You are on page 1of 4

Brain Research Bulletin 195 (2023) 141–144

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Brain Research Bulletin


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/brainresbull

Interaction between reconsolidation and extinction of fear memory


Satoshi Kida
Department of Applied Biological Chemistry, Graduate School of Agriculture and Life Sciences, The University of Tokyo, Tokyo 113-8657, Japan

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Memory retrieval is not a passive process. When a memory is retrieved, it returns to a labile state and undergoes
Reconsolidation reconsolidation to be re-stored. The discovery of this memory reconsolidation has had a major impact on memory
Extinction consolidation theory. In other words, it suggested that memory is more dynamic than expected and can be
Fear memory
modified through reconsolidation. Conversely, a conditioned fear memory undergoes memory extinction after
Consolidation
Retrieval
retrieval, and it is thought that extinction does not reflect its erasure, but rather new inhibitory learning of the
original conditioned memory. We have investigated the relationship between memory reconsolidation and
extinction by comparing their behavioral, cellular, and molecular mechanisms. Memory reconsolidation and
extinction have opposite functions on contextual fear and inhibitory avoidance memories; reconsolidation
maintains or strengthens fear memory, whereas extinction weakens it. Importantly, reconsolidation and
extinction are contrasting memory processes not only at the behavioral level but also at cellular and molecular
levels. Furthermore, our analysis revealed that reconsolidation and extinction are not independent processes, but
interact with each other. Interestingly, we also found a "memory transition process" that switches the fear
memory process from reconsolidation to extinction after retrieval. Investigating the mechanisms of reconsoli­
dation and extinction will contribute to our understanding of the dynamic nature of memory.

Short-term memory is labile. A labile memory is stabilized through factor Zif268 (Lee et al., 2004), and specific brain regions (Taubenfeld
“memory consolidation” to generate a stable long-term memory (Silva et al., 2001), indicating that the mechanisms of consolidation and
et al., 1998; Kida and Serita, 2014). However, Nader, Ledoux, and col­ reconsolidation are similar, but not identical.
leagues have shown that even a consolidated memory, following Importantly, a memory is not always destabilized following retrieval,
memory retrieval, returns to the labile state (destabilization) and is and there are boundary conditions to induce the destabilization of a
restabilized through “memory reconsolidation” for re-storage (Nader retrieved memory, e.g., the duration of memory retrieval, the strength of
et al., 2000a, 2000b; Kida, 2029, 2020). The discovery of memory the memory, and the age of the memory (Kida, 2020; Suzuki et al.,
reconsolidation indicated that memories are more flexible than expected 2004). For example, a memory is less destabilized if the retrieval
and had a big impact on our understanding of memory consolidation. duration is short, or if the memory is strong, or old (Suzuki et al., 2004).
The functional roles of memory reconsolidation have been suggested to
1. Mechanisms and function of memory reconsolidation update, strengthen, and/or modify a retrieved memory (Kida, 2019,
2020). In the contextual fear conditioning task, rodents receive elec­
Behavioral evidence showing that a retrieved memory is disrupted trical footshocks in a conditioning chamber to form a contextual fear
by the inhibition of protein synthesis suggests that it becomes labile memory and retrieve this memory when they are re-exposed to the
(destabilized) and restabilized through memory reconsolidation (Nader chamber. Importantly, this re-exposure to the chamber initiates either
et al., 2000a, 2000b; Kida, 2019, 2020). This observation also indicates memory reconsolidation or extinction; a short or long duration of the
that the molecular mechanism of memory reconsolidation is similar to re-exposure induces memory reconsolidation or extinction, respectively
that of consolidation. Importantly, a common molecular signature of (Mamiya et al., 2009; Suzuki et al., 2004) (Fig. 1). Therefore, in the
memory consolidation and reconsolidation is the essential requirement contextual fear conditioning test, it is difficult to investigate the func­
for gene expression activated by the transcription factor CREB (Kida, tional roles of reconsolidation under conditions in which the interaction
2020; Kida et al., 2002; Tronson and Taylor, 2007). However, memory between reconsolidation and extinction does not occur and only
reconsolidation involves specific molecules, such as the transcription reconsolidation is induced.

