You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology Copyright # 2007 by

2007, Vol. 36, No. 4, 503–514 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Perceived Romantic Competence, Importance of Romantic


Domains, and Psychosocial Adjustment
Heather A. Bouchey
University of Vermont

This study investigated romantic competence during late adolescence, including


preliminary psychometric properties of a new measure and its concurrent
association with psychosocial adjustment. Six aspects of perceived romantic
competence (romantic appeal, sexual competence, communication with part-
ners, maintenance of relationships, establishing equivalent power balance in
relationships, and feeling accepted by romantic partners) were hypothesized
to predict unique variance in psychosocial adjustment. Self-report data were
collected from 586 college students, a primarily female and European American
sample. Results revealed that four domains predicted variance in psychosocial
functioning. In addition, participants’ valuing of both maintaining romantic
relationships and positive partner characteristics moderated the effects of
perceived competence on self-worth.

In recent years, scholars have become increasingly preliminary test of this premise, a novel measure
interested in studying the nature and function of romantic self-concept, one composed of both
of romantic relationships during adolescence. individuals’ perceived competence in different
The typical features and developmental course of aspects of romantic relationships and their per-
adolescent romantic experiences have been deli- ceived importance or valuing of each of these
neated (Bouchey & Furman, 2003; Collins, 2003), aspects, was investigated with college students.
and the extent to which parental, peer, and roman- Younger college students (i.e., 18- to 20-year-olds)
tic relationships are linked continues to receive are typically considered ‘‘late adolescents’’ by
attention (Connolly, Furman, & Konarski, 2000; developmental scholars (Rice & Dolgin, 2005;
Furman, Simon, Shaffer, & Bouchey, 2002). How- Steinberg, 2005).
ever, the processes by which adolescents them- In this article, preliminary data in support of the
selves make sense of their romantic experiences hypothesized structure of romantic self-concept
remain relatively underexplored to date. are presented, as are data suggesting that certain
For instance, we know little about how adoles- aspects of romantic competence uniquely predict
cents structure, integrate, and access the infor- psychosocial adjustment. In addition, the potential
mation that they garner through their romantic moderating role of the individual’s perceived
experiences. One possibility is that adolescents importance of each romantic domain on his or
form distinct competence beliefs within the roman- her self-worth is discussed. First, however, a brief
tic domain, as others have posited occurs across discussion of the study’s theoretical underpinnings
broader domains of functioning and social roles is in order.
(e.g., Harter, 1999; Masten et al., 1995). As a

This research was supported by National Institutes of The Structure of Self-Concept


Health Institutional Training Grant T32 HD07109-24, Univer-
sity of Michigan.
The conceptual framework for this research
I thank Valerie Simon and Laura Shaffer Hand for their
contributions to this study and Timothy R. Stickle for his sta- relied on contemporary theory regarding the self
tistical advice. I also thank Carrie Hommel, Lisa Andrews, Matt in adolescence and young adulthood. A hallmark
Renck, Tracy Petherbridge, Melanie Steeves, Laurie Kaufman, of self-organization during this developmental
Meredith Adams, Audra Boden, Kara Egasti, William Murphy, period is an increase in the number of domains
David Guzzman, Joanne Kalisz, Susan Varni, and Anka
encapsulated by one’s self-concept (Marsh &
Vujanovic for their help in data collection and=or data entry.
Correspondence should be addressed to Heather A. Bouchey, Hattie, 1996). Harter (1999) pointed out that
Department of Psychology, University of Vermont, Burlington, self-representations become more multidimen-
VT 05405. E-mail: Heather.Bouchey@uvm.edu sional and differentiated once the capacity for

503
BOUCHEY

abstract thought emerges in early to middle ado- competent for particular aspects of romantic func-
lescence. Adolescents can create abstract and tioning, before assessing ‘‘how much’’ they were
integrated self-representations focused on the self like those people. Finally, the measure was designed
in different contexts (i.e., broader peer group, to simultaneously assess both competence and
romantic partners, part-time jobs, etc.). importance components of the romantic domain.
Based on this multiple selves phenomenon, late Previous work on interpersonal competence has
adolescents’ self-representations within one social tended to focus solely on assessments of com-
sphere—romantic relationships—were further petence, without examining the extent to which
explored in the research presented here. Just as adolescents care about whether they are competent
adolescents form distinct perceived competencies in different arenas of social functioning.
linked to different interaction partners or contexts,
they were also expected to possess distinct com-
Domains of Romantic Competence
petence perceptions for different aspects of the self
within the same domain. Thus, this study investi- As an initial step toward investigating adoles-
gated a number of separate, albeit modestly cents’ perceived romantic competence, six domains
related, components of perceived romantic com- were assessed. Four of these domains could be
petence during late adolescence. considered ‘‘individually’’ based romantic compe-
Previous research has revealed that high school tencies, focused on respondents’ perceived ability,
students’ perceived competence regarding the peer comfort, and preference for certain aspects of
group, close friends, and romantic friends are romantic functioning. The latter two domains
distinct constructs (Connolly & Konarski, 1994). could be construed as more ‘‘partner’’ focused
However, the Romantic Competence scale in that aspects of competence, as they directly incorporate
work was based solely on the Romantic Appeal characteristics of respondents’ partners as well as
subscale of the Self-Perception Profile for Adoles- broader relational features. The six initial domains
cents (SPP-A; Harter, 1988). Consistent with the were (a) romantic appeal, adolescents’ perceived
contemporary theorized structure of adolescent ability to get an attractive potential dating partner
self-concept, it was expected in this study that ado- to go out with him or her (from the original SPP-
lescents would perceive themselves to be com- A); (b) sexual competence, adolescents’ perceived
petent (or incompetent) for a variety of different experience and confidence in the sexual arena;
domains, all related to romantic functioning. A (c) communication, adolescents’ perceived ability
central goal of this research was to demonstrate to disclose important information to their part-
that additional components of perceived romantic ners; (d) relationship maintenance, adolescents’
competence, above and beyond romantic appeal, perceived ability to stay in romantic relationships;
could be empirically validated. A measure osten- (e) power balance, adolescents’ success at estab-
sibly tapping into a number of these domains lishing an equal balance of power in romantic
was accordingly developed. relationships; and (f) partner acceptance, the
This conceptualization is different from existing extent to which adolescents felt understood and
approaches that assess interpersonal competence validated as a person by their romantic partners.
in several respects. First, whereas previous scales Each of these domains was selected because
have focused on adolescents’ perceptions of broad extant literature supports its salience for healthy
interpersonal skills across a variety of domains romantic functioning during adolescence or young
and relationships (e.g., Buhrmester, Furman, adulthood. For instance, romantic involvement is
Wittenberg, & Reiss, 1988; Grover, Nangle, & a normative experience for nearly all Western mid-
Zeff, 2005), this investigation was designed to dle and late adolescents (Carver, Joyner, & Udry,
assess perceived competence among different 2003), and feeling capable of attracting potential
facets of romantic relationships per se. Thus, the dating partners (i.e., romantic appeal) has long
extent to which late adolescents differentiated been considered a central component of adoles-
among aspects of their functioning within the same cents’ romantic self-perceptions (Connolly &
social role was examined. Second, this approach Konarski, 1994). Late adolescents who profess
was different from most previous work on an inability to initiate dating relationships (barring
interpersonal competence in that it adopted a cultural or contextual factors that preclude such
‘‘structured alternative format’’ to evaluate com- activities; see Diamond, Savin-Williams, & Dube,
petence dimensions. To adequately assess per- 1999) could therefore be construed as ‘‘incom-
ceived romantic competence while reducing social petent’’ with respect to arguably the most basic
desirability (see Harter, 1988), respondents were aspect of romantic functioning. It was thus
required to first select whether they were ‘‘more deemed important to include this aspect of com-
like’’ people portrayed as either incompetent or petence in the study presented here.

