You are on page 1of 14

J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:1708–1720

DOI 10.1007/s10964-012-9878-0

EMPIRICAL RESEARCH

Linking Social Anxiety and Adolescent Romantic Relationship


Functioning: Indirect Effects and the Importance of Peers
Karen R. Hebert • Jessica Fales • Douglas W. Nangle •

Alison A. Papadakis • Rachel L. Grover

Received: 17 September 2012 / Accepted: 22 November 2012 / Published online: 2 December 2012
 Springer Science+Business Media New York 2012

Abstract Peer relationships undergo dramatic shifts in pathway whereby social anxiety was associated with
form and function during adolescence, at the same time the impairment in same-sex friendships; functioning in same-
incidence of socially evaluative fears sharply rises. Despite sex friendships was associated with functioning in
well-established links between social anxiety and broader other-sex friendships, which was associated, in turn, with
interpersonal functioning, there is a dearth of research functioning in romantic relationships. While the hypothe-
evaluating the impact of social anxiety on functioning in sized indirect pathway was significant among both boys and
close relationships during this developmental stage. The girls, there was greater continuity of functioning between
present study examines the impact of social anxiety on same- and other-sex friendships for girls. These findings
functioning in close friendships and romantic relationships highlight the importance of examining the multiple down-
during adolescence. From a developmental psychopathol- stream effects of social anxiety on perceived social func-
ogy perspective, it was expected that social anxiety would tioning in adolescence, and suggest that continuity may exist
influence functioning (quality, length, satisfaction) in for maladaptive patterns of socialization, particularly across
romantic relationships through its influence on functioning developmentally salient close relationships.
in same- and other-sex friendships. Participants included
314 adolescents (60.5 % female, 14–19 years of age) with Keywords Romantic relationships  Social anxiety 
a prior or current history of romantic relationship Other-sex friends  Same-sex friends  Relationship quality
involvement. Structural equation modeling was used to test
a mediation model positing an indirect pathway from social
anxiety to romantic relationship functioning through Introduction
functioning in close same- and other-sex friendships. Given
known gender differences in social anxiety and relationship Adolescents are particularly vulnerable to social discom-
functioning, gender also was explored as a potential fort and evaluative fears at a time when peer interactions
moderator. Results supported the hypothesized indirect increase in frequency and importance. Indeed, the link
between social anxiety and compromised adolescent social
functioning is well documented (see Newman Kingery
K. R. Hebert
Psychology Department, Barnard College of Columbia et al. 2010, for a review). In normative samples, higher
University, 415 Milbank Hall, 3009 Broadway, New York, NY levels of social anxiety have been found to relate to lower
10027, USA peer acceptance (Erath et al. 2010; Flanagan et al. 2008; La
Greca and Lopez 1998; La Greca and Harrison 2005) and
J. Fales (&)  D. W. Nangle
Psychology Department, University of Maine, 301 Little Hall, increased peer victimization (Siegel et al. 2009; Storch
Orono, ME 04469, USA et al. 2003; Storch and Masia-Warner, 2004). Not only do
e-mail: jessica.fales@seattlechildrens.org socially anxious youth have fewer reciprocated close
friendships (Erath et al. 2010), but their existing friend-
A. A. Papadakis  R. L. Grover
Psychology Department, Loyola University Maryland, 4501 ships also tend to be of lower quality. For instance, higher
North Charles Street, Baltimore, MD 21210, USA social anxiety has been associated with increased negative

123
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:1708–1720 1709

interactions and decreased support from one’s best friend, Other leading models of adolescent romantic relationships
and lower perceived companionship in adolescent girls’ explain this continuity with attachment theory (e.g.,
closest friendships (La Greca and Harrison 2005; La Greca Downey et al. 1999; Furman and Wehner 1994). Such
and Lopez 1998). theories posit that children develop an internal working
Given the salience and developmental importance of model (IWM) of relationships based on early interactions
romantic relationships in adolescence, it is surprising that with their caregivers that then shapes their future close
much less is known about how social anxiety and adoles- relationships. This IWM is thought to serve as a foundation
cent romantic relationships may be related. By mid-ado- for how to initiate, interact with, and maintain other types
lescence, most teens have begun dating and have been of relationships, including friendships and romantic rela-
involved in at least one romantic relationship (Carver et al. tionships (see Engels et al. 2001; Hazan and Shaver 1987).
2003; Feiring 1996). Consistent with developmental theory Thus, both developmental and attachment models theorize
(e.g., Erikson 1968; Sullivan 1953), adolescent romantic some consistency in the quality of adolescent same- and
relationships fulfill a number of critical functions including other-sex friendships and their romantic relationships, and
the enhancement of self-worth, social status, and sense of suggest a developmental trajectory from same- to other-sex
belonging, as well as the provision of social support, friendships, then leading to romantic relationships.
companionship, and intimacy (Collins 2003; Connolly and Supporting such theories, empirical work demonstrates
Konarski 1994; Feiring 1996; Furman and Buhrmester the importance of same- and other-sex peers in facilitating
1992; Furman and Hand 2006). These relationships also the onset of romantic relationships. Dunphy (1963) first
provide a context for meeting sexual needs and help to documented the rapid structural progression of the ado-
establish sexual identity and romantic self-concept (Con- lescent social network from same-sex peer affiliations, to
nolly and Goldberg 1999). Because the quality of these mixed-sex groups, to other-sex romantic dyads. In early
early romantic relationships in mid-adolescence is predic- adolescence, youth maintain membership in small, isolated,
tive of the quality and level of commitment in early adult same-sex peer groups. Within the context of the same-sex
relationships (Seiffge-Krenke 2003), it is important to peer groups, teens experience increased interactions
examine factors such as social anxiety that may influence between male and female cliques in group settings, which
longer term romantic relationship functioning. The fol- foreshadow the emergence of the mixed-sex cliques or
lowing section describes how friends and the larger peer ‘‘crowds.’’ Finally, the crowd slowly disintegrates as cli-
group promote the emergence of romantic relationships ques are reorganized into loose associations of steadily
during adolescence and then presents a developmental dating couples. In their longitudinal investigation, Con-
psychopathology framework for understanding how social nolly et al. (2000) observed structural shifts consistent with
anxiety may influence normative social trajectories. We those described by Dunphy such that the size of one’s
then briefly consider gender differences in social anxiety same-sex group in grade 9 predicted the size of the other-
and social functioning during this developmental stage, sex peer group in grade 10, as well as the likelihood of
before describing the present study. having a romantic partner in grade 11. Interestingly, the
same-sex peer network appeared to exert its influence on
A Theoretical Guide and Its Empirical Support romantic involvement indirectly through its influence on
the establishment of the other-sex friendship network.
The developmental literature provides a strong conceptual Similarly, in a longitudinal study following youth from
base for examining possible associations between social ages 9 through 18, Feiring (1999) found that the size of
anxiety and adolescent romantic functioning. In this liter- youth’s other-sex network increased dramatically during
ature, the establishment of romantic relationships is viewed early adolescence and having more other-sex friends
as part of a trajectory that begins with same- and other-sex appeared to facilitate the emergence of romantic relation-
peer affiliations. Major developmental theorists have long ships, as those adolescents with a larger other-sex peer
observed linkages across the peer and romantic domains. network began dating earlier than adolescents with smaller
Sullivan (1953) proposed that youth first learn emotional networks. Likewise, in a sample of adolescents in grades
intimacy skills in the context of their same-sex friendships. 10–12, La Greca and Mackey (2007) found that adoles-
With the onset of puberty and an emergent interest in the cents’ number of close same-sex friends was associated
other sex, adolescents then apply the social skills and concurrently with their number of other-sex friends, which
interpersonal competencies learned in their same-sex was associated in turn with having a romantic partner.
friendships to their relationships with the other sex. Thus, Overall, these findings suggest that greater access to and
early friends (both same- and other-sex) are thought to interactions with peers, particularly other-sex peers, seem
provide a framework for developing intimacy in more to facilitate the development of romantic relationships
mature peer relationships, including romantic relationships. during adolescence.

