You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Consumer Research, Inc.

Nostalgia and Consumption Preferences: Some Emerging Patterns of Consumer Tastes


Author(s): Morris B. Holbrook
Source: Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 20, No. 2 (Sep., 1993), pp. 245-256
Published by: Oxford University Press
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2489272
Accessed: 29-09-2015 01:13 UTC

REFERENCES
Linked references are available on JSTOR for this article:
http://www.jstor.org/stable/2489272?seq=1&cid=pdf-reference#references_tab_contents

You may need to log in to JSTOR to access the linked references.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/
info/about/policies/terms.jsp

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content
in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship.
For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Oxford University Press and Journal of Consumer Research, Inc. are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and
extend access to Journal of Consumer Research.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 128.6.218.72 on Tue, 29 Sep 2015 01:13:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
Nostalgia and Consumption Preferences:
Some Emerging Patterns of Consumer
Tastes

MORRIS B. HOLBROOK*

As studied in recent consumer research, the role of nostalgia involves both (1) a
link between age and the development of consumer tastes (over time) and (2) the
relationshipof nostalgia proneness (a psychographic variable)to patterns of consumer
preferences. However, questions remain concerning whether the influence of nos-
talgia proneness and age can be regarded as two independent aspects of nostalgic
consumption. This research reports a pair of studies that investigate this issue with
respect to motion picture preferences among subjects. Study 1 uses an age-ho-
mogeneous sample to examine 20 items that assess nostalgia proneness, to derive
a nostalgia scale, to form a spatial representation of movie preferences, and to test
the fit of a vector reflecting differential nostalgia-related tastes in this preference
space. Study 2 generalizes these results to a sample heterogeneous with respect
to age and shows that age and nostalgia proneness appear to work independently
as separate aspects of nostalgic preference patterns.

As a basic aspect of the human condition, the phe- to building some theoretical connections between age
nomenon of nostalgia has received attention from and the development of nostalgia-related consumer
such disciplines as history (Hobsbawm 1983; Lowenthal tastes. Most recently, Holak and Havlena (1992) ex-
1985), psychology (Fodor 1950; Nawas and Platt 1965), amine the recurring person-, object-, and event-related
sociology (Davis 1979), anthropology (McCracken themes in personal accounts of nostalgic consumption
1988), environmental psychology (Taylor and Konrad experiences; Stern (1992) applies the viewpoint of lit-
1980), and other more eclectic approaches in social sci- erary criticism to an analysis of nostalgia in advertising
ence (Campbell 1987; Csikszentmihalyi and Rochberg- as it reflects antecedents found in historical romances
Halton 1981). Further, by virtue of the potential con- and sentimental novels; and Hirsch (1992) summarizes
nection of nostalgic feelings to consumption experiences research connecting nostalgic emotions to odors via the
of many kinds, several consumer researchers have ex- sense of smell.
amined various questions related to nostalgia and to
consumption associated with the "dear departed past."
For example, Holbrook (1989) traces nostalgic themes
Defining Nostalgia
in the songs of David Frishberg. Belk (1990, 1991) deals In general, nostalgia refers to a longing for the past,
with the meanings of possessions in establishing one's a yearning for yesterday, or a fondness for possessions
"sense of past." Belk et al. (1988; 1991) apply this gen- and activities associated with days of yore. For example,
eral perspective to the specific case of collecting and Davis (1979) views "simple nostalgia" as "a positively
explore the manner in which collections of memora- toned evocation of a lived past" involving a negative
bilia, souvenirs, and other relics preserve memories that feeling toward the present or future as manifested by a
enhance people's lives. Havlena and Holak (1991) re- "belief that THINGS WERE BETTER . . . THEN
view the use of nostalgic appeals in advertising and other THAN NOW" (p. 18). Building on this view of nos-
commercial settings, while Holbrook and Schindler talgia, Holbrook and Schindler (1991) define it as "a
(1991) consider these and other phenomena en route preference (general liking, positive attitude, or favorable
affect) toward objects (people, places, or things) that
*Morris B. Holbrook is the William T. Dillard Professor of Mar- were more common (popular, fashionable, or widely
keting, Graduate School of Business, Columbia University, 504 Uris circulated) when one was younger (in early adulthood,
Hall, New York, NY 10027. The author thanks Robert M. Schindler in adolescence, in childhood, or even before birth)"
for his helpful comments on an earlier draft of this paper. He also (p. 330).
gratefully acknowledges the support of the Columbia Business School's
Faculty Research Fund.
Four aspects of the latter definition deserve emphasis.
First, preference designates the consumer's degree of
245
? 1993by JOURNALOF CONSUMERRESEARCH,Inc.* Vol. 20 o September1993
All rightsreserved.0093-5301/94/2002-0005$2.00

