You are on page 1of 12

Cleaner and Responsible Consumption 12 (2024) 100175

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Cleaner and Responsible Consumption


journal homepage: www.journals.elsevier.com/cleaner-and-responsible-consumption

The effects of perceptions of environmental health risk and environmental


risk on sustainable infectious waste management behaviours among
citizens in Bangkok, Thailand
Piyapong Janmaimool a, *, Jaruwan Chontanawat b, c, Surapong Chudech b
a
ASEAN Institute for Health Development, Mahidol University, 999 Salaya, Nakhon Pathom Province, 73170, Thailand
b
Department of Social Sciences and Humanities, School of Liberal Arts, King Mongkut’s University of Technology Thonburi, 126 Pracha-Uthit Rd., Thungkru, Bangkok,
10140, Thailand
c
The Joint Graduate School of Energy and Environment (JGSEE), King Mongkut’s University of Technology Thonburi, 126 Pracha-Uthit Rd., Thungkru, Bangkok,
10140, Thailand

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: This study aims to examine how people’s perceived characteristics of environmental risks and environmental
Sustainable infectious waste management health risks, as associated with COVID-19 infectious waste problems, affect their participation in sustainable
behaviors infectious waste management behaviours (i.e. infectious waste minimization, waste collection awareness, and
Environmental health risk perception
infectious waste segregation). Questionnaires were conducted with 521 people living in Bangkok, Thailand.
Environmental risk perception
Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was carried out to validate the study variables, followed by structural
Pro-environmental behaviour
Sustainable development equation modelling (SEM) to test how perceived characteristics of environmental risks (perceived ecological
threat occurrence to the environment and perceived severity of the threat to the environment), the environ­
mental health risks themselves (perceived ecological threat occurrence to human health and perceived severity of
the threat to human health) and socio-economic characteristics influence individuals’ sustainable infectious
waste management behaviours. The results revealed that perceived ecological threat occurrence to the envi­
ronment and human health did not statistically affect all types of sustainable infectious waste management
behaviours, but perceived severity of the ecological threat to human health significantly affected individuals’
infectious waste minimization and segregation. Perceived severity of the ecological threat to the environment
only affected infectious waste collection awareness. In addition, the respondents’ age and education level
positively and significantly affected most types of behaviours, whereas income only had a negative effect on
respondents’ infectious waste minimization. These results provide implications for the development of
communication strategies to motivate people to participate in sustainable infectious waste management
behaviours.

1. Introduction self-administered rapid test kits in daily life. Most PPE and rapid test kits
become infectious waste after being used (Jung et al., 2021). In addition,
Since the coronavirus outbreak became a global pandemic in March they are mostly made from plastics, which directly impact the envi­
2020, several countries have recommended that their citizens wear face ronment once they are disposed of and later degrade (Rume and Islam,
masks in public to reduce the transmission of the virus (Cai et al., 2020; 2022). This makes the drastic increases in the quantities of infectious
Pandit et al., 2021), and to use antigen-detecting rapid diagnostic tests waste most concerning, requiring their effective management (Jung
(Ag-RDT) to detect and control infection (World Health Organization, et al., 2021). According to the WHO (2022) and Das, (2021), a massive
2021). New normal living in response to the COVID-19 outbreak has increase in infectious waste from the COVID-19 pandemic has exacer­
caused a massive increase in infectious waste upon the practising of bated environmental problems and posed a serious threat to human
health protection measures, such as using personal protective equip­ health and environmental quality. Many scholars, including Roberts
ment (PPE; e.g. hand gloves, face masks, face shields, etc.) and et al. (2020), Bouchet et al. (2021) and Hartanto and Mayasari (2021),

* Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: piyapong.jan@mahidol.ac.th (P. Janmaimool), Jaruwan.cho@kmutt.ac.th (J. Chontanawat).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clrc.2024.100175
Received 27 July 2023; Received in revised form 20 January 2024; Accepted 25 January 2024
Available online 5 February 2024
2666-7843/© 2024 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-
nc-nd/4.0/).
P. Janmaimool et al. Cleaner and Responsible Consumption 12 (2024) 100175

have confirmed that the increase in the quantity of infectious waste is those healthcare facilities have implemented infectious waste manage­
due to the growth in the production, patronage and use of face masks ment systems which can help mitigate environmental and health im­
specifically. The WHO (2022) further reported that since the emergence pacts. Another major source of infectious waste is from households. With
of COVID-19 until December 2021, over 140 million COVID-19 test kits limited access to medical support, increased numbers of infected cases
had been procured by the United Nations (UN), and those test kits had have to self-medicate at home. Additionally, the use of PPE by general
the potential to generate approximately 2600 tons of general waste. people also contributes to a massive increase in the quantity of infectious
Improper disposal of this infectious waste can cause environmental waste. This situation is very concerning as it has been reported that a
contamination as well, for once in the environment, face masks and great amount of household infectious waste was not properly managed.
other plastic waste begin to degrade upon weathering. During these This study, therefore, focuses on the management of infectious wastes
processes, the plastics in the waste transform into microfibers that can generated from households. Several studies have revealed that the way
enter both land and aquatic ecosystems (Mohamed et al., 2022; Saliu people use and dispose of PPE and rapid test kits could destroy the
et al., 2021). These microfibers are composed of micro and nano plastics environment and impact human health (Liu, 2020; Shutterstock, 2021;
(183–1247 particles per piece) as well as organic pollutants that can Singh et al., 2022). Nevertheless, inappropriate littering of used face
then enter the ecological food chain and contaminate environmental masks has steadily increased in many countries (Shutterstock, 2021).
systems and human food (Bouwmeester et al., 2015; Chang et al., 2020). For instance, during a beach cleaning of Britain’s most popular tourist
Rathinamoorthy and Raja Balasaraswathi (2022) and Shen et al. (2021) beaches in September 2020, more than 100 face masks were recovered
found that when exposed to natural weathering, disposable face masks (Liu, 2020). In addition, many people are not aware of waste separation;
could increase the release of microfibers from 71.06% to 99.99%. The thus, they mix recyclables with other waste. This increases the volume of
WHO (2018) added that pathogens and toxic pollutants released to the general waste that goes to landfills and hinders the proper management
environment during the process of infectious waste disposal and treat­ of infectious waste, such as high-temperature incineration. Mixing
ment can pose a serious health risk to humans, as microplastics can enter several types of waste also prevents the recovery of recyclables. Singh
and accumulate in human lungs, placenta, hair, skin and blood (Abbasi et al. (2022) indicated that the low cost of face masks and people’s lack
et al., 2019; Goodman et al., 2021; Leslie et al., 2022), which affects cell of awareness of the negative impact of infectious waste on the envi­
morphology and inhibits cell proliferation (Goodman et al., 2021). Yee ronment and human health yield a great level of consumption and the
et al. (2021) also indicated that people who are exposed to microplastics waste’s improper disposal. Therefore, the way people use PPE and
may develop inflammation, apoptosis and metabolic homeostasis. dispose of it should become more sustainable and safer (Jung et al.,
Ecological and environmental impacts caused by the improper 2021). The WHO (2017) suggested an effective health care waste
management of infectious waste are also concerning. Several aquatic management method which includes five steps: waste classification and
animals have been affected by the presence of used face masks in surface segregation; waste minimization; waste handling and collection; waste
water resources, as animals often mistakenly consume plastic materials on-site transport; and waste treatment and disposal. To complete this
from face masks as food, and thus suffer with malnutrition (Keiron et al., effective health care management, people who produce infectious waste
2020). Fauna can also be affected by littered face masks; one report from should particularly take part in waste minimization, segregation and
Columbia explained that a bird died because its body and beak were collection.
wrapped up in a discarded face mask (Boyle, 2020). In addition, the Encouraging people to participate in sustainable infectious waste
improper management of infectious waste generated from used PPE can management behaviours is also very challenging, as people’s decision to
increase the chance of viral infection, with Haque et al. (2021a,b) perform a behaviour is based on varying factors. Risk communication
finding that the COVID-19 virus can adhere to the surface of tested has been proved to be effective in enhancing people’s awareness and
materials and infectious waste for a long time. Consequently, the virus motivation to participate in sustainable behaviours (Zeng et al., 2020)
can be transmitted to humans upon contact with the infected waste. and preventive behaviours (You et al., 2023). Risk communication en­
Despite its necessity, the management of infectious waste is complex ables people to construct a risk perception which finally motivates them
and challenging. Though untreated infectious waste is disposed of in to perform environmental related behaviours (Maartensson and Loi,
landfills, environmental contaminations, such as that of surface and 2022) or preventive behaviours (Cooper et al., 2020). Namely, when
underground water, can still occur if the landfills are not properly built. people feel that they or their valued things might be negatively impacted
Incineration of infectious waste is widely practised to avoid this issue; by a potential threat, they are likely to response to the risk by per­
however, this releases other pollutants into the air and generates ash forming adaptive and response actions (Boss et al., 2015). As indicated
residue. The dioxins and furans generated from incinerated materials in Protection Motivation Theory (PMT; Rogers, 1975), individuals’
containing chlorine in particular can cause serious health impacts estimation of the level of threat to themselves and their valued things (e.
(WHO, 2018). In this way, it is important to find a sustainable way to g. personal health, the health of others or the environment) can
manage infectious waste. Several countries also now face infectious contribute to their decision to perform actions that mitigate risk. An
waste management problems due to the great volume of waste which effective risk communication is therefore important to enhance people’
exceeds the capacity of their waste management systems (Knowlton, risk perception and motivation to perform a concerned behaviour.
2019). According to the U.S. EPA (2020), developing countries produce To develop effective risk communication strategies, it is important to
COVID-19 infectious waste at a rate of approximately 2.5 kg per bed per understand types of risk messages which can have a significant impact
day. In Thailand, the disposal of infectious waste became a critical on people’ concerned behavers (Lin and Petersen, 2008). Understanding
problem in August 2021: Approximately 432.5 tons of infectious waste is significant associations between individuals’ perception of risk charac­
produced per day, but local incinerators have the capacity to dissolve teristics and concerned behaviours can help identifying potential risk
only 305 tons per day (WHO, 2022). In response to this issue, the Bureau messages (Fitzpatrick-Lewis et al., 2010). However, the concept of risk
of Environmental Health, Thailand employed industrial waste process­ itself has diverse dimensions such as possibility of threat occurrence and
ing factories and upscaled local incinerators to dispose of the excess severity of its consequence (Sjöberg et al., 2004). In addition, risks can
waste. be divided into several types based on objects or things that a hazard or
The major sources of infectious waste from COVID-19 are from harmful event can affect. For instance, environmental risk perception
healthcare establishments such as health centres, hospitals, quarantine concerns disruptions or impactful events on the environment or
facilities, clinics, autopsy centres and laboratories. Infectious wastes ecological elements, such as plants, animals and even non-biotic ele­
generated by these healthcare establishments are highly infectious ments (Slimak and Dietz, 2006). In turn, environmental health risk
waste including used sharps, liquid waste from diagnostic machines, perception concerns the impact of environmental conditions on people’s
reagents, spent cartridges and blood collection tubes. However, most of health and well-being (Asuzu, 1994). Therefore, risk messages can be