123
1710 J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:1708–1720

Also consistent with developmental and attachment Gender Differences in Social Anxiety and Social
models of romantic relationships, there is emerging evi- Functioning
dence to suggest that the quality of one’s relationships
with same- and other-sex friends is predictive of romantic The literature on social anxiety during adolescence sug-
relationship functioning (Cheung and McBride-Chang gests that, in community samples of youth, social anxiety
2011; Connolly et al. 2000; Feiring 1999; La Greca and symptoms are more prevalent among female adolescents
Mackey 2007). Furman (1999) suggests that youth (Essau et al. 1999; Inderbitzen-Nolan and Walters 2000; La
develop affiliative skills (e.g., intimacy, companionship, Greca and Lopez 1998; Rao et al. 2007). Although these
support) within the context of their same-sex relationships differences are somewhat consistently observed, they tend
and then transfer those skills to their other close rela- to be small in magnitude. Further, although girls are more
tionships. In support of this contention, Feiring (1999) likely to endorse higher global social anxiety, one recent
found that adolescents with more other-sex friends were study has found that boys tend to endorse greater social
more likely to identify intimacy and support as important avoidance and escape behaviors than girls (Inglés et al.
features of their romantic relationships and were also 2011). Whether this difference in symptom expression
more likely to have romantic relationships of longer results in more impaired social functioning for boys com-
duration. This provides indirect support for the notion that pared to girls is currently unknown. The very few studies
greater exposure to, and interaction with, the other sex that have directly examined gender differences in the social
enhances one’s skill with such relationships, which in turn functioning of socially anxious youth have yielded mixed
may lead to better quality romantic relationships. In one of results (Erath et al. 2007; Inderbitzen et al. 1997; La Greca
the few studies that has directly explored the role of same- and Lopez 1998; La Greca and Harrison 2005; Vernberg
and other-sex friendship quality as predictors of romantic et al. 1992). When gender differences have been reported,
relationship quality, Connolly et al. (2000) found that both it appears that social anxiety is more strongly linked to
social support and negative interactions in same- and problems in close relationships for girls (La Greca and
other-sex friendships were associated with similar indices Lopez 1998; Vernberg et al. 1992) and problems in broader
of quality in one’s concurrent romantic relationship (see peer interactions for boys (Erath et al. 2007; Inderbitzen
also Connolly and Johnson 1996; Furman and Wehner et al. 1997). Nevertheless, it is unclear whether this dis-
1994). This finding suggests that there is some degree of tinction would continue to hold when considering adoles-
continuity of functioning across different types of close cent romantic relationship functioning, particularly given
relationships. that dating often occurs in a group context during this
From a developmental psychopathology perspective, developmental stage (Roisman et al. 2009) and traditional
social anxiety likely would alter this more normative gender norms place the onus on males to be more active
pathway. For example, Connolly and Goldberg (1999) agents in dating relationships (Eaton and Rose 2011).
suggest that social withdrawal might result in difficulties It is also important to note that females consistently
at each ‘‘level’’ of peer experience. On the first level, report higher relationship quality across all types of rela-
withdrawn youth are likely less accepted by their same- tionships (Colarossi and Eccles 2000; De Goede et al.
sex peers. Since same-sex peers facilitate the emergence 2009; Furman and Buhrmester 1992), seem to more highly
of the mixed-sex peer groups, impairment with same-sex value dyadic relationships (Berndt 1992; Maccoby 1990;
peers likely would limit one’s exposure to and interactions Rose and Rudolph 2006), and more quickly progress to
with the other-sex. In turn, mixed-sex interactions are romantic relationships (Carver et al. 2003) than their male
thought to provide opportunities to develop skills specific counterparts. For these reasons, girls may be expected to
to other-sex relationships, as well as provide increased evidence greater continuity of functioning across relation-
opportunities to meet potential dating partners. As such, ship types, compared to boys. Given that so little is known
for more socially anxious adolescents, romantic relation- about how social anxiety impacts functioning in adoles-
ships may be delayed or non-existent (Connolly and cents’ close relationships, it is unclear whether girls’
Goldberg 1999; Dunphy 1963). Another possibility is that advantages in dyadic relationships would afford them extra
some teens may enter into romantic relationships without protection in the face of more global social anxiety. If
an established skill set and/or a lack of supportive friends, social anxiety is detrimental to functioning in close rela-
which could lead to poorer functioning in those romantic tionships, its impact may be more notable for girls than
relationships and poorer relationship quality. Thus, expe- boys. That said, given the importance of the broader peer
riencing social anxiety may have far-reaching conse- group for the emergence of romantic relationships, social
quences in peer relationships during this critical period of anxiety may uniquely impact romantic functioning for
social development. males. Regardless, gender is an important variable to

123
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:1708–1720 1711

consider when evaluating the impact of social anxiety on same-sex friendships, other-sex friendships, and romantic
social functioning during adolescence. relationships. In our model, we examine how social anxiety
is associated with affiliations in the same- and other-sex
peer group domains, and as well as functioning in romantic
The Present Study relationships. We particularly were interested in the con-
tinuity of functioning across these relationship types
Based on the proposed social withdrawal pathway of among youth who have initiated romantic relationships. By
Connolly and Goldberg (1999) and the broader develop- studying adolescents who have initiated romantic rela-
mental literature, the aim of the present study was to test tionships, we were able to examine the continuity of
whether social anxiety has downstream effects on func- functioning across the three social domains. We anticipated
tioning within romantic relationships by sequentially that symptoms of social anxiety would be associated with
affecting functioning in same- and other-sex friendships fewer same-sex friends and poorer relationship function-
among adolescents who have initiated dating relationships ing. This impairment in the same-sex domain was expected
(see Fig. 1). Key to the study development was the phase- to predict similar impairment in the other-sex domain.
based developmental-contextual model, first proffered by Impairment in one’s other-sex friendships was expected to
Dunphy (1963) and later expanded upon by others (Brown be associated with poorer romantic relationship quality,
1999; Connolly and Goldberg 1999), which proposes that shorter relationships, and less overall dating satisfaction
interactions within the friendship network and larger peer among youth who have initiated those relationships. Thus,
group facilitate the emergence of romantic relationships we propose that social anxiety will have a broad impact on
during adolescence. As adolescents develop romantic social functioning during adolescence, with its influence
interests, their same-sex peer network expands to include extending to difficulties in romantic relationships. We
other-sex peers. Increased access to and interactions with predict that social anxiety will be related to romantic
the other-sex are then thought to promote the development relationship quality; however, this association is not
of romantic relationships. thought to be direct. Instead, it is expected to be an indirect
A developmental psychopathology framework suggests association through its sequential associations with same-
that social anxiety would affect the developmental pathway and other-sex friendship functioning. Functionally, because
of romantic relationships. As such, it is important to our model only considered youth who already had initiated
explore the effects of social anxiety on functioning in dating, we conceptualized our sample as being at the same