E-mail address: akida@g.ecc.u-tokyo.ac.jp.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2023.02.009
Received 27 September 2022; Received in revised form 11 February 2023; Accepted 15 February 2023
Available online 16 February 2023
0361-9230/© 2023 The Author. Published by Elsevier Inc. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-
nc-nd/4.0/).
S. Kida Brain Research Bulletin 195 (2023) 141–144

strengthen them.

2. Function and mechanisms of fear memory extinction

A conditioned fear memory is retrieved by re-exposure to the


conditioned stimulus (CS) such as a conditioned context and tone. Fear
memory retrieval induces fear responses in rodents such as freezing.
However, fear responses are attenuated under conditions in which the
unconditioned stimulus (US), e.g., an electrical shock, is not given for a
long period during re-exposure to the CS. This behavioral change is
termed "memory extinction" (Myers and Davis, 2002) (Figs. 1 and 2).
Importantly, memory extinction is thought not to reflect erasure or
forgetting of the original conditioned fear memory since behavioral
evidence showed that fear responses to the CS can be recovered after a
Fig. 1. Reconsolidation and extinction of contextual fear memory. Retrieval of long period (spontaneous recovery) or even when a weak CS is given,
a contextual fear memory by the short (e.g., 3 min) or long (e.g., 30 min) re-
even after extinction learning (Myers and Davis, 2002). Memory
exposure to the conditioning chamber induces memory reconsolidation or
extinction is thought to represent new inhibitory learning that generates
extinction, respectively. Activation of gene expression is observed in the
amygdala/hippocampus or the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC)/amygdala inhibitory circuits suppressing fear responses. Similar to memory
when the retrieved memory is reconsolidated or extinguished, respectively. consolidation and reconsolidation, long-term extinction memory is sta­
bilized through gene expression activated by CREB (Fukushima et al.,
We have focused on the inhibitory avoidance (IA) task to understand 2014, 2021; Mamiya et al., 2009; Suzuki et al., 2004).
the function of reconsolidation (Fukushima et al., 2014, 2021) (Fig. 2).
In this task, mice are first placed in a light compartment and then given 3. Interaction between reconsolidation and extinction
an electrical footshock immediately after entering a dark compartment
from the light compartment. This experience generates an IA memory to Reconsolidation and extinction play opposite roles in states of fear
avoid the dark compartment, even though mice prefer dark places. memory; memory reconsolidation maintains or strengthens fear mem­
When the mouse is re-exposed to the light compartment, the IA memory ory, whereas memory extinction weakens it. The reconsolidation and
is retrieved, but it is not extinguished unless the mouse learns that the extinction processes of contextual fear memory have been compared at
electrical footshock is not given once it re-enters the dark compartment; the behavioral and molecular levels. A short duration of contextual fear
therefore, re-exposure to the light compartment induces only IA memory memory retrieval, e.g., 3 min re-exposure to the conditioning chamber,
reconsolidation without extinguishing this memory. Interestingly, the induces reconsolidation, whereas a long duration, e.g., 30 min re-
time spent in the light compartment (test session) after re-exposure to exposure, induces extinction (Mamiya et al., 2009; Suzuki et al., 2004)
the light compartment only (re-exposure session) is significantly (Fig. 1). Expectedly, inhibition of gene expression after short
increased compared to that duration during re-exposure despite the re-exposure to the conditioning chamber disrupts the retrieved memory
absence of an electrical footshock, indicating that the IA memory is by blocking memory reconsolidation. In contrast, inhibition of gene
enhanced after memory retrieval (Fukushima et al., 2014). In contrast, expression after long re-exposure blocks long-term extinction without
blocking gene expression during re-exposure to the light compartment affecting the original contextual fear memory. These observations are
by protein synthesis inhibition or genetic inhibition of CREB-mediated not surprising since reconsolidation and extinction both depend on new
transcription disrupts the retrieved IA memory (Fukushima et a, gene expression. However, the observation that inhibition of gene
2014). These observations suggest that IA memory is enhanced through expression blocks long-term extinction indicates that the retrieved
memory reconsolidation following memory retrieval. Therefore, our contextual fear memory is not disrupted, even though gene expression is
findings suggest that one of the functions of memory reconsolidation is inhibited when the memory is retrieved by prolonged re-exposure.
to reinforce memories and that destabilization opens a window to Therefore, these behavioral observations suggest that the reconsolida­
tion and extinction of contextual fear memory are not independent
processes and interact with each other (Kida, 2019, 2020; Suzuki et al.,
2004). It is important to note that this interaction may not be observed
in other types of memory such as amygdala-dependent cued fear
memory (Duvarci et al., 2006).
Phosphorylation of CREB is a crucial step for the activation of CREB-
mediated transcription (Kida and Serita, 2014). Increases in CREB
phosphorylation are observed in the hippocampus/amygdala and
amygdala/medial prefrontal cortex when contextual fear memory is
reconsolidated by a short reminder or extinguished by a long one,
respectively (Mamiya et al., 2009) (Fig. 1). Interestingly, these obser­
vations indicate that CREB phosphorylation in the hippocampus to
Fig. 2. Reconsolidation, transition, and extinction of IA memory. Retrieval of induce reconsolidation with a short reminder is no longer observed
an IA memory by the re-exposure to the light compartment (0 min re-exposure because extinction learning (within-session extinction) progresses with
to the dark compartment) induces memory reconsolidation that strengthens the a long reminder (Mamiya et al., 2009). In other words, the hippocampal
retrieved memory, whereas retrieval of this memory by the re-exposure to the CREB phosphorylation that is observed following short re-exposure is
dark compartment (e.g., 3–10 min) after the entry from the light compartment
abrogated by extinction learning. Furthermore, reconsolidation and
induces memory extinction. Brief re-exposure to the dark compartment (e.g.,
extinction of contextual fear memory differ in the time course of the
1 min) cancels the induction of reconsolidation but is insufficient for memory
extinction. Activation of gene expression is observed in the medial prefrontal increase in CREB phosphorylation in the amygdala; although increased
cortex (mPFC)/amygdala/hippocampus or mPFC/amygdala when the retrieved CREB phosphorylation is observed at 30 min after a short re-exposure
memory is reconsolidated or extinguished, respectively. This activation of gene inducing reconsolidation, this time point is the same as that for the
expression when memory is reconsolidated is not induced by brief re-exposure end of the extended re-exposure that induces extinction, but no increase
to the dark compartment. in CREB phosphorylation is observed (Mamiya et al., 2009). These