2
P. Janmaimool et al. Cleaner and Responsible Consumption 12 (2024) 100175

diverse. Considering risks associated with COVID-19 infectious waste participation in sustainable infectious waste management to avoid
which can have negative impacts on both human health and the envi­ environmental and health impacts. The WHO (2017) suggested ways to
ronment, individuals’ risk perception can be therefore divided into two manage infectious waste, with some stages of these methods requiring
types including environmental risk and environmental health risk per­ people’s active participation such as waste classification and segrega­
ceptions. Accordingly, potential risk messages related to COVID-19 in­ tion. Omar et al. (2012) indicated that waste segregation helps effi­
fectious waste can be very diverse. To develop effective risk ciently manage infectious waste by reducing the amount of waste that
communication strategies for promoting sustainable infectious waste must be treated as hazardous. In addition, waste segregation can help
management behaviours, it is therefore important to understand a type reduce the cost of waste treatment (WHO, 2017). Regarding COVID-19
of risk perception and risk messages which can have a significant effect waste management, the WHO (2021) recommended that waste gener­
on the behaviours. ated from PPE use should be disposed of in a strong and noticeable bag
Regarding available current studies, though many studies have that is closed before it is collected by the responsible organizations.
revealed that risk perception can motivate people to perform environ­ In turn, general non-infectious health care waste should be properly
mental behaviours, none of these explore how several types of risk separated from infectious waste, such as pathological or sharp waste.
perception (e.g. environmental risk perception and environmental Mosquera et al. (2014) and Windfeld and Brooks (2015) proposed that
health risk perception) can significantly affect environment-related be­ the separation of recyclable waste from potentially infectious waste
haviours, particularly sustainable COVID-19 infectious waste manage­ must be taken into consideration as well. If recyclable waste is efficiently
ment behaviours. Knowing the types of risk perception that affect segregated, it will minimize the volume of waste that is sent for treat­
people’s decisions to perform sustainable behaviours can inform the ment and disposal, and help maintain resource efficiency as well as
development of related communication strategies. The main goals of this material recovery. Another waste management strategy is waste mini­
study are to gain insight understanding on how to develop risk mization, which is the most preferable approach, because it involves
communication strategies for promoting sustainable infectious waste producing as little waste as possible (WHO, 2017). Waste minimization
management behaviours and to identify target groups which risk aims to prevent or reduce the generation of waste by emphasizing source
communication should be implemented with. Based on these goals, the reduction and recycling. In the context of COVID-19 prevention, ac­
association between individuals’ sustainable infectious waste manage­ cording to the UK’s Department for Environment, Food & Rural Affairs
ment behaviours and variables related to environmental risk and envi­ (2021) and the WHO (2021), people should use cloth face masks instead
ronmental health risk perceptions, and the association between the of single-use face masks to prevent and reduce the amount of infectious
behaviours and socio-economic characteristics will be explored. waste generated. In addition, people are encouraged to use sustainable
For the objectives of this study, this study first aims to explore reusable masks that are made from recycled materials, such as recycled
participation in sustainable infectious waste management behaviours plastic or water bottles (Block, 2020; Gupta et al., 2021; Haque et al.,
among people living in Bangkok, Thailand, which has been facing a 2021a,b; Leal Filho et al., 2021). The WHO (2021) also promoted the use
great amount of infectious waste as a highly populated area. It also ex­ of safe and reusable PPE.
plores how these people perceive environmental risk and environmental Waste collection is important to effective infectious waste manage­
health risk characteristics in relation to COVID-19 infectious waste­ ment as well. According to the WHO (2017), infectious waste must be
–related problems. In addition, this study aims to examine how those safely collected before being sent for treatment and disposal. However, it
perceived characteristics as well as the citizens’ socio-economic char­ is also important to ensure that facilities such as infectious waste con­
acteristics (i.e. income, age and education) affect their participation in tainers or bags are sufficiently provided for people to properly dispose of
sustainable infectious waste management behaviours, including infec­ infectious waste. At the same time, people should be aware of its proper
tious waste minimization, collection awareness and segregation. The collection and disposal by not littering or leaving used PPE in the
results could provide implications for the development of communica­ environment. Several reports have revealed that a great number of used
tion strategies that promote public engagement in sustainable infectious face masks were found in natural areas such as beaches and oceans (Liu,
waste management behaviours. 2020; Shutterstock, 2021). Based on this discussion, to achieve sus­
tainable infectious waste management, there are several actions that
2. Literature reviews, conceptual framework and hypotheses people should perform. This study therefore explores people’s partici­
pation in these sustainable infectious waste management behaviours,
2.1. Sustainable COVID-19 infectious waste management behaviours notably waste minimization, segregation and collection.

According to the WHO (2017), infectious waste refers to waste that is 2.2. Environmental risk and environmental health risk perceptions
suspected to contain pathogens in adequate concentrations to cause
disease transmission. Infectious waste is generally contaminated with To encourage people’s participation in sustainable infectious waste
blood or other body fluids, while highly infectious waste is contami­ management behaviours, this study focuses on the influence of in­
nated with laboratory cultures and microbiological stocks, including dividuals’ perceived risks associated with COVID-19 infectious waste­
used sharps, liquid waste from diagnostic machines, reagents, spent –related problems. As stated by many scholars, a massive increase in
cartridges and blood collection tubes. Materials that have been in con­ infectious waste can be a major threat to the environment and human
tact with infected patients are also considered infectious waste. Upon health (Bouchet et al., 2021; Hartanto and Mayasari, 2021; Roberts
the outbreak of COVID-19, general citizens became infectious waste et al., 2020). Thus, it can be assumed that once individuals construct
producers, as they have been encouraged to wear face masks for pro­ risks associated with this large amount of COVID-19 infectious waste,
tection against the virus, and to use Ag-RDT to detect infection. Inevi­ they might be aware of its negative impacts and decide to perform
tably, the increased use of disposable PPE and diagnosis kits has led to a sustainable infectious practices.
just as massive increase in potentially infectious waste which may pose Theoretically, risk perception can be defined as an individual’s
health and environmental threats. subjective assessment of the probability of a specified type of threat
Our world is now facing a challenging environmental problem that occurrence and potential adverse consequences (Sjöberg et al., 2004).
demands people’s participation in pro-environmental practices (Steg Similarly, Renn (2017) defined risk perception as individuals’ expecta­
and Vlek, 2009). Massive increases in the amount of infectious waste tion of how a harmful event can occur and how much harm it can cause.
generated by individuals and improper management of this waste have Rogers’s (1975) PMT defines risk perception as individuals’ estimation
exacerbated this environmental problem. Therefore, in this new normal of the level of threat to themselves and their valued things (e.g. personal
living, several organizations and scholars have suggested people’s health, the health of others or the environment). Individuals’ perceived