504
COMPETENCE, IMPORTANCE, AND ADJUSTMENT

Sexual competence is also likely to be a core Kawaguchi, & Rostosky, 1999). In contrast, when
domain of romantic competence, especially for adolescents feel supported, respected, and valued
older adolescents. In addition to romantic involve- by their romantic partners, they are more likely
ment, a sizeable majority of late adolescents report to report satisfaction with their relationship (see
having experienced sexual intercourse within the Fitness, 2006). These latter aspects of romantic
context of their romantic relationships (Carver functioning are also presumed to underlie the
et al., 2003). As well, sexual agency and sexual establishment of intimacy (Reis & Shaver, 1988).
self-reflection have been recently identified as
important components of early and late adolescent
self-concept (Horne & Zimmer-Gembeck, 2006; Links with Adjustment
O’Sullivan, Meyer-Bahlburg, & McKeague, 2006).
Satisfaction with sexual aspects of one’s life is also Competence in each of these six domains should
an important predictor of romantic relationship also logically be linked to psychosocial adjust-
quality (Sprecher, 2002). Thus, adolescents’ per- ment. For instance, because it is normative for
ceived satisfaction with the amount and type of older adolescents to engage in intimate, long-term
sexual activity they had engaged in was assessed. relationships, competence in communication and
Open communication has also been considered relationship maintenance should be associated
a central component of healthy marital and with adolescents’ overall sense of self-worth as a
romantic relationships (Dindia & Timmerman, person as well as their perceived social com-
2003; Fitzpatrick & Sollie, 1999; Sprecher & petence. It also stands to reason that those adoles-
Hendrick, 2004). Young adults who value com- cents with greater perceived romantic competence
munication, emotional involvement, and intimacy would feel less social anxiety (LaGreca & Lopez,
report more satisfaction with their romantic rela- 1998). Presumably individuals’ confidence arising
tionships (Sanderson & Cantor, 1997). Moreover, from previous romantic experience helps facilitate
self-disclosure and intimacy in romantic relation- the developmentally salient and complex task of
ships are likely to become increasingly important initiating and negotiating romantic relationships
as individuals move through adolescence (Collins during late adolescence. Moreover, perceived
& Sroufe, 1999). Accordingly, respondents’ per- inequity in romantic relationships has been found
ceived competence at communicating important to relate to depression and lower self-esteem dur-
information to their romantic partners was ing late adolescence, particularly among girls
assessed in the current study. (Galliher et al., 1999; Welsh et al., 1999). Feeling
In terms of relationship maintenance, norma- understood and validated by one’s romantic part-
tive studies indicate that late adolescents are typi- ner is also presumably linked to enhanced psycho-
cally involved in romantic relationships of longer logical well-being. Accordingly, competence in
duration than are younger adolescents (Carver each of these domains was expected to (statisti-
et al., 2003; Feiring, 1999), and they tend to value cally) predict psychosocial adjustment.
commitment-relevant aspects of romantic relation-
ships (Levesque, 1993; Montgomery, 2005).
Further, partners’ level of commitment has been Competence in Important Domains
associated both concurrently and longitudinally
with couple well-being (Drigotas, Rusbult, & A second aim of this study emanated from
Verette, 1999). Thus in this study, late adolescents’ William James’s (1890) classic theory. James
perceived comfort with and skill at maintaining argued that the ratio of one’s success to one’s pre-
relationships was considered a central aspect of tensions or aspirations would most strongly
romantic competence. impact how a person felt about him- or herself.
With respect to more partner-oriented aspects Harter (1999) tested James’s ideas by investigating
of romantic competence, establishing a balance adolescents’ perceived competence in certain
of power and feeling accepted and loved by one’s domains coupled with their perceived importance
partner appear to be central components of well- of doing well in that domain. She found that when
functioning relationships. For instance, Galliher, adolescents highly value competence in a certain
Rostosky, Welsh, and Kawaguchi (1999) reported domain—for example, sports—but feel that they
that perceived level of commitment to the relation- are not doing well in that domain, they are more
ship, feelings of entrapment, and control over sex- likely to experience low self-worth. In contrast,
ual matters were predictors of female individuals’ those adolescents who feel that they aren’t doing
well-being. Moreover, perceived inequality of well in sports but don’t particularly care about
supportive behavior in romantic relationships that domain maintain their overall sense of
may signal relational difficulties (Welsh, Galliher, self-worth.