142
S. Kida Brain Research Bulletin 195 (2023) 141–144

differences in the molecular signatures of reconsolidation and extinction 4.1. Interaction of fear and extinction neurons
suggest an interaction between them at the molecular level.
The above findings suggest that the transition of memory phases
4. Mechanisms of fear memory transition from reconsolidation from reconsolidation to extinction is regulated by a specific process. In
to extinction contrast, there is evidence that competition between “fear” neurons and
“extinction” neurons determines the states of fear in the amygdala. Fear
Since the reconsolidation and extinction of contextual fear memory neurons in the basolateral amygdala (BLA) are activated during the fear
are induced by re-exposure to the same conditioning chamber, the time response to a CS following cued fear conditioning, whereas extinction
point at which memory processes are shifted from reconsolidation to neurons in this brain region are activated in response to the same cue
extinction is unclear (Fig. 1). In contrast, the IA task allows discrimi­ after extinction learning (Herry et al., 2008). Interestingly, fear and
nation of the reconsolidation and extinction processes at the time point extinction neurons in the medial intercalated neurons clusters between
at which mice enter the dark compartment from the light compartment, the BLA and central amygdala have also been identified that are located
as described above (Fig. 2). In this task, reconsolidation is induced when upstream of the fear and extinction neurons in the BLA and determine
the mice stay only in the light compartment, whereas extinction is the states of the fear response through mutual inhibition (Hagiwara
induced when the mice stay in the dark compartment for 3–10 min after et al., 2021). These observations suggest that the fear response is regu­
entry from the light compartment without receiving an electrical shock lated by the competition between the two neural circuits for fear and
(Fig. 2) (Fukushima et al., 2014, 2021). Importantly, similar to the ob­ extinction. This supports the hypothesis that a new inhibitory circuit is
servations in contextual fear conditioning, inhibition of gene expression formed by extinction learning and suppresses the fear responses.
blocks only long-term extinction without disrupting the retrieved IA Interestingly, cell ensembles responding to the CS in the basolateral
memory when it is extinguished, even though the mouse is re-exposed to regions of the amygdala are transformed after fear conditioning as if
the light compartment before entering the dark compartment and the IA they were responding to a US and do not return to their original en­
memory is retrieved (Fukushima et al., 2014, 2021). This observation sembles even after extinction learning (Grewe et al., 2017). This in­
confirms the interaction between reconsolidation and extinction at the dicates that cell ensembles are qualitatively different before and after
behavioral level in the IA task. extinction learning, suggesting that they are also shifted from the state of
More interestingly, neither reconsolidation nor extinction is reconsolidation to extinction. This may reflect the competition in which
observed when mice are re-exposed to the dark compartment for only new inhibitory circuits suppress the activity of the fear circuits inducing
1 min after entry from the light compartment; extinguishing or weak­ the fear responses.
ening of the IA memory is not observed, whereas the retrieved IA In summary, the reconsolidation and extinction of contextual fear
memory is not disrupted by protein synthesis inhibition (Fukushima and IA memories are not independent and interact with each other since
et al., 2021) (Fig. 2). Most importantly, although re-exposure only to the the induction of reconsolidation can be canceled at the behavioral,
light compartment induces memory reconsolidation and activates gene cellular, and molecular levels once fear memory is extinguished.