3
P. Janmaimool et al. Cleaner and Responsible Consumption 12 (2024) 100175

risks can motivate them to take adaptive actions (Boss et al., 2015), as importance to environmental values may decide to perform actions to
per the psychometric paradigm (Slovic, 2000), individuals’ risk mitigate environmental problems. The threat associated with a massive
perception is based on their rational thinking process and their inter­ increase in COVID-19 infectious waste can generate such negative
pretation and understanding of risk characteristics. Slovic (2000) environmental change, and thus affect human health.
emphasized individuals’ interpretation of hazard characteristics (i.e.
threat occurrence and threat severity) that enable people to construct
2.3. Conceptual framework and hypotheses
risks. Once individuals recognize the possibility of threat occurrence,
they start assessing the risk that they may face. Accordingly, individuals
This study aims to explore how individuals’ perceived environmental
assess the threat’s catastrophic consequence(s) and judge the extent of
risk and environmental health risk characteristics (including threat
the potential impacts that they or their value things would undergo.
occurrence and severity) affect their participation in sustainable infec­
Individuals who are aware of potential threat occurrence and cata­
tious waste management behaviours (see Fig. 1). As stated by Slovic
strophic consequences tend to also construct their ability to face risks.
(2000), the ways people construct risks could shape how they act or
Several scholars have confirmed that, to perceive risk, an individual,
respond to those risks. Duan and Fortner (2010) proposed that the public
group of people or society needs to evaluate the probability of hazard
perception of various environmental risks leads to public concerns, and
occurrence, as well as the adverse consequences of a negative outcome
then to the decision to perform actions responding to those risks. Based
(Slovic, 1987; Wachinger et al., 2013). Slovic (1987) added that the
on the Health Belief Model (HBM; Rosenstock, 1974), once individual
adverse consequences caused by a hazard may happen to the environ­
construct risks, those individuals value the benefits from behavioural
ment or human health, and individuals’ judgement of adverse conse­
changes and consider barriers that they may face when practising health
quences can be influenced by hazard features and/or personal beliefs.
behaviours. Finally, they decide whether they should practise those
Risk perception can be further classified into several types based on
concerning behaviours. Regarding the perception of environmental
objects or things that can be potentially affected by a hazard or harmful
risks, Lin et al. (2021) and Lieske et al. (2014) found that when in­
event. In this study, which focuses on the threat from a massive increase
dividuals who are concerned about the environment have perceived
in COVID-19 infectious waste, the threat can have negative impacts on
environmental risks, their sense of urgency and responsibility can be
both environmental systems and human health. Thus, risk perception in
constructed or enhanced. Accordingly, they consider practising behav­
this study is classified into two types: environmental risk perception and
iours that benefit the environment. For instance, Pahl et al. (2005) and
environmental health risk perception. Regarding the first, Slimak and
Schmitt et al. (2019) revealed that people’s pro-environmental behav­
Dietz (2006) defined environmental risk perception as the perceived
iours are influenced by their risk perception. Jeong et al. (2021)
impacts of disruption or an event on the environment or ecological el­
demonstrated that pro-environmental intention is significantly associ­
ements, such as plants, animals and humans. Any disruptions or harmful
ated with the perception of marine microplastic pollution. Similarly, for
events that create adverse impacts on the environment can be called
the perception of environmental health risks, which focuses on health
ecological threats, which Beck (1992) claimed mostly arise from the
impacts caused by an ecological threat, PMT indicates that people adopt
modernization process. Related, Yu and Xie (2006) and Zhang (2013)
defensive or protective actions to reduce the likelihood of facing a risk
defined environmental risk perception in reference to an individual’s
when confronted with a harmful event (Rogers, 1975). Terpstra et al.
perceived severity of an ecological threat, possibility of ecological threat
(2009) and Bubeck et al. (2012) likewise found that people’s perceived
occurrence and persistence of the risk.
flood risks to their health and well-being contributed to their practice of
Besides posing a risk to the environment, threats can also pose a risk
flood mitigation measures. Additionally, Linden (2015) and Lacroix and
to human health through negative changes in environmental elements.
Gifford (2018) reported that climate change risk perception stimulated
This type of risk can be called environmental health risk. As stated by the
people to practise energy-saving behaviours.
WHO (2016) and the Australian Government Department of Health
With regard to the massive increase in COVID-19 infectious waste
(2014), environmental health refers to conditions in the environment
due to people’s extensive use of PPE and self-administered rapid test kits
such as air, water and soil that can influence human health and
in daily life, a risk to both the environment and human health can arise,
well-being. Environmental health risk is therefore defined as the risk
as discussed in the introduction. Therefore, it can be assumed that
associated with environmental factors (air, water, soil, food, light, noise
people’s perceptions of the characteristics of environmental and envi­
and hygiene) (Asuzu, 1994). Environmental health risks cause negative
ronmental health risks positively affect their sustainable infectious
impacts for people and often lead to their preventive responses (Estévez
waste management behaviours, including infectious waste minimization
et al., 2015; Smith and Mayer, 2018). Namely, when people are aware of
and infectious waste segregation (see Fig. 2). Additionally, this study
environmental health risks and pay attention to them, they are likely to
treated individuals’ awareness of proper infectious collection as a type
realize the importance of environmental problem mitigation measures
of sustainable infectious waste management behaviour, though it has
and the improvement of environmental quality. Consequently, they are
different features from another two types of behaviour explored in this
more willing to act pro-environmentally. Similarly, upon perceiving
study. Individuals’ infectious waste collection awareness can be asso­
environmental risks, people who care about the environment and give
ciated with practices that can ensure a proper collection and

Fig. 1. Perceptions of environmental risk and environmental health risk characteristics and sustainable actions.
Source: Created by authors (2022)

4
P. Janmaimool et al. Cleaner and Responsible Consumption 12 (2024) 100175

Fig. 2. Conceptual Framework


Note: see glossary of acronyms in Table 1.

management of infectious waste. Those practices are such as not waste management behaviours, and younger ages might be more likely
forgetting a used face mask in a public area or carefully keeping in-use to participate in those behaviours due to their capacity to access
face masks. This current study assumed that if individuals perceived the environment-related information. Regardless of outcome, knowing how
potential environmental and health impacts caused by infectious waste, each population responds to environmental and health challenging
they would be aware of their masks and put some efforts to ensure that problems can provide implications for the development of effective
those used masks will be properly managed. Thus, levels of forgetfulness communication policies (Smith and Kingston, 2021).
and the times that individuals lost their used masks can vary among Education level is another socio-economic factor that has been
individuals with different degree of risk perception. widely explored for its association with environment-related behav­
Furthermore, this study assumes that a population’s socio-economic iours, because it can be connected to environmental variables (Berger,
characteristics might also affect sustainable infectious waste manage­ 1997; Hines et al., 1986). Several studies have revealed that people with
ment behaviours. One socio-economic characteristic which has been a higher educational level tend to have a higher level of environmental
widely explored for its relationship with environment-related behav­ concerns as well (Gifford and Nilsson, 2014; Liere and Dunlap, 1980).
iours is age. Knowing which age groups in a population are willing or Meyer (2015) explained that educational attainment can encourage
hesitant to act sustainably can imply the need for a communication or people to care more about the welfare of their society, including the
education campaign for each age group. Several studies conducted be­ environment, and that education enables people to realize the external
tween 1970 and 1980 have already revealed that younger groups are benefits of their behaviours or actions. This study thus assumes that
more likely to act sustainably (Buttel, 1979; Hines et al., 1986; Liere and individuals with a higher education level are more likely to participate
Dunlap, 1980). In contrast, later studies indicated a positive association in infectious waste management behaviours due to their ability to un­
between age and pro-environmental behaviour, wherein older groups derstand environmental situations, and to recognize the importance of
are more likely to participate in environmental behaviours than younger infectious waste management behaviours.
groups (Otto and Kaiser, 2014; Wiernik et al., 2013), as it is possible that Income level can also be associated with sustainable behaviours in
older groups have more experiences with adverse environmental events several ways. First, some environmental behaviours are costly, meaning
(e.g. diversity loss, sea level rise, disaster events or environmental low-income people can hardly participate in them. Second, many low-
pollution). As such, they realize the importance of environmental income people live in areas that are vulnerable to environmental
behaviour practices. However, some scholars have argued that younger threats, such as climatic phenomena. Thus, they could have experiences
groups are better informed or educated about environment-related is­ with adverse environmental consequences and be more aware of envi­
sues due to their capability to access digital communications or the ronmental problems, leading to the decision to act environmentally. For
internet. Consequently, younger people have more understanding of instance, Hallegatte and Rozenberg (2017) and the International Mon­
environmental issues such as climate change, and become more likely to etary Fund (2017) revealed that climate change phenomena mostly
act environmentally (Bedard and Tolmie, 2018; Furlow and Knott, negatively influence low-income people with limited resources. In turn,
2014). This current study assumes that age is associated with infectious Hardoy and Pandiella (2009) stated that low-income people might well