505
BOUCHEY

The interplay between competence and impor- Procedures


tance was tested in this study to reveal support
Measure construction. Constructs to be
for James’s theory within the romantic domain.
included in this study were initially generated after
In particular, it was expected that adolescents’
a thorough review of empirical literature as well as
self-worth would be highest when adolescents both
a series of informal discussions among three
valued competence within a particular romantic
doctoral-level scholars, each with expertise in the
domain and concomitantly felt that they were, in
arena of adolescent romantic relationships.
fact, competent in that domain. In contrast, self-
Twenty-two items were generated by the author
worth should be lowest when adolescents placed
to capture the six initially hypothesized com-
high value on a domain but felt relatively
petence domains (except for romantic appeal,
incompetent in that same domain.
which comprised original items from the SPP-A).
To summarize, it was hypothesized that each
Based on Harter’s (1988) methodology, 14 items
domain of romantic competence would have a
were generated to capture perceived importance
unique direct effect on psychosocial adjustment.
of competence in the six domains. All items were
In particular, given the normative salience of
completed and reviewed by a separate group of
romantic involvement for late adolescents, the
five doctoral-level scholars, each with expertise in
domains of romantic competence were expected
the arena of adolescent relationships. Reviewers
to positively predict a unique and significant por-
were instructed to complete the measure, noting
tion of variability in participants’ global self-worth
any items that were unclear or did not seem to
(i.e., self-esteem) and social acceptance. In con-
adequately tap into the notion of ‘‘competence’’
trast, negative relations were expected between
in romantic relationships. None of the respondents
competence and social anxiety, given that adoles-
noted significant difficulties with items or the
cents with low perceived romantic competence
measure’s conceptualization.
may feel particularly anxious in situations involv-
ing peers.
In terms of the role of perceived importance as Empirical validation of measure. Participants
a moderator of adolescents’ competence percep- completed self-report questionnaires in small
tions, it was expected that when adolescents placed groups of 10 to 30 per session. Participants were
greater value on competence in certain romantic told that they could discontinue participation at
domains the effect of competence in that domain any time with no penalty, that there were no right
on their self-worth would be more pronounced. or wrong answers, and that they should think
Because this latter prediction was relatively about romantic or dating partners ‘‘in general’’
exploratory, specific hypotheses were not gener- rather than specific partners when responding.
ated for each romantic domain. At the Midwestern university, students were
required to participate in research or complete an
equivalent writing assignment as part of their
course requirements. At the New England univer-
sity, students were given course extra credit for
participation. This research was fully approved
by the Institutional Review Board at each univer-
Method
sity before data collection began. In addition, all
participants provided written informed consent
Participants
before beginning the questionnaire.
Participants were 586 undergraduate students
recruited from introductory-level psychology
Measures
courses at two universities: a large public univer-
sity in the Midwest and a smaller public university All constructs in this study were assessed with
in New England. Mean age of participants was self-report questionnaires. Mean-level ratings for
18.96 years (SD ¼ .87). Sixty-nine percent of the participants who reported on at least 75% of the
sample was female. A large majority of parti- items were used for all scales.
cipants (83%) identified themselves as European The format of the Romantic Self-Concept
American, with 8% Asian, 3% African American, Questionnaire (RSC-Q; see next) and two of the
3% Latino, and 3% identifying as Other. Forty- psychological adjustment scales was similar to that
five percent of this sample was currently involved of the original SPP-A. Items were structured so
in a romantic relationship of at least 4 weeks in that participants first chose whether one of two
duration. Ninety-eight percent identified them- statements was most like them and subsequently
selves as heterosexual. marked how true that statement was for them.