expression, as measured by c-fos induction, and proteasome-dependent Importantly, this transition of memory phases from reconsolidation to
protein degradation in the hippocampus, amygdala, and medial pre­ extinction of fear memory is actively controlled in an ERK-dependent
frontal cortex, this activation of gene expression and protein degrada­ manner. It is important to understand further the molecular and
tion is not observed after re-exposure to the dark compartment for 1 min cellular mechanisms underlying this memory transition process. Since
following re-exposure to the light compartment (Fukushima et al., the amygdala shows distinct populations of fear and extinction neurons
2021). In contrast, extended exposure to the dark compartment that is and regulation of fear memory states through competition between
sufficient for extinction learning, i.e., 3–10 min, re-activates those these neurons, it is also important to identify reconsolidation and
changes in gene expression (Fukushima et al., 2021). These observations extinction memory engram neurons in the amygdala and other brain
indicate that this brief re-exposure to the dark compartment for 1 min regions and understand the mechanisms underlying the transition of
cancels the changes in gene expression that are observed after memory phases through their interactions.
re-exposure to the light compartment (Fig. 2). These findings suggest
that a "transition" process of the retrieved IA memory from reconsoli­ Declaration of Competing Interest
dation to extinction inhibits the induction of destabilization/reconsoli­
dation and puts the retrieved memory in a state that enables the The authors declare no conflict of interest.
initiation of memory extinction. It is important to note that a similar
transition of retrieved memory was suggested by measuring extracel­ Data Availability
lular signal-regulated kinase (ERK) phosphorylation following memory
retrieval in contextual fear conditioning (Merlo et al., 2014, 2018). No data was used for the research described in the article.
Phosphorylation of ERK increases in the hippocampus, amygdala,
and medial prefrontal cortex during memory transition from reconsoli­ Acknowledgment
dation to extinction by brief, i.e., 1 min, re-exposure to the dark
compartment (Fukushima et al., 2021). This observation suggests that S.K. was supported by Grant-in-Aids for Scientific Research (A)
this transition process shows specific molecular signatures, although (15H02488, 18H03944, 19H01047, 22H00358), Scientific Research (B)
there is no activation of gene expression and protein degradation. More (23300120 and 20380078), and Challenging Exploratory Research
interestingly, pharmacological inhibition of ERK activation in any of (24650172, 26640014, 17K19464, 20K21265, 22K19142); Grant-in-
those brain regions disinhibits the cancellation of reconsolidation; the Aids for Scientific Research on Priority Areas—Molecular Brain Sci­
retrieved IA memory is enhanced even though the animals are ence (18022038 and 22022039); Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research on
re-exposed to the dark compartment for 1 min (Fukushima et al., 2021). Innovative Areas (Research in a proposed research area) (24116008,
This observation suggests that the memory transition process actively 24116001, 23115716, 17H06084, 17H05961, 17H05581, 18H05428,
inhibits the induction of memory reconsolidation. In this transition 18H05434, 19H04917, 22H05486); JST [Moonshot R&D]
process, ERK may act as a molecular switch to abrogate reconsolidation [JPMJMS2298]; MEXT-Supported Program for the Strategic Research
and enables the initiation of extinction learning. Foundation at Private Universities (S1311017); Core Research for Evo­
lutional Science and Technology (CREST), Japan; The Sumitomo
Foundation, Japan; the Takeda Science Foundation, Japan; The Naito
Foundation, The Uehara Memorial Foundation, Daiichi Sankyo

