-frequency gain for clear and “non- axis reports on the number of chosen re-
boomy” perception. To proceed with the sponses. The number of comparisons is the
comparison, the clinician sets one memory displayed parameter. If one draws a hori-
(A) to the original setting of the hearing aid, zontal line at a particular probability (or
and the other memory (B) to the alternate level of significance), one can read off the
setting of the hearing aid. In this case, mem- number of times a particular setting should
ory B should have 5 dB less gain in the low be selected for the chosen response to have
frequency (5 dB is the default step size) than that probability. The probability for up to 10
memory A. The clinician instructs the wear- comparisons is provided. For example, if the
ers, turns on the test stimulus, and lets the line is drawn at p = 0.3, one can see that a
wearer listen through settings A and B. After subject has to choose the same response two
the wearer has decided on the preferred set- out of two times, or three out of three times
ting, the content of A and B will be updated to ensure that the choice has less than 30%
so that one contains the preferred setting chance of being in error. However, if four
and the other contains the setting for the comparisons are made, the same individual
next comparisons. Comparison continues has to choose the same setting three out of
until the termination rule is met. The specific four times to reach that probability level. If
instructions and the example of how a po- 10 comparisons are made, the same response
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 147

CHAPTER 4 ■ PAIRED COMPARISONS AS A FINE-TUNING TOOL IN HEARING AID FITTINGS 147

Figure 4–8. Significance level for n


selection when m trials of paired
comparison are performed. (From
Kuk F, Lau C. The application of
binomial probability theory to
paired comparison judgments. Am J
Audiol 1995b;4(1):37–42. © Ameri-
can Speech-Language-Hearing As-
sociation. Reprinted by permission.)

has to be selected seven times before one can (performed the necessary number of times)
say with 70% certainty that the chosen re- specifies the final setting.
sponse is truly preferable to the wearer. As If one desires to perform adaptive compar-
one can imagine, the more stringent the cri- ison manually to optimize the final settings,
terion (lower probability), the more times a one would need a commercial hearing aid
particular response has to be chosen. Al- that satisfies the following two requirements.
though most statistical analysis uses a 0.05 The first is that the electroacoustic dimen-
or 0.1 level of significance, we have found sion(s) under comparison must have fixed
that a more relaxed criterion of p = 0.3 can intervals so that comparisons can proceed in
produce relatively accurate and reliable se- discrete steps. Almost all commercial pro-
lection for clinical purposes. grammable and digital hearing aids satisfy
this requirement. The second requirement is
that the unit must have at least (N + 1) tem-
complaints involving more than
porary or permanent memories to allow
one parameter
paired comparison in N dimensions, that is,
There may be two alternatives to solving two memories for one dimension, three
complaints that require adjustment in more memories for two dimensions, and so on.
than one parameter. For example, a com- One can illustrate how a modified simplex
plaint of “unclear speech” may require low- procedure can be manually implemented on
ering the gain in the low-frequency channel a commercial digital hearing aid for selection
and increasing the gain in the mid-/high- of optimal high- and low-frequency settings.
frequency channels. With a commercial sys- Because settings on two electroacoustic di-
tem that has only two memories, one ap- mensions are compared (i.e., N = 2), the hear-
proach is simply to compare the original ing aid must have at least three memories
setting with another setting that has de- (i.e., N + 1) to facilitate comparison. A matrix
creased low-frequency gain and increased representation of the combinations of settings
mid-frequency gain stored in the second similar to that illustrated in Figure 4–7 must
memory. The result of the single comparison be prepared prior to the comparison so that
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 148