5
P. Janmaimool et al. Cleaner and Responsible Consumption 12 (2024) 100175

understand the effect of climate change, but not the causes or appro­ (extremely low) to 5 (completely agree) or (extremely high) were used to
priate counteractions. Therefore, they might not participate in explore the participants’ perceptions of potential environmental impacts
pro-environmental behaviours. These conflicting findings show that the caused by COVID-19 infectious waste. For the measurement of perceived
relationship between income level and environmental behaviours is still characteristics of environmental health risks, this study adapted ques­
ambiguous. This study therefore only generally assumes that income tions from Flynn et al. (1994) and Zhang (2013). These questions aimed
level affects individuals’ participation in sustainable infectious waste to explore how individuals perceive the seriousness of the impact of
management behaviours. environmental hazards on human health, as well as the possibility of
human society being influenced by environmental hazards. Five-point
3. Methods Likert scale questions ranging from 1 (completely disagree) or
(extremely low) to 5 (completely agree) or (extremely high) were also used
3.1. Study area for these questions. For socio-economic variables, the participants were
asked to indicate their actual age, monthly income and highest educa­
Bangkok, the capital city of Thailand, was selected as the study area tional degree.
for this research, because it is highly populated and sees a great amount The questionnaire developed for this study was validated using the
of infectious waste generation due to the massive use of PPE among its content validity technique, wherein experts in the field of environmental
residents. According to the Environment Department’s 2022 Annual studies evaluate the questionnaire for its ability to measure the study
Report, Bangkok Metropolitan Administration (BMA) (2022), during the variables. After revisions in accordance with the experts’ suggestions,
COVID-19 outbreak in 2021, though the total amount of solid waste the questionnaire was tested among 30 people who were not research
generation in Bangkok decreased, the amount of plastic and infectious participants to test its reliability. The results of this pre-survey revealed
waste increased. The Environmental Department, the BMA also reported the Cronbach’s alpha coefficient values for each questionnaire variable
that in 2021, approximately 74.17 tons of infectious waste were daily to be greater than 0.70 (Cronbach, 1951), and that for the entire ques­
generated in the BMA, whereas there were only 43.24 tons per day tionnaire to be 0.82.
generated in 2020. This drastic increase was attributed to the intensive
use of health care materials by both health care sectors and households. 3.3. Participants and data collection
To manage infectious waste, the BMA already provides infectious waste
incineration, though it only has the capacity to dispose of 50 tons per People who lived in Bangkok, Thailand and practised individual
day of infectious waste – less than the total amount of infectious waste COVID-19 protection measures were the research participants of this
generated in the BMA per day. Therefore, the BMA requires a more study. The exclusion criteria were people who worked in health care
effective infectious waste management system; otherwise, it could sectors, such as medical staff; who were infected with COVID-19 at the
potentially face critical environmental and health problems. time of the study; or who were responsible for caring for COVID-19-
infected people. According to recommendations from the WHO
3.2. Research tools (2020a, b), these groups of people are required to put on durable or
advanced face masks (e.g. N95 masks), so they can hardly practise
A questionnaire was used to collect the data in this study. The certain types of sustainable infectious waste management behaviours,
questionnaire items are presented in Table 1 according to each study such as using cotton or recycled face masks.
variable. The participants’ sustainable infectious waste management The sample size for this study was determined based on Cochran
behaviours were divided into three categories based on recommenda­ et al.’s (1953) calculation formula, because the total population was
tions from the WHO (2017). The first category is waste minimization, unknown. The Cochran formula allows researchers to calculate a sample
which refers to people’s efforts to reduce the amount of generated waste size based on a desired level of precision, desired confidence level, and
by adopting strategies such as reusing or recycling. Regarding the the estimated proportion of the attribute presented in the population
minimization of COVID-19 waste, the WHO (2021) suggested using (Biglari et al., 2019). In calculating the sample size for this research, the
cotton or recycled masks. Therefore, the participants in this study were confidence level was set at 95% (error at 5%), and the result revealed an
asked to indicate their frequency of participating in this type of behav­ appropriate sample size of 385 participants. However, to address errors
iour based on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (regularly) in some cases and enhance the reliability of the results, this research
(Liu and Wu, 2013). The second type of sustainable infectious waste distributed 550 questionnaire sheets to the sampling group via a random
management behaviour is infectious waste collection awareness. As sampling technique.
suggested by the WHO (2017), infectious waste must be carefully Bangkok metropolitan area contains 50 districts. This study selected
collected, and should not be released to the environment. To measure 5 districts with the largest population as of 2021 (Bangkok Adminis­
the participants’ participation in infectious waste collection awareness, trative Strategy Division, 2022). Those districts are such as Sai mai (206,
a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (regularly) was used, 831), khlong sam wa (206,437), Bang khae (192,431) Bang Khen (186,
with the participants asked to indicate whether they were aware of the 200) and Bang Khun Thian (184,944). A convenience sampling tech­
release of their used face masks into the environment. The last type of nique was used to recruit research participants from those 5 districts.
sustainable waste management behaviour is infectious waste segrega­ One hundred and ten residents in each target district were invited to
tion, which the WHO (2021) and the UK’s Department for Environment, participate in the data collection, and both face-to-face (F2F) and online
Food & Rural Affairs (2021) suggested for PPE, including face masks and questionnaire surveys were employed to collect data. The questionnaires
self-administrated rapid test kits, to avoid negative impacts on the were conducted from 15 October to November 9, 2022. In total, 521
environment and human health. Namely, recyclable waste should be samples were included in the analysis; the remaining 29 samples were
separated from hazardous waste that is contaminated with blood or not completed and therefore excluded. Most participants were female
other body fluids. For this study, the participants were asked to indicate (320), accounting for 61.4% of the sample, while the number of male
their participation in a number of infectious waste segregation activities participants was 181 (34.7%). The participants who did not want to
based on different 5-point Likert scales e.g. “1 (never) to 5 (always)"). identify their sex amounted to 3.8% (20 persons). Participants between
In measuring the variables related to individuals’ perceived char­ the ages of 20 and 30 years old had the highest proportion of involve­
acteristics of environmental risks, including perceived environmental ment (39.9%), followed by participants between 31 and 50 (18.2%). The
impact occurrence and perceived harmfulness of environmental im­ elderly group (older than 60 years old) amounted to 12.7%, and par­
pacts, this study adapted questionnaire items from Zhang (2013). ticipants between 51 and 60 came in at 15.2%. The minority consisted of
Five-point Likert scale questions ranging from 1 (completely disagree) or participants younger than 20 at only 4.0%. Most participants had a

6
P. Janmaimool et al. Cleaner and Responsible Consumption 12 (2024) 100175

bachelor’s degree (52.21%), whereas those with an education level Table 1


below a bachelor’s degree accounted for 12.48% of participants. Others Questionnaire items.
had educational degrees higher than a bachelor’s degree (25.34% for a Constructs Items Response
master’s degree, and 9.98% for a doctoral degree). Most participants had Categories
an income lower than 15,000 baht (440 USD; 37%). The proportion of Infectious Waste A1: Have you used cotton face masks 1 = Never
participants with an income between 15,000 and 35,000 baht was Minimization (WM) to prevent COVID-19 infection? 5 = Regularly
15.3%, and that of participants with an income between 35,001 and 60, A2: Have you used face masks
000 baht was 19.9%. Other participants had an income greater than 60, produced from recycled materials to
prevent COVID-19 infection?
000 baht (17.7%). Infectious Waste D1: Have you forgotten your medical 1 = Regularly
Collection Awareness face masks in public areas such as 5 = Never
3.4. Ethical considerations (WCA) restaurants, parks, markets, etc.?
D2: Have you lost your in-use face
masks during the day?
This study was approved by the ethical committee of King Mongkut’s
Infectious Waste S1: I place used medical face masks in 1 = Never
University of Technology Thonburi, Bangkok, Thailand on October 4, Segregation (WS) a plastic bag, and seal or tie the bag 5 = Regularly
2022 (approval number: KMUTT-IRB, 2022/0928/252). In collecting before throwing it away.
the data, the participants were first asked to give their consent to S2: Before throwing away used
participate, then informed that their participation was voluntary. antigen test kits, I separate elements
contaminated with biological
material (e.g. the swab, buffer tube
3.5. Data analysis and cassette) from other
uncontaminated materials, and place
All collected data were inspected before the analyses, which were them inside a sealed plastic bag.
S3: Before throwing away used
divided into three parts. First, analyses of descriptive statistics, such as
antigen test kits, I separate recyclable
percentage, mean and standard deviation, were conducted to reveal materials, and throw them away in a
participant characteristics and the values of the study variables. Second, place for recyclable waste.
confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed to test the scales’ S4: I throw away the infectious waste
construct and discriminant validity (Devellis, 2003). Based on the I generated into an infectious waste
bin or container.
measurement model results, validity fitting indexes such as χ2/df, CFA, Perceived Characteristics of Environmental Risks
TLI, RMSEA and RMR were calculated. The factor loading for each item Environmental Impact O1: An increase in the quantity of 1 = Completely
in each study variable was also calculated, with items that had factor Occurrence (EIO) infectious waste, including medical disagree
loading values lower than 0.60 removed (Hair et al., 2010). In addition, face masks and (Antigen-test Kits) 5 = Completely
ATKs, may cause several types of agree
Cronbach’s alpha (α) coefficients, average variance extracted (AVE) and
environmental pollution, such as air,
combined reliability (CR) were determined to test the measurement water or soil pollution.
reliability and validity of each study variable. Finally, the hypothetical O2: An increase in the quantity of
model was evaluated via structural equation modelling (SEM) analysis. infectious waste, including medical
SPSS 22.0 and Amos 23 were utilized for the data analyses. face masks and ATKs, may increase
the quantity of ocean waste.
O3: Animals could be adversely
4. Results affected by an increased quantity of
infectious waste, including medical
4.1. The measurement model face masks and ATKs.
Harmfulness of H1: How severely would an increase 1 = Extremely
Environmental in the quantity of infectious waste, low
First, the fitness of the measurement model to the data was evaluated Impacts (HEI) including medical face masks and 5 = Extremely
via CFA (Thompson, 2007). The results showed that the measurement ATKs, exacerbate existing high
model was acceptable (X2 = 96.434, df = 77, p = 0.066, X2/df = 1.252, environmental problems, such as
GFI = 0.980, TLI = 0.994, CFI = 0.997, AGFI = 0.955, RMSEA = 0.022). global warming or environmental
pollution?
Each item had factor loadings above 0.6 (Hair et al., 2010), with all H2: How severely would infectious
ranging from 0.601 to 0.962 (see Table 2), thereby showing convergent waste affect animal species?
validity. The CR values were between 0.741 and 0.922, which were H3: How severely would infectious
greater than the standard value of 0.7 (Fornell and Larcker, 1981), and waste impact ecosystems?
Perceived Characteristics of Environmental Health Risks
thus indicated good reliability of the latent variable measurement
Health Impact HO1: Improper disposal of infectious 1 = Completely
model. AVE was also calculated to reflect the average amount of vari­ Occurrence (HIO) waste, including medical face masks disagree
ance that a construct can explain in its indicators. A suitable convergent and ATKs, may cause the greater 5 = Completely
validity should have an AVE equivalent to or greater than 0.5 (Hair spreading of COVID-19. agree
et al., 2010). For this study, AVE values ranged from 0.502 to 0.855, HO2: Environmental pollutants
caused by the improper management
exceeding the generally accepted minimum of 0.5 (Fornell and Larcker, of COVID-19 infectious waste may
1981). Finally, Cronbach’s α coefficients were calculated to assess the negatively impact human health.
internal reliability of the measures. The results showed that Cronbach’s Harmfulness of Health HH1: How severely would 1 = Extremely
α coefficients ranged from 0.753 to 0.921, all above the threshold of 0.7 Impacts (HHI) environmental pollutants caused by low
the improper management of 5 = Extremely
(Fornell and Larcker, 1981).
infectious waste impact human high
In addition, correlation analyses were performed to verify discrimi­ health?
nant validity of the variables. According to Pearson’s correlation anal­ HH2: To what extent do you think
ysis, the correlations between the independent variables (EIO, HEI, HIC that human health could deteriorate
and HHI) and the dependent variables (MW, WCA and WS) were sta­ in the long run due to environmental
pollutants caused by the improper
tistically significant (p < 0.05; Fornell and Larcker, 1981), confirming management of infectious waste?
discriminant validity. In addition, there was no multicollinearity issue,
as the coefficient values were not greater than 0.60 (Gao et al., 2017; see
Table 3). Therefore, SEM could be performed.

