You are on page 1of 14

History's Mysteries Demystified: Becoming a Psychologist–Historian

Author(s): Kelli Vaughn-Blount, Alexandra Rutherford, David Baker and Deborah Johnson
Reviewed work(s):
Source: The American Journal of Psychology, Vol. 122, No. 1 (Spring 2009), pp. 117-129
Published by: University of Illinois Press
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/27784381 .
Accessed: 02/10/2012 06:18

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

University of Illinois Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The
American Journal of Psychology.

http://www.jstor.org
History'sMysteriesDemystified:
Becoming a Psychologist-Historian
Kelli Vaughn-Blount
ofCentralOklahoma
University

Alexandra Rutherford
YorkUniversity

David Baker
ofAkron
University

Deborah Johnson
of Southern
University Maine

More than40 yearsago, psychologist-historianRobertWatson argued thatthe studyof history


was of particularsalience to psychology.Inthisarticlewe explore the relationshipbetween

psychologyand history playsa vital role inthedisci


and argue thatthepsychologist-historian
We providea briefoverviewof theemergenceof the history
plineof psychology. of psychology
as a professionalsubdiscipline, are,explainwhy theyare
describewho psychologist-historians
needed, and detail how to join theirranks.
We argue that increasinghistoricalsophistication
among psychologists will have beneficialeffectson researchand teaching,and we inviteall
psychologiststo participateinthemakingof psychology'shistory.

None of us can
escape history.... History
can from the study of history, since psychology in itself,is
not be denied; the choice is between making stillanother view ofhuman behavior and experience"
ita conscious determinant of our behavior as (Watson, 1966, p. 64). In an even earlier article in the
or us un American Psychologist, he identified history as a ne
psychologists, allowing it to influence
awares. There is no other alternative. (Watson, glected area and gently accused American psycholo
1966, p. 64) gists ofboth geographic and historical provincialism.
He made a plea forpsychologists' greater attention
More than40 years ago, psychologist-historian Rob to and competence in history (Watson, i960). Sub
ertWatson suggested that the study of history was sequently,Watson probably did more than any other
of particular salience to psychology. As the study of single person to establish the history of psychology
human experience and thought, he argued, history as a professional subdiscipline within
psychology
adds another dimension to the science ofpsychology. (see Evans, 1982; Hilgard, Leary, & McGuire, 1991;
As he noted, "psychology is in a position to profit Watson, 1975).

American Journalof Psychology

122,No. l pp. 117-129 ? 2009 by theBoard ofTrustees of theUniversity of Illinois


Spring 2009, Vol.
Although Watson's scholarly contributions to the is therefore in itselfa psychological phenomenon.
...
history of psychology now appear somewhat dated, [W]e are looking at Psychology's role in the dy
and most contemporary psychologists probably do namic psychological process bywhich human nature
not recognize his name, his basic idea that
history and constantly recreates, re-forms, and regenerates itself"

have much to offerone another remains a


psychology (Richards, 2002, p. 7).
vital one. It continues tobe cultivated by historians of Despite these compelling arguments for the syn
psychology to extremely fruitfulintellectual ends. As ergy between psychology and history, it is clear that
Kurt Danziger noted, "Human subjectivity,the reality history and historical methods do not often inform
behind the objects of psychological investigation, is the day-to-day work of the psychological scientist.
itself strongly implicated in the historical process, Historians ofpsychology have come tooccupy a rela
both as agent and as product. Moreover, thehistory of tionalposition vis-a-vis theirnonhistorian colleagues

psychology and thehistory of human subjectivity are not unlike thatbetween any two specialists indiffer
not independent of one another.... So the ent subdisciplines in an increasinglyfragmentedfield.
grounds
for claiming a certain priority forhistory are much Just as the social psychologist may not have the time
stronger in the case of psychology than in the case of or theneed
(at least in termsof her ability topublish
thenatural sciences" (Danziger, 1994, p. 475). Roger research inher own field) tofind out the latestdevel
Smith, an eminent historian of the human sciences, opments in, for example, physiological psychology,
has also addressed the relationship between psychol one would not expect thatshewould necessarily seek

ogy and history.He has done so by focusing on the out relevant historical research.However, one could

implications of reflexivity for this relationship (as argue thatthereis an importantdifferencebetween the
have others; see Gergen, 1973;Morawski, 2005). To relationship between any two subfields inpsychology
brieflycharacterize his argument, he pointed out that and the relationship between any one subfield and
because knowledge about the self changes the self, Whether you are a social psychologist, physi
history.
or clinician,
psychology's very object of empirical investigation ological psychologist, developmentalist,
is changed in theprocess of psychological investiga odds are there is body of historical scholarship on
a
tion and theorizing.Historical methods allow us to your field, topic, or concern. Additionally, knowledge
examine this process, so history is itself a form of of this scholarship or a historical sensibility toward

self-knowledge without which psychology remains your research may appreciably enhance your ability
incomplete (Smith, 2007). tomake novel contributions, avoid past errors, and
Historian Peter Stearns (2007) stated that "all perhaps even ask your research questions in subtly
definitions of history's utility relyon two fundamen differentways.
tal facts (1)History helps us understand people and Now that thehistory of psychology has become
societies and (2)History helps us understand change its own specialized area, practiced both by psychol
and how the societywe live in came tobe" (pp. 5-7) ogists trained in historical methods and by profes
and added thathistorical research "focuses attention sional historians, have insurmountable barriers been
on the complex processes of social erected between those indoctrinated into this small
change including
the factors thatare causing change around us today" circle by virtue of formal trainingand experience and