506
COMPETENCE, IMPORTANCE, AND ADJUSTMENT

Respondents sometimes encountered a positive This scale consisted of the five original items from
stem followed by a negative stem, and sometimes the SPP-A, except a 6-point rating scale was used
this pattern was reversed. (see earlier). A sample item for this scale is, ‘‘Some
There were two modifications to the original people are happy with themselves most of the time
SPP-A format. Scales in the study presented here BUT other people are often not happy with them-
employed 6-point ratings ranging from not at all selves.’’ Harter (1988) reported reliabilities of .80
true to very true. The number of rating points to .89 for this scale.
was increased from the original 4-point format to
capture more variability in participants’ responses
and reduce skewness in scale scores. In addition, Social acceptance. The Social Acceptance
the word teenagers from the original SPP-A was subscale of the SPP-A assessed the extent to which
changed to people in this study. the respondent ‘‘feels that he=she is easy to like.’’
This scale consisted of the five original items from
Romantic self-concept. The RSC-Q comprised the SPP-A, with a 6-point rating scale (see earlier).
two components: perceived competence in and A sample item for this scale is, ‘‘Some people find
perceived importance of distinct romantic domains. it hard to make friends BUT for other people it’s
pretty easy.’’ Harter (1988) reported reliabilities
of .77 to .90 for this scale.
Perceived competence. Adolescents’ percep-
tions of their romantic competence were assessed
Social anxiety. The Social Anxiety Scale for
with 22 items that ostensibly assessed competence
Adolescents (LaGreca & Lopez, 1998) assessed
in the following domains: romantic appeal, sexual
respondents’ perceptions of social evaluative anxi-
experience and knowledge, communication,
ety and perceived distress in social situations.
relationship maintenance, power balance, and
Scores from both the Fear of Negative Evaluation
partner acceptance. Leading stems for items for
(FNE) and Social Avoidance Specific to New
the final scales are included in Table 1. For the
Situations or Unfamiliar Peers (SADN) subscales
original romantic appeal scale, Harter (1988)
were used. The FNE subscale consisted of eight
reported reliabilities of .75 to .85. Cronbach alpha
items (e.g., ‘‘I worry about what others say about
reliabilities for the scales in this study are dis-
me’’), whereas the SADN scale consisted of six
played in Table 2.
items (e.g., ‘‘I get nervous when I meet new
people’’). All items were rated using a 5-point scale
Perceived importance. Adolescents’ percep- ranging from 1 (not at all) to 5 (all the time).
tions of how important competence was in each LaGreca and Lopez reported reliabilities of .91
romantic domain were assessed with 14 items (FNE) and .83 (SADN) for this scale.
(e.g., ‘‘Some people don’t care that much about
Dating history. As indeces of their previous
whether they are in a committed romantic
romantic experiences, participants provided self-
relationship BUT other people think it’s important
reports of the total number of romantic partners
to be in a committed relationship’’). Scores for
they had had and the longest duration (in months)
perceived importance were composed of the aver-
of their romantic involvement.
age of 2 to 4 items for each domain. The format
of these items mirrored the structure of the Impor-
tance Rating Scale of the SPP-A, except that a Results
6-point scale was used (see earlier). Intercorrela-
tions among the importance items for domains The analysis plan for this study consisted of
are listed in Table 2. principal components analysis followed by a series
of descriptive, correlational, and regression
analyses.
Psychological adjustment. Four measures
were used to assess psychological adjustment.
Internal reliability for all four measures was very Factor Structure and Psychometric Properties
good (Cronbach a reliabilities ¼ .84–.90). of the RSC-Q
To test whether the hypothesized six subscales
of the RSC-Q competence measure emerged from
Global self-worth. The Global Self-Worth sub- the data, a principal components analysis with
scale of the SPP-A was used to assess the extent to six specified factors and varimax rotation was con-
which the respondent ‘‘likes oneself as a person.’’ ducted. Factorability of the correlation matrix was

507
Table 1. Factor Structure and Loadings for Competence Items of the Romantic Self-Concept Questionnaire

508
Factor
1 2 3 4 5
Positive Partner Characteristics
Some people feel that their partners respect them as a person (PA) .78
Some people experience a pretty equal balance of power in their relationships (PB) .76
Some people are not pleased with how their partner makes them feel as a person (PA, RC) .75
Some people feel that their romantic partners accept them for who they are (PA) .69
Some people are not happy with how their partner thinks of them (PA, RC) .63
Some people are not pleased with their romantic partner’s level of commitment in the .58
relationship (PB, RC)
Maintaining Relationships
Some people are not comfortable being in a long-term relationship (MN, RC) .80
Some people prefer not to be involved in a long-term, committed relationship (MN, RC) .80
Some people have the social skills that allow them to stay in long-term romantic .74
relationships (MN)
Some people are able to stay in a relationship with someone they like (MN) .74
Some people are happy with their own level of commitment in romantic relationships (PB) .49
Some people put the same amount of effort into their relationships as their partners do (PB) .44 .46
Communication
Some people are not very comfortable sharing personal information with their partners (CM, RC) .74
Some people find it easy to tell their partners what they like or need (CM) .67
Some people have a hard time communicating with their romantic partner (CM, RC) .66
Some people find it easy to talk with their romantic partners (CM) .49 .60
Some people find it difficult to have romantic relationships (MN, RC) .47
Romantic Appeala
Some people feel that others their age will be romantically attracted to them (RA) .76
Some people feel that if they are romantically interested in someone, that person will .72
like them back (RA)
Some people feel that they are fun and interesting on a date (RA) .63
Some people are able to please their partners in a physical=sexual wayb (SC) .58
Some people usually don’t go out with people they would really like to date (RA, RC) .43
Some people are not dating the people they are really attracted toc (RA, RC)
Sexual Competence
Some people feel that they could benefit from more sexual experience (SC, RC) .85
Some people are happy with the amount of sexual activity they engage in (SC) .76
Some people feel that they could use more knowledge about sex (SC, RC) .44
Note: Factor loadings below .40 are not shown. Original scale for item included after each item: PA ¼ Partner Acceptance; PB ¼ Power Balance; CM ¼ Communication; MN ¼ Maintenance;
RA ¼ Romantic Appeal; SC ¼ Sexual Competence. RC ¼ reverse coded.
a
Adapted from Harter (1988).
b
This item was not included in the Romantic Appeal subscale, to keep the measure consistent with Harter’s (1988) original subscale.
c
Although this item did not load on any of the five factors, it was included in the Romantic Appeal subscale to keep the measure consistent with Harter’s (1988) original subscale.
COMPETENCE, IMPORTANCE, AND ADJUSTMENT

Table 2. Zero-Order Correlations Between Competence Domains and Psychological Adjustment