148 STRATEGIES FOR SELECTING AND VERIFYING HEARING AID FITTINGS

one can manually track listener responses lected or verified frequency-gain response
and update memory content accurately. will yield valuable information on the ap-
Assume that one follows the same se- propriateness of the selected device. Regular
quence of comparisons shown in Figure 4–7. follow-up examinations and readjustment of
In the first round of comparison, one may settings (if necessary) may also enhance the
store the frequency-gain setting represented wearer’s satisfaction with the hearing aid.
by cell (2L, 2H) into memory no. 1. Frequency- The technique of paired comparison is an
gain responses represented by cells (3L, 2H) important tool that can enhance the success
and (2L, 3H) can be stored in memories 2 and of a hearing aid fitting. It should be regarded
3, respectively. Memory assignment is arbi- as part of a fitting battery. Integration of re-
trary. The same sequence of comparison il- sults on more than one measure is necessary
lustrated in Figure 4–1 can be followed. It is to ensure maximum wearer satisfaction with
extremely important to indicate on the ma- the selected hearing aid.
trix the winner of each pair of comparison so
that settings for the next round of compari-
son can be easily determined. In the example References
illustrated in Figure 4–7, settings represented Balfour P, Hawkins D. A comparison of sound quality
by cells (3L, 2H), (3L, 1H), and (4L, 2H) are judgments for monaural and binaural hearing aid
stored in each memory of the hearing aid for processed stimuli. Ear Hear 1992;13:331–339.
Berger K, Hagberg E. Gain usage based on hearing aid
the second round of comparison. In this way, experience and subject age. Ear Hear 1982;3:235–237.
memory update is only necessary after every Box G. Evolutionary operation: a method for increasing
round of comparison. Obviously, manual industrial productivity. Appl Stat 1957;26:679–685.
Byrne D. Effects of frequency response characteristics
tracking of wearer response and the frequent on speech discrimination and perceived intelligibil-
update can be tedious and labor intensive. ity and pleasantness of speech for hearing-impaired
Thus, the use of a computer is extremely listeners. J Acoust Soc Am 1986;80:494–504.
Byrne D. Evaluation measures of speech intelligibility
helpful and necessary when paired compar- and quality-Research and clinical application. In:
isons are made in more than one electro- Studebaker GA, Bess F, Beck L, eds. Vanderbilt
acoustic dimension. Hearing-Aid Report II. Parkton, Maryland:York Press,
1991, pp. 195–200.
Byrne D, Cotton S. Evaluation of the National Acoustic
Laboratories’ new hearing aid selection procedure.
Integration with Other Measures J Speech Hear Res 1988;31:178–186.
Byrne D, Dillon H. Comparative reliability of warble
The paired comparison technique is a rela- tone thresholds under earphones and in sound field.
Aust J Audiol 1981;3:12–14.
tive measure. The result of the comparison Byrne D, Dillon H. The National Acoustic Laboratories’
does not guarantee that the particular pro- (NAL) new procedure for selecting the gain and fre-
grammable or digital hearing aid includes quency response of a hearing aid. Ear Hear 1986;7:
257–265.
the best frequency-gain response or process- Byrne D, Parkinson A, Newall P. Hearing aid gain
ing algorithm for the specific hearing aid and frequency response requirements for the se-
wearer or if the hearing aid is appropriate verely/profoundly hearing-impaired. Ear Hear 1990;
11:40–49.
for the wearer. Although this is a potential Cox R, Alexander G. Hearing aid benefit in everyday
limitation of this technique, one can easily environments. Ear Hear 1991;12:127–139.
overcome it with the use of additional mea- Cox R, Bisset J. Prediction of aided preferred listening
levels for hearing aid gain prescription. Ear Hear
sures to ensure wearer satisfaction for the 1982;3:66–71.
selected frequency-gain response. For exam- Eisenberg L, Dirks D. Reliability and sensitivity of
ple, the use of real-ear measures could en- paired comparisons and category rating in children.
J Speech Hear Res 1995;38:1157–1167.
sure that the wearer is actually using some Eisenberg L, Dirks D, Gornbein J. Subjective judgments
amplification from the hearing aid and not of speech clarity measured by paired comparisons
responding to “phantom attributes” during and category rating. Ear Hear 1997;18:294–306.
Eisenberg L, Levitt H. Paired comparison judgments for
paired comparison. Frequently, direct ques- hearing aid selection in children. Ear Hear 1991;12:
tioning of wearer satisfaction with the se- 417–430.
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 149