(p. 10). Psychological knowledge and practice are thosewho have an avid interestbut no formal train
themselves social products that are heavily impli ing inhistory? Our position is thatall psychologists
cated in the complex processes of social change that can benefit from and potentially contribute to the
Stearns alludes to.As some historians ofpsychology history of psychology. Furthermore, the history of
have pointed out, the cultural authority of psychol psychology has much togain from the increasing and
ogy is now a taken-for-granted,even invisible aspect increasingly sophisticated participation ofpsycholo
of life inmany parts of theworld, especially inNorth gists across specialties. As Danziger pointed out,
America (seeHerman, 1995;Rose, 1992;Ward, 2002).
Finally,British historian ofpsychology Graham Rich People who have training inpsychology and
ards put it thisway: "For thehistorian of Psychology then enter into history have an advantage over
who is also a psychologist, the discipline's history theprofessional historians in that these people

118 VAUGHN-BLOUNT ET AL.


would typicallyhave had a hands-on, direct of the emergence and development of the
history of
experience ofwhat it is like to do psychologi psychology to familiarize readers with the field.
cal research or to be engaged inprofessional
practice. They will know what it is like from the Historyofpsychology:Originsand development
inside, sometimes throughyears of experience. In 1991, Ernest Hilgard, David
Leary, and Gregory
are in a
They position, at least potentially, to McGuire wrote a chapter on the history of
psychol
communicate with their fellow psychologists in ogy for theAnnual Review ofPsychology series.1 In
a way that
perhaps professional historians can this chapter theyprovided an excellent overview of
not do. (cited in Brock, 2006, p. thehistory of thehistory of
5) psychology, with sections
on representative authors and books,
historiography,
Donald Dewsbury (2003), a prominent com and teaching.To date, it is the
only such chapter that
parative psychologist and historian of psychology, has appeared in this series.2 In this section, we sum
described three different profiles marize some of thematerial covered in their 1991 re
characterizing
psychologists' involvement with history: the dab view, supplemented with developments in the field
bler, the retread, and the straight-liner. Dabblers thathave occurred over thepast 15years.
are well trained in other fields of
psychology but As Hilgard et al. (1991) pointed out,
shortly after
have an interest in the history of psychology as an
psychology was established as a scientificdiscipline in
add-on to theirmain line of work. Retreads may the late 1800s, synoptic accounts of its
history began
begin as dabblers but become more and more pas to appear. One of the firstAmerican textbooks was
sionate about history.They begin to studyhistorical G. Stanley Hall's The Founders ofModern
Psychol
methods and become more engaged in the profes ogy (1912).Hall was also the founder of theAmerican
sional world of historians of psychology, eventually
Journal ofPsychology, so it seems fitting tomention
putting theirprevious research area aside to focus his work. However, the best-known
psychologist
almost exclusively on historical scholarship. historian isprobably Edwin G. Boring, who in 1929
Finally,
Dewsbury defined the straight-liners as a more re published his now-classic History ofExperimental
cent invention, made possible with the advent of
Psychology. A second edition of Boring's book ap
graduate programs in the history of psychology. peared in 1950. Boring excised large areas of psychol
Straight-liners identify their interest in history at ogy (i.e., applied or professional psychology) from
an early stage and seek out his account, a strategy thatwas particularly problem
graduate training in the
field. Importantly,Dewsbury stated that "the work atic because, until recently,his textbook served as
of these differenthistorians is all useful" (Dewsbury, the template for subsequent
history of psychology
2003, p. 144) and noted that each group brings a textbooks (O'Donnell, 1979).At
midcentury, much of
slightlydifferent sensibility to theirhistorical work. thehistorical work in
psychology was concerned with
In this article,we hope toprovide information that thegreatmen and great theories of experimental psy
will be especially helpful to the first two groups,
chology and was often celebratory or ceremonial in
dabblers and retreads, although students consid nature. Itwas not until the late 1960s, inpart
spurred
a
ering straight-line approach will also find some by the increasing professionalization of thehistory of
useful information. as a field and inpart
psychology by developments in
In this articlewe have fourprimary aims: topro the history of science generally, that the shortcom
vide a brief, updated disciplinary overview of the
ings and limitations of an exclusive reliance on this
origins and development of history of psychology; kind of history began to be discussed. In 1966, his
to explain why it is important to have torian of science Robert Young surveyed the
psychologist existing
historians in everydepartment ofpsychology; to offer histories of the behavioral sciences and proclaimed
a series of
practical suggestions forhow any interested them largely presentist, beset by perseveration (the
psychologist can become a psychologist-historian; same stories were
being told time and time again),
and to enumerate theprofessional resources thatare and concerned almost exclusively with great men,
available to thosewho wish topursue thisvocation. great ideas, and great dates (Young, 1966).
We will startby providing a briefhistory and overview This was a call to arms in a subdiscipline thatwas

BECOMING A PSYCHOLOGIST-HISTORIAN 119


just beginning to formally coalesce. The watershed (1989) noted, itwas not until themid-1970s that
year for the history of psychology was 1965. In that historians of psychology began to act on Young's
year, theAmerican Psychological Association (APA) critique and produce scholarship thatmoved be
Division 26, History of Psychology, was formed. yond ceremonial aims and could appropriately be
The Archives of theHistory ofAmerican Psychol called critical history. Psychologists became more
ogy inAkron, Ohio, was established. The Journal aware of thedistinctions between institutional,bio

of theHistory of theBehavioral Sciences (JHBS) was graphical, social, cultural, and intellectual history
founded, with Robert Watson as its firsteditor.Two and of the challenges of combining internalist (in
years later, in 1967, the graduate program in thehis sider) with externalist (outsider) vantage points. She
tory and theory of psychology at theUniversity of explained that critical histories tended to be more
New Hampshire was opened, followed a year later contextual, inclusive, and historicist than traditional
by the founding of another professional organization, histories. Furumoto also noted that practitioners
Cheiron, theSociety for theHistory of theSocial and of the new, critical history were more likely to use
Behavioral Sciences (withwhich JHBS became affili archival and primary documents in order to avoid
topass
ated). Further developments unfolded internationally repeating anecdotes and myths that tended
in thedecades thatfollowed (see Table 1fora fulltime fromone textbook generation to thenext. She noted
line of these developments). thatFranz Samelson (1974) had recentlyhighlighted