7
P. Janmaimool et al. Cleaner and Responsible Consumption 12 (2024) 100175

Table 2
The measurement model.
Variable Items M SD Factor Loadings (>0.6) CR (>0.7) AVE (≥0.5) Cronbach’s Alpha (>0.7)

Infectious Waste Minimization (WM) A1 3.132 1.474 0.886 0.922 0.855 0.918
A2 2.841 1.602 0.962
Infectious Waste Collection Awareness (WCA) D1 4.518 0.899 0.934 0.920 0.851 0.921
D2 4.392 0.955 0.911
Infectious Waste Segregation (WS) S1 3.664 1.329 0.662 0.799 0.502 0.791
S2 4.088 1.201 0.724
S3 3.743 1.350 0.828
S4 3.330 1.405 0.601
Environmental Impact Occurrence (EIO) O1 4.386 0.919 0.768 0.821 0.604 0.798
O2 4.180 1.029 0.795
O3 4.205 1.007 0.768
Harmfulness of Environmental Impacts (HEI) H1 4.311 0.958 0.796 0.768 0.526 0.822
H2 4.081 0.985 0.705
H3 3.810 1.134 0.668
Health Impact Occurrence (HIO) HO 1 4.242 0.944 0.778 0.741 0.588 0.753
HO 2 3.877 1.014 0.756
Harmfulness of Health Impacts (HHI) HH 1 3.745 1.053 0.933 0.912 0.838 0.913
HH 2 3.735 1.095 0.898

4.2. Structural equation model assessment Finally, the TLI value of 0.996 also exceeded the acceptable minimum of
0.90. From the SEM, it can be concluded that the proposed model was
SEM analysis was performed to confirm whether the participants’ statistically acceptable (see Fig. 3).
perceived environmental risk and environmental health risk character­
istics, as well as socio-demographic characteristic variables (age, income
and education level), affected their participation in sustainable infec­ 4.3. Effects of environmental risk and environmental health risk
tious waste management behaviours (infectious waste minimization, perceptions on infectious waste management behaviours
collection awareness and segregation). First, the overall fit of the model
with the data was examined. The results revealed that the model was not According to the SEM analysis results, each type of infectious waste
fit with the data, and that perceived environmental impact occurrence management behaviour could be predicted by a different set of vari­
(EIO) and perceived health impact occurrence (HIO) did not signifi­ ables. For the infectious waste minimization behaviours, the squared
cantly affect all types of sustainable infectious waste management be­ multiple correlations value of 0.40 was derived. This indicated that
haviours. In addition, income did not significantly affect infectious approximately 40% of the variance in infectious waste minimization
waste collection awareness or segregation behaviours, and education behaviours could be accounted for by a linear combination of the vari­
did not significantly affect waste segregation behaviours. Therefore, to ables perceived harmfulness of health impacts, age, income and edu­
improve the model’s fit, paths without statistically significant affect (p cation level (see Fig. 2). Most notably, perceived harmfulness of health
> 0.05) were removed from the structural model. Consequently, the impacts (β = 0.347; t = 8.646; p < 0.001), age (β = 0.466; t = 9.928; p <
results showed that the model perfectly fit the data. 0.001) and education (β = 0.310; t = 6.945; p < 0.001) positively and
Several validity fitting indices were also statistically accepted. For significantly affected the participants’ infectious waste minimization
instance, the χ2 value was not statistically significant (χ2 = 78.936; df = behaviours, whereas income (β = − 0.140; t = − 2.810; p < 0.001)
67; p = 0.161), and χ2/df was 1.170, which was not greater than 5.0 negatively and significantly affected these behaviours. Considering in­
(Hair et al., 2010). The GFI value, which was greater than 0.90, indi­ fectious waste collection awareness, the results showed a squared mul­
cated a close fit between the observed data and the model (GFI = 0.982; tiple correlation value of 0.20, indicating that 20% of the variance in
Hair et al., 2010). The RMSEA value was less than 0.08, which indicated infectious waste collection awareness could be accounted for by the
a reasonable approximation of the data (RMSEA = 0.018; Brown and linear combination of the variables perceived harmfulness of environ­
Cudeck, 1993). To explain the discrepancy function adjusted for sample mental impacts, age and education level. From these results, perceived
size, the CFI value was calculated, and the result revealed an acceptable harmfulness of environmental impacts (β = 0.434; t = 8.607; p < 0.001),
value of 0.998, which was greater than 0.90 (Hu and Bentler, 1999). The age (β = 0.098; t = 2.224; p < 0.01) and education level (β = 0.089; t =
IFI value of 0.988 was also acceptable, as it was greater than 0.90, 2.060; p < 0.01) positively and significantly affected waste collection
indicating the proposed model’s acceptability (Meyers et al., 2006). awareness. Lastly, the results indicated a squared multiple correlation
value of 0.19 for the variance in infectious waste segregation

Table 3
The results of correlation analyses.
Constructs EIO HEI HIO HHI Education Income Age WM WCA WS

Environmental Impact Occurrence (EIO)


Harmfulness of Environmental Impacts (HEI) .546**
Health Impact Occurrence (HIO) .505** .538**
Harmfulness of Health Impacts (HHI) .518** .513** .580**
Education .087* .051 .062 .010
Income − .022 − .040 − .051 − .092* .492**
Age − .053 − .097* − .020 − .106* .326** .600**
Infectious Waste Minimization (WM) .258** .267** .309** .279** .353** .251** .429**
Infectious Waste Collection Awareness (WCA) .441** .378** .450** .333** .143** .031 .090* .164**
Infectious Waste Segregation (WS) .361** .358** .457** .349** .076 .063 .143** .234** .319**

** Statistic significance at 0.001 level.


* Statistic significance at 0.05 level.

8
P. Janmaimool et al. Cleaner and Responsible Consumption 12 (2024) 100175

Fig. 3. The study’s structural equation modelling (SEM; **p < 0.001).

behaviours. This indicated that 19% of the variance in waste segregation environmental health risk characteristics (threat occurrence and
behaviours could be accounted for by a linear combination of the vari­ severity), as perceived by participants, have the ability to predict sus­
ables perceived harmfulness of environmental impacts and age. Both tainable infectious waste management behaviours; even though many
perceived harmfulness of environmental impacts (β = 0.419; t = 7.657; previous studies have revealed that environmental risk and environ­
p < 0.001) and age (β = 0.180; t = 3.873; p < 0.001) positively and mental health risk perceptions can predict sustainable behaviours (Pahl
significantly affected these behaviours (see Table 4). et al., 2005; Schmitt et al., 2019; Zeng et al., 2020). Surprisingly, the
results did reveal that perceived occurrence of ecological threat to the
5. Discussion, recommendations and conclusion environment and to human health did not significantly affect all types of
sustainable infectious waste management behaviours. These findings
The objective of this stay was to examine how people’s perceived contradict Li et al. (2022), who found that the perceived possibility of
characteristics of environmental risks and environmental health risks, as ecological threat occurrence and threat severity affected the
associated with COVID-19 infectious waste problems, as well as the pro-environmental behaviours of college students in China. This implies
socio-economic characteristics (i.e. income, age and education) affect that people’s understanding of the possibility of threat occurrence to the
their participation in sustainable infectious waste management behav­ environment and human health does not sufficiently encourage them to
iours (i.e. infectious waste minimization, waste collection awareness, participate in all types of sustainable infectious waste management
and infectious waste segregation). CFA and SEM analyses were per­ behaviours.
formed to test the effect of independent variables on dependent This study also demonstrated that both perceived severity of
variables. ecological threat to the environment (harmfulness of environmental
This study demonstrated that not all environmental and impact) and to human health (harmfulness of health impacts) signifi­
cantly affect sustainable infectious waste management behaviours.
Specifically, perceived severity of ecological threat to human health
Table 4 predicted infectious waste minimization and segregation behaviours,
Path coefficient estimate of the revised model. whereas perceived severity of ecological threat to the environment
Paths Estimate S.E. C.R. Beta could predict only infectious waste collection awareness. These results
imply that people are more likely to practise sustainable behaviours if
Harmfulness of Health Impacts → 0.476 0.055 8.646 0.347**
Infectious Waste Minimization
they realize that environmental impacts could affect human health to a
Harmfulness of Environmental 0.362 0.042 8.607 0.434** certain degree. As stated by Lu et al. (2015), people may realize the
Impacts → Infectious Waste environmental impacts caused by their practices, but the decision to
Collection Awareness perform risk reduction measures (i.e. avoidance of practices that
Harmfulness of Health Impacts → 0.346 0.045 7.657 0.419**
potentially generate environmental impacts) is based on people’s
Infectious Waste Segregation
Income → Infectious Waste − 0.100 0.035 − 2.810 − 0.140** awareness of how much the environmental impacts would negatively
Minimization affect their lives. For this study, a higher level of perceived harmfulness
Age → Infectious Waste Collection 0.054 0.024 2.224 0.098* of health impacts caused by the environmental pollutants stemming
Awareness
from a massive amount of COVID-19 infectious waste might have
Age → Infectious Waste Minimization 0.414 0.042 9.928 0.466**
Age → Infectious Waste Segregation 0.096 0.025 3.873 0.180**
increased the participants’ concerns about their health; thus, they were
Education → Infectious Waste 0.511 0.074 6.945 0.310** more likely to take part in sustainable infectious waste management
Minimization behaviours. Lee et al. (2022) likewise found that people’s decision to
Education → Infectious Waste 0.09 0.044 2.060 0.089* purchase sustainable face masks was significantly influenced by their
Collection Awareness
health concerns. Similarly, Verma et al. (2019) reported that