CHAPTER 4 ■ PAIRED COMPARISONS AS A FINE-TUNING TOOL IN HEARING AID FITTINGS 149

Gilman S, Dirks D, Stern R. The effects of occluded ear Kuk F, Tyler R, Mims L. Subjective ratings of noise re-
impedance on the eardrum SPL produced by hear- duction hearing aids. Scand Audiol 1990;19:237–244.
ing aids. J Acoust Soc Am 1981;70:370–386. Leijon A, Lindkvist A, Ringdahl A, Israelsson B. Pre-
Hagerman B, Gabrielsson A. Questionnaires on desir- ferred hearing aid gain in everyday use after pre-
able properties of hearing aids. Scand Audiol 1985;14: scriptive fitting. Ear Hear 1990;11:299–303.
109–111. Leijon A, Lindkvist A, Ringdahl A, Israelsson B. Sound
Harris R, Goldstein D. Effects of room reverberation quality and speech reception for prescribed hearing
upon hearing aid quality judgments. Audiology 1979; aid frequency responses. Ear Hear 1991;12:251–260.
18:253–262. Levitt H. Transformed up-down methods in psycho-
Hawkins D, Naidoo S. Comparison of sound quality acoustics. J Acoust Soc Am 1970;49:467–477.
and clarity with asymmetrical peak clipping and Levitt H. Adaptive testing in audiology. Scand Audiol
output limiting compression. J Am Acad Audiol 1993; Suppl 1978;6:241–291.
4:221–228. Levitt H. Adaptive procedures for hearing aid prescrip-
Jeffers J. Quality judgments in hearing aid selection. tion and other audiological applications. J Am Acad
J Speech Hear Dis 1960;25:259–266. Audiol 1992;3:119–131.
Kuk F. Preferred insertion gain of hearing aids in listen- Levitt H, Collins J, Dubno J, Resnick S, White R. Devel-
ing and reading-aloud situations. J Speech Hear Res opment of a Protocol for the Prescriptive Fitting of a
1990;33:520–529. Wearable Master Hearing Aid. CSL research report no.
Kuk F. Evaluation of the efficacy of a multimemory 11, National Institute of Neurological and Commu-
hearing aid. J Am Acad Audiol 1992;3:338–348. nicative Disease and Stroke. New York: City Univer-
Kuk F. A screening procedure for modified simplex in sity of New York, 1978.
frequency-gain response selection. Ear Hear 1994;15 Levitt H, Sullivan J, Neuman A, Rubin-Spitz J. Experi-
(1):62–70. ments with a programmable master hearing aid. J
Kuk F. Recent approaches to fitting nonlinear hearing Rehab Res Dev 1987;24:29–54.
aids. In: Roeser RJ, Valente M, Hosford-Dunn H, eds. Montgomery A, Schwartz D, Punch J. Tournament
Audiology: Diagnosis, Treatment, and Practice Manage- strategies in hearing aid selection. J Speech Hear Dis
ment, vol 2. New York: Thieme, 2000, pp. 261–290. 1982;47:363–372.
Kuk F. Adaptation to enhanced dynamic range com- Murray N, Byrne D. Performance of hearing-impaired
pression (EDRC)—examples from the SENSO P38 and normal hearing listeners with various high-fre-
hearing aid. Semin Hear 2001;22(2):161–171. quency cut-offs in hearing aids. Aust J Audiol 1986;8:
Kuk F, Harper T, Doubek K. Preferred real-ear insertion 21–28.
gain for listening in different speech and noise lev- Neuman A, Bakke M, Hellman S, Levitt H. Effect of
els. J Am Acad Audiol 1994;5(2):99–109. compression ratio in a slow-acting compression
Kuk F, Lau C. Effects of initial setting on convergence to hearing aid: paired comparison judgments of qual-
optimal hearing aid setting in a simplex method. Br J ity. J Acoust Soc Am 1994;96:1471–1478.
Audiol 1995a;29:263–269. Neuman A, Bakke M, Mackersie C, Hellman S. Effect of
Kuk F, Lau C. The application of binomial probability release time in compression hearing aids: paired
theory to paired comparison judgments. Am J Audiol comparison judgments of quality. J Acoust Soc Am
1995b;4(1):37–42. 1995;98:3182–3187.
Kuk F, Lau C. Comparison of preferred frequency gain Neuman A, Levitt H, Mills R, Schwander T. An evalua-
settings obtained with category rating and modified tion of three adaptive hearing aid selection strate-
simplex procedure. J Am Acad Audiol 1996a;7(5):322– gies. J Acoust Soc Am 1987;82:1967–1976.
331. Pascoe D. Frequency responses of hearing aids and
Kuk F, Lau C. Effect of hearing aid experience on pre- their effects on the speech perception of hearing-
ferred insertion gain selection. J Am Acad Audiol impaired subjects. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol Suppl
1996b;7(4):274–281. 1975; 23:1–40.
Kuk F, Ludvigsen C. Variables affecting the use of gen- Pascoe D. An approach to hearing aid selection. Hear In-
eral-purpose prescriptive formulae to fit modern strum 1978;29(6):2–16, 36.
nonlinear hearing aids. J Am Acad Audiol 1999;10(8): Pearsons K, Bennett R, Fidell S. Speech Levels in Various
458–465. Noise Environments. Bolt Beranek and Newman re-
Kuk F, Pape N. The reliability of a modified simplex port no. 32, Canoga Park, CA, 1977.
procedure in hearing aid frequency response selec- Preminger J, Neuman A, Bakke M, Walters D, Levitt H.
tion. J Speech Hear Res 1992;35:418–429. An examination of the practicality of the simplex
Kuk F, Pape N. Relative satisfaction for frequency re- procedure. Ear Hear 2000;21:177–193.
sponses selected with a simplex procedure in differ- Punch J. Quality judgments of hearing aid processed
ent listening conditions. J Speech Hear Res 1993;36: speech and music by normal and otopathologic lis-
168–177. teners. J Am Audiol Soc 1978;3:179–188.
Kuk F, Plager A, Pape N. Hollowness perception in Punch J, Beck E. Low frequency response of hearing
noise reduction hearing aids. J Am Acad Audiol 1992; aids and judgments of aided speech quality. J Speech
3:39–45. Hear Dis 1980;45:325–335.
Kuk F, Potts L, Valente M, Lee L, Picirrili J. Evidence of Punch J, Beck L. Relative effects of low frequency am-
acclimatization in subjects with severe-to-profound plification on syllable recognition and speech qual-
hearing loss. J Am Acad Audiol 2002 (to be published). ity. Ear Hear 1986;7:57–62.
Kuk F, Tyler R. Relationship between consonant recog- Punch J, Howard M. Listener-assessed intelligibility of
nition and subjective ratings of hearing aids. Br J Au- hearing aid-processed speech. J Am Audiol Soc 1978;
diol 1990;24:171–177. 4:69–76.
12843.C04.PGS 3/8/02 11:05 AM Page 150