Number Close SS Satisfaction w/SS Social Support Number Close OS Satisfaction w/OS Social Support
Friends (PRQ) Friends (DHQ) (NRI) Friends (PRQ) Friends (DHQ) (NRI)

.39 .42 .58 .52 .42 .63


Path 2
Same Sex (SS) .45/.49*** Other Sex (OS)
Functioning Functioning

Path 1
Path 3
-.27/-.27**
Path 4 .37/.31**
-.11/.00 Path 5
-.12/.00
Romantic
Social Anxiety (SA)
Relationship (RR)
(SAS-A)
Path 6 Functioning
-.08/-.08 .02/.00
-.09/-.08
.39 .62 .78
-.11/-.11*
Age .17/.17*
Typical Length Satisfaction Social Support
(covariate)
(DHQ) w/Partner (DHQ) (NRI)

Fig. 1 SEM model testing the indirect effect of social anxiety on estimates. All factor loadings were significant and did not differ
romantic relationship functioning through same- and other-sex across constrained and unconstrained models. N = 314. *p \ .05.
friendships. Parameter estimates are for the unconstrained model *p \ .01. ***p \ .001
(Model 1)/constrained model (Model 2; see text). All are standardized

123
1712 J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:1708–1720

‘‘developmental stage.’’ Nevertheless, we included chro- were reporting about a romantic relationship that was less
nological age as a covariate to account for its potential clearly defined, without a clear beginning and end, which also
influence on major study variables. might be associated with lower satisfaction and social support.
As reviewed in the previous section, gender is also an Participants ranged in age from 14 to 19 years
important variable to consider in our proposed model. (M = 16.25, SD = 1.25; 60.5 % female). Consistent with
While we predicted that the model would function similarly the surrounding area, the current sample was predomi-
for boys and girls, we also sought to explore whether gender nantly Caucasian (94.9 %), with a smaller percentage
might serve as a moderator, that is, whether the strength of identifying as African American (2.9 %) or endorsing an
the associations might be different for boys versus girls. ‘‘other’’ category (2.1 %). In terms of self-reported sexual
Given that the extant literature would support competing orientation, 93.9 % of those who responded identified as
hypotheses, we made no specific predictions regarding the heterosexual, 5.7 % identified as bisexual, and less than
direction of the moderating effects. However, we did pre- 1 % identified as gay/lesbian (.3 %). Over half of the
dict that girls would evidence greater continuity of func- sample (53.1 %) reported a current dating relationship and
tioning across relationship types, given their likely greater a similar percentage (57.6 %) reported that they were
experience and facility with close dyadic relationships. sexually active. For minor youth, active parental consent
and the adolescent’s verbal assent were obtained prior to
participation. Participants ages 18 or above provided their
Method own informed consent. Data were collected in either two
one-hour sessions or one two-hour session in participating
Participants and Procedures classrooms. Participants were provided with freestanding
privacy screens while completing study questionnaires.
Participants included 456 high school students recruited from
four schools in Northern New England, with a participation Measures
rate of 72 % across classrooms. As the focus of the current
study was the effect of social anxiety on functioning in Social Anxiety
romantic relationships, the total sample was reduced to 381
students who had initiated dating, indicated by a self reported To assess social anxiety, adolescents were administered the
current or past dating relationship. The present study specifi- Social Anxiety Scale for Adolescents (SAS-A; La Greca
cally included those participants who reported a past/current 1998). The scale consists of 18 self-statements rated on a
history of dating and who provided relationship quality ratings 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from not at all (1) to all the
for a specific current or most recent dating partner. Although time (5), plus 4 filler items. The SAS-A has a three-factor
there were no differences in social anxiety between partici- structure: fear of negative evaluation (FNE), which measures
pants who had initiated dating and those who had not (‘‘non- social evaluation anxiety; social anxiety and distress in
daters’’) (t(445) = 0.83, p = .410), non-daters reported less general situations (SAD-General), which measures distress
same- and other-sex friendship social support (t(402) = that is experienced generally when in situations with peers;
-3.74, p \ .001; t(385) = -4.96, p \ .001, respectively), and social anxiety and distress in new situations (SAD-New),
less satisfaction with their other-sex friendships (t(441) = which is a measure of distress specific to new situations with
-2.23, p = .026) and dating life (t(432) = -5.05, p \ .001), unfamiliar peers. A total score was computed by summing
and fewer other-sex close friendships (t(368) = -3.21, the scores from the three subscales and was used in all
p = .001). Of the sample of 381, 314 (190 females, 124 analyses. The SAS-A is one of the most frequently used
males) participants had complete data for the variables of measures to assess social anxiety in adolescents and has
interest. While listwise deletion has its drawbacks, this demonstrated good to excellent internal consistency and
approach was necessary to obtain bootstrapped confidence reliability (Garcı́a-Lopez et al. 2001; La Greca 1998; Myers
interval estimates to test for the hypothesized indirect effect. et al. 2002). Research also has supported the criterion and
Within those who were excluded for missing data, the largest concurrent validity of the scale (Myers et al., 2002). In the
percentage (32 %) was excluded for missing data regarding present study, scores on the SAS-A ranged from 18 to 78
the typical length of their romantic relationships. Those who (M = 42.20, SD = 12.14) with excellent internal consis-
were excluded reported lower same-sex social support tency (Cronbach’s a = .93).
(t(368) = -2.29, p = .007), dating satisfaction (t(368) =
-3.12, p = .002), and social support within the focal Peer Network
romantic relationship (t(379) = -4.36, p \ .001); no other
differences were significant, including social anxiety. Based on The size and gender composition of the participant’s peer
this pattern, we speculate that perhaps those who were excluded networks were assessed through the Peer Relationships