By the late 1960s, the institutionalpresence of the the origin myth process, the retrospective selection

history of psychology was well established, at least of great thinkersand classic experiments tobuttress
in theUnited States. However, as Laurel Furumoto the legitimacy ofpresent views and to impart a sense

TABLE 1. Developmentsintheprofessionalization of psychology


of thehistory

Year Founding event


1965 APA'sDivision26, Historyof Psychology
Archivesof theHistoryofAmericanPsychology
Journalof theHistoryof theBehavioralSciences
1967 Graduateprograminthehistory ofNew Hampshire
and theoryof psychologyat theUniversity
1968 Cheiron,theSocietyfortheHistoryof theSocial and BehavioralSciences
1979 Storiae Criticadella Psicologia (Italy)
1980 Revistade Historicade la Psicologia (Spain)
1981 and theoryof psychologyatYorkUniversity
Graduateprograminthehistory inToronto
1982 CheironEurope (now the EuropeanSocietyfortheHistoryof theHumanSciences)
1984 Historyand Philosophyof PsychologySectionof theBritishPsychologicalSociety
1988 Association
Historyand Philosophyof PsychologySectionof theCanadian Psychological
Historyof theHumanSciences journal
1989 Historicalsectionof theGermanPsychological
Association
ForumfortheHistoryof theHumanSciencesof theHistory
of ScienceSociety
1998 AmericanPsychological
AssociationestablishesAPA historianposition

HistoryofPsychologytheofficialjournalof Division26
2004 BritishPsychologicalSociety'sHistoryof Psychology
Centreofficially
opened
Historyand Theoryof PsychologyStudentNetwork(http://www.yorku.ca/htnet/)
2007 programinthePsychology
Theoryand Historyof Psychology CollegeDublin
University
Department,

120 VAUGHN-BLOUNT ET AL.


of continuity and tradition about the Why everydepartmentneeds a psychologist-historian
development
of psychology. Benjamin Harris
(1980, p. 219) also As thediversity and specialization within psy
emphasized the importance of addressing the issue
of "what constitutes a good historical question" and chology continue to increase, thehistorical
perspective may be evenmore important, as
suggested that historians broaden their focus be
the only vantage point fromwhich we
yond narrative description and ceremonial accounts might
maintain some sense of coherence in the field.
tomore analytical tasks.
The 1990s saw theproliferationof increasingly so (Hilgard et al., 1991,p. 100)

phisticated textbooksand specialized monographs and The field of psychology has become an
increasingly
a
burgeoning journal literatureby both psychologist fractionated and complex enterprise,
encompassing
historians and professional historians. Although Hil the ever-changing facets of human behavior. As G.
gard et al. noted in 1991 thatthenumber ofpublications W. F. Hegel (1837/1953, p. xii) noted, history is the
concerned with teaching the history of "movements of the idea." The history of
psychology psychology
remained modest at thatpoint, this too has comprises the record of themovements of human
changed,
largely through thedevelopment of online resources. ideas in the context of the science that attempts to
In addition to the print resources mentioned in the understand them.However, psychologist-historians
1991 review (e.g.,Benjamin, 1979,1981;McGuire, 1990; differ frompsychological scientists in their relation
Raphelson, 1982), Ludy Benjamin published two edi ship to their subject matter. Whereas the psycho
tionsofAHistory ofPsychology inLetters (1993,2006), at least must
logical scientist, theoretically, attempt
which enable teachers to expose students to thejoys to remain detached fromhis or her research
subject,
of archival researchwithout leaving theclassroom and thepsychologist-historian "must care
enough about
provide researchers with a window on the archives it tomake it come alive for others"
(Leahey, 1987, p.
without leaving the office. In 2001, Hendrika Vande viii). It is thisdifference and the intricacy of the sub
Kemp compiled a bibliography of references from the ject matter thatnecessitate the attention and employ
psychology journal literatureon teaching thehistory ment ofpsychologist-historians in everypsychology
of psychology that includes more than 100 articles. department.
As for the status of the history of It has now become rare fordepartments of psy
psychology
course itself,a survey Perlman andMcCann
by (1999) chology to include facultymembers who are gener
reported thatas of 1997, a history and systems course alists. In thepast, the generalist was
usually the one
was
being offered at 82% of the doctoral institutions person in thedepartment who had the "big picture"
they surveyed and at 54% of institutions overall (in view of psychology; that is, the
generalist could see
cluding doctoral, comprehensive, baccalaureate, how the various parts of psychology fit
together.
and 2-year colleges). At all types of institutions the Such fitting is now best suited to the
history of psy
percentages had increased since similar data were chology. The historian of psychology serves a num
collected in 1975. Fuchs and Viney (2002) provided ber of purposes. First, the historian of
psychology
a
turn-of-the-century status report on the history teaches thehistory of psychology to undergraduate
course and concluded that
although the course was and graduate students.An importantpart of
teaching
offered inmost departments of psy thehistory of psychology is
being regularly introducing students to
was often
chology and required ofmajors, itwas historiography and helping them understand that
almost never taught by instructorswho were actu historical research and inquiry are meaningful, sys
ally publishing and doing research in thefield.They tematic activities thatcan garner new knowledge and
concluded, "The continued vitalityof scholarship in, produce new synthesis (see Table 2 forother benefits
and the teaching of, the history of psychology will to students). Despite the fact that there are many
depend on the future involvement of psychologists well-trained historians of psychology, this course is
who believe that,as one of our respondents expressed often taught by a person with an interest in thefield
it, 'nobody should be called a without a but with sparse knowledge of new historical research
psychologist
thorough background in history'" (p. 13). methods and the literature thathas been generated

BECOMING A PSYCHOLOGIST-HISTORIAN 121


TABLE 2. Ways inwhich historiansof psychologyhelpquickenthecriticalfacultiesof students