9
P. Janmaimool et al. Cleaner and Responsible Consumption 12 (2024) 100175

individuals’ concerns about health and well-being motivated their climate change and disasters (Hallegatte and Rozenberg, 2017; Inter­
adoption of green purchasing behaviours. Therefore, this study strongly national Monetary Fund, 2017), meaning that they can construct more
highlights the importance of individuals’ perceived severity of ecolog­ environmental concerns and decide to participate in sustainable be­
ical threats to human health in promoting sustainable infectious waste haviours. However, this reason might not explain this study’s findings,
management behaviours. as income only affected one type of infectious waste management
Though many previous studies have concluded that environmental behaviour (waste minimization), and this behaviour could in fact help
behaviours are influenced by perceived environmental risks (Lacroix people save money in using daily PPE. For instance, using cotton or
and Gifford, 2018; Slovic, 2016), this study indicated that perceived recyclable masks could be less expensive than using single-use face
severity of ecological threat to the environment only affects individuals’ masks, so the low-incomes may have no choice than this for their eco­
engagement in infectious waste collection awareness, which, in this nomic reason.
study, was a measure that directly affects the environment. Notably, not Based on the results of this study, recommendations to promote
performing this measure (e.g. not disposing of infectious waste in a people’s participation in sustainable infectious waste management be­
rubbish bin but littering) can directly impact the environment. For haviours can be proposed. First, communication about the adverse im­
instance, the practice like losing wastes in public spaces can directly and pacts of environmental pollutants (as caused by infectious waste
immediately create a negative impact on the environment such as birds management problems) on human health and the environment can be
trapped in used face masks or contaminated water due to massive in­ very influential in promoting people’s participation in sustainable waste
fectious wastes in water resources. Thus, people with high level of management behaviours. This study’s results showed that perceptions of
perceived harmfulness of environment impact will try to avoid this the harmfulness of health and environmental impacts as caused by waste
problematic behaviour. management problems significantly influence these behaviours. Second,
For the other types of infectious waste management behaviours environmental education provided in educational institutes is always
(waste minimization and segregation), the study found that perceived important to garner concern about environmental issues, thus making
threat severity to the environment was not significant. In this way, in­ people more likely to respond to those issues. While at school or uni­
dividuals’ concerns about their health, which might be influenced by versity, people should be educated on these troubling topics, such as
environmental pollutants from infectious waste management problems, climate change, environmental health–related issues and environmental
could more heavily promote their practice of sustainable infectious pollutants. Moreover, younger generations can serve as a target group
waste management behaviours. However, the role of environmental risk when promoting sustainable behaviours, as this study found that
perception, particularly perceived severity of ecological threat to the younger age groups are less likely to perform infectious waste man­
environment, cannot be neglected, as it might affect some types of in­ agement behaviours.
fectious waste management behaviours that directly affect the
environment. 6. Limitation of the study
Furthermore, this study found that socio-economic factors signifi­
cantly affect several types of infectious waste management behaviours. Infectious waste management behaviors explored in this study could
Notably, participant age positively affected all three behaviours, with be explained by investigated variables 19–40%. There are other poten­
the older age group in particular seemingly more active in participating tial variables (such as self-efficacy, environmental knowledge, social
in sustainable infectious waste management behaviours. Some previous norms, etc.) which are not included in this study. Therefore, further
studies have found similar results (Otto and Kaiser, 2014; Wiernik et al., studies which could cooperate those variables are recommended. In
2013), explaining that older people have more experiences with adverse addition, the result of this study might not be able to generalize to the
environmental consequences; thus, they are more aware of environ­ whole population of Bangkok city because demographic characteristics
mental issues and more easily decide to partake in environmental be­ of research participants are different from ones of target population.
haviours. Another possible reason is that older groups might perceive a This difference can limit the ability to generalize the study results.
higher susceptibility to environmental impacts than younger groups, as
younger people generally have better physical health conditions. Older CRediT authorship contribution statement
people therefore feel more sensitive to environmental health–related
problems, so they are more likely to perform sustainable behaviours to Piyapong Janmaimool: W. Jaruwan Chontanawat: Supervision,
mitigate risk. This association is further supported by the study’s addi­ Methodology, Investigation, Data curation, Conceptualization. Sur­
tional finding that participants with high perceived harmfulness of apong Chudech: Visualization, Supervision, Software, Data curation,
health impacts caused by infectious waste management problems ten­ Conceptualization.
ded to be more active in performing sustainable waste management
behaviours. Declaration of competing interest
This study also revealed the significant and positive effect of edu­
cation level on waste minimization and collection awareness. This The authors declare that they have no known competing financial
finding is consistent with Kagawa (2007) and Emanuel and Adams interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence
(2011), who found that people with a higher education level tend to the work reported in this paper.
more actively participate in environmental behaviours. Spending longer
years in educational institutes, people could be more educated about Data availability
issues related to environmental responsibilities. Consequently, they
could have more environmental concerns that lead to performing sus­ Data will be made available on request.
tainable behaviours effectively (Gifford and Nilsson, 2014; Liere and
Dunlap, 1980). Environmental education in educational institutes is Acknowledgement
therefore important to promote sustainable behaviours.
Regarding income, this study found that income only affects waste This study was financially supported by the School of Liberal Arts,
minimization (e.g. using cotton or recyclable masks), and negatively at King Mongkut’s University of Technology Thonburi, Bangkok, Thailand.
that. This meant that participants with lower incomes tended to be more The scholarship number is 2566201. Authors would like also thank all
likely to participate in this type of behaviour. Many previous studies participants who supplied valuable data for this study.
have explained that low-income people are more likely to receive
negative impacts from environmental problems, such as pollution,

10
P. Janmaimool et al. Cleaner and Responsible Consumption 12 (2024) 100175

References Conference on Advance Computing and Innovative Technologies in Engineering,


