You are on page 1of 13

Challenges In Measuring a New Construct: Perception of Voluntariness for Research and

Treatment Decision Making


Author(s): Victoria A. Miller, William W. Reynolds, Richard F. Ittenbach, Mary Frances Luce,
Tom L. Beauchamp and Robert M. Nelson
Source: Journal of Empirical Research on Human Research Ethics: An International Journal, Vol.
4, No. 3 (September 2009), pp. 21-31
Published by: Sage Publications, Inc.
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1525/jer.2009.4.3.21
Accessed: 08-01-2016 09:20 UTC

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/
info/about/policies/terms.jsp

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content
in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship.
For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Sage Publications, Inc. is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Journal of Empirical Research
on Human Research Ethics: An International Journal.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 131.94.16.10 on Fri, 08 Jan 2016 09:20:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
JER0403_03 9/3/09 5:05 PM Page 21

Challenges in Measuring a New Construct 21

C HALLENGES IN M EASURING A N EW C ONSTRUCT: P ERCEPTION OF


VOLUNTARINESS FOR R ESEARCH AND T REATMENT D ECISION M AKING

V ICTORIA A. M ILLER to as constructs (often said to be concepts in the


The Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia humanities), and measures are considered proxies for
these constructs. When we assess the relationships
W ILLIAM W. R EYNOLDS between measures, we infer relationships between the
Richard Stockton College of New Jersey constructs they are intended to measure.
R ICHARD F. I TTENBACH Empirical research ethics has been a growing area, and
Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center with that growth comes the need for reliable and valid
measures of phenomena that are relevant to ethical
M ARY F RANCES LUCE problem solving. The process of scale development for
Duke University subjective phenomena involved in ethical problem solv-
T OM L. B EAUCHAMP ing, such as quality of life and decision making, is com-
Georgetown University plex, especially with imprecisely defined or inadequately
measured constructs. In this situation, refining the con-
R OBERT M. N ELSON struct definition and operationalizing the construct can
U.S. Food and Drug Administration be challenging. Failure to fully address these processes
can result in a number of unanticipated consequences,
ABSTRACT: RELIABLE AND VALID MEASURES OF RELEVANT
including problems with validity and biased estimates of
constructs are critical in the developing field of the empir-
relationships between variables (MacKenzie, 2003).
ical study of research ethics. The early phases of scale devel-
Although many papers describe the development of new
opment for such constructs can be complex. We describe
questionnaires to measure subjective health-related con-
the methodological challenges of construct definition and
structs, most focus on the psychometrics of the final
operationalization and how we addressed them in our
measure and lack important details about the early stages
study to develop a measure of perception of voluntariness.
of scale development. To move forward and to provide
We also briefly present our conceptual approach to the
useful insights for solving ethical problems in real-world
construct of voluntariness, which we defined as the percep-
research settings, attention to these early phases of scale
tion of control over decision making. Our multifaceted
development for research ethics-related constructs is
approach to scale development ensured that we would
critical. Without reliable and valid measures of such con-
develop a construct definition of sufficient breadth and
structs, research findings are of questionable value.
depth, that our new measure of voluntariness would be
The purpose of this paper is to describe how we
applicable across disciplines, and that there was a clear link
addressed the challenges of construct definition and
between our construct definition and items. The strategies
operationalization when developing an instrument to
discussed here can be adapted by other researchers who
measure the perception of voluntariness in parents
are considering a scale development study related to the
making protocol-based treatment decisions (defined
empirical study of ethics.
below) for their seriously ill children. We defined per-
KEY WORDS: voluntariness, decision making, method- ception of voluntariness as the perception of control the
ology, scale development individual perceives over a specific decision. Our goal is
not to discuss well-accepted principles of measurement
Received: June 15, 2009; revised July 13, 2009 such as scaling responses or statistical evaluation of
items, but to expand on the details of a project to opera-
tionalize a construct that has never been measured
before and about which there may be some debate in the
measur- literature. We will show the way in which a multidisci-

S
CALE DEVELOPMENT IS CONCERNED WITH
ing phenomena that we believe to exist but that plinary, multifaceted approach facilitated our progress
cannot be observed directly (DeVellis, 1991). In in the early phases of scale development. The strategies
the social sciences, these phenomena are referred we describe can be considered by others when faced

Journal of Empirical Research on Human Research Ethics, PP. 21–31. PRINT ISSN 1556-2646, ONLINE ISSN 1556-2654. © 2009 BY JOAN SIEBER .
ALL RIGHTS RESERVED. PLEASE DIRECT ALL REQUESTS FOR PERMISSIONS TO PHOTOCOPY OR REPRODUCE ARTICLE CONTENT THROUGH THE
UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA PRESS ’ S RIGHTS AND PERMISSIONS WEBSITE , HTTP :// WWW. UCPRESSJOURNALS . COM / REPRINTINFO. ASP.
DOI: 10.1525/jer.2009.4.3.21

This content downloaded from 131.94.16.10 on Fri, 08 Jan 2016 09:20:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
JER0403_03 9/3/09 5:05 PM Page 22