Theme Methods

Orientingstudents Guidingstudentsas theyattemptto integratedisparatetopicstheyhave encoun


tered;helpingthemrecognizeand come to termswith theenormousconceptualand
in
methodologicaldiversity thefieldof psychology.
Broadeningawareness Increasingstudents'awarenessof otherdimensionsof thediscipline:itsinstitutional
and organizationalunderpinnings, contextsof
the roleplayedby the sociocultural
the of
psychology, politics psychology, and themarginalization groupsand per
of
in
spectives psychology.
Criticalanalysisand evaluation Assistingstudentsas theydevelop theirabilityto critically assess thequalityof
discussions
information; of the relative
value of primary,
secondary, and tertiary
(and beyond) sources isessential inhistorycourses and illuminating beyondthe
history course.The of
study history enrichesand potentially liberates
psychologists
and studentsbecause itsuppliessubjectmatterthatgivesbackgroundand context,
perspectiveand outlooktowhat otherwisemightbe narrowprofessional
intellectual
activities.

preconceptions
Transcending A carefulstudyof historycan highlightthedangersof presentism(examiningthe
pastwith the standards
and of thepresent)and thevalue of past-mind
sensibilities
edness (situatingaccounts intheirown context),and this lessonhas implications
for
students' day-to-day interpersonal interactions and academic work.

with thesemethods. A best-case scenario is thathe be able to recognize and correct the errors for the
or she has an avid interest and the time and students. Because it is difficult to keep up with the
energy
to devote to course. literature in even one specialty area, the untrained
developing the
This lack of familiaritywith current historical history of psychology professor is unlikely to keep
methods and literatureperpetuates errors in thebody up with this additional area. Such an expectation is
of historical knowledge passed down to students in unreasonable and unlikely in this age of information
the discipline. Lack of formal study in the field can overload.

lead professors to choose textbooks that contain er Second, historians of psychology can help col
rors and misrepresent information to students.
Roger leagues see the interconnections between specialty
Thomas (2007, p. 493) stated, areas in
psychology. With increasing specialization
comes a strong
tendency toward isolation. Psycholo
Many or perhaps most who teach thehistory of
gists tend to stick togetherwithin theirown special
psychology do not do research in thehistory of ized area, and the result is a failure to see thebigger
psychology. Errors such as those revealed in the
picture of psychology. Students and professionals in
present work and in thework of others ... point
to theneed to psychology often are exposed to thebarest outlines of
give careful consideration to the historical psychology without ever being allowed to
adoption of any textbook, especially when itad
delve more deeply. Imagine ifevery course informed
dresses topics in thehistory ofpsychology. Less
students thatErikson had a theoryofdevelopment but
experienced teachersmay want to consult more never explained what itwas, or that
although there is
experienced teachers of history of psychology a statisticcalled a z-score it is
and may want, when possible, to read critical re really importantonly to
learn the analysis of variance. This is similar towhat
views of the textbooks theyare considering.
has been occurring in students' historical education.
Thomas also pointed out that any textbook is They are offerednames, theories,and possibly a para
to contain errors, and the professional must
likely graph or two of historical background in each course

122 VAUGHN-BLOUNT ET AL.


throughout theirdegree but are rarelygiven the op historians are originally trained in other specialties
portunity tounderstand the context inwhich a theory because of the small number of graduate programs in
was derived or inwhich a person worked. For the discipline. Availability of formal graduate train
example,
teachers and students of statistics rarelyhave the op ing remains limited,but some foundational programs
portunity to consider the implications of the fact that exist, including those found at York University, the
thedominance ofmany statisticalmethods was often University of New Hampshire, the University of
based on thepolitical prowess of theiroriginators and Calgary, and University College Dublin. In the last
was shaped by thepractical problem forwhich the sta several years postdoctoral opportunities have also
tisticwas developed (see Cowles, 2001; Porter, 1986), become available at such
prestigious institutions as
This informationis importantnot only in itsown right theMax Planck Institute
(History of Science Divi
but also in thecurrent and futureprofessional's ability sion) and the of Exeter. Ifyou are already
University
to thinkcritically about the intellectual and in a but are interested in receiving
political graduate program
environmentofpsychology.Wayne Viney summarized training in the area, do not despair. There are many
itbest by pointing out that "breadth of perspective
psychologist-historians working in various graduate
should be an educational goal for all students of psy programs throughout North America and interna
and historical studies make
chology unique contribu tionally. If you are a closet historian working in an
tions to such a goal" (Rutherford,2004, p. other area, then you can be sure that there ismentor
290). This
is especially truewhen we consider thateven themost who also specializes in your primary field
waiting to
seasoned professional remains a perpetual student of hear fromyou.
Many psychologist-historians are self
the ever-changing landscape of psychology. trained, and itmay simply be a matter of taking the
The question professionals and students should
plunge and diving into the literature.
be asking is not "Why does our need a
department WHAT TO READ.

psychologist-historian?" but rather "Why don't we


Many volumes have been written on the practice,
have one?" David Baker, director of theArchives for
theory,and methods of historical research (i.e., his
theHistory ofAmerican
Psychology, reminds us that
toriography). It isperilous to recommend only one or
the firstthing a clinician or counselor does is take a
two of these sources to the
beginning psychologist
personal history,yet psychologists continue toques historian because opinions and positions on how best
tion the value of theirown (Prieto, 2001). Furumoto
to conduct
history range verywidely and are tied to
(1995, p. 126) took this line of reasoning a step further, theoretical
allegiances. However, an accessible start
asking the discipline as a whole to accept psycho isLudmilla
ingpoint Jordanova's History inPractice
logical "historicism as a legitimate approach to the
(2006). The slightlymore advanced student might
production of knowledge." Perhaps it is time forhir consult Jan Golinski's Making Natural
Knowledge:
ing committees to recognize the invaluable inclusion Constructivism and theHistory of Science
of the trained psychologist-historian. The historian (2005),
which, as the titlesuggests, focuses more specifically
does not advocate a unified psychology but on issues in
helps
to elucidate the commonalties that run conducting historical studies of science.
through all As readers will quickly learn, the
strands of psychological science and practice. Found variability of skills
required of thehistorian is equal to thatof a seasoned
in the history of
psychology are a host of questions, experimentalist endowed with extensive knowledge
assumptions, hypothesis, biases, beliefs, and prac of hermeneutics, discourse analysis,
tices that illuminatewhat itmeans to be human and anthropology,
sociology, and historicism. Gilderhus
tobe a psychologist. (2007, p. 133)
summed itup thisway:

For students of history,no matter whether


Becominga psychologist-historian3 novices or professionals, the responsibilities
BECOMING EDUCATED. of reading,writing, and research hold
special
There aremany ways to become a
professional, but importance. As consumers and producers of
most paths involve a or program of educa
period scholarship, historians need to cultivate par
tion.As Dewsbury (2003) noted, many ticular kinds of skills.Reading requires certain
psychologist

BECOMING A PSYCHOLOGIST-HISTORIAN 123


powers of retention and synthesis, a capacity important issues about the role of critical history in
towork over
large bodies of information and to science education. Itpresents many of the arguments
establish a measure of intellectual possession. both pro (e.g., historical analysis develops critical

Writing calls for an ability to communicate thinkers)and


con a
(e.g., history is diversion from the
clearly, in the case of history, inplain, jargon realwork of thediscipline; criticalhistoryundermines
freeprose; and research compels, among other the lessons of scientific training) thatfuturehistorians

things,orderly, systematic, and imaginative ofpsychology are likely to encounter if they teach the
forms of inquiry.The degree towhich students history ofpsychology in a psychology department.
of history can attain such capabilities will deter
JOINING SOCIETIES.
mine their successes or failures.
Professional societies offer insight and access to the
Puente, Matthews, and Brewer (1992, p. 3) sum field toboth students and professionals. The United
marized Robert Young and Thomas Leahey's rules States, Canada, Britain, and Europe all have their
of thenew history of psychology: own
primary societies for the study of the history
of psychology. The APA Division 26 (Society for
Present mindedness must be replaced by past
theHistory of Psychology) is thebest known to the
mindedness. Ceremonial historymust give way
American psychological community.The Canadian
to critical analysis. "Great men" approaches to
Psychological Association and the British Psycho
history should give way to Zeitgeist approaches. encom
logical Society both offer similar divisions
Primary sources and archivalmaterial should
be used whenever possible. passing history and philosophy. In addition, there
are twohighly respected international organizations.
Greene (1994, p. 95) included three important The firstisCheiron, the International Society for the
for teachers to bestow on students in this
strategies History of Behavioral and Social Sciences, and the
new a
history: "Knowing how to establish scholarly second is the European Society for theHistory of
project by placing one's own ideas amidst what oth theHuman Sciences. Each of these societies can be
ers have said. sources as both intellectual and sites.
Citing easily located through their
Web
social touchstones. Justifyingone's decisions in de
PUBLISHING IN JOURNALS.
terminingwhat is important." Publication opportunities are always a concern for
We suggest thata systematic self-studyof thehis
psychologists, and psychologist-historians share this
toryof psychology begin with two books: A History concern. Historical work can often be found in stan
ofExperimental Psychology, by E. G. Boring (1929, dard psychological journals such as theAmerican
1952), and The Norton History of theHuman Sci
Journal ofPsychology, theAmerican Psychologist,
ences,byRoger Smith (1997).We recommend Boring
and Teaching ofPsychology, There are several field
because itput thehistory of psychology on themap,
no means
it influenced work in the field forhalf a century, it is specific journals as well. A brief but by
exhaustive list includes thejournalHistory ofPsychol
the result of a prodigious amount of scholarly effort,
and itrepresents a particular view ofwhat psychology ogy,published by theAPA's aforementionedDivision
26, Society for theHistory of Psychology, theJournal
should be (i.e., narrowly scientific). In comparing
of theHistory of theBehavioral Sciences, History of
Boring with Smith, the student will encounter dif theHuman Sciences, andHistory and Philosophy of
ferentperspectives on what has been (historically)
the British Psychological
as the appropriate content of psychology, Psychology, published by
regarded a
the appropriate methods for thediscipline, and even Society. Several history journals have cultivated
market forhistory of psychology, including theHis
itsappropriate moniker. Comparing these twoworks
also raises historiographic issues such as the func toryof Science Society's journal Isis and theBritish
tions history serves for thediscipline. Journal for theHistory ofScience.
A thirdmust-read isBrush's (1974) article "Should ACCESSING ARCHIVES (CLOSE TO HOME).

theHistory of Science Be Rated X?" (See Tables 3, are finding thatmany


Today's historical researchers
a
4, and 5 for additional resources.) This article raises primary sources are no longer available only in dis

124 VAUGHN-BLOUNT ET AL.


TABLE 3. Additionalbooks forbeginningself-study

Title Publication year Authors


of Pioneersof Psychology
Portraits 1991-2006 (6 volumes) GregoryKimble,Charlotte
White,
MichaelWertheimer,C.Alan Boneau,
Donald Dewsbury,and LudyBenjamin(Eds.)
FromSeance toScience:A History 2004 LudyBenjaminand David Baker
of theProfessionof Psychology
inAmerica
Even theRatWas White:A Historical 2003 (2ndedition) RobertGuthrie
Viewof Psychology
Handbook of PsychologyVolume 1, 2003 Donald Freedheim(Ed.)and
Historyof Psychology Weiner (Ed.)
Irving
Models ofAchievement:Reflections 1983,1988, 2001 Agnes O'Connell and NancyRusso
ofEminentWomen inPsychology (3 volumes)
EvolvingPerspectiveson theHistory 2001 Wade Pickren(Ed.)and Donald Dewsbury
of Psychology (Ed.)
theSubject:Historical
Constructing 1994 KurtDanziger
Origins PsychologicalResearch
of
A Guide toManuscriptCollectionsin 1982 Michael Sokal and PatriceRafail
theHistoryofPsychologyand
RelatedAreas
Editors foreach series varied. Order representsyear of firstappearance as editor.For search purposes Kimble is listedas firstnamed editor for
Volumes l-V and Dewsbury is firstforVolume VI.