Variable Name Global Self-Worth Social Acceptance Fear Neg Eval Social Avoidance a M(SD)
Competence
1. Partner Chars. .48 .32 .28 .20 .86 4.84a (.92)
2. Maintenance .36 .17 .21 .12 .86 4.59b (1.05)
3. Communication .42 .26 .23 .28 .82 4.51b (1.03)
4. Romantic Appeal .56 .45 .44 .40 .77 4.02c (.96)
5. Sexual Competence .27 .19 .19 .19 .70 3.95c (1.23)
Importance
1. Partner Chars. .24 .19 .06 .05 .44a 5.53a (.59)
2. Maintenance .05 .02 .07 .09 .53 4.22b (1.15)
3. Communication .21 .11 .02 .00 .46 4.76c (1.02)
4. Romantic Appeal .12 .09 .01 .02 .39 5.36e (.75)
5. Sexual Competence .09 .12 .02 .01 .32 4.13b (1.03)
Note: Ns for correlations range from 538 to 554. Ns for means range from 537 to 566. For correlations, j.09j < jrj < j.11j, p < .05.
j.11j < jr j < j.15j, p < .01. j rj > j.15 j, p < .001. a ¼ Cronbach’s alpha reliability for competence domain; average correlation among
items for importance domains. Subscripts for competence and importance means denote significant t-test differences within the domain
at p < .005. Neg Eval ¼ negative evaluation; Chars. ¼ characteristics.
a
Average correlation among four importance items for original acceptance and power balance scales.

acceptable (Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sam- validity and reliability of the RSC–Q. For
pling adequacy ¼ .93), Bartlett’s Test of instance, with respect to criterion validity, all five
Sphericity ¼ 6144.74 (325), p < .001. The solution competence scores were positively associated with
yielded five factors with eigenvalues greater than the duration of adolescents’ longest romantic
1.0. As indicated in Table 1, the first factor was involvement (rs ¼ .15 to .42, p < .001), and com-
a combination of romantic partner acceptance munication, romantic appeal, and sexual com-
and power balance items (termed ‘‘positive partner petence were also positively related to the total
characteristics’’ for the remainder of this article). number of dating partners adolescents reported
The remaining factors were relatively consistent (rs ¼ .14, p < .01 to .25, p < .001). Perceived
with the originally hypothesized structure. The romantic competence was thus correlated with
second factor comprised primarily maintenance the extensiveness and intensity of romantic
items, the third factor represented primarily com- involvement. Cohen’s reliability coefficients indi-
munication items, and the fifth factor comprised cated good reliability for the competence sub-
three sexual competence items. Only four of the scales, and correlations between the items for
original romantic appeal items loaded together, each importance subscale were all significant and
and one of the sexual competence items loaded positive (see Table 2).
with this factor as well (see Factor 4, Table 1). In addition, a subsample (n ¼ 75) of the orig-
Together, these five factors accounted for inal participants recompleted the RSC-Q 1 month
59.14% of the variance in the items. The analysis after originally participating. Test–retest correla-
yielded a relatively clear solution, with only two tions for this subsample indicated good reliability.
cross-loadings greater than .40 (see Table 1). Correlations for competence ranged from .77 (sex-
Based on these results, the following mean sub- ual competence) to .86 (relationship maintenance),
scales of romantic competence were constructed: all p < .001. Correlations for importance ranged
(a) Positive Partner Characteristics, comprising from .48 (romantic appeal) to .77 (sexual com-
the six highest loading items for Factor 1; (b) petence), all p < .001.
Relationship Maintenance, comprising the six
highest loading items for Factor 2; (c) Communi-
Descriptive Analyses
cation, comprising the five highest loading items
for Factor 3; (d) Romantic Appeal, comprising As illustrated in Table 2, participants reported
the original five items from the SPP-A (see Table moderately high levels of competence across
1); and (e) Sexual Competence, comprising the domains of the RSC–Q. A series of pairwise t tests
three highest loading items for Factor 5. Cross- with Bonferroni error correction revealed that
loading items were assigned only to the factor that positive partner characteristics were rated highest,
they loaded highest on to minimize multicollinear- whereas competence in both romantic appeal and
ity among subscales. the sexual domain were rated lowest. Importance
Concurrent and short-term longitudinal corre- ratings also indicated that participants placed at
lation analyses indicated preliminary evidence for least moderate value on being competent in each

509
BOUCHEY

of these aspects of romantic functioning. Adoles- negatively associated with social avoidance, com-
cents reported that it was most important to have petence at maintaining relationships was related
partners with positive characteristics. The domains to less social acceptance and more social avoid-
of sexual competence and relationship mainte- ance. Neither competence at communication nor
nance were the least valued. maintenance was associated with fear of negative
evaluation. Overall, romantic competence pre-
dicted a moderate to sizeable amount of variance
Competence and Psychological Adjustment in psychosocial adjustment (R2 ¼ .19 to .37).
In two regression analyses (for social accept-
The first hypothesis stated that romantic com- ance and social avoidance), the sign of the coef-
petence would be positively associated with psy- ficient for maintenance was reversed from that of
chosocial adjustment. As expected, zero-order the zero-order correlation between maintenance
correlations revealed that competence was signifi- and adjustment (see Table 2); follow-up analyses
cantly related to adjustment in the hypothesized examined whether maintenance served as a
direction. Negative correlations (rs ¼ .12, p < .01 suppressor variable (see Cohen, Cohen, West,
to .44, p < .001) were obtained for social anxiety & Aiken, 2003; Maassen & Bakker, 2001). Simul-
scales, whereas positive correlations (rs ¼ .17 to taneous multiple regressions run without mainte-
.56, all p < .001) were obtained for social accept- nance in the model revealed that it was indeed a
ance and global self-worth (see Table 2). Overall, suppressor of the effect of positive partner features
the more competent participants felt in the roman- on social acceptance (b ¼ .16, p < .01 without
tic domain, the better psychosocial adjustment maintenance in the equation; R2 ¼ .21) and the
they reported. effect of communication on social avoidance
A series of multiple regression analyses was (b ¼ .14, p < .01 without maintenance in the
conducted to test whether each component of equation; R2 ¼ .16). In essence, within these two
romantic competence was a significant and unique analyses participants’ perceived competence at
predictor of psychosocial adjustment. Simul- maintaining relationships served to suppress error
taneous regression was selected because direct, variance in positive partner features or communi-
rather than total, effects on adjustment were of cation, ultimately enhancing their effects on the
primary interest. Analyses were conducted separ- criterion (Cohen et al., 2003). These two variables
ately with global self-worth, social acceptance, fear were significant predictors of outcomes even in the
of negative evaluation, and social avoidance as the absence of maintenance, however. The coefficients
criterion variable. Results for these analyses are for romantic appeal and sexual competence did
demonstrated in Table 3. Perceived romantic not change noticeably in either of these analyses.
appeal was the strongest predictor of all four
measures of adjustment. As well, positive partner
features were consistently linked in the expected
Perceived Importance as a Moderator
direction to adjustment, except for social
of Romantic Competence
avoidance. Effects for perceived competence at
maintaining romantic relationships and communi- The second hypothesis predicted a moderating
cation with romantic partners were more mixed. relation between importance and competence
Whereas communication competence was in predicting global self-worth. A series of four
positively associated with global self-worth and hierarchical regressions (see Cohen et al., 2003),