22 V. Miller, W. Reynolds, R. Ittenbach, M. Luce, T. Beauchamp, R. Nelson

with similar circumstances. First, we briefly summarize Our collaborative research team consists of three prin-
the background and methods of the project. Next, we cipal investigators from three institutions, as well as three
discuss the importance of construct definition and the additional individuals from the primary site. The three
strategies we used to define the construct of voluntari- principal investigators include a physician with expertise
ness. We then discuss construct operationalization and in research ethics (RN), a philosopher who has written
the strategies used to develop items for a self-report extensively on informed consent (TB), and a researcher
measure of voluntariness. Finally, we describe the con- in marketing with a focus on decision making and affect
struction of the final experimental item pool, which was (MFL). Additional on-site members include a clinical
then tested for its psychometric properties. psychologist (VM), a qualitative social science researcher
(WR), and a quantitative psychologist (RI).1
Description of Project In the first phase of the study, focus groups were con-
ducted to examine themes related to voluntariness.
A fundamental principle of research ethics is that the Individual interviews supplemented the use of focus
validity of consent reflects the degree to which consent is groups due to scheduling difficulties and concerns
informed and voluntary. The empirical literature on con- about socially desirable responding in a group format.
sent has focused on disclosure, understanding, and deci- In addition, we determined that we would not lose any
sion making, with little attention paid either to whether valuable information about perception of voluntariness
the decision was made voluntarily or to the conceptual by shifting to the use of interviews. The focus group
conditions of voluntariness. Absent a measure of whether data up to that point allowed us to identify what ques-
consent decisions are voluntary, we are left with only a tions were most important to ask. Participants included
partial picture of the adequacy of the consent process. The parents who had made protocol-based treatment deci-
goal of the overall project was to develop a reliable and sions for their seriously ill children, clinician-investigators
valid instrument to measure the perception of voluntari- who were involved in obtaining permission from these
ness in parents making decisions about protocol-based parents, and study coordinators and other non-physi-
treatment for their seriously ill children. Protocol-based cian clinicians involved in the consent process (e.g.,
treatment refers to protocols that offer an intervention to social workers, nurses). Participants were approached
the child, whether proven or unproven (i.e., research), from several divisions at a pediatric teaching hospital,
and require a signed consent document. These decisions including oncology, the neonatal intensive care unit,
can be quite stressful for parents, as the decisions typically and the cardiac intensive care unit. Focus groups and
involve a wealth of potentially complex information, must interviews were audio-taped, transcribed, and analyzed
be made within a short period of time, involve uncertain for themes related to voluntariness. These data were
outcomes, and often occur in the emotional context of a used to refine our construct definition and generate can-
new diagnosis, relapse (e.g., cancer), or illness exacerba- didate items to be included in the experimental item
tion (e.g., cystic fibrosis). There has been much debate pool. In the second phase of the study, we administered
about the extent to which individuals with a life-threaten- the experimental item pool and additional instruments
ing illness can make voluntary decisions and resist entice- to parents within 10 days of making a decision for pro-
ments or manipulations of hope; this debate extends to tocol-based treatment for their seriously ill child. The
parents of seriously ill children and applies to both items were tested for their psychometric properties to
research and treatment decisions. We intend for our determine which items would remain in the final
instrument to be used in either research or treatment instrument. (Empirical data and information about
contexts, as we expect that the measurement of perception informed consent procedures and IRB approval will be
of voluntariness is similar for these two types of decisions. reported elsewhere.)
However, the factors that may influence perceptions of
voluntariness are likely to be different across contexts. Defining the Construct of Perception of Voluntariness
This question can be tested in future research that utilizes
our new instrument. This new instrument will redress the OVERVIEW OF THE CHALLENGE
current imbalance in the assessment of both treatment The first step when developing a new scale is to define
and research decision making and allow us to explore the (i.e., specify the meaning of) the construct of interest. A
factors that influence perception of voluntariness. clear definition of the construct and its boundaries is
Research using this new tool will also facilitate the devel- critical, because this definition will guide the generation
opment of guidelines for ensuring the voluntariness of and selection of items to be included in the experimen-
decisions made in treatment and research settings. tal item pool. Different construct definitions will lead to

This content downloaded from 131.94.16.10 on Fri, 08 Jan 2016 09:20:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
JER0403_03 9/3/09 5:05 PM Page 23

Challenges in Measuring a New Construct 23

different sets of items. “The boundaries of the phenom- excluded from the item pool. Typically, the identification
enon must be recognized so that the content of the scale of related constructs is an ongoing process that emerges
does not inadvertently drift into unintended domains” in conjunction with the development of a concise defini-
(DeVellis, 1991, p. 51). In addition, an ambiguous defi- tion of the construct of interest. In our study, we took a
nition creates difficulty when deciding about the inclu- multifaceted approach to this process, through the use of
sion and exclusion of specific items based on their a literature review, focus groups and interviews, and
psychometric properties. For example, low correlations multidisciplinary group discussions. Each facet con-
between individual items and the total score of a ques- tributed to the development of our definition of percep-
tionnaire can result from poor items or items that assess tion of voluntariness, and laid the foundation for
a multidimensional construct. The wrong theory about enhanced construct representation and decreased con-
the construct can also result in items that end up having struct misspecification in the new instrument.
no predictive or explanatory power, which will have
meant wasted time and resources (Streiner & Norman, STRATEGIES USED TO REFINE THE CONSTRUCT DEFINITION
1995). In addition, the process of construct validation, Literature Review. We started with an examination of
which is concerned with whether the indicators appro- voluntariness in the literature. One broad definition is
priately reflect the construct (Wallander, 1992), depends that voluntariness is the free power of choice without
on a clear and unambiguous definition. undue influence or coercion (Nuremberg Code, 1949),
A challenge related to construct definition is how to but this definition fails to specify what it means to be
differentiate the construct of interest from other con- free of undue influence and coercion. Building on the
structs. This differentiation will be important when test- notion of “choice,” Wall (2001) argues that “voluntari-
ing later for convergent and discriminant validity, both ness is the degree of control that an agent has over
of which are aspects of construct validity. Convergent his own behavior” (Wall, 2001, p. 130). In the case of
validity refers to how closely the new measure is related informed consent, the relevant behavior of interest
to measures of similar constructs, or other measures of would be decision making. Similarly, Faden and
the same construct. These correlations should be mod- Beauchamp (1986) analyze voluntariness as the degree
erate to high, but not so high that the two instruments to which an individual controls a decision and link the
appear to be measuring exactly the same thing. notion of control to the individual’s resistance to influ-
Conversely, discriminant validity refers to whether the ence. Their approach favors a subjective interpretation
measure is unrelated to measures of dissimilar con- of influence, because there is likely to be wide variability
structs. If there is an unexpected correlation between in individuals’ resistance to particular influences (Faden
measures, this may have resulted from faulty measure- & Beauchamp, 1986). We can know only if an influence
ment or an incorrect theory of the construct of interest. was controlling the individual’s decision by assessing the
When defining the construct, it is important to consider individual’s subjective perception; the mere presence of
how the construct of interest is expected to relate (or not an influence cannot tell us anything about the individ-
relate) to other constructs (Campbell & Fiske, 1959; ual’s response to that influence. The distinction between
Cronbach & Meehl, 1955). This pattern of relationships a subjective and objective interpretation of influence was
will provide evidence regarding how well the new important from the outset of the study, as we proposed
instrument and the construct it purports to measure to develop a scale for measuring voluntariness from a
perform in relationship to other constructs. parent’s subjective perspective. Our approach focuses on
Other constructs may: (1) be related to the construct of perception of voluntariness, but we also recognize that
interest, (2) confuse interpretation of the construct of some conditions are incompatible with voluntariness
interest, or (3) moderate relationships between the con- (e.g., lying with the intent to lead a person to believe
struct of interest and other constructs. Distinguishing what is false and to act on the false belief renders the
between constructs is important to determine in action non-voluntary) even if the individual is unaware
advance, because this will guide item development and of the influence and perceives the decision as voluntary.
the analytic plan. For example, items that reflect corre- In contrast to a conceptualization of perception of vol-
lated constructs may generate a scale that meets the sta- untariness as control over the decision, Roberts presents
tistical criteria for unidimensionality, especially when the multiple definitions of voluntariness, including ‘the indi-
number of items is high (McGrath, 2005). That is, vidual’s ability to act in accordance with one’s authentic
covariation of items does not necessarily imply concep- sense of what is good, right, and best in light of one’s
tual redundancy. As such, construct clarity at the outset situation, values, and prior history’ (Roberts, 2002,
is crucial so that items reflecting different constructs are p. 707). Faden and Beauchamp (1986) frame authenticity