123
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:1708–1720 1713

Questionnaire (PRQ; Connolly & Konarski, 1994). The participants appropriately categorize their close relation-
adolescents were asked to identify same-age peers whom ships (i.e., participants cannot choose a sibling, relative, or
they believe constitute their peer group (i.e., ‘‘people who boy/girlfriend as their closest other-sex friend). The NRI
you like, to whom you feel close, and with whom you consists of 33-items, each rated on a 5-point scale, that
spend time’’). Frequency counts are typically used to score measure positive (intimacy, companionship, reliable alli-
the PRQ (Connolly et al. 2000; Williams, Connolly, Pepler, ance, instrumental aid, admiration, affection, satisfaction,
& Craig, 2005), and in the present study same- and other- and nurturance) and negative (conflict, antagonism, relative
sex peer networks were indexed with a frequency count of power) relationship qualities. Factor analyses of the mea-
the number of same- and other-sex adolescents, respec- sure have supported two dimensions of relational quality:
tively, listed by each student. One month test–retest reli- social support and negative interaction (Adler and Furman
abilities for number of friends has been found to be good 1988). Items assessing negative relationship qualities were
(range = .72–.76), while modest to moderate correlations not included in the present study (see also Connolly and
have been found between self- and peer-reports of friend- Konarski 1994; Faith et al. 2011). This decision was
ship networks (Connolly & Konarski, 1994). Connolly and informed by a growing body of research suggesting that the
Konarski suggest that PRQ scores are best thought of as an difficulties socially anxious youth experience in their close
adolescent’s perception of their peer network. relationship are more strongly associated with reduced
social support than increased negative interactions or
Dating History conflict (see Davila and Beck 2002; Kashdan et al. 2007;
Rodebaugh 2009; Starr and Davila 2008). We computed a
The dating history questionnaire (DHQ; Furman and broadband measure of social support for each of the three
Wehner 1992) is a 20-item measure that assesses the nature relationship domains (same, other, and romantic relation-
of adolescents’ current relationship status and dating his- ships) by averaging the positive items within that domain.
tory. The adolescent was asked to respond to questions The broadband social support scale has been found to have
regarding when he or she began dating activities, number satisfactory internal consistency, with mean alphas (across
of dating partners, and the way he or she would describe relationship types) equal to .80 (Furman and Buhrmester
him or herself when in a dating relationship. The first five 1985). In the present study, internal consistency alphas for
items of the questionnaire focus on close friendships with this scale were .95 for same-sex, .96 for other-sex, and .97
both same- and opposite-sex friends. The items on this for romantic relationships. Additionally, test–retest reli-
questionnaire are not scored to determine a total score, but ability over a one-month period for the social support scale
rather are examined item by item to determine features of ranges from .66–.77 across relationship types (Connolly
previous dating relationships. Of interest in the present and Konarski 1994).
investigation were the items assessing satisfaction with
same- and other-sex friendships as well as the participant’s
level of satisfaction with their dating life in general. For Results
these items, participants rated the extent to which they
were satisfied with their relationship on a 5-point scale, Preliminary Analyses
with higher scores indicating greater satisfaction. We were
also interested in how long individuals’ romantic rela- Bivariate correlations for study variables are presented in
tionships typically lasted, with longer relationships indi- Table 1. Means and standard deviations for the full sample
cating better relationship functioning. Response options and by gender are given in Table 2. Female participants
ranged from 1 (2 weeks or less) to 6 (more than 1 year), were on average older and reported more social support in
with higher scores being indicative of longer lasting rela- same-sex friendships, more other-sex friends, and more
tionships. Participants in the present study reported their social anxiety than the male participants (see Table 2). Age
typical relationship length was on average approximately was associated negatively with social anxiety and associ-
3–6 months (M = 3.90, SD 1.20). ated positively with typical romantic relationship length
and satisfaction with romantic relationships (see Table 2).
Relationship Quality There were no other gender differences or associations
between age and study variables.
The Network of Relationships Inventory (NRI; Furman and
Buhrmester 1985) was used to assess the quality of the Mediation/Indirect Effect Analysis
adolescents’ closest same- and other-sex friendships and
their current or most recent romantic relationship. The NRI Structural equation modeling was used to examine the
provides explicit instructions to help ensure that effect of social anxiety on romantic relationship

123
1714 J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:1708–1720

Table 1 Bivariate correlations


Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

1. Age –
2. Social anxiety (SAS-A) -.11* –
Same-sex friendship functioning
3. Number of close friends (PRQ) .00 -.12* –
4. Satisfaction (DHQ) -.02 -.09 .19** –
5. Social support (NRI) -.06 -.16** .22*** .22*** –
Other-sex friendship functioning
6. Number of close friends (PRQ) -.04 -.11 .32*** .09 .17** –
7. Satisfaction (DHQ) -.03 -.09 .13* .67*** .05 .20*** –
8. Social support (NRI) -.09 -.12* .12* .14* .47*** .33*** .25*** –
Romantic relationship functioning
9. Typical length (DHQ) .16** .09 -.02 .03 -.01 -.03 .03 .01 –
10. Satisfaction (DHQ) .13* -.08 .02 .10 -.01 .12* .19** .09 .21*** –
11. Social support (NRI) .06 -.04 .00 .03 .17** .16** .08 .20** .32*** .47*** –
N = 314
* p \ .05, ** p \ .01, *** p \ .001. All two-tailed

Table 2 Means and Standard Deviations


Variable Both genders Females Males Gender comparison
M SD Range M SD M SD t p

Age 16.25 1.25 14–19 16.49 1.24 16.10 1.24 2.74 .006
Social anxiety (SAS-A) 42.20 12.14 18–78 43.72 12.07 39.87 11.91 -2.77 .006
Same-sex friendship functioning
Number of close friends (PRQ) 4.84 2.51 0–14 5.06 2.50 4.51 2.51 -1.92 .056
Satisfaction (DHQ) 4.34 1.05 1–5 4.40 1.08 4.24 1.01 -1.30 .195
Social support (NRI) 82.25 16.16 21–105 85.63 15.98 77.08 15.08 -4.74 \.001
Other-sex friendship functioning
Number of close friends (PRQ) 3.19 2.28 0–11 3.43 2.36 2.84 2.12 -2.25 .025
Satisfaction (DHQ) 4.19 1.02 1–5 4.17 1.08 4.23 0.93 0.37 .710
Social support (NRI) 70.38 18.61 21–104 70.41 18.87 70.34 18.28 -0.03 .975
Romantic relationship functioning
Typical relationship length (DHQ) 3.90 1.20 1–6 3.99 1.21 3.75 1.17 -1.77 .077
Satisfaction (DHQ) 3.77 1.02 1–5 3.85 0.97 3.65 1.09 -1.61 .109
Social support (NRI) 78.44 19.78 21–105 78.74 20.05 77.98 19.43 -0.33 .742
N = 314

functioning through same- and other-sex friendship func- romantic relationships), we allowed the covariance
tioning, controlling for age. The hypothesized model is between the errors for related indicators (e.g., NRI items)
given in Fig. 1. First, we examined an unconstrained model to vary freely in all models.
(Model 1) in which all direct and indirect paths between the The overall model fit was excellent for the uncon-
latent factors were allowed to vary freely. Then as a test of strained model (Model 1: v2(31) = 31.833, p = .425;
the hypothesized indirect effect, we examined a con- CFI = .998, TLI = .997, rmsea = .009), and there was no
strained model (Model 2) in which the paths that were not significant decrement in model fit going from the uncon-
part of the hypothesized indirect effect (marked paths 4, 5, strained model to the constrained model (Dv2(3) = 3.398,
and 6 in Fig. 1) were constrained to zero. To account for p = .334; Model 2: v2(34) = 35.231, p = .410; CFI =
shared method effects due to similar measures across .998, TLI = .997, rmsea = .011; see Fig. 1). The lack of
relationships (same- and other-sex friendships and decrement in model fit and the path estimates in the