Table 3. Standardized Regression Coefficients for Romantic Self-Concept Domains as Predictors of Psychosocial
Adjustment

Psychosocial Adjustment
Global Self-Worth Social Acceptance Fear of Neg Eval Social Avoidance
Variables b R2 b R2 b R2 b R2
Positive Partner Char’s. .24 .20 .11 .01
Relationship Maintenance .02 .16 .03 .23
Communication .11 .05 .01 .24
Romantic Appeal .43 .41 .43 .37
Sexual Competence .07þ .37 .06 .22 .06 .21 .01 .19
Note: N ¼ 532–534. Neg Eval ¼ negative evaluation; Chars. ¼ characteristics.
þ
p < .10.  p < .05.  p < .01.  p < .001.

510
COMPETENCE, IMPORTANCE, AND ADJUSTMENT

Table 4. Perceived importance and Competence as Predictors of Global Self-Worth

Step 1 Step 2
2
Domain Comp b Import b R Comp b Import b Intxn b R2 DR2
PC .44 .20 .23 .42 .16 .11 .24 .01
MN .45 .15 .15 .47 .17 .12 .18 .01
CM .42 .01 .18 .42 .01 .06 .18 .00
RA .56 .06þ .32 .55 .06þ .04 .32 .006
SC .27 .02 .07 .27 .02 .02 .07 .00
Note: N ¼ 537–551. Comp ¼ mean-centered competence term; Import ¼ mean-centered importance term; Intxn ¼ Competence 
Importance interaction term; PC ¼ Partner Characteristics; MN ¼ Maintenance; CM ¼ Communication; RA ¼ Romantic Appeal;
SC ¼ Sexual Competence.
þ
p < .10.  p < .05.  p < .001.

one for each romantic domain, was conducted to documenting positive links between romantic
test this prediction. involvement and self-esteem (Samet & Kelly,
Results revealed a statistically significant inter- 1987). It is important to point out that the most
action between importance and competence for salient aspect of competence for adolescents’ con-
two of the domains: positive partner characteris- current adjustment was their perceived romantic
tics and competence in maintaining relationships appeal. Yet each of the three competence domains
(see Table 4). Specifically, adolescents with the previously noted did exhibit a unique direct effect
lowest self-esteem were those that placed the high- on at least one aspect of psychosocial adjustment.
est value on staying in romantic relationships but Hypothesized effects were not demonstrated
also felt less capable of doing so. Results were in for either perceived sexual competence or skill at
the opposite direction for positive partner charac- relationship maintenance. The broad-based lack
teristics. Here, adolescents with the highest self- of findings for sexual competence in this study is
esteem were those that both valued these relational interesting, as it suggests that, in the face of other
features and felt that their partners typically pos- relational competencies, late adolescents’ per-
sessed them. Adolescents low in positive partner ceived competence in the sexual domain is not
characteristics did not show differential self-worth relevant to their adjustment, at least in terms of
as a function of importance. the psychosocial outcomes assessed here. Of
course, the reliability obtained for the sexual com-
petence scale in this study was somewhat lower
Discussion that that of the other competence scales, and
future research with more sensitive measures of
Consistent with hypotheses, late adolescents’ perceived sexual competence should be undertaken
competence in three of the five assessed romantic to either replicate or refute this pattern.
domains was positively linked with psychosocial Surprisingly, competence at maintaining roman-
adjustment. To the extent that adolescents felt tic relationships did not independently predict
more romantically appealing and reported both variance in psychosocial outcomes. It functioned
acceptance from partners and an equivalent bal- primarily as a suppressor variable in this investi-
ance of power in their relationships, they were gation. Although this pattern could certainly have
more likely to feel good about themselves and resulted from some degree of multicollinearity
report more acceptance and less anxiety in the between competence at maintaining relationships
social domain. When adolescents felt skilled at and both communication competence and having
communicating with partners, they also reported partners with positive features (relevant correla-
higher self-esteem and were less avoidant of novel tions were .68 and .61), the mean-level pattern of
social situations. These results suggest that indivi- findings suggests a slightly different interpretation.
duals with low perceived romantic competence are Namely, these late adolescents were not very
at heightened risk for poor self-esteem and com- invested in maintaining their romantic relation-
promised well-being. Of course, it is also possible ships, a pattern somewhat inconsistent with
that poorer psychosocial adjustment predicts com- frameworks positing greater commitment to rela-
promised romantic competence or that bidirec- tionships by late adolescence (see Collins & Sroufe,
tional associations exist between competence and 1999; Montgomery, 2005). Indeed, the most ‘‘deva-
adjustment at any one point in time. Nonetheless, lued’’ domains in this study (maintenance and sex-
the findings are consistent with previous work ual competence) were the two least linked with