TABLE 4. Articlesforbeginners

Author Year Title Published in


B. J.Lovett 2006 Thenewhistory
ofpsychology:
A reviewand critique ofPsychology
History 9(1), 17-37
C. J.Goodwin 1997 Thevitalroleofpsychology's
historyinintroductory ofPsychology,
Teaching 24, 218-221
courses: An interviewwith Ludy T. Benjamin, Jr.

K. Danziger 1994 Does thehistory have a future?


ofpsychology and Psychology
Theory 4, 67-84
D. J.Coon 1992 of senseand science:American
Testingthe limits American Psychologist, 47,143-151

experimental combatspiritualism,
psychologists
1880-1920
L. Furumoto 1989 Thenewhistory ofpsychology. In I.S. Cohen Washington, DC: American

(Ed.),TheG. StanleyHall LectureSeries(Vol.9). Association


Psychological
R.Smith 1988 Does thehistory have a subject?
ofpsychology History of Human Sciences, 1,147-177

J.M. O'Donnell 1979 The crisis of experimentalism in the 1920s: American Psychologist, 34, 289-295
E. G. Boring and his uses of history

R.I.Watson 1975 Thehistoryofpsychologyas a specialty:


A Journal
of theHistory
of the
view
personal of its 15
first years Behavioral Sciences, 11, 5-14

R.I.Watson 1966 The roleand use ofhistoryinthepsychology Journal


of theHistory
of the
curriculum Behavioral Sciences, 2, 64-69

R. I.Watson 1960 Thehistory A neglectedarea


ofpsychology: American Psychologist, 15, 251-255

BECOMING A PSYCHOLOGIST-HISTORIAN 125


resourcesforbeginners
TABLE 5. Internet

Resource Web site


Advances intheHistory
of PsychologyBlog http://ahp.yorku.ca/
APA Referenceson TeachingtheHistory
of Psychology http://www.apa.org/archives/refthp.html
Classics intheHistoryof Psychology http://psychclassics.yorku.ca/
Historyand Theoryof PsychologyStudentNetwork http://www.yorku.ca/htnet/
ofWomen HeritageSite
SocietyforthePsychology http://www.psych.yorku.ca/femhop/
podcast series
ThisWeek intheHistoryof Psychology http://www.yorku.ca/christo/podcasts/

tant librarybut are also readily available within a few One example of such a project is to take textbook
or historical
short clicks. The explosion in electronic archiving descriptions of classic experiments
means thatfarmore primary source material is avail figures that students have used in other courses,
able on the Internet than ever before. Scanning of have them locate theoriginal materials related to the
archival documents, out-of-print books, gray litera excerpts (many classic papers are available online
ture, and other resources now
provides
round-the through theClassics in theHistory of Psychology
clock access to aworld ofdata. A variety ofdatabases Web site), and compare the accuracy and validity of
allow researchers to locate archival materials, and a the information and interpretation presented. Stu
number of important archives forpsychologists have dents should be asked to consider the cultural and
doc
online informationand search aids (seeTable 6). Oral generational context inwhich both theoriginal
are ument and the textbookwere created and the effect
history interviewswith important psychologists
increasingly becoming available online
as well. For these biases might have on the interpretation.The

example, complete transcripts of interviews with firstauthor did this recently in a history and systems
course with great results. In addition, studentswere
Kenneth and Mamie Phipps Clark, two of themost
African American psychologists of the20th asked to research and perform as historical figures
important
century, are available at theColumbia Oral History at a mock conference where theywere to interpret
Research Office Web site on Notable New York information and respond as they thought thefigure
ers (see http://www.columbia.edu/cu/lweb/digital/ would. Not only did the students explore historical

eolleetions/nny/notable_ny.html). online resources and increase theirunderstanding of


Electronic resources make itpossible to intro the value of primary sources, but they also learned
duce students at all levels to historical inquiry and how difficult itwas to negotiate thedangers of pre
research. As archivalmaterials appear online, teach sentism and linguistic misgivings in dealing with
ers can design activities that encourage students to issues across multiple timeperiods.
use and learn the value of original source materials. It is important to understand that electronic

TABLE 6. SamplingofWeb-based archivalresources

Archival resources Web site


ArchiveGrid http://www.archivegrid.org/web/index.jsp
Archivesof theHistoryofAmericanPsychology http://www3.uakron.edu/ahap/

Association
Archivesof theAmericanPsychological http://www.apa.org/archives/

ofCongress
Library http://www.loc.gov/rr/

126 VAUGHN-BLOUNT ET AL.


resources are still far from complete, and accuracy in exploring the following questions: What can we
remains a concern. A largeproportion of archivalma learn fromhistory thatwould enable us to document
terialmay never be available electronically. Overreli the rigor and validity (and perhaps the necessity) of
ance on material that is available qualitative approaches to psychology? What can
electronically could
lead to incomplete research, and in reality nothing us about how
history tell psychological knowledge
can replace the
reliability and satisfaction ofworking is influenced by who produces it,under what cir
with archivalmaterial in the archive itself.University cumstances, inwhich contexts, and using which
archives or localmunicipal archivesmay contain ma methods? How have race, gender, sexual identity,
terial to support historical projects for the researcher and other constructs been handled by psycholo
who would like toundertake a historical project but gists, and what effectshas thishad on theproducts,
has limited funds for travel.Many archives also offer practices, and uses of psychology?
grants for travel and expenses to support relevant Finally, students need to be exposed to the his
projects. The Archives of theHistory ofAmerican toryof the discipline at the beginning, middle, and
a
Psychology inAkron, Ohio, is treasure trove for end of their formal education. Offering one history
historians of psychology and offers grants to help and systems course over the duration of a degree