This content downloaded from 131.94.16.10 on Fri, 08 Jan 2016 09:20:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
JER0403_03 9/3/09 5:05 PM Page 24

24 V. Miller, W. Reynolds, R. Ittenbach, M. Luce, T. Beauchamp, R. Nelson

more broadly as requiring either “stability or consis- decision in a particular way and to facilitate the genera-
tency” in the values underlying choice. Although quite tion of new themes. As each group progressed, ques-
different from the notion of control and not widely dis- tions became more specific and prompted for reactions
cussed in the literature, we decided to consider authen- to different definitions of voluntariness.
ticity as a potentially important aspect of perception of A review of the data suggested that key stakeholders
voluntariness that could be explored further in focus in the informed consent process have varying interpre-
groups. We did not want to reject prematurely any tations of voluntariness. In general, research personnel
themes that might be relevant to the construct. Thus, at and clinician-investigators approached voluntariness
this early point, we framed perception of voluntariness from a regulatory perspective and discussed potential
as consisting of themes of control and authenticity. influences on voluntariness (e.g., inducements, time
There is a paucity of literature related to authenticity; pressure), rather than the construct of voluntariness per
the theme of control was the primary focus of further se. Overall, data from the parent focus groups and
literature review. We examined control-related con- interviews were more useful, presumably because par-
structs in the psychological literature, such as locus of ents are better able to reflect on their experiences regard-
control and perceived behavioral control. Locus of con- ing decision making for their seriously ill children.
trol, as originally conceptualized by Rotter (1966), is Parents reported that a sense of control meant they per-
related to beliefs about whether outcomes are causally ceived that the decision was up to them, were aware of
dependent on variations in one’s own behavior versus the option to reject or withdraw from the protocol, and
external factors. In contrast, perceived behavioral con- perceived that the decision was made without feeling
trol refers to the perception of control over behaviors, pressured, intimidated, or coerced. One new theme that
rather than the outcomes of those behaviors (Ajzen, parents identified was the experience of dissociation
2002). According to Ajzen (2002), “perceived behav- from the decision-making process as a result of feeling
ioral control . . . is comprised of two components: self- shocked by their child’s recent medical diagnosis.
efficacy (dealing largely with the ease or difficulty of Similarly, physicians noted that parents are often
performing a behavior) and controllability (the extent to “paralyzed” at the time of diagnosis and that voluntary
which performance is up to the actor)” (p. 680). The decision making appears difficult in this context.
examination of these two constructs was deemed useful, Presumably, this is one way in which a lack of control
by helping us to identify aspects of control that were can be manifested. The theme of authenticity did not res-
potentially relevant to perception of voluntariness, par- onate with participants. This appeared to be the result of
ticularly because philosophical treatments of voluntari- difficulties parents encountered in the attempt to grasp
ness may not translate easily into a measurable and apply the notion of authenticity to decision making
psychological construct. Our focus on psychological about their children’s serious illness. Overall, the focus
constructs related to control was also consistent with our group data allowed us to explore specific factors that
emphasis on the perceptions of the individual making may reflect a perception of control (or lack thereof) in
the decision rather than what is actually happening in the context of protocol-based treatment decisions.
the environment. Multidisciplinary Group Discussions. Multidisciplinary
Focus Groups and Interviews. The various conceptu- group discussions were utilized to discuss the literature
alizations of voluntariness and related constructs (i.e., review and focus group data and served as a vehicle
freedom from coercion, control/non-control, locus of through which our definition of perception of voluntari-
control, perceived behavioral control, self-efficacy, and ness evolved over time. The use of a multidisciplinary
authenticity) were explored further in focus groups and group of experts has several advantages, including
individual interviews. We were also interested in gener- access to the most recent thinking in the field and a
ating additional themes related to perception of volun- range of knowledge, experience, and opinions about
tariness, because participants’ views about voluntariness the construct (Streiner & Norman, 1995). Furthermore,
may differ from psychological and philosophical this approach assures that the construct is tackled
treatments of the construct. Prior to the focus groups, from multiple angles, rather than one particular view-
we developed a detailed interview guide to ensure point (Streiner & Norman, 1995). This is particularly
that we explored key themes identified in the literature. important when the construct intersects with several
The groups began with open-ended questions about the disciplines (e.g., philosophy, medicine, psychology, bio-
consent process. We avoided use of the words “volun- ethics). If approached from a single point of view, the
tariness,” “control,” and “authenticity” at the beginning work would have limited usefulness and applicability to
of the groups, to prevent biasing participants to view the a broader audience. In our study, a multidisciplinary