123
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:1708–1720 1715

unconstrained model indicate there is no significant direct Next, we tested whether the strength of the paths that
effect of social anxiety on other-sex friendship or romantic constitute the indirect effect (paths 1, 2, and 3) were different
relationship functioning (paths 4 and 6, respectively) or of for boys versus girls. Parameter comparison tests in Model 4
same-sex friendship functioning on romantic relationship indicated that there is no significant gender difference in the
functioning (path 5; see Fig. 1). Although we predicted a effect of social anxiety on same-sex friendship functioning
total effect of social anxiety on romantic relationship (path 1; z = 0.221, p = .825) or in the effect of other-sex
functioning, there was no total effect in the unconstrained friendship functioning on romantic relationship functioning
model (Model 1; unstandardized estimate = -0.001; 95 % (path 3; z = -0.709, p = .478). Therefore, in the final
bias-corrected, bootstrapped CI based on 5,000 samples: model (Model 5), we began with Model 4 and constrained
-0.006, 0.004; p = .635). However, consistent with our paths 1 (social anxiety to same-sex functioning) and 3 (other-
predictions, in the constrained model (Model 2) there was a sex functioning to romantic relationship functioning) to be
significant indirect effect of social anxiety on romantic equivalent for boys and girls but allowed path 2 to vary freely
relationship functioning through same- and other-sex rela- by gender. The final model for the multigroup analysis
tionship functioning (unstandardized estimate = -0.002; (Model 5) had very good overall fit, v2(86) = 93.875,
95 % bias-corrected, bootstrapped CI based on 5,000 p = .263, CFI = .987, TLI = .983, rmsea = .017, which
samples: -0.004, -0.001; p = .001). Further, the paths was not significantly different from the fit of Model 4,
that constitute the indirect effect were significant and in the Dv2(2) = 0.565, p = .754. The lack of change in model fit
predicted directions; social anxiety was associated with further indicated the gender equivalence for paths 1 and 3. In
significantly poorer same-sex relationship functioning Model 5 the association between same-sex and other-sex
(b = -0.27, p = .001), and poorer same-sex friendship friendship functioning (path 2) was stronger for girls than
functioning was associated with significantly poorer other- boys (z = 2.166, p = .030; see Table 3 for all path estimates
sex friendship functioning (b = 0.49, p \ .001), which for Model 5 by gender). Finally, the indirect effect of social
was, in turn, associated with significantly poorer romantic anxiety on romantic relationship functioning through same
relationship functioning (b = 0.31, p = .006). and other-sex friendship functioning was significant for both
genders (see Table 3).
Gender Moderation Analysis

To examine whether the patterns found in the constrained and Discussion


unconstrained models above (Models 1 and 2) varied across
gender, we conducted a multigroup analysis to compare the The present study examined a process by which social
strength and direction of the direct and indirect paths (marked anxiety may affect functioning within romantic relation-
1–6 in Fig. 1) for boys versus girls. In order to establish that ships among adolescents who have initiated dating rela-
the direct paths (paths 4, 5, and 6) were invariant across gender tionships. Prior research has demonstrated a natural
and equal to zero for both genders, we began the multigroup progression for romantic relationship formation during
analysis with Model 3, which was the unconstrained model adolescence in which teens transition from same- to mixed-
described above (Model 1) but with paths 1–6 (and the paths sex peer groups, and finally to dyadic relationships with
involving age; see Fig. 1) allowed to vary across boys and romantic partners (Connolly et al. 2000; Dunphy 1963).
girls. Then, we estimated a constrained model (Model 4), This developmental trajectory formed the rationale for our
which was the same as Model 3 but paths 4, 5, and 6 con- hypothesized model. According to the model, social anxi-
strained to zero for both genders. Both models had excellent ety is hypothesized to impact relationship functioning with
fit, and there was no decrement in overall model fit due to the same-sex peers, other-sex peers, and romantic partners,
imposition of the constraints in Model 4 (unconstrained where better relationship functioning was defined as having
model: v2(78) = 91.114, p = .147, CFI = .978, TLI = more friends, longer relationships, more social support in
.969, rmsea = .023; constrained model: v2(84) = 93.309, those relationships, and greater satisfaction with the rela-
p = .228, CFI = .984, TLI = .980, rmsea = .019; model fit tionships. More specifically, we proposed that there is an
difference test: Dv2(6) = 2.195, p = .901). The lack of dec- indirect pathway such that social anxiety is associated with
rement in model fit from Model 3 to Model 4 and the path lower functioning in same-sex friendships, which in turn is
estimates for paths 4, 5, and 6 in the unconstrained model associated with lower functioning in other-sex friendships.
(Model 3) indicate that for both genders there is no significant Lower functioning in other-sex friendships was then
direct effect of social anxiety on other-sex friendship or expected to be associated with lower romantic relationship
romantic relationship functioning (paths 4 and 6, respectively) functioning. Given known gender differences in social
or of same-sex friendship functioning on romantic relation- anxiety and relationship functioning, gender was explored
ship functioning (path 5). as a potential moderator of the proposed indirect pathway.

123
1716 J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:1708–1720

Table 3 Parameter estimates by gender for the final multigroup develop (Connolly and Goldberg 1999). This has implica-
model (Model 5) tions for the development of romantic relationships as well,
Path Unstd SE p whereby socially withdrawn youth may enter romantic
relationships with fewer skills gleaned from prior social
Boys (n = 124)
relationships (e.g., establishing intimacy, negotiating con-
Path 1: SA ? SSa -0.301 .065 \.001 flict; see Newman Kingery et al. 2010 for a review).
Path 2: SS ? OS 0.323 .134 .016 One important area for future research concerns the
Path 3: OS ? RRa 0.014 .005 .003 specific mechanisms linking social anxiety to poorer
Age ? SA -0.995 .861 .247 functioning across close relationships. From an attachment
Age ? SS -0.501 .621 .420 perspective, youth may develop cognitive expectancies for
Age ? OS -1.238 .675 .067 dyadic relationships based on their earlier close relation-
Age ? RR 0.064 .028 .021 ships (Engels et al. 2001; Hazan and Shaver 1987). Youth
Indirect Effect -0.001 .001 .005 who navigate same and other-sex friendships with some
SA ? SS ? OS ? RR [-0.004, facility may develop representations of themselves as
0.000]b,c skillful and desirable interaction partners who are capable
Girls (n = 190) of loving and worthy of being loved. Youth who struggle in
Path 1: SA ? SSa -0.301 .065 \.001 these earlier important relationships may instead develop
Path 2: SS ? OS 0.848 .226 \.001 internal representations of themselves as unworthy or
Path 3: OS ? RRa 0.014 .005 .003 unlovable and themselves as unskillful interaction partners.
Age ? SA -0.780 .708 .271 Such cognitive expectancies ultimately may guide behav-
Age ? SS 0.118 .633 .852 ior. For example, youth who fear negative evaluation and/
Age ? OS -0.955 .679 .160 or rejection may avoid entering relationships altogether as
Age ? RR 0.069 .029 .016 a means to avoid the inevitable. Alternatively, youth who
Indirect Effect -0.003 .002 .002 do enter relationships with negative preconceptions may
SA ? SS ? OS ? RR [-0.008, - engage in a host of behaviors meant to preserve the rela-
0.001]b tionship but ultimately having the opposite effect. For
Paths 4, 5, and 6 were constrained to be zero. Unstd unstandardized example, youth who have a heightened anticipation of
parameter estimates. N = 314 rejection based on prior experiences may repeatedly make
a
Parameter constrained to be equal across gender requests for assurance that they are liked or loved. Socially
b
95 % bias corrected bootstrap CIs from 5,000 bootstrap samples anxious youth may also attempt to preserve relationships
C
Software rounding limits precision and explains the inconsistency be avoiding conflict through self-silencing behaviors.
between the CI and p value Excessive reassurance seeking and self-silencing are
merely two maladaptive interpersonal processes that may
Our results suggest that social anxiety has a significant have relevance for the close relationships of socially anx-
impact on romantic relationship functioning during ado- ious youth; however, it would seem that there are many
lescence. Consistent with the proposed model, the effect possible mechanisms and likely complex interactions that
appears to be indirect. Youth with greater social anxiety might partially explain links between social anxiety and
were found to have poorer functioning in their same- and relationship functioning.
other-sex friendships, which was associated, in turn, with With respect to gender differences, the girls in our
poorer functioning in the romantic domain. Though the sample endorsed greater symptoms of social anxiety,
effect was modest, it appears that impairment in the same- greater perceived social support in their same-sex best
sex peer group may mark the initial deviation from nor- friendships, and more other-sex friends than their male
mative social development for socially anxious teens. The counterparts. Interestingly, the strength of the association
extant literature suggests that the same-sex peer network is between social anxiety and functioning across relationship
a place where teens can develop values, a sense of identity, types did not differ according to gender. This is somewhat
and the social skills necessary for success in other peer surprising, given prior research supporting stronger asso-
contexts (Furman 1999). Without these provisions, socially ciations between social anxiety and impairment in close
anxious adolescents may have difficulty in subsequent relationships for girls versus boys (e.g., La Greca and
stages of development. For example, socially anxious or Lopez 1998). One possible explanation for our failure to
withdrawn youth with smaller same-sex networks may find a difference is that our measures of social functioning
have fewer opportunities to be involved with other-sex were more broadly defined than in previous studies, which
peers and also may be less skillful at developing and have instead focused more on intimacy and companionship
maintaining higher quality friendships once they do as indicators of relationship quality. Thus, our study may