defray travel costs for researchers conducting rel program sounds sufficient,but it is not. The com
evant
projects. plex history and myriad nuances of our discipline
cannot be adequately
conveyed in one course. The
Conclusion:Thefutureof thehistoryofpsychology trade-offfor such limited exposure is often the exclu
One of theongoing tasks forhistorians ofpsychology sion of information about traditionallymarginalized
is advocacy. The place of history of psychology re groups such as women and minorities. One way this
mains tenuouswithin thepsychology curriculum, and could be addressed, besides additional courses, is
thediscipline remains somewhat marginalized within through the inclusion of historical material in psy
the field. History tells us that forces fargreater than chology textbookswritten by experts and the greater
the individual push and pull culture, and perhaps employment of trained psychologist-historians in
there is littlewe can do to influence the fate of the psychology departments.
history ofpsychology, but thereare enough examples In addition, psychology departments must be
of thepower of the individual to shape events to give persuaded of thevalue of thehistorical method, a skill
us
hope. The power of thehistorical record to inform thatmust be learned but is rarely taught at theunder
our our actions is
thinkingand guide something that graduate level. Psychologists takegreat effortto train
everyone inpsychology needs to know more about. students in experimental techniques and technical

Knowledge of the history of psychology makes us writing; however, experiments and empirical papers
better students, teachers, citizens, and psychologists, represent only a fraction of the research and written
and we need to findways to convey this potential. work thatpsychology studentswill be asked to pro
There are many ways this can be done. We can ad duce in the course of theirprofessional careers. As
vocate in our departments forcourses and faculty in teachers, researchers, and students,
we must
acquire
the history of psychology, we can lobby accrediting and understand the critical analytic and interpretive
bodies such as theAPA to include history as a core skills that can be developed through the practice of
competency, and we can be good teachers. In the end historical research. Peter Stearns (2007) noted that
we need todo as historians must cultivate the ability to assess evi
Roger Smith (2007) and others have
done and explain why historymatters.We hope this dence, assess
conflicting interpretations, and evaluate
article has made one small step in thisdirection. past examples of change. The practice of historical
For the sake of fostering pluralism in psychol research,with theguidance of a trainedpsychologist
ogy and retaining vitality and relevance, we must historian, would enable students to develop these
produce more historical analyses ofmarginalized professionally necessary abilities. Students must be
groups and marginalized intellectual traditions in gin publishing from the outset. Analyzing, forming,
psychology. In particular, therewould be great value and communicating critical arguments, interpreting

BECOMING A PSYCHOLOGIST-HISTORIAN 127


as many as
evidence, and evaluating current scientific claims in
Lawrence College. The
panel produced questions
it answered. Overwhelming interest and support for theAPA
the context of theirpolitics and history are some of
event were the original inspiration for this article.
themost crucial abilities we can provide the future

psychologist. REFERENCES
We began thisarticleby invokingRobertWatson's
Benjamin, L. T. Jr. (1979). Instructional strategies in the his
40-year-old suggestion thatthe study ofhistorywas of of
tory psychology. Teaching ofPsychology, 6,15-17.
we
particular salience topsychology. Along theway, A
Benjamin,L. T.Jr. (1981).Teachinghistoryofpsychology:
have substantiated this statementby explaining who handbook. New York: Academic Press.

come from,
psychologist-historians are,where they (1993). A history ofpsychology in letters.
Benjamin, L. T.

are needed, how to join them, and where Dubuque, IA: Brown 8c Benchmark.
why they as a
are we find ourselves stillwondering Benjamin, L. T. (2005). A history of clinical psychology
they going, yet a at its future). An
profession inAmerica (and glimpse
whether we have properly explained and resolved
nual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1(1), 1-30.
the apparent mystery of thediscipline of thehistory in letters (2nd
Benjamin, L. T. (2006). A history ofpsychology
of psychology. It occurs to us thatour field is less a MA: Blackwell.
ed.). Maiden,
mystery than awell-kept secret.The secret is that the Boring, E. G. (1929). A history of experimental psychology.

history of psychology and psychologist-historians


New York: Century.

offer the discipline of psychology and itspractitio Boring,E. G. (1950).A historyofexperimental


psychology
ners a looking (2nd ed.). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
glass not only into the study of being
Brock, A. C. the history of psychol
human but also for itsmovement through the lens (2006). Rediscovering
ogy: An interview with Kurt Danziger. History ofPsychol
of psychology while providing an interpretation of
ogy, 9,1-16.
how it encountered itself along theway. The only the history of science be rated X?
Brush, S. G. (1974). Should
mystery appears tobe why psychology has yet to fully Science, 1^5(4130), 1164-1172.

recognize the salience of that endeavor. Cowles, M. (2001). Statistics in psychology:


An historical per

spective (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.


NOTES the history of psychology have a
Danziger, K. (1994). Does
1.A review of the history of clinical psychol future? Theory & Psychology, 4, 467-484.
specialized
of
ogy is also available in the series Annual Review of Clinical Dewsbury, D. A. (2003). Archival adventures in the history
In D. Baker (Ed.), Thick descrip
Psychology(seeBenjamin,2005). comparative psychology
2. Two other chapters have focused on history or histori tionandfine texture:
Studies in thehistoryofpsychology
of Akron Press.
cal matters but with very different aims: Conrad Mueller's (pp. 143-161). Akron, OH: University
1979 chapter, "Some Origins of Psychology as a Science," Evans, R. B. (1982). Robert I.Watson and the history of psy

written on the occasion of psychology's 100 th anniversary at theUniversity of New Hampshire.


chology program
as dated from the
opening ofWundt's Leipzig laboratory in Journal of History of theBehavioral
the Sciences, 18,

1879, and Dean Simonton's 2003 chapter, "Qualitative and 312-316.