This content downloaded from 131.94.16.10 on Fri, 08 Jan 2016 09:20:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
JER0403_03 9/3/09 5:05 PM Page 25

Challenges in Measuring a New Construct 25

approach facilitated a definition of perception of volun- addition, the development of actual items must attend to
tariness that would be coherent and acceptable from issues of clarity and content validity, to assure that the
multiple perspectives. Meetings with the on-site team items assess all relevant content (i.e., content validity)
members occurred weekly, while meetings with the and avoid tapping into other constructs (i.e., discrimi-
entire team were held on a quarterly basis. nant validity). Potential sources of items include focus
As we refined our construct definition, the group groups, key informant interviews, clinical observation,
addressed the differentiation between perception of vol- theory, expert opinion, and existing measures (DeVellis,
untariness and related constructs. This differentiation is 1991; Streiner & Norman, 1995). As mentioned earlier,
important so that the measure does not become tainted the most important guide to choosing items is the defi-
by unrelated factors (MacKenzie, 2003). For example, nition of the construct. In classical scale development,
the decision about whether to include authenticity in our the assumption is that responses to items are caused by
definition of voluntariness was a source of debate in the the construct, such that each item should give an indi-
early stages of the project and was compounded by the cation of the strength of the latent variable (DeVellis,
fact that it is not widely discussed with clarity and preci- 1991). That is, higher scores on any single item are
sion in the literature. Some team members were hesitant indicative of higher levels of the construct of interest. In
to discard authenticity as a component of voluntariness addition, the instrument should include multiple items
before we had actual data to shed light on this issue. to assess similar content. Redundancy in assessing
Others argued for a more concise definition of volun- the construct is important for developing a scale with
tariness before the item pool was finalized and data col- adequate psychometric properties (DeVellis, 1991).
lection began. This assertion was due to the concern Because items with poor psychometric properties are
that the inclusion of multiple themes in our definition of omitted after testing, the experimental scale is usually
voluntariness would result in a scale that would be con- longer than the final scale. For this reason, the experi-
taminated by other constructs and difficult to interpret. mental item pool can tolerate more redundancy than
Group discussions addressed the tensions among these the final instrument. However, the need for redundancy
different perspectives until a consensus could be should also be balanced with the overall length of the
reached. We eventually reached consensus that the per- item pool. When the response burden to participants is
ception of authenticity, although of interest both philo- too high, recruitment may become more difficult
sophically and psychologically, is largely irrelevant to a and/or the quality of responses may decline (Schmitt &
study of perception of voluntariness. An inauthentic Stuits, 1985). Strategies we used to operationalize per-
choice may be voluntary, and a non-voluntary choice ception of voluntariness included adaptation of existing
may be consistent with ‘one’s situation, values, and prior instruments and generation of new items based on focus
history’ (p. 707; Roberts, 2002). group data and literature review.

FINAL CONSTRUCT DEFINITION STRATEGIES USED TO OPERATIONALIZE THE CONSTRUCT


Our final construct definition, consistent with the views Adaptation of Existing Instruments. The process of
of Faden and Beauchamp (1986) and Wall (2001), was operationalization was made easier by linking percep-
that perception of voluntariness is the degree of control tion of voluntariness to control-related constructs in the
the individual perceives that he or she has over the spe- psychological literature. Particular attention during item
cific decision about protocol-based treatment. Both Wall development was paid to measures of locus of control
(2001) and Faden and Beauchamp (1986) refer to con- and self-efficacy. Additional instruments related to deci-
trol as a continuum; these conceptualizations are consis- sion making were also examined for their relevance to
tent with control-related constructs in the psychological perception of voluntariness. Relevant instruments that
literature (e.g., Ajzen, 2002). we examined included the Self-Determination Scale
(Sheldon, Ryan, & Reis, 1996), the Rotter Locus of
Operationalizing the Construct Control Scale (Rotter, 1966), the Multidimensional
Health Locus of Control Scale (Wallston, Wallston, &
OVERVIEW OF THE CHALLENGE DeVellis, 1978), the General Self-Efficacy Scale
Operationalization is the process of linking a construct (Bosscher & Smit, 1998; Sherer et al., 1982), the
definition to one or more specific, concrete indicators Decision Self-Efficacy Scale (Bunn & O’Connor, 1996;
that can be measured, such as items on a self-report Cranney et al., 2002), the Admission Experience
questionnaire. This can be challenging when there are Survey- Short Form (Gardner et al., 1993), and the
no existing instruments to measure the construct. In Decisional Conflict Scale (Bunn & O’Connor, 1996;

This content downloaded from 131.94.16.10 on Fri, 08 Jan 2016 09:20:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
JER0403_03 9/3/09 5:05 PM Page 26

26 V. Miller, W. Reynolds, R. Ittenbach, M. Luce, T. Beauchamp, R. Nelson

O’Connor, 1995). Items from these instruments were retained and adapted when the team agreed that they
used to facilitate the generation of items that assessed might be potential indicators of perception of voluntari-
perception of voluntariness. ness. Table 1 shows examples of original source items
We created a spreadsheet that included verbatim from these instruments and the adapted items used in
items from the instruments listed above; items were our final experimental item pool. For example, items
linked with their original source so that we could track from the locus of control scales were relevant because
the derivation and original meaning of items as they they assessed beliefs about control, but they were not, in
were edited. In group discussions, each item was exam- and of themselves, appropriate indicators of perception
ined for its potential usefulness as an indicator of per- of voluntariness. These items assess a trait-like charac-
ception of voluntariness (e.g., whether responses to teristic of individuals (e.g., an enduring characteristic of
items were indicative of voluntariness according to our the person that applies across situations), refer to gen-
construct definition). Items were discarded when they eral domains of functioning, and assess perceptions of
bore no relevance to the construct, and items were control over important outcomes. As such, items related

TABLE 1. Adapted Instruments and Items.