123
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:1708–1720 1717

tap into features that are featured more prominently in anxiety. Perhaps an adolescent might avoid a large social
boys’ friendships (i.e., number of friends, instrumental aid) gathering with peers in favor of spending time alone with
and as such, any potential gender differences may have his or her romantic partner, which would increase the
been cancelled out. romantic relationship quality. In this way, social anxiety
In line with expectations, the continuity effect of same- could lead to more avoidance of social situations (an
sex functioning on other-sex functioning was weaker for alternate mediator), which could lead to better romantic
boys than for girls. Although all of the boys in the present relationship quality. In doing so, this and other alternate
study have identified some romantic involvement, at this mediators that have the opposite effect would diminish the
stage in their development they may have less interest or total overall effect of social anxiety on romantic relation-
fewer opportunities to develop close other-sex friendships. ship functioning. The potential existence of such alternate
Girls, who may be more socially advanced (De Goede et al. mediators does not diminish the importance of the indirect
2009), may evidence greater continuity in functioning pathway supported by our model.
across relationship types as a function of their greater The present study provides a useful starting point for
experience and involvement with both types of relation- future research investigating the impact of social anxiety
ships. This speculation is somewhat borne out by our on the formation, quality, and maintenance of close rela-
findings that girls reported more other-sex friends in their tionships, including romantic relationships, and has
friendship networks. At present, our understanding of the important clinical implications. Our findings are consistent
developmental significance of other-sex friendships is quite with extant literature linking social anxiety to social
limited. Nevertheless, the current study suggests that these impairment during adolescence. It also extends prior
types of relationships are critical to our understanding of research by suggesting that risk factors like fewer friends
romantic relationship functioning during adolescence. and poorer quality friendships significantly contribute to
Indeed, same-sex functioning only affected romantic rela- the likelihood of success in one’s future romantic rela-
tionships through its association with other-sex relationship tionships. Problematic romantic relationships have been
functioning. associated with a host of negative sequalae, including
Finally, consistent with the results from the total sample, increased risk for depression and nonsuicidal self-injury
the indirect effect of social anxiety on romantic relation- (Adam et al. 2011; Levesque et al. 2010; Vujeva and
ship functioning was significant for both boys and girls. Furman 2011), greater engagement in risky health behav-
This suggests that, regardless of gender differences in the iors (Jones and Furman 2011) and increased risk for dating
strengths of associations between the variables, the overall violence (Kerr and Capaldi 2011). Identification of vari-
pattern of the model was consistent across gender. For both ables that likely contribute to poor relationship quality
boys and girls, social anxiety had no independent or direct across all types of relationships (e.g., limited conflict res-
influence on romantic relationship functioning; rather, it olution skills, low assertiveness, negative cognitive
exerted an influence through its effect on same-sex expectancies) and early intervention may help protect
friendship functioning, which in turn was associated with socially anxious youth from experiencing these more distal
other-sex friendship functioning, which in turn was asso- negative consequences.
ciated with functioning in romantic relationships. Although this study fills a number of important gaps in
Interestingly, and somewhat unexpectedly, social anxi- the extant literature, several limitations provide directions
ety had no total effect on romantic relationship functioning for future research. In particular, the cross-sectional nature
when we examined both the indirect and direct effects of the study precludes us from determining the direction of
together. This finding of an indirect effect with no total effects (e.g., whether social anxiety impacts friendship
effect can happen when there are other mediators (some- quality or friendship quality impacts social anxiety). Given
times ones not included in the model) that have an effect on that social anxiety concerns emerge at the same time that
the final endogenous variables (i.e., romantic relationship peers take on greater importance in the social world, it may
functioning) and those mediators’ effects go in the opposite be impossible to disentangle their likely reciprocal effects.
direction of the mediators in the model (i.e., same and Indeed, results from the very few prospective studies
opposite sex functioning; Hayes 2009). In our case, we evaluating links between social anxiety and peer relation-
suspect that there may be such a mediator, or group of ships have demonstrated that peer problems uniquely pre-
mediators, at play. For example, because we limited our dict future social anxiety (Teachman and Allen 2007;
analyses to adolescents who have a current or past romantic Tillfors et al. 2012) and that social anxiety uniquely pre-
relationship, it may be that adolescents who have overcome dicts peer problems, including poorer quality friendships
their social anxiety enough to have such a relationship may (Siegel et al., 2009; Tillfors et al. 2012; Vernberg et al.
become more invested in their romantic relationship as a 1992). More prospective designs, particularly those that
way to avoid other social situations that provoke social include romantic relationship functioning, may help to