ofHistoricalData," which ismore nar Fuchs, A. H., 8c Viney, W. (2002). The course in the history
Quantitative Analyses
of psychology: Present status and future concerns. His
rowly concerned with historiometry,
3. The firstauthor of this article asked the seemingly tory ofPsychology, 5, 3-15.
"How do you become a
psychologist Furumoto, L. (1989). The new history of psychology. In I.
simple question,
Series (Vol.
historian" of the second author 2 years ago. We decided that S. Cohen (Ed.), The G. Stanley Hall Lecture
the question was best addressed DC: American
by panel of psychologist 9, pp. 9-34). Washington, Psychological
historians because there is never just one way to become a Association.
in any field. This led to a panel presentation at L. textbook history of psychology and
practitioner Furumoto, (1995). On
the 2007 APA convention in San Francisco aptly titled "How scientizing history. Psychological Inquiry, 6(2), 124-126.
a as
to Become Psychologist-Historian: A Beginner's Guide." Gergen, K. M. (1973). Social psychology history. Journal of
We were very fortunate to be joined by David B. Baker, direc Personality and Social Psychology, 26, 309-320.
tor,Archivesof theHistory of American Psychology, Akron, Gilderhus, M. T. (2007). History and historians: A historical
Indiana University at Bloomington; Pearson.
Ohio; James Capshew, introduction (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ:
James Goodwin, Western Carolina University; Christopher Golinski,J. (2005). Making natural knowledge: Constructiv
D. Green, York University, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; Ann ism and the history of science. Chicago: University of Chi

Johnson, University of Saint Thomas; Deborah Johnson, cago Press.


S. (1994). The of learning to think like a
University of Southern Maine; and Elizabeth Johnston, Sarah Greene, problems

128 VAUGHN-BLOUNT ET AL.


historian: Writing history in the culture of the classroom. Richards, G. (2002). Putting
psychology in itsplace:
A criti
Educational Psychologist, 29(2), 9-96. cal, historical overview. New York: Routledge.
Hall, G. S. (1912). The founders ofmodern psychology. New Rose, N. (1992). Engineering the human soul: Analyzing psy
York: Appleton. chological expertise. Science in Context, 5, 351-369.
B. (1980). Ceremonial versus critical
Harris, history of psy Rutherford, A. (2004). Where history, philosophy, and psy
chology. American Psychologist, 35, 218-219. chology meet: An interview with Wayne Viney. Teaching
Hegel, G. W. F. (1953). Reason in history: A general introduc ofPsychology, 31, 289-295
tion to the philosophy of history (R. S. Hartman, Trans). F. (1974). History,
Samelson, origin myth, and ideology: "Dis
New York: Bobbs-Merrill. (Original work published 1837) covery" of social psychology. Journal for theTheory of
Herman, E. (1995). The romance ofAmerican psychology: Social Behaviour, 4, 217-231.
Political culture in the age of experts. Berkeley: University Simonton, D. K. (2003). Quantitative and qualitative
analy
of California Press. ses of historical data. Annual Review
ofPsychology, ?4,
E. R., Leary, D. E., 8c McGuire, G. R. (1991). The 617-640.
Hilgard,
history of psychology: A survey and critical assessment. Smith, R.(1997). The Norton history of the human sciences.
Annual Review ofPsychology, 42, 79-107. New York: WW. Norton.
L. in practice. New York: Oxford Smith, R. (2007). Being human: Historical
Jordanova, (2006). History knowledge and the
University Press. creation of human nature. New York: Columbia Univer

Leahey, T. H. (1987). A history ofpsychology: Main currents


sity Press.
inpsychologicalthought
(2nded.). EnglewoodCliffs,
NJ: Stearns, P. N. (2007,July). Why study history? American
Prentice Hall. Historical Association. Retrieved March 15,2008, from
McGuire, F. L.
(1990). Psychology aweigh: A history of clinical http://www.historians.org/pubs/Free/WhyStudyHistory
psychology in theUnited States Navy, igoo-ig88. Wash .htm
DC: American Psychological Association. Thomas, R. K. errors among recent
ington, (2007). Recurring history
Morawski,J. G.
(2005). Reflexivity and the psychologist. His of psychology textbooks. American Journal ofPsychology,
tory of theHuman Sciences, 18, 77-105. 120, 477-495

Mueller, C. G. (1979). Some origins of psychology as a sci Vande Kemp, H. (2001). History of psychology texts and re
ence. Annual Review lated resources. History 8c Theory of Psychology Eprint
ofPsychology, 30, 9-29.
O'Donnell, J.M. (1979). The crisis of experimentalism in the Archive. Retrieved October 10,2007, from http://
1920s: E. G. and his uses of American
Boring history. Psy htpprints.yorku.ca/archive/00000026/
chologist, 34, 289-295. Ward, S. C.
(2002). Modernizing themind: Psychological

Perlman, B., 8c McCann, L. I. (1999). The most frequently knowledge and the remaking of society.Westport, CT:
listed course in the undergraduate psychology curricu Praeger.
lum. Teaching ofPsychology, 26, 177-182. Watson, R. I. (i960). The history of A neglected
psychology:
Porter, T. M. area. American
(1986). The Rise of statistical thinking 1820 Psychologist, 15, 251-255.
igoo. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Watson, R. I. (1966). The role and use of history in the psy

Prieto, L. (2001). The archives of the history of American chology curriculum. Journal of theHistory of theBehav
psychology: An interview with David B. Baker. Teaching ioral Sciences, 2, 64-69.

28(4), 299-302.
ofPsychology, Watson, R. I. (1975). The
history of psychology
as a
specialty:
Puente, A. E., Matthews, J. R., 8c Brewer, C. L. (Eds.). (1992). A personal view of its first 15 years.
Journal of theHistory
A history.Washington,
Teaching psychology in America: of theBehavioral Sciences, 11, 5-14.
DC: American Psychological Association. Young, R. M. (1966). Scholarship and the history of the be
A. C. course as the capstone
Raphelson, (1982). The history havioural sciences. History of Science, 5, 1-51.
of the psychology curriculum. Journal of theHistory of
theBehavioral Sciences, 18, 279-285.

BECOMING A PSYCHOLOGIST-HISTORIAN 129

You might also like