What It Example of 1st Revision for Final Revision for


Instrument Measures Original Item New Instrument New Instrument

Self- Beliefs about whether one “What I do is often not “I was not really free • “I was not free to
determination perceives oneself as the what I’d choose to do.” to decide what decide what I wanted.”
Scale origin of one’s actions I wanted.” (reverse-scored)
(“trait autonomy”). • “I was free to decide
what I wanted.”
Rotter Locus Beliefs about whether “By taking an active part “By taking an active “I was actively involved
of Control outcomes are causally in political and social part in decision in this decision.”
Scale dependent on variations affairs, the people can making, I was able to
in one’s own behavior. control world events.” control the decision
that was made.”
Multi-dimensional Beliefs about whether “If I get sick, it is my “It was my own “I had an influence
Health Locus one’s health outcomes own behavior which behavior that on the decision about
of Control are causally dependent determines how soon determined the the protocol.”
Scale on variations in one’s I get well again.” decision that
own behavior. was made.”
General Beliefs about one’s ability “When I make plans, “When confronted with Omitted
Self- to successfully execute I am certain I can this decision, I was
efficacy behaviors required to make them work.” certain that I could
Scale produce particular make the decision
outcomes. for myself.”
Decision Beliefs about one’s ability “I feel confident that “When making this “I was able to express
Self- to make effective I can express my decision, I felt my point of view
efficacy decisions about concerns about each confident that I about the decision.”
Scale treatment options. choice.” could express my
medical team.”
Decisional Beliefs about one’s “I have enough support “When making this “I was the one to choose.”
Conflict uncertainty and effective from others to make decision, I felt
Scale decision making (e.g., feeling a choice.” supported in making
the choice is informed, my own choice.”
values-based, and likely
to be implemented) for
a specific decision.
Admission Perceptions of coercion “People tried to force me “Someone took this • “Someone took this
Experiences when being admitted for to come to the hospital.” decision away ”decision away from
Survey—Short psychiatric hospitalization. from me.” me.”(reverse-scored)
Form • “Others made this
decision against my
wishes.” (reverse-
scored)

This content downloaded from 131.94.16.10 on Fri, 08 Jan 2016 09:20:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
JER0403_03 9/3/09 5:05 PM Page 27

Challenges in Measuring a New Construct 27

to locus of control were adapted so that they (1) would go into the experimental item pool. For exam-
assessed control during a specific period of time, (2) ple, some team members felt that items related to the
referred to the specific decision about protocol-based perception of options (e.g., “There were multiple
treatment or research, and (3) assessed perceptions of options worth considering”) should be included in the
the decision-making process and the decision itself, item pool. Other team members, coming from a meas-
not the outcomes associated with the decision. Items urement perspective, asked if this was consistent with
from the other measures were adapted in a similar man- our definition of perception of voluntariness. That is,
ner, so that they eventually bore little resemblance to would we be willing to say that agreement with this
their source items but instead reflected the construct of statement was indicative of higher levels of voluntari-
voluntariness as we had defined it. ness? In addition, some team members noted that the
The goal of this stage of scale development was to focus group data suggested that the availability of
ensure that we were being inclusive in our approach to options is independent of the individual’s control over
item generation, so items were discarded only after the decision. By discussing these questions, eventually
extensive group discussion. For example, we eventually we decided that these items should not be included in
concluded that items reflecting self-efficacy were not the item pool, but that it would be desirable to include
relevant indicators of perception of voluntariness, them somewhere in the questionnaire packet. We
because self-efficacy refers to confidence in one’s ability agreed that responses to these items could inform our
to perform a task or behavior in the future, not a task understanding of voluntariness and generate hypothe-
that has already been completed (e.g., a decision that ses for future research, by providing preliminary data
was made). about the conditions under which perception of volun-
Generation of New Items. In addition to discarding tariness is more or less likely.
items that were not relevant to perception of voluntari- To clarify our thinking about how to use additional
ness, we generated items to assess content that was not items to inform our understanding of perception of vol-
addressed by existing measures. For example, some untariness, we utilized a matrix, with one construct (e.g.,
focus group participants noted that their lack of control perception of options) on the horizontal axis and the
during decision making was expressed through feel- other (perception of voluntariness) on the vertical axis
ings of detachment and passivity. As such, we added (Table 2). The four individual cells reflect the various
items to assess these experiences to the experimental intersections of the two constructs; for example, parents
item pool (e.g., “I was passive in the face of this deci- who perceived that there were no options but still felt in
sion”). New items were added to the spreadsheet as control of the decision are reflected in the bottom left cell
they were generated, to ensure that we did not discard of the matrix. The matrix illustrated how, after data col-
potentially relevant items prematurely. Similar to the lection was complete, we could assign parents to one of
items adapted from existing instruments, these items the four categories, based on their perception of volun-
were revisited over time and discarded or edited as tariness scores and their responses to the item about
appropriate. perception of options. This approach has enabled us to
The use of a multidisciplinary team ensured that we identify separate constructs and the boundaries of vol-
targeted all relevant content and that items made sense untariness and to examine the factors that differentiate
conceptually from multiple perspectives. Throughout between the parents in the different cells. For example,
this process, team members struggled with differentiat- in cases in which it was not possible to fill in off-diagonal
ing perception of voluntariness from related constructs, cells, we were not able to conclude that the relevant con-
which was important for determining which items struct overlapped with voluntariness.