123
1718 J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:1708–1720

determine whether the proposed effects, reversed effects, Acknowledgments KRH conceived of the study, its design and
or bidirectional effects best capture the complex interre- coordination as part of her doctoral dissertation; JF drafted the
manuscript and aided in interpretation of the data; DWN conceived of
lated nature of social relationships in adolescence. Relat- the study, participated in its design and coordination, and critically
edly, future research would benefit from expanding the age revised the manuscript for important intellectual content; AAP per-
range to include both younger and older adolescents, and formed the statistical analysis, aided in interpretation of the data and
looking at outcomes related to dating involvement (as helped to draft the manuscript; RLG: aided with statistical analysis
and interpretation, and offered critical revisions to the manuscript.
opposed to functioning in a pre-existing relationship) such
that the full trajectory could be better observed.
Characteristics of the study sample also may limit
generalizability of results in a number of important ways. References
Our participants were predominantly Caucasian, urban/
Adam, E. K., Chyu, L., Hoyt, L. T., Doane, L. D., Boisjoly, J.,
suburban, and self-identified as heterosexual. There are Duncan, G. J., & …, McDade, T. W. (2011). Adverse adolescent
known differences in the development of romantic rela- relationship histories and young adult health: Cumulative effects
tionships for each of these factors (see Atav and Spencer of loneliness, low parental support, relationship instability,
intimate partner violence, and loss. Journal of Adolescent
2002; Carver et al. 2003, Poteat 2007; Poteat et al. 2009;
Health, 49(3), 278–286.
Simon et al. 1992; Spencer and Bryant 2000). Further Adler, T. F., & Furman, W. (1988). A model for children’s relationships
research that systematically considers these characteristics and relationship dysfunctions. In S. Duck, D. F. Hay, S. E. Hobfoll,
will be necessary to better understand how, for whom, and W. Ickes, B. M. Montgomery, S. Duck, …, B. M. Montgomery
(Eds.), Handbook of personal relationships: Theory, research
under what circumstances, social anxiety impacts the
and interventions (pp. 211–229). Oxford, UK: Wiley.
development of romantic relationships for youth. Atav, S., & Spencer, G. A. (2002). Health risk behaviors among
Finally, this study relied exclusively on self-report. As adolescents attending rural, suburban, and urban schools: A
with any investigation in which the type of assessment comparative study. Family & Community Health, 25, 53–64.
Berndt, T. J. (1992). Friendship and friends’ influence in adolescence.
remains consistent across constructs, one runs the risk of
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1(5), 156–159.
common method variance. Nevertheless, we felt that self- Brown, B. (1999). ‘You’re going out with who?’ Peer group
report methodology was the most appropriate means of influences on adolescent romantic relationships. In W. Furman,
assessing symptoms of social anxiety and perceptions of B. Brown, C. Feiring, W. Furman, B. Brown, & C. Feiring
(Eds.), The development of romantic relationships in adoles-
relationship quality across domains. Assessing dating his-
cence (pp. 291–329). New York, NY, USA: Cambridge Univer-
tory, as well as the composition of peer networks during sity Press.
adolescence, has been notoriously challenging for several Carver, K., Joyner, K., & Udry, J. (2003). National estimates of
reasons. For example, national estimates of romantic rela- adolescent romantic relationships. In P. Florsheim & P. Flors-
heim (Eds.), Adolescent romantic relations and sexual behavior:
tionship prevalence indicate that slightly over half of all
Theory, research, and practical implications (pp. 23–56).
reported adolescent romantic relationships are not recip- Mahwah, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
rocally agreed upon (Carver et al. 2003), with even more Cheung, S., & McBride-Chang, C. (2011). Relations of gender,
dismal rates of agreement regarding the timing of a specific gender-related personality characteristics, and dating status to
adolescents’ cross-sex friendship quality. Sex Roles, 64(1–2),
relationship. A related challenge observed in the present
59–69.
study was that a sizable proportion of eligible participants Colarossi, L. G., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). A prospective study of
were not included because they failed to provide an esti- adolescents’ peer support: Gender differences and the influence
mate of the length of their romantic relationship. Future of parental relationships. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
29(6), 661–678.
research would benefit from the inclusion of peer report or
Collins, W. (2003). More than myth: The developmental significance
use of observational methodologies. of romantic relationships during adolescence. Journal of
The present investigation represented an initial exploration Research on Adolescence, 13(1), 1–24.
into the role of social anxiety in the development of romantic Connolly, J., Furman, W., & Konarski, R. (2000). The role of peers in
the emergence of heterosexual romantic relationships in adoles-
relationships from a developmental psychopathology per-
cence. Child Development, 71(5), 1395–1408.
spective. Overall, the findings suggest that socially evaluative Connolly, J., & Goldberg, A. (1999). Romantic relationships in
concerns impact functioning in romantic relationships adolescence: The role of friends and peers in their emergence
through their impact on broader peer functioning. Results and development. In W. Furman, B. Brown, C. Feiring,
W. Furman, B. Brown, & C. Feiring (Eds.), The development
supported a developmental sequence whereby impairment in
of romantic relationships in adolescence (pp. 266–290).
same-sex friendships predicted impairment in other-sex New York, NY, USA: Cambridge University Press.
friendships, which in turn predicted poorer functioning in the Connolly, J. A., & Johnson, A. M. (1996). Adolescents’ romantic
romantic domain; this suggests that continuity may exist for relationships and the structure and quality of their close
interpersonal ties. Personal Relationships, 3(2), 185–195.
maladaptive patterns of socialization. Additional research
Connolly, J. A., & Konarski, R. (1994). Peer self-concept in
examining the impact of social anxiety on close relationships adolescence: Analysis of factor structure and of associations
during adolescence clearly is warranted.