287–292. IEEE. https://doi.org/10.1109/ICACITE51222.2021.9404765. (Accessed
25 August 2022).
Abbasi, S., Keshavarzi, B., Moore, F., Turner, A., Kelly, F.J., Dominguez, A.O.,
Hair, J., Black, W., Babin, B., Anderson, R., 2010. Multivariate Data Analysis: A Global
Jaafarzadeh, N., 2019. Distribution and potential health impacts of microplastics
Perspective. Pearson, London.
and microrubbers in air and street dusts from Asaluyeh County, Iran. Environ. Pollut.
Hallegatte, S., Rozenberg, J., 2017. Climate change through a poverty Lens. Nat. Clim.
244, 153–164.
Change 7, 250–256.
Asuzu, M., 1994. Definition and History in Occupational Health. Ibadan: Africa Link
Haque, M.S., et al., 2021a. Coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) induced waste
Book, 1994.
scenario: a short overview. J. Environ. Chem. Eng. 9 (1).
Australian Government Department of Health, 2014. Overview of Environmental Health.
Haque, M.S., Sharif, S., Masnoon, A., Rashid, E., 2021b. SARS-CoV-2 pandemic-induced
March 2023.
PPE and single-use plastic waste generation scenario. Waste Manag. Res. 39, 3–17.
Bangkok Administrative Strategy Division, 2022. In: Available at. https://webportal.
Hardoy, J., Pandiella, G., 2009. Urban poverty and vulnerability to climate change in
bangkok.go.th/pipd, 3 March, 2022).
Latin America. Environ. Urbanization 21, 203–224.
Beck, U., 1992. Risk Society, towards a New Modernity. Sage Publications, London.
Hartanto, B.W., Mayasari, D.S., 2021. Environmentally friendly non-medical mask: an
Bedard, S.A.N., Tolmie, C.R., 2018. Millennials’ green consumption behaviour: exploring
attempt to reduce the environmental impact from used masks during COVID 19
the role of social media. Corp. Soc. Responsib. Environ. Manag. 25, 1–9.
pandemic. Sci. Total Environ. 760, 144143.
Berger, I.E., 1997. The demographics of recycling and the structure of environmental
Hines, J.M., Hungerford, H.R., Tomera, A.N., 1986. Analysis and synthesis of research on
behavior. Environ. Behav. 29 (4), 515–531.
responsible environmental behavior: a meta-analysis. J. Environ. Educ. 18 (2), 1–8.
Biglari, T., Maleksaeidi, H., Eskandari, F., Jalali, M., 2019. Livestock insurance as a
Hu, L., Bentler, P.M., 1999. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis:
mechanism for household resilience of livestock herders to climate change: evidence
Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 6,
from Iran. Land Use Pol. 87, 104043, 2019), Article.
1–55.
Block, I., 2020. Petit Pli makes expandable and reusable face mask from recycled plastic
International Monetary Fund, 2017. World economic outlook: seeking sustainable
bottles. Dezeen. https://www.dezeen.com/2020/07/14/petit-pli-beta-msk-face-mas
growth: short- term recovery, long-term challenges. In: Chapter 3: the Effects of
k-coronavirus-recycled-sustainable/. (Accessed 25 August 2022).
Weather Shocks on Economic Activity: How Can Low-Income Countries Cope?.
Boss, S., Galletta, D., Lowry, P.B., Moody, G.D., Polak, P., 2015. What do systems users
Available at: https://www.imf.org/en/Publications/WEO/Issues/2017/09/19/wo
have to fear? Using fear appeals to engender threats and fear that motivate
rld-economic-outlook-october-2017.
protective security behaviors. MIS Q. 39 (4), 837–864.
Jeong, D., Yoon, A., Chon, J., 2021. The impact of the perceived risk and conservation
Bouchet, A., Boucher, J., Schutzbach, K., Senn, N., Genton, B., Vernez, D., 2021. Which
commitment of marine microplastics on tourists’ pro-environmental behavior. Sci.
strategy for using medical and community masks? A prospective analysis of their
Total Environ. 774, 144782.
environmental impact. BMJ Open 11 (9), e049690. Article.
Jung, S., Lee, S., Dou, X., Kwon, E.E., 2021. Valorization of disposable COVID-19 mask
Bouwmeester, H., Hollman, P.C.H., Peters, R.J.B., 2015. Potential health impact of
through the thermo-chemical process. Chem. Eng. J. 405. Article 126658.
environmentally released micro-and nanoplastics in the human food production
Kagawa, F., 2007. Dissonance in students’ perceptions of sustainable development and
chain: experiences from nanotoxicology. Environ. Sci. Technol. 49, 8932–8947.
sustainability: implications for curriculum change. Int. J. Sustain. High Educ. 8,
Brown, M.W., Cudeck, R., 1993. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In: Bollen, K.A.,
317–338.
Long, J.S. (Eds.), Testing Structural Equation Models. Sage, Newbury Park, CA, USA,
Keiron, P.R., Cressida, B., Simon, K., Steve, F., 2020. Face mask pollution could lead to an
pp. 136–162.
environmental disaster. Available at: https://www.mic.com/p/face-mask-pollution-
Boyle, L., 2020. Bird Dies after Getting Tangled in Coronavirus Face Mask Independent
could-lead-to-environmental-disaster-32135830. (Accessed 19 August 2021).
(New York).
Knowlton, K.U., 2019. Pathogenesis of SARS-CoV-2 induced cardiac injury from the
Bubeck, P., Botzen, W., Aerts, J., 2012. A review of risk perceptions and other factors that
perspective of the virus. J. Mol. Cell. Cardiol. 147, 12–17.
influence flood mitigation behavior. Risk Anal. 9, 1481–1495.
Lacroix, K., Gifford, R., 2018. Psychological barriers to energy conservation behavior: the
Buttel, F.H., 1979. Age and environmental concern: a multivariate analysis. Youth Soc.
role of worldviews and climate change risk perception. Environ. Behav. 50 (7),
10 (3), 237–256.
749–780.
Cai, J., Sun, W., Huang, J., Gamber, M., Wu, J., He, G., 2020. Indirect virus transmission
Leal Filho, W., Salvia, A.L., Minhas, A., Paço, A., Dias-Ferreira, C., 2021. The COVID-19
in cluster of COVID-19 cases, Wenzhou, China, 2020. Emerg. Infect. Dis. 26 (6),
pandemic and single-use plastic waste in households: a preliminary study. Sci. Total
1343.
Environ. 793, 148571.
Chang, X., Xue, Y., Li, J., Zou, L., Tang, M., 2020. Potential health impact of
Lee, S.E., Kim, S.J., Oh, K.W., et al., 2022. Purchase intention toward sustainable masks
environmental micro-and nanoplastics pollution. J. Appl. Toxicol. 40, 4–15.
after COVID-19: the moderating role of health concern. Fash Text 9, 43.
Cochran, W.G., Mosteller, F., Tukey, J.W., 1953. Statistical problems of the Kinsey
Leslie, H.A., van Velzen, M.J.M., Brandsma, S.H., Vethaak, A.D., Garcia-Vallejo, J.J.,
report. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 48 (264), 673–716.
Lamoree, M.H., 2022. Discovery and quantification of plastic particle pollution in
Cooper, C.M., Langman, J.B., Sarathchandra, D., Vella, C.A., Wardropper, C.B., 2020.
human blood. Environ. Int. 163, 107199.
Perceived risk and intentions to practice health protective behaviors in a mining-
Li, X., Liu, Z., Wuyun, T., 2022. Environmental value and pro-environmental behavior
impacted region. Int. J. Environ. Res. Publ. Health 17, 7916.
among young adults: the mediating role of risk perception and moral anger. Front.
Cronbach, L.J., 1951. Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests. Psychometrika
Psychol. 13, 771421.
16, 297–334.
Liere, K.D.V., Dunlap, R.E., 1980. The social bases of environmental concern: a Review of
Das, A.K., et al. (2021). COVID-19 pandemic and healthcare solid waste management
hypotheses, explanations and empirical evidence. Publ. Opin. Q. 44 (2), 181–197.
strategy - a mini-review. Sci. Total Environ., 778.
Lieske, D.J., Wade, T., Roness, L.A., 2014. Climate change awareness and strategies for
Devellis, R.F., 2003. Scale Development: Theory and Applications. Sage Publications,
communicating the risk of coastal flooding: a Canadian Maritime case example.
Thousand Oaks, CA, USA.
Estuar. Coast Shelf Sci. 140, 83–94.
Duan, H., Fortner, R., 2010. A cross-cultural study on environmental risk perception and
Lin, T., Cao, X., Huang, N., Xu, L., Li, X., Zhao, Y., Lin, J., 2021. Social cognition of
educational strategies: implications for environmental education in China. Int.
climate change in coastal community: a case study in Xiamen City, China. Ocean
Electron. J. Environ. Educ. 1 (1), 1–19.
Coast. Manag. 207, 104429.
Emanuel, R., Adams, J.N., 2011. College students’ perceptions of campus sustainability.
Lin, I.H., Petersen, D., 2008. Risk Communication in Action: the Tools Ff Message
Int. J. Sustain. High Educ. 12, 79–92.
Mapping. U.S. Environmental Protection Agency, Washington, DC. EPA/625/R-06/
Environment Department, 2022. Annual Report. Bangkok Metropolitan Administration,
012.
2022. https://webportal.bangkok.go.th/public/user_files_editor/231/Doc_Websi
Linden, S., 2015. The social-psychological determinants of climate change risk
te/AnnualReport/AnnualReport2022.pdf. (Accessed 19 May 2023).
perceptions: towards a comprehensive model. J. Environ. Psychol. 41, 112–124.
Estévez, R.A., Anderson, C.B., Pizarro, J.C., Burgman, M.A., 2015. Clarifying values, risk
Liu, A., 2020. Disposable masks raise environmental concerns. Available at: https://dail
perceptions, and attitudes to resolve or avoid social conflicts in invasive species
ytrojan.com/2020/10/08/disposable-masks-raise-environmental-concerns/.
management. Conserv. Biol. 29 (1), 19–30.
(Accessed 25 August 2022).
Fitzpatrick-Lewis, D., Yost, J., Ciliska, D., Krishnaratne, S., 2010. Communication about
Liu, X., Wu, J., 2013. Environmental value and pro-environmental behavior of college
environmental health risks: a systematic review. Environ. Health 9, 67.
students: mediating role of environmental concern. Stud. Psychol. Behav. 11,
Flynn, J., Slovic, P., Mertx, C.K., 1994. Gender, race, and perception of environmental
780–785.
health risks. Risk Anal. 14 (6), 1101–1108.
Lu, Y., Song, S., Wang, R., Liu, Z., Meng, J., Sweetman, A.J., Jenkins, A., Ferrier, R.C.,
Fornell, C., Larcker, D.F., 1981. Structural equation models with unobservable variables
Li, H., Luo, W., 2015. Impacts of soil and water pollution on food safety and health
and measurement error: algebra and statistics. J. Market. Res. 18, 382–388.
risks in China. Environ. Int. 77, 5–15.
Furlow, N.E., Knott, C., 2014. Who’s Reading the label? Millenials’ use of environmental
Maartensson, H., Loi, N.M., 2022. Exploring the relationships between risk perception,
product labels. JABE 10, 1–12.
behavioural willingness, and constructive hope in pro-environmental behaviour.
Gao, L., Wang, S., Li, H., 2017. Application of the extended theory of planned behavior to
Environ. Educ. Res. 28 (4), 600–613.
understand individual’s energy saving behavior in workplaces. Conserv. Recycl. 127,
Meyer, A., 2015. Does education increase pro-environmental behavior? Evidence from
107–113.
Europe. Ecol. Econ. 116, 108–121.
Gifford, R., Nilsson, A., 2014. Personal and social factors that influence pro-
Meyers, L.S., Gamst, G., Guarino, A.J., 2006. Applied Multivariate Research: Design and
environmental concern and behaviour: a review. Int. J. Psychol. 49 (3), 141–157.
Interpretation. Sage, London.
Goodman, K.E., Hare, J.T., Khamis, Z.I., Hua, T., Sang, Q.X.A., 2021. Exposure of human
Mohamed, B.A., Fattah, I.M.R., Yousaf, B., et al., 2022. Effects of the COVID-19 pandemic
lung cells to polystyrene microplastics significantly retards cell proliferation and
on the environment, waste management, and energy sectors: a deeper look into the
triggers morphological changes. Chem. Res. Toxicol. 34 (4), 1069–1081.
long-term impacts. Environ. Sci. Pollut. Res. 29, 46438–46457.
Gupta, R., Sinha, V., Damania, A., 2021. Sustainable masks revisiting the design and
features of eco-friendly cost effective masks. In: Gupta, R. (Ed.), 2021 International