TABLE 2. Example of Matrix Approach to Differentiating Voluntariness from Related Constructs.

Perception of Voluntariness

Yes No

Perception Yes There were several options, and I was There were several options, but I was
of Options in control of choosing one. not in control of choosing one.
No There was only one option, and I was in There was only one option, and I was
control of choosing it (e.g., It was my not in control of choosing it.
choice but I had no choice).

This content downloaded from 131.94.16.10 on Fri, 08 Jan 2016 09:20:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
JER0403_03 9/3/09 5:05 PM Page 28

28 V. Miller, W. Reynolds, R. Ittenbach, M. Luce, T. Beauchamp, R. Nelson

Construction of the Experimental Form which is considered to be one source of information


regarding the construct validity of an instrument
In tandem with the adaptation of existing instruments (Thompson & Daniel, 1996).2
and generation of new items, we edited and deleted
items for clarity in order to arrive at our final experi- Summary
mental item pool. Attention was paid to reading level,
jargon, ambiguity, length of items, and double-barreled We have highlighted two challenges that can arise when
items, which contain more than one question, each of developing an instrument to measure a construct that is
which can be answered in a different way. Each of these not clearly defined in the literature and has never been
factors can threaten the validity of the instrument if it measured before: refining the construct definition and
impacts how individuals respond to the items. Items operationalizing the construct. Addressing these two
were revised by individual team members and discussed challenges constituted a major portion of the project,
and edited further in group meetings. Any single item spanning a period of ten months from the first focus
may have gone though several revisions, as we refined group to the final item pool. This time period does not
our construct definition, adapted and generated items, include the work that went into exploring potential
and examined the items for redundancy, structure, and meanings of the construct and sources of items prior to
clarity. While devising the experimental item pool, we the first focus group. It is surprising that construct defi-
also finalized the additional instruments and demo- nition and operationalization are rarely discussed in
graphic variables that would be administered with the much detail, given their importance for accurate meas-
questionnaire packet. These instruments were chosen urement and validity. The development of reliable and
with an eye toward establishing the construct validity of valid measures of key constructs in human research
the new scale and informing our understanding of per- ethics is essential to helping IRB members and re-
ception of voluntariness. We included measures of searchers assess decision making (e.g., voluntariness,
mood (Profile of Mood States–Bipolar; Lorr & McNair, understanding) in ethically problematic settings. In addi-
1988), coping (Coping Inventory for Stressful tion, our new measure of perception of voluntariness can
Situations; Endler & Parker, 1990), decision-making be used in future research to test important ethical ques-
preferences (Krantz Health Opinion Survey; Krantz, tions related to undue influences on both treatment and
Baum, & Wideman, 1980) (Autonomy Preference research decision making in medical settings.
Index; Ende et al., 1989), trust (Trust in Physician Scale;
Pearson & Raeke, 2000) (Trust in Medical Researchers; Best Practices
Hall et al., 2006), self-efficacy for decision making
(Decision Self-Efficacy Scale; Bunn & O’Connor, 1996, A number of recommendations can be made regarding
Cranney et al., 2002), and social desirability (Social scale development for constructs relevant to human
Desirability Scale-17; Stober, 2001). These instruments research ethics. The use of a multidisciplinary team was
were adapted using minor wording changes so that they particularly fruitful for us. For example, this approach
were applicable to parents making medical decisions for enabled us to do collective interpretive work that could
their children. not have been achieved individually (Siltanen, Willis, &
After piloting the experimental item pool and other Scobie, 2008). Given the complex nature of voluntari-
questionnaires with 17 parents from the target popula- ness, the benefits of a team approach were enhanced by
tion, items were discarded or revised as appropriate. the fact that team members viewed the issue through
Reasons for discarding or revising items included repet- different lenses. This allowed us to give careful thought
itiveness, double-barreled format, and ambiguous to existing literature and avoid the biases that may result
meaning. After this process, the final experimental item when viewing a construct from one particular view-
pool contained 28 items, all of which assessed the par- point. We were also careful not to reject prematurely
ent’s perception of control over decision making. The potential themes or items that might be relevant to the
next phase of the study involved administration of the construct. Although this process created tension at cer-
final experimental item pool and additional instruments tain points and involved revisiting the same debates
to parents, within 10 days of making a decision for pro- over time, a multidisciplinary approach allowed us to
tocol-based treatment for their seriously ill children. develop a more comprehensive understanding of our
Classical test theory guided our approach to scale con- construct and to ensure that the end product (i.e., an
struction (Streiner & Norman, 1995). This approach instrument to measure perception of voluntariness)
includes exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis, would be acceptable across different audiences. Both

This content downloaded from 131.94.16.10 on Fri, 08 Jan 2016 09:20:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
JER0403_03 9/3/09 5:05 PM Page 29