123
J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:1708–1720 1719

with peer experience. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 4(3), Furman, W., & Wehner, E. A. (1994). Romantic views: Toward a
385–403. theory of adolescent romantic relationships. In R. Montemayor,
Davila, J., & Beck, J. (2002). Is social anxiety associated with G. R. Adams, T. P. Gullotta, R. Montemayor, G. R. Adams, &
impairment in close relationships? A preliminary investigation. T. P. Gullotta (Eds.), Personal relationships during adolescence
Behavior Therapy, 33(3), 427–446. (pp. 168–195). Thousand Oaks, CA, USA: Sage.
De Goede, I. A., Branje, S. T., & Meeus, W. J. (2009). Developmental Garcı́a-Lopez, L., Olivares, J., Hidalgo, M., Beidel, D. C., & Turner,
changes and gender differences in adolescents’ perceptions of S. M. (2001). Psychometric properties of the social phobia and
friendships. Journal of Adolescence, 32(5), 1105–1123. anxiety inventory, the Social Anxiety Scale for Adolescents, the
Downey, G., Bonica, C., & Rincon, C. (1999). Rejection sensitivity Fear of Negative Evaluation Scale, and the Social Avoidance and
and adolescent romantic relationships. In W. Furman, B. Brown, Distress Scale in an adolescent Spanish-speaking sample.
C. Feiring, & W. Furman (Eds.), The development of romantic Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 23(1),
relationships in adolescence (pp. 148–174). New York: Cam- 51–59.
bridge University Press. Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation
Dunphy, D. C. (1963). The social structure of urban adolescent peer analysis in the new millennium. Communication Monographs,
groups. Sociometry, 26(2), 230–246. 76, 408–420.
Eaton, A., & Rose, S. (2011). Has dating become more egalitarian? Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an
A 35 year review using Sex roles. Sex Roles, 64(11–12), attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
843–862. ogy, 52(3), 511–524.
Engels, R. E., Finkenauer, C., Meeus, W., & Deković, M. (2001). Inderbitzen, H. M., Walters, K. S., & Bukowski, A. L. (1997). The
Parental attachment and adolescents’ emotional adjustment: The role of social anxiety in adolescent peer relations: Differences
associations with social skills and relational competence. among sociometric status groups and rejected subgroups.
Journal of Counseling Psychology, 48(4), 428–439. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 26(4), 338–348.
Erath, S. A., Flanagan, K. S., & Bierman, K. L. (2007). Social anxiety Inderbitzen-Nolan, H. M., & Walters, K. S. (2000). Social Anxiety
and peer relations in early adolescence: Behavioral and cognitive Scale for Adolescents: Normative data and further evidence of
factors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35(3), 405–416. construct validity. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29(3),
Erath, S. A., Flanagan, K. S., Bierman, K. L., & Tu, K. M. (2010). 360–371.
Friendships moderate psychosocial maladjustment in socially Inglés, C. J., Piqueras, J. A., Garcı́a-Fernández, J. M., Garcı́a-López,
anxious early adolescents. Journal of Applied Developmental L. J., Delgado, B., & Ruiz Esteban, C. (2011). Gender and age
Psychology, 31(1), 15–26. differences in cognitive, psychophysiological, and behavioral
Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: youth and crisis. Oxford, UK: Norton responses of social anxiety in adolescence. Psychology in Spain,
& Co. 15(1), 80–86.
Essau, C. A., Conradt, J., & Petermann, F. (1999). Frequency and Jones, M. C., & Furman, W. (2011). Representations of romantic
comorbidity of social phobia and social fears in adolescents. relationships, romantic experience, and sexual behavior in
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37(9), 831–843. adolescence. Personal Relationships, 18(1), 144–164.
Faith, M. A., Fiala, S. E., Cavell, T. A., & Hughes, J. N. (2011). Kashdan, T. B., Volkmann, J. R., Breen, W. E., & Han, S. (2007).
Mentoring highly aggressive children: Pre-post changes in Social anxiety and romantic relationships: The costs and benefits
mentors’ attitudes, personality, and attachment tendencies. The of negative emotion expression are context-dependent. Journal
Journal of Primary Prevention, 32(5–6), 253–270. of Anxiety Disorders, 21(4), 475–492.
Feiring, C. (1996). Concept of romance in 15-year-old adolescents. Kerr, D. R., & Capaldi, D. (2011). Young men’s intimate partner
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 6(2), 181–200. violence and relationship functioning: Long-term outcomes
Feiring, C. (1999). Other-sex friendship networks and the develop- associated with suicide attempt and aggression in adolescence.
ment of romantic relationships in adolescence. Journal of Youth Psychological Medicine, 41(4), 759–769.
and Adolescence, 28(4), 495–512. La Greca, A. (1998). Manual for the Social Anxiety Scales for
Flanagan, K. S., Erath, S. A., & Bierman, K. L. (2008). Unique children and adolescents. Miami, FL: University of Miami.
associations between peer relations and social anxiety in early La Greca, A. M., & Harrison, H. (2005). Adolescent peer relations,
adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child and friendships, and romantic relationships: Do they predict social
Adolescent Psychology, 37(4), 759–769. anxiety and depression? Journal of Clinical Child and
Furman, W. (1999). Friends and lovers: The role of peer relationships Adolescent Psychology, 34(1), 49–61.
in adolescent romantic relationships. In W. Collins, B. Laursen, La Greca, A. M., & Lopez, N. (1998). Social anxiety among
W. Collins, & B. Laursen (Eds.), Relationships as developmental adolescents: Linkages with peer relations and friendships.
contexts (pp. 133–154). Mahwah, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 26(2), 83–94.
Associates Publishers. La Greca, A. M., & Mackey, E. (2007). Adolescents’ anxiety in
Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1985). Children’s perceptions of the dating situations: The potential role of friends and romantic
personal relationships in their social networks. Developmental partners. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology,
Psychology, 21(6), 1016–1024. 36(4), 522–533.
Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1992). Age and sex differences in Levesque, C., Lafontaine, M., Bureau, J., Cloutier, P., & Dandurand,
perceptions of networks of personal relationships. Child Devel- C. (2010). The influence of romantic attachment and intimate
opment, 63(1), 103–115. partner violence on non-suicidal self-injury in young adults.
Furman, W., & Hand, L. (2006). The slippery nature of romantic Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 39(5), 474–483.
relationships: Issues in definition and differentiation. In Maccoby, E. E. (1990). Gender and relationships: A developmental
A. C. Crouter, A. Booth, A. C. Crouter, & A. Booth (Eds.), account. American Psychologist, 45(4), 513–520.
Romance and sex in adolescence and emerging adulthood: Risks Myers, M. G., Stein, M. B., & Aarons, G. A. (2002). Cross validation
and opportunities (pp. 171–178). Mahwah, NJ, USA: Lawrence of the Social Anxiety Scale for Adolescents in a high school
Erlbaum Associates Publishers. sample. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 16(2), 221–232.
Furman, W., & Wehner, E. A. (1992) Dating history questionnaire. Newman Kingery, J., Erdley, C. A., Marshall, K. C., Whitaker, K. G.,
Unpublished measure, University of Denver. & Reuter, T. R. (2010). Peer experiences of anxious and socially

123
1720 J Youth Adolescence (2013) 42:1708–1720

withdrawn youth: An integrative review of the developmental Spencer, G. A., & Bryant, S. A. (2000). Dating violence: A
and clinical literature. Clinical Child and Family Psychology comparison of rural, suburban, and urban teens. Journal of
Review, 13(1), 91–128. Adolescent Health, 27(5), 302–305.
Poteat, V. (2007). Peer group socialization of homophobic attitudes Starr, L. R., & Davila, J. (2008). Differentiating interpersonal
and behavior during adolescence. Child Development, 78(6), correlates of depressive symptoms and social anxiety in
1830–1842. adolescence: Implications for models of comorbidity. Journal
Poteat, V., Espelage, D. L., & Koenig, B. W. (2009). Willingness to of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 37(2), 337–349.
remain friends and attend school with lesbian and gay peers: Storch, E. A., Brassard, M. R., & Masia-Warner, C. L. (2003). The
Relational expressions of prejudice among heterosexual youth. relationship of peer victimization to social anxiety and loneliness
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38(7), 952–962. in adolescence. Child Study Journal, 33(1), 1–18.
Rao, P. A., Beidel, D. C., Turner, S. M., Ammerman, R. T., Crosby, Storch, E. A., & Masia-Warner, C. (2004). The relationship of peer
L. E., & Sallee, F. R. (2007). Social anxiety disorder in victimization to social anxiety and loneliness in adolescent
childhood and adolescence: Descriptive psychopathology. Beha- females. Journal of Adolescence, 27(3), 351–362.
viour Research and Therapy, 45(6), 1181–1191. Sullivan, H. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry. New
Rodebaugh, T. L. (2009). Social phobia and perceived friendship York, NY, USA: W. W. Norton & Co.
quality. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23(7), 872–878. Teachman, B. A., & Allen, J. P. (2007). Development of social
Roisman, G. I., Booth-LaForce, C., Cauffman, E., & Spieker, S. anxiety: Social interaction predictors of implicit and explicit fear
(2009). The developmental significance of adolescent romantic of negative evaluation. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,
relationships: Parent and peer predictors of engagement and 35(1), 63–78.
quality at age 15. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38(10), Tillfors, M., Persson, S., Willén, M., & Burk, W. J. (2012).
1294–1303. Prospective links between social anxiety and adolescent peer
Rose, A. J., & Rudolph, K. D. (2006). A review of sex differences in relations. Journal of Adolescence, 35(5), 1255–1263.
peer relationship processes: Potential trade-offs for the emotional Vernberg, E. M., Abwender, D. A., Ewell, K. K., & Beery, S. H.
and behavioral development of girls and boys. Psychological (1992). Social anxiety and peer relationships in early adoles-
Bulletin, 132(1), 98–131. cence: A prospective analysis. Journal of Clinical Child
Seiffge-Krenke, I. (2003). Testing theories of romantic development Psychology, 21(2), 189–196.
from adolescence to young adulthood: Evidence of a develop- Vujeva, H. M., & Furman, W. (2011). Depressive symptoms and
mental sequence. International Journal of Behavioral Develop- romantic relationship qualities from adolescence through emer-
ment, 27(6), 519–531. ging adulthood: A longitudinal examination of influences.
Siegel, R. S., La Greca, A. M., & Harrison, H. M. (2009). Peer Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 40(1),
victimization and social anxiety in adolescents: Prospective and 123–135.
reciprocal relationships. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, Williams, T., Connolly, J., Pepler, D., & Craig, W. (2005). Peer
38(8), 1096–1109. victimization, social support, and psychosocial adjustment of
Simon, R. W., Eder, D., & Evans, C. (1992). The development of sexual minority adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,
feeling norms underlying romantic love among adolescent 34(5), 471–482.
females. Social Psychology Quarterly, 55(1), 29–46.

123
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without
permission.

You might also like