11
P. Janmaimool et al. Cleaner and Responsible Consumption 12 (2024) 100175

Mosquera, M., Andres-Prado, M.J., Rodriguez-Caravaca, G., Latasa, P., Mosquera, M.E. Terpstra, T., Lindell, M.K., Gutteling, J.M., 2009. Does communicating (flood) risk affect
G., 2014. Evaluation of an education and training intervention to reduce health care (flood) risk perceptions? Results of a quasi-experimental study. Risk Anal. 8,
waste in a tertiary hospital in Spain. Am. J. Infect. Control 42, 894–897. 1141–1155.
Omar, D., Nazli, S.N., Karuppannan, S.A.L., 2012. Clinical waste management in district Thompson, B., 2007. Factor analysis. In: Ritzer, G. (Ed.), The Blackwell Encyclopedia of
hospitals of tumpat, batu pahat and taiping. In: Abbas, M.Y. (Ed.), Aice-Bs, vol. 68, Sociology. Wiley, Hoboken, NJ.
pp. 134–145, 2012 Cairo. US.EPA, 2020. Recycling and sustainable management of food during the coronavirus
Otto, S., Kaiser, F.G., 2014. Ecological behavior across the lifespan: why (COVID-19) public health emergency. Available at: https://www.epa.gov/corona
environmentalism increases as people grow older. J. Environ. Psychol. 40, 331–338. virus/recycling-and-sustainable-management-food-during-coronavirus-covid19-publ
Pahl, S., Harris, P.R., Todd, H.A., Rutter, D.R., 2005. Comparative optimism for ic-health. (Accessed 2 March 2022).
environmental risks. J. Environ. Psychol. 25, 1–11. UK’s Department for Environment, 2021. In: Food & Rural Affairs. Available at: https
Pandit, P., Maity, S., Singha, K., Annu Uzun, M., Shekh, M., Ahmed, S., 2021. Potential ://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/media/60103f71d3bf7f05bc42d294/waste-m
biodegradable face mask to counter environmental impact of Covid-19. Clean Eng. anagement-plan-for-england-2021.pdf, 2 March 2022).
Technol. 4, 100218. Verma, V.K., Chandra, B., Kumar, S., 2019. Values and ascribed responsibility to predict
Rathinamoorthy, R., Raja Balasaraswathi, S., 2022. Mitigation of microfibers release consumers’ attitude and concern towards green hotel visit intention. J. Bus. Res. 96,
from disposable masks - an analysis of structural properties. Environ. Res. 214, 206–216.
114106. Wachinger, G., Renn, O., Begg, C., Kuhlicke, C., 2013. The risk perception paradox –
Renn, O., 2017. Risk Governance: Coping with Uncertainty in a Complex World implications for governance and communication of natural hazards Risk. Analysis
Routledge. 33, 1049–1065.
Roberts, K.P., Bowyer, C., Kolstoe, S., Fletcher, S., 2020. Coronavirus face masks: an Wiernik, B.S., Ones, D., Dilchert, S., 2013. Age and environmental sustainability: a meta-
environmental disaster that might last generations. The Conversation. https://theco analysis. J. Manag. Psychol. 28 (7/8), 826–856.
nversation.com/coronavirus-face-masks-an-environmental-disaster-that-might-last- Windfeld, E.S., Brooks, M.S.L., 2015. Medical waste management - a review. J. Environ.
generations-144328. (Accessed 25 August 2022). Manag. 163, 98–108.
Rogers, R.W., 1975. A protection motivation theory of fear appeals and attitude change World Health Organisation, 2018. Health-care waste. Available at: https://www.who.
1. J. Psychol. 91 (1), 93–114. int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/health-care-waste.
Rosenstock, I.M., 1974. Historical origins of the health belief model. Health Educ. World Health Organisation, 2016. Environmental Health [Online]. (Accessed 2 May
Monogr. 2 (4), 328–335. 2022).
Rume, T., Islam, S.M.D., 2022. Environmental effects of COVID-19 pandemic and World Health Organisation, 2020a. Rational Use of Personal Protective Equipment for
potential strategies of sustainability. Heliyon 6 (9). Coronavirus Disease (COVID-19) and Considerations during Severe Shortages.
Saliu, F., Veronelli, M., Raguso, C., Barana, D., Galli, P., Lasagni, M., 2021. The release https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/rational-use-of-personal-protective-equi
process of microfibers: from surgical face masks into the marine environment. pment-for-coronavirus-disease-(covid-19)-and-considerations-during-severe-shorta
Environ. Adv. 4, 100042. ges. (Accessed 11 February 2023).
Schmitt, M.T., Mackay, C., Droogendyk, L.M., Payne, D., 2019. What predicts World Health Organisation, 2020b. Global Analysis of Health Care Waste in the Context
environmental activism? the roles of identification with nature and politicized of COVID-19 available at: https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/9789240039
environmental identity. J. Environ. Psychol. 61, 20–29. 612. (Accessed 11 February 2023).
Shen, M., Zeng, Z., Song, B., Yi, H., Hu, T., Zhang, Y., Zeng, G., Xiao, R., 2021. Neglected World Health Organisation, 2021. Antigen-detection in the diagnosis of SARS-CoV-2
Microplastics Pollution in Global COVID-19: Disposable Surgical Masks. Sci Total infection. Available at: https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/antigen-detecti
Environ., p. 790 on-in-the-diagnosis-of-sars-cov-2infection-using-rapid-immunoassays. (Accessed 11
Shutterstock, 2021. Used face mask floating sea mediterranean. Available at: https February 2023).
://www.shutterstock.com/image-photo/used-face-mask-floating-sea-mediterrane World Health Organization, 2017. Safe Management of Wastes from Health-Care
an-1778173526. (Accessed 30 October 2022). Activities. https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/259491/WHO-FW
Singh, D., Aryan, Y., Chavan, D., Tembhare, M., Dikshit, A.K., Kumar, S., 2022. Mask C-WSH-17.05-eng.pdf. (Accessed 19 January 2023).
consumption and biomedical waste generation rate during Covid-19 pandemic: a World Health Organization, 2022. Global Analysis of Health Care Waste Inthe Context of
case study of central India. Environ. Res. 212, 113363. COVID-19: Status, Impacts and Recommendations. World Health Organization,
Sjöberg, L., Moen, B., Rundmo, T., 2004. Explaining Risk Perception: an Evaluation of Geneva. https://www.who.int/publications/i/item/9789240039612. (Accessed 19
the Psychometric Paradigm in Risk Perception Study. Norwegian University of May 2023).
Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway. Yee, M.S.L., Hii, L.W., Looi, C.K., Lim, W.M., Wong, S.F., Kok, Y.Y., Tan, B.K., Wong, C.
Slimak, W.M., Dietz, T., 2006. Personal values, beliefs, and ecological risk perception. Y., Leong, C.O., 2021. Impact of microplastics and nanoplastics on human health.
Risk Anal. 26 (6), 1689–1705. Nanomaterials 11 (2), 496.
Slovic, P., 1987. Perception of risk. Science 236, 280–285. You, Z., Zhan, W., Zhang, F., 2023. Online information acquisition affects food risk
Slovic, P., 2000. The Perception of Risk. Earthscan Publications, London, UK; Sterling, prevention behaviours: the roles of topic concern, information credibility and risk
VA, USA. perception. BMC Publ. Health 23, 1899.
Slovic, P., 2016. Understanding perceived risk: 1978-2015 Environment. Sci. Pol. Yu, Q.Y., Xie, X.F., 2006. Characteristics of environmental risk perception. Psychol. Sci.
Sustain. Dev. 58, 25–29. 29, 362–365.
Smith, E.K., Mayer, A., 2018. A social trap for the climate? Collective action, trust and Zeng, J., Jiang, M., Yuan, M., 2020. Environmental risk perception, risk culture, and pro-
climate change risk perception in 35 countries. Global Environ. Change 49, 140–153. environmental behavior. Int. J. Environ. Res. Publ. Health 17, 1750.
Smith, M.A., Kingston, S., 2021. Demographic, attitudinal, and social factors that predict Zhang, W., 2013. The effect of Chinese environmental value on multi-featured
pro-environmental behavior. Sustain. Clim. Change 14 (1), 47–54. environmental decision making. In: Doctoral Dissertation. Zhejiang University,
Steg, L., Vlek, C., 2009. Encouraging pro-environmental behavior: an integrative review Hangzhou.
and research agenda. J. Environ. Psychol. 29, 309–317, 2009.

12

You might also like