Challenges in Measuring a New Construct 29

our construct definition and item pool were subjected to competence and understanding and appreciation of the
multiple revisions throughout this process. The dissem- elements of consent. Attention to psychometrics, valid-
ination of empirical findings to multiple audiences to ity, appropriate settings for use, and feasibility would be
which human subjects research is relevant is consistent important, so that researchers in this area can choose
with our multidisciplinary approach. which measures will best fit their needs.
Additional strategies included literature reviews,
qualitative methods, and adaptation of existing meas- Educational Implications
ures. For example, the use of focus groups and inter-
views ensured that our definition of perception of It is important for investigators working in the area of
voluntariness was informed by the views of key stake- human research ethics to understand key concepts
holders in the informed consent process. The integra- related to instrument development and use instruments
tion of all of these components was critical in the early with acceptable levels of reliability and validity. Journal
phases of scale development, by facilitating careful clubs to discuss these issues are one good forum in
exploration of the construct definition, assuring that our which to become familiar with currently used instru-
item pool assessed all relevant domains of the construct, ments in empirical ethics research, critique those instru-
placing our construct definition and instrument in the ments, and generate ideas for the participants’ own
context of existing literature, and providing a strong work. Workshops or conferences can also be used in
foundation for enhancing the construct validity of the this manner. As mentioned earlier, literature reviews
final instrument. should be undertaken to review existing instruments
Though this process took time, it must not be short- and identify gaps that should be addressed in future
changed in the instrument development process. instrument development studies. These reviews are
Additional research, using the new measure of percep- important for investigators and would also be useful for
tion of voluntariness, is needed before recommenda- familiarizing other members of the research ethics com-
tions can be made for enhancing the voluntariness of munity, such as IRB members, with the benefits and
both research and treatment decisions made in medical shortcomings of available measures.
settings.
Acknowledgments
Research Agenda
This research was supported in part by grants from the
Once the Decision Making Control Instrument is com- National Science Foundation (SES-0527618; Drs.
plete, additional research using the instrument is needed Nelson, Luce, and Beauchamp) and the National
to explore different questions related to informed con- Institutes of Health/National Cancer Institute
sent. For example, how does the presence of a serious (R21CA118377-01A1; Dr. Nelson) and an Institutional
medical condition influence voluntary choice? Under Development Grant to The Center for Research
what conditions do financial inducements for research Integrity, Department of Anesthesiology and Critical
participation pose an undue influence to informed con- Care Medicine, The Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia.
sent? How do features of the informed consent process
affect the voluntariness of a choice? Are there patient Author Note
or clinician characteristics that make voluntary choice
less likely? Answers to these questions are needed to Address correspondence to: Victoria Miller, The
develop interventions to enhance the voluntariness of Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia, 34th St. and Civic
both research and treatment decisions made in medical Center Blvd., CHOP North Room 1515, Philadelphia,
settings. PA 19104. Phone: 267-426-5259; Fax: 267-426-5035;
Interventions aimed at improving informed consent E-MAIL: millerv@email.chop.edu.
would be foolhardy unless soundly based on empirical
research using reliable and valid measures of the key Authors’ Biographical Sketches
constructs that are relevant to human subject research
and clinical ethics. It is likely that rigorous scale devel- Victoria A. Miller is a pediatric psychologist at The
opment studies need to be undertaken for constructs Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia and Assistant
such as autonomy, vulnerability, and cultural compe- Professor of Anesthesiology and Critical Care Medicine
tence. In addition, it would be beneficial to conduct sys- at the University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine.
tematic reviews of existing measures of decision making Her current research focuses on informed consent, child

This content downloaded from 131.94.16.10 on Fri, 08 Jan 2016 09:20:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
JER0403_03 9/3/09 5:05 PM Page 30

30 V. Miller, W. Reynolds, R. Ittenbach, M. Luce, T. Beauchamp, R. Nelson

assent, and parent-child collaborative decision making 2009), co-authored (with Ruth Faden) A History and
for the management of childhood chronic illness. Theory of Informed Consent (Oxford University Press,
William Reynolds is Assistant Professor of Social Work 1986), and authored Philosophical Ethics (McGraw Hill,
at The Richard Stockton College of New Jersey. His 1st ed., 1982; 3rd ed., 2001). In 2004, Beauchamp was
research has focused on the informed consent and assent given the Lifetime Achievement Award of the American
decisions of parents, adolescents, and children in the con- Society of Bioethics and Humanities (ASBH) in recogni-
text of pediatric medical research. He is a co-investigator tion of his contributions and publications in bioethics
on the project described in the present article. and the humanities. Earlier, in 1994, Indiana University
Richard F. Ittenbach is Associate Professor of Pediatrics awarded Beauchamp its “Memorial Award for Furthering
in the Division of Biostatistics and Epidemiology, Greater Understanding and Exchange of Opinions
Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center. His current between the Professions of Law and Medicine.”
research focuses on the intersection of traditional scale Robert M. Nelson is Pediatric Ethicist in the Office of
development methods and generalized linear models and Pediatric Therapeutics, Office of the Commissioner at the
applications. Increasingly, his work emphasizes opera- U.S. Food and Drug Administration. The work reported
tional definitions of emerging biobehavioral constructs for in this article was conducted prior to Dr. Nelson joining
children with life-threatening illnesses and disabilities. the U.S. Food and Drug Administration, and do not rep-
Mary Frances Luce is Thomas A. Finch, Jr. Professor resent the views and/or policies of the FDA or the U.S.
of Business Administration at The Fuqua School of Department of Health and Human Services. Dr. Nelson’s
Business at Duke University. She is interested in con- research explores aspects of child assent and parental per-
sumer behavior, medical decision making, and the mission such as adolescent risk perception, the develop-
effects of negative emotion on decision behavior. She co- ment of a child’s capacity to assent, and the degree to
authored Emotional Decisions: Tradeoff Difficulty and which parental choice is perceived as voluntary.
Coping in Consumer Choice.
Tom L. Beauchamp is a member of the Department of End Notes
Philosophy and also Senior Research Scholar at the
1
Kennedy Institute of Ethics, Georgetown University. In The social science researcher (WR) and quantitative
1976, he wrote the bulk of The Belmont Report (1978) for psychologist (RI) moved to other institutions but
the National Commission for the Protection of Human remained involved in the project.
2
Subjects of Biomedical and Behavioral Research. He co- The final instrument will be available on the
authored (with James Childress) Principles of Biomedical University of California Press website once it is pub-
Ethics (Oxford University Press, 1st ed., 1979; 6th ed., lished, pending appropriate copyright approval.