511
BOUCHEY

adjustment. It is possible that at a different time maladjustment because of how strongly they value
frame, such as later in adulthood when the press certain aspects of romantic functioning would be
for finding a marital partner increases, the links fruitful. If the latter is true, then scholars should
between individuals’ investment in maintaining put more effort into further refining what is meant
long-term relationships and adjustment would be by and how best to assess romantic competence.
stronger. It is also worthwhile to note that individ- It is also worth noting that, contrary to expecta-
ual differences in the ratio between perceived tions, results did not support a unique construct of
competence and importance did predict self-worth perceived power balance in the study presented
(see next). here. Rather, the original power balance items per-
In addition to the direct associations between taining to partner features loaded with the original
competence and psychosocial adjustment, findings partner acceptance items, whereas the original
also revealed that adolescents’ self-esteem was dif- power balance items pertaining to the respondent’s
ferentially associated with both the extent to which effort and commitment in the relationship loaded
they experienced positive partners and were com- with the original maintenance items. These pat-
petent at maintaining relationships, as a function terns are interesting in two respects. First, they
of how much they valued these domains. Thus, suggest that late adolescents may be inclined to
when adolescents both expressed that it was clump together their perceptions of different
important to have acceptance and balance in rela- aspects of their partners’ behavior and inclina-
tionships and felt that they did, they had very high tions, rather than retain distinct conceptions of
levels of self-esteem. Of interest, it was not the case what partners do and believe. Second, the results
that those who highly valued positive partner fea- indicate that our original conceptualization may
tures but did not report experiencing them felt the have misattributed the meaning of romantic com-
worst about themselves. Instead, adolescents with mitment and effort for late adolescents. Whereas
few positive partner characteristics evidenced the these components of romantic functioning were
lowest self-esteem, regardless of their importance initially thought to capture ways in which relation-
ratings, demonstrating the strong association of ships could be ‘‘imbalanced,’’ they were actually
perceived partner characteristics with respondents’ more relevant to perceived competence at main-
self-worth in this sample. taining relationships for this sample of late adoles-
In contrast, when adolescents who highly cents. The potential overlap between power
valued being in a romantic relationship felt that balance and relationship maintenance in romantic
they were not skilled at staying in them, they did relationships deserves further empirical attention.
show lower levels of self-worth. In other words, It is important to highlight limitations regard-
perceived competence at staying in romantic rela- ing this study. First, the data were cross-sectional
tionships was a liability only for those who wanted and all self-report. Longitudinal and multimethod
a long-term relationship to begin with. These approaches would clearly add to this work. Also,
results may indicate a unique subset of late adoles- the convenience sample, drawn from introductory
cents who are seeking out long-term romantic psychology classes, was overwhelmingly female.
partnerships but feel unable to successfully achieve Given this sex makeup, it is possible that the con-
them. Future study should inquire whether such a nections revealed among romantic self-concept
group is at heightened risk for psychopathology in and adjustment are particularly salient to young
addition to reduced self-esteem. women. Other researchers have reported gender
Although these two moderating effects are differences in the links between romantic percep-
interesting, it is important to point out that only tions and both depression and self-esteem (Galliher
two of the six moderating analyses revealed signifi- et al., 1999; Welsh et al., 1999), and future research
cant effects. Moreover, the significant effects that on romantic competence should more readily
did emerge were quite small in magnitude. Given examine gender as a potential moderator variable.
the moderate correlations obtained among the Likewise, the current sample was primarily
importance items in this study, it is possible that European American and heterosexual. As recently
the psychometric properties of that measure pre- noted (Bouchey & Furman, 2003), there exists a
cluded the discovery of truly significant effects. paucity of research on the romantic experiences
At present, it remains unclear whether more sensi- of ethnic and sexual minority adolescents in the
tive indeces of perceived importance would reveal United States. As such, we know very little about
stronger moderation effects or, alternatively, how broader cultural aspects of late adolescents’
whether perceived competence is truly the primary lives inform their romantic expectations and per-
force driving effects on psychosocial functioning. ceptions. The sample here was unique in that
If the former is true, identifying unique subsets it comprised highly educated, late adolescent
of adolescents who are at risk for psychological youth. Given the connections among romantic