150 STRATEGIES FOR SELECTING AND VERIFYING HEARING AID FITTINGS

Punch J, Jenison R, Allan J, Durrant J. Evaluation of Studebaker G, Sherbecoe R. Magnitude estimation of


three strategies for fitting hearing aids binaurally. the intelligibility and quality of speech in noise. Ear
Ear Hear 1991;12:205–215. Hear 1988;9:259–267.
Punch J, Montgomery A, Schwartz D, Walden B, Prosek Studebaker G, White R, Hoffnung S. Evaluation of a
R, Howard M. Multidimensional scaling of quality paired comparison technique for selecting hearing
judgments of speech signals processed by hearing aid characteristics. Paper presented at the meeting
aids. J Acoust Soc Am 1980;68:458–466. of the American Speech Language and Hearing As-
Punch J, Parker C. Pairwise listener preferences in hear- sociation, 1978, San Francisco, CA.
ing aid evaluation. J Speech Hear Res 1981;24:366– Studebaker G, White R, Hoffnung S. Evaluation of a
374. paired comparison elimination paradigm as a
Purdy S, Pavlovic C. Reliability, sensitivity and validity method of selecting hearing aids. Paper presented at
of magnitude estimation, category scaling and the meeting of the Acoustical Society of America,
paired comparison judgments of speech intelligibil- 1979a, Cambridge, MA.
ity by older listeners. Audiology 1992;31:254–271. Studebaker G, White R, Hoffnung S, Cox R. Studies of a
Robbins H, Monro S. A stochastic approximation paired comparison hearing aid selection procedure.
method. Ann Math Stat 1951;22:400–407. Paper presented at the meeting of the American
Schwartz D, Walden B, Prosek R. Electroacoustic corre- Speech Language and Hearing Association, 1979b,
lates of hearing aid quality judgments. Paper pre- Atlanta, GA.
sented at the meeting of the American Speech Lan- Sullivan J, Levitt H, Hwang J, Hennessey A. An experi-
guage and Hearing Association, 1979, Atlanta, GA. mental comparison of four hearing aid prescription
Skafte M. The 1999 hearing instrument market—the methods. Ear Hear 1988;9:22–32.
dispensers’ perspective. Hear Rev 2000;7(6):8–40. Tecca J, Goldstein D. Effect of low frequency hearing aid
Skinner M, Miller J. Amplification bandwidth and intel- response on four measures of speech perception. Ear
ligibility of speech in quiet and noise for listeners Hear 1984;5:22–29.
with sensorineural hearing loss. Audiology 1983;22: Thurstone L. A law of comparative judgment. Psycho-
253–279. acoust Rev 1927;34:273–286.
Stelmachowicz P, Lewis D, Carney E. Preferred hearing Wall L, Gans R. Test-retest reliability of a forced-choice
aid frequency responses in simulated listening envi- procedure for determining the most comfortable
ronments. J Speech Hear Res 1994;37:712–719. loudness level of speech. Ear Hear 1984;5:118–122.
Studebaker G. Hearing aid selection: an overview. In: Watson N, Knudsen V. Selective amplification in hear-
Studebaker G, Bess F, eds. The Vanderbilt Hearing-Aid ing aids. J Acoust Soc Am 1940;11:406–419.
Report: State of the Art Research Needs. Upper Darby, White R, Studebaker G. An evaluation of elimination
PA: Monographs in Contemporary Audiology, 1982: tournament strategies for hearing aid selection.
147–155. Paper presented at the meeting of the American
Studebaker G, Bisset J, Van Ort D. Paired comparison Speech Language and Hearing Association, 1978,
judgments of relative speech intelligibility. Paper San Francisco, CA.
presented at the meeting of the Acoustical Society of Witter H, Goldstein D. Quality judgments of hearing
America, 1980, Los Angeles, CA. aid transduced speech. J Speech Hear Res 1971;14:
Studebaker G, Bisset J, Van Ort D, Hoffnung S. Paired 312–322.
comparison judgments of relative intelligibility in Zerlin S. A new approach to hearing aid selection. J Speech
noise. J Acoust Soc Am 1982;72:80–92. Hear Res 1962;5:370–376.

You might also like