References

A JZEN , I. (2002). Perceived behavioral control, self-efficacy, C RONBACH , L. J. & M EEHL , P. E. (1955). Construct validity in
locus of control, and the theory of planned behavior. Journal psychological tests. Psychological Bulletin, 52(4), 281–302.
of Applied Social Psychology, 32(4), 665–683. D E V ELLIS , R. F. (1991). Scale Development: Theory and
B OSSCHER , R. J. & S MIT, J. H. (1998). Confirmatory factor Applications (Applied Social Research Methods Series, Vol.
analysis of the general self-efficacy scale. Behavior Research 26). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
and Therapy, 36(3), 339–343. E NDE , J., K AZIS , L., A SH , A., & M OSKOWITZ , M. A. (1989).
B UNN , H. & O’C ONNOR , A. (1996). Validation of client deci- Measuring patients’ desire for autonomy: Decision making
sion-making instruments in the context of psychiatry. and information-seeking preferences among medical patients.
Canadian Journal of Nursing Research, 28(3), 13–27. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 4(1), 23–30.
C AMPBELL , D. T. & F ISKE , D. W. (1959). Convergent and dis- E NDLER , N. S., & PARKER , J. D. (1990). Multidimensional
criminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. assessment of coping: A critical evaluation. Journal of
Psychological Bulletin, 56(2), 81–105. Personality and Social Psychology, 58(5), 844–854.
C RANNEY, A., O’C ONNOR , A. M., JACOBSEN , M. J., T UGWELL , FADEN , R. R., & B EAUCHAMP, T. L. (1986). A history and the-
P., A DACHI , J. D., O OI , D. S., WALDEGGER , L., G OLDSTEIN , ory of informed consent. New York: Oxford University Press.
R., & W ELLS , G. A. (2002). Development and pilot testing of G ARDNER , W., H OGE , S. K., B ENNETT, N., R OTH , L. H.,
a decision aid for postmenopausal women with osteoporosis. L IDZ , C. W., M ONAHAN , J., ET AL . (1993). Two scales for
Patient Education and Counseling, 47(3), 245–255. measuring patients’ performance and perceptions of coercion

This content downloaded from 131.94.16.10 on Fri, 08 Jan 2016 09:20:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
JER0403_03 9/3/09 5:05 PM Page 31

Challenges in Measuring a New Construct 31

during hospital admission. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, S CHMITT, N. & S TUITS , D. M. (1985). Factors defined by neg-
20, 307–321. atively keyed items: The result of careless respondents?
HALL , M. A., C AMACHO, F., L AWLOR , J. S., D E P UY, V., Applied Psychological Measurement, 9(4), 367–373.
S UGARMAN , J., & W EINFURT, K. (2006). Measuring trust SHELDON, K. M., RYAN, R., & REIS, H. T. (1996). What makes for
in medical researchers. Medical Care, 44(11), 1048–1053. a good day? Competence and autonomy in the day and in the
KRANTZ, D. S., BAUM, A., & WIDEMAN, M. V. (1980). Assessment person. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22(12),
of preferences for self-treatment and information in health 1270–1279.
care. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(5), S HERER , M., M ADDUX , J. E., M ERCADANTE , B., P RENTICE -
977–990. D UNN , S., JACOBS , B., & R OGERS , R. W. (1982). The self-
L ORR , M. & M C NAIR , D. M. (1988). Profile of Mood States: efficacy scale: Construction and validation. Psychological
Bipolar Form (manual). San Diego, CA: Educational and Reports, 51, 663–671.
Industrial Testing Service. S ILTANEN , J., W ILLIS , A., & S COBIE , W. (2008). Separately
M AC K ENZIE , S. B. (2003). The dangers of poor construct con- together: Working reflexively as a team. International Journal
ceptualization. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, of Social Research Methodology, 11(1), 45–61.
31(3), 323–326. STÖBER, J. (2001). The Social Desirability Scale-17 (SDS-17):
M C G RATH , R. E. (2005). Conceptual complexity and con- Convergent validity, discriminant validity, and relationship
struct validity. Journal of Personality Assessment, 85(2), with age. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 17(3),
112–124. 222–232.
Nuremberg Code. (1949). In Trials of War Criminals before the STREINER , D. L. & NORMAN , G. R. (1995). Health Measurement
Nuremberg Military Tribunals under Control Council Law No Scales: A Practical Guide to their Development and Use. New
10, Vol. 2, Nuremberg, October 1946–April 1949. Washington, York: Oxford University Press.
DC: U.S. Government Printing Office. T HOMPSON , B. & DANIEL , L. G. (1996). Factor analytic evi-
O’C ONNOR , A. M. (1995). Validation of a decisional conflict dence for the construct validity of scores: A historical over-
scale. Medical Decision Making, 15(1), 25–30. view and some guidelines. Educational and Psychological
P EARSON , S. D. & R AEKE , L. H. (2000). Patients’ trust in Measurement, 56(2), 197–208.
physicians: Many theories, few measures, and little data. WALL , E. (2001). Voluntary action. Philosophia, 28(1–4), 127–136.
Journal of General Internal Medicine, 15(7), 509–513. WALLANDER , J. L. (1992). Theory-driven research in pediatric
R OBERTS , L. W. (2002). Informed consent and the capacity for psychology: A little bit on why and how. Journal of Pediatric
voluntarism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(5), 705–712. Psychology, 17(5), 521–535.
R OTTER , J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal WALLSTON , K. A., WALLSTON , B. S., & D E V ELLIS , R. (1978).
versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Development of the multidimensional health locus of control
Monographs, 80, 1–28. (MHLC) scales. Health Education Monographs, 6, 160–170.

This content downloaded from 131.94.16.10 on Fri, 08 Jan 2016 09:20:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
JER0403_03 9/3/09 5:05 PM Page 32

This content downloaded from 131.94.16.10 on Fri, 08 Jan 2016 09:20:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like