143
S. Kida Brain Research Bulletin 195 (2023) 141–144

Foundation of Life Science, The Food Science Institute Foundation Kida, S., 2019. Reconsolidation/Destabilization, extinction and forgetting of fear
memory as therapeutic targets for PTSD. Psychopharmacology 236 (1), 49–57.
(Ryoushoku-kenkyukai), Danone Research Grant from Danone Institute
Kida, S., 2020. Function and mechanisms of memory destabilization and reconsolidation
of Japan Foundation, and The Science Research Promotion Fund; and after retrieval. Proc. Jpn. Acad. Ser. B Phys. Biol. Sci. 96, 95–106.
The Promotion and Mutual Aid Corporation for Private Schools of Kida, S., Serita, T., 2014. Functional roles of CREB as a positive regulator in the
Japan. formation and enhancement of memory. Brain Res. Bull. 105, 17–24.
Kida, S., Josselyn, S.A., deOrtiz, S.P., Kogan, J.H., Chevere, I., Masushige, S., Silva, A.J.,
2002. Nat. Neurosci. 5, 348–355.
References Lee, J.L., Everitt, B.J., Thomas, K.L., 2004. Science 304, 839–843.
Mamiya, N., Fukushima, H., Suzuki, A., Matsuyama, Z., Homma, S., Frankland, P.W.,
Duvarci, J., Mamou, C.B., Nader, K., 2006. Extinction is not a sufficient condition to Kida, S., 2009. J. Neurosci. 29, 402–413.
prevent fear memories from undergoing reconsolidation in the basolateral amygdala. Merlo, E., Milton, A.L., Goozée, Z.Y., Theobald, D.E., Everitt, B.J., 2014. Reconsolidation
Eur. J. Neurosci. 24, 249–260. and extinction are dissociable and mutually exclusive processes: behavioral and
Fukushima, H., Zhang, Y., Archbol, G., Ishikawa, R., Nader, K., Kida, S., 2014. molecular evidence. J. Neurosci. 34, 2422–2431.
Enhancement of fear memory by retrieval through reconsolidation. eLife 3, e02736. Merlo, E., Milton, A.L., Everitt, B.J., 2018. A novel retrieval-dependent memory process
Fukushima, H., Zhang, Y., Kida, S., 2021. Active transition of fear memory phase from revealed by the arrest of ERK1/2 activation in the basolateral amygdala. J. Neurosci.
reconsolidation to extinction through ERK-mediated prevention of reconsolidation. 38, 3199–3207.
J. Neurosci. 41, 1288–1300. Myers, K.M., Davis, M., 2002. Neuron 36, 567–584.
Grewe, B.F., Gründemann, J., Kitch, L.J., Lecoq, J.A., Parker, J.G., Marshall, J.D., Nader, K., Schafe, G.E., Le Doux, J.E., 2000a. Nature 406, 722–726.
Larkin, M.C., Jercog, P.E., Grenier, F., Li, J.Z., Lüthi, A., Schnitzer, M.J., 2017. Nader, K., Schafe, G.E., Le Doux, J.E., 2000b. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 1, 216–219.
Neural ensemble dynamics underlying a long-term associative memory. Nature 543 Silva, A.J., Kogan, J.H., Frankland, P.W., Kida, S., 1998. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 21,
(7647), 670–675. Mar 30. 127–148.
Hagiwara, K.M., Bukalo, O., Zeller, M., Aksoy-Aksel, A., Karalis, N., Limoges, A., Rigg, T., Suzuki, A., Josselyn, S., Frankland, P., Masushige, S., Silva, A., Kida, S., 2004.
Campbell, T., Mendez, A., Weinholtz, C., Mahn, M., Zweifel, L.S., Palmiter, R.D., J. Neurosci. 24, 4787–4795.
Ehrlich, I., Lüthi, A., Holmes, A., 2021. Intercalated amygdala clusters orchestrate a Taubenfeld, S.M., Milekic, M.H., Monti, B., Alberini, C.M., 2001. Nat. Neurosci. 4,
switch in fear state. Nature 594 (7863), 403–407 (Jun). 813–818.
Herry, C., Ciocchi, S., Senn, V., Demmou, L., Muller, C., Luthi, A., 2008. Nature 454, Tronson, N.C., Taylor, J.R., 2007. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 8, 262–275.
600–606.

144

You might also like