512
COMPETENCE, IMPORTANCE, AND ADJUSTMENT

competence, importance of romantic domains, and (pp. 685–721). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associ-
adjustment documented in this study, investigating ates.
Drigotas, S. M., Rusbult, C. E., & Verette, J. (1999). Level of
how these patterns may be similar (or not) for indi- commitment, mutuality of commitment, and couple well-
viduals from diverse ethnic and socioeconomic being. Personal Relationships, 6, 389–409.
backgrounds will be an informative next step in Feiring, C. (1999). Other-sex friendship networks and the devel-
this research area. Finally, and perhaps most opment of romantic relationships in adolescence. Journal
important, this investigation must be recognized of Youth and Adolescence, 28, 495–512.
Fitness, J. (2006). Emotion and cognition in close relationships.
as a preliminary attempt to investigate the domains In P. Noller & J. Feeney (Eds.), Close relationships: Func-
of (late) adolescents’ perceived romantic compe- tions, forms, and processes (pp. 285–303). New York:
tencies. Future work will no doubt reveal other Psychology Press.
important domains of romantic competence. As Fitzpatrick, J. & Sollie, D. L. (1999). Influence of individual
well, it is clear that more systematic investigation and interpersonal factors on satisfaction and stability in
romantic relationships. Personal Relationships, 6, 337–350.
of the RSC–Q is needed, including replication with Furman, W., Simon, V. A., Shaffer, L. A., & Bouchey, H. A.
multiple samples of both younger and older ado- (2002). Adolescents’ working models and styles for rela-
lescents and concurrent validity checks. It is hoped tionships with parents, friends, and romantic partners.
that this initial investigation will serve as a spring- Child Development, 73, 241–255.
board for additional research in the intriguing Galliher, R. V., Rostosky, S. S., Welsh, D. P., & Kawaguchi,
M. C. (1999). Power and psychological well-being in
realm of romantic competence. late adolescent romantic relationships. Sex Roles, 40,
689–710.
Grover, R. L., Nangle, D. W., & Zeff, K. R. (2005). The mea-
sure of adolescent heterosocial competence: Development
References and internal validation. Journal of Clinical Child and
Adolescent Psychology, 34, 282–291.
Bouchey, H. A., & Furman, W. (2003). Dating and romantic Harter, S. (1988). The self-perception profile for adolescents.
experiences in adolescence. In G. R. Adams & Unpublished manual, Denver, CO: University of Denver.
M. Berzonsky (Eds.), The blackwell handbook of ado- Harter, S. (1999). Developmental approaches to self processes.
lescence (pp. 313–329). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. New York: Guilford.
Buhrmester, D., Furman, W., Wittenberg, M., & Reis, H. Horne, S. & Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J. (2006). The female sexual
(1988). Five domains of interpersonal competence in peer subjectivity inventory: Development and validation of a
relations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, multidimensional inventory for late adolescents and
55, 991–1008. emerging adults. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 30,
Carver, K., Joyner, K., & Udry, J. R. (2003). National estimates 125–138.
of adolescent romantic relationships. In P. Florsheim (Ed.), James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. Chicago: Encyclo-
Adolescent romantic relations and sexual behavior: Theory, pedia Britannica.
research, and practical implications (pp. 25–36). Mahwah, LaGreca, A. M. & Lopez, N. (1998). Social anxiety among
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. adolescents: Linkages with peer relations and friendships.
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26, 83–95.
Applied multiple regression=correlation analysis for the Levesque, R. J. R. (1993). The romantic experience of adoles-
behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). (pp. 255–301). Mahwah, NJ: cents in satisfying love relationships. Journal of Youth
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. and Adolescence, 22, 219–251.
Collins, W. A. (2003). More than myth: The developmental Maassen, G. H. & Bakker, A. B. (2001). Suppressor variables in
significance of romantic relationships during adolescence. path models: Definitions and interpretations. Sociological
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 13, 1–24. Methods & Research, 30, 241–270.
Collins, W. A. & Sroufe, L. A. (1999). Capacity for intimate rela- Marsh, H. W. & Hattie, J. (1996). Theoretical perspectives on
tionships: A developmental construction. In W. Furman, the structure of self-concept. In B. A. Bracken (Ed.), Hand-
B. B. Brown, & C. Feiring (Eds.), The development of book of self–concept: Developmental, social, and clinical
romantic relationships in adolescence (pp. 125–147). New considerations (pp. 38–90). Oxford, England: Wiley.
York: Cambridge University Press. Masten, A. S., Coatsworth, J. D., Neemann, J., Gest, S. D.,
Connolly, J., Furman, W., & Konarski, R. (2000). The role of Tellegen, A., & Garmezy, N. (1995). The structure and
peers in the emergence of heterosexual romantic relation- coherence of competence from childhood through ado-
ships in adolescence. Child Development, 71, 1395–1408. lescence. Child Development, 66, 1635–1659.
Connolly, J. A. & Konarski, R. (1994). Peer self-concept in Montgomery, M. (2005). Psychosocial intimacy and identity:
adolescence. Analysis of factor structure and associations From early adolescence to emerging adulthood. Journal
with peer experience. Journal of Research in Adolescence, of Adolescent Research, 20, 346–374.
43, 385–403. O’Sullivan, L. F., Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L., & McKeague,
Diamond, L. M., Savin-Williams, R. C., & Dube, E. M. (1999). I. W. (2006). The development of the sexual self-concept
Sex, dating, passionate friendships, and romance: Intimate inventory for early adolescent girls. Psychology of Women
peer relations among lesbian, gay, and bisexual adoles- Quarterly, 30, 139–149.
cents. In W. Furman, B. B. Brown, & C. Feiring (Eds.), Reis, H. T. & Shaver, P. (1988). Intimacy as an interpersonal
The development of romantic relationships in adolescence process. In S. W. Duck (Ed.), Handbook of personal rela-
(pp. 175–210). New York: Cambridge University Press. tionships (pp. 367–389). New York: Wiley.
Dindia, K. & Timmerman, L. (2003). Accomplishing romantic Rice, F. P. & Dolgin, K. G. (2005). The adolescent: Develop-
relationships. In J. O. Greene & B. R. Burleson (Eds.), ment, relationships, and culture (11th ed.). Boston: Allyn
Handbook of communication and social interaction skills & Bacon.

513
BOUCHEY

Samet, N. & Kelly, E. W. (1987). The relationship of steady Steinberg, L. (2005). Adolescence (7th ed.). New York:
dating to self-esteem and sex role identity among adoles- McGraw-Hill.
cents. Adolescence, 22, 231–245. Welsh, D. P., Galliher, R. V., Kawaguchi, M. C., & Rostosky,
Sanderson, C. A. & Cantor, N. (1997). Creating satisfaction in S. S. (1999). Discrepancies in adolescent romantic couples’
steady dating relationships: The role of personal goals and and observers’ perceptions of couple interaction and their
situational affordances. Journal of Personality and Social relationship to depressive symptoms. Journal of Youth
Psychology, 73, 1424–1433. and Adolescence, 28, 645–666.
Sprecher, S. (2002). Sexual satisfaction in premarital relation-
ships: Associations with satisfaction, love, commitment,
and stability. The Journal of Sex Research, 3, 1–7.
Sprecher, S. & Hendrick, S. S. (2004). Self-disclosure in intimate
relationships: Associations with individual and relation-
ship characteristics over time. Journal of Social and Clinical Received August 11, 2005
Psychology, 23, 857–877. Accepted November 13, 2006

514

You might also like