This content downloaded from 128.6.218.72 on Tue, 29 Sep 2015 01:13:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
246 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

liking toward various objects used in consumption. Nostalgia Proneness


Second, these objects refer to any kind of product
(broadly defined to cover both goods and services, in- In addition to these effects due to age, nostalgic phe-
cluding such aspects of popular culture as recordings, nomena may also characterize consumers of the same
movies, or other entertainment). Third, with respect to age to different degrees. In preliminary work on this
the concept more common . . . when one was younger, topic, I have argued that-even for people of compa-
differences of opinion exist on whether nostalgia per- rable ages-some individuals might show higher pro-
tains only to one's own store of remembered events pensities toward nostalgia proneness than others. From
from a "personally experienced past" (Davis 1979, p. this perspective, we may regard nostalgia proneness as
8) or reaches back historically so that it "engulfs the a potential facet of individual character-a psycho-
whole past" (Lowenthal 1985, p. 6). As indicated by its graphic variable, aspect of life-style, or general customer
inclusion of "before birth," this definition accepts a characteristic-that may vary among consumers, in-
broad interpretation of "when one was younger" and, dependent of time- or age-related factors. To explore
in that sense, favors the side of this debate espoused by the potential effects of nostalgia proneness as a psycho-
Lowenthal. Thus, while nostalgia might attach itself to graphic variable that differs among members of the same
experiences recalled from one's own youth (Davis age cohort at a moment in time, I proposed a 20-item
1979), it might also focus on the womb (Fodor 1950), index of nostalgia proneness, assessed the role of this
on objects "recalled" via collective memory from a his- nostalgia index in explaining preferences toward a large
torical era (Lowenthal 1985), or on combining ancient set of 125 products, and found encouraging reliability
materials into invented traditions that serve the needs and concurrent validity when applied to a sample ho-
of the present (Hobsbawm 1983). Fourth, this definition mogeneous with respect to age (Holbrook 1993).
focuses on the temporal orientation of one's product-
related attitudes and does not necessarily bear on the Purpose of the Study
degree of sentimentality or other bittersweet, wistful
In sum, previous research has suggested the impor-
feelings that may attend those positive affective re-
tance of two consumption-related phenomena asso-
sponses toward objects from days of yore.
ciated with nostalgia. The first concerns the role of age
Nostalgia and Age in the development of consumer tastes. The second in-
volves nostalgia proneness as a psychographic variable
It appears that the targets of nostalgia and general
that differs even among consumers from the same age
levels of nostalgic consumption experienced by different
cohort. Further work is needed, however, to refine our
consumers depend on developmental changes that oc-
measure of nostalgia proneness, to assess its reliability,
cur over time and that are therefore associated with
and to validate its ability along with age to explain pat-
age. On this theme, a general review by Davis (1979)
terns of preferences toward products that provide im-
suggests that "in Western society it is adolescence, and
portant nostalgia-related consumption experiences.
for the privileged classes early adulthood as well, that
Toward that end, this article reports two studies.
affords nostalgia its most sumptuous banquets" (p. 57);
"the tides of nostalgia which . . . wash over middle- Study 1 involves a complete reanalysis of the data col-
lected earlier on a sample of respondents homogeneous
aged persons typically carry them back to the songs,
with respect to age (Holbrook 1993). Study 2 generalizes
films, styles, and fads of their late teens" (p. 60).
the results of study 1 to a sample of respondents that
Recent empirical evidence has supported aspects of
vary over a wide range of ages so as to validate the first
this proposed link between age and nostalgic tastes. For
set of findings on an age-heterogenous sample of con-
example, Holbrook and Schindler (1989) found a non-
sumers.
monotonic relationship in which preferences for mu-
sical recordings peaked for those that had been popular
when the respondent was in the vicinity of about 23.5 METHOD
years old. This result corresponds to the conventional
wisdom that "adolescence . . . [is] the formative years
General Procedure
for music preferences" so that "the most important fac- Studies 1 and 2 used identical questionnaires to col-
tor determining people's musical tastes is their age" lect data on respondents' preferences toward 62 movies,
(Stipp 1990, p. 48). their agreement with 20 statements reflecting nostalgia
Analogous findings appeared in a study by Schuman proneness, and their demographic characteristics on
and Scott (1989). These authors examined the relation- such variables as gender and age. As already mentioned,
ship between age and the tendency toward attributing the studies differed in that the first set of respondents
importance to various historical events. They found came from the same age cohort whereas the second rep-
maximal memorability for episodes that had occurred resented a wide range of ages. Although some prelim-
during an age cohort's "teens or early 20s" and con- inary results for the first study appear elsewhere (Hol-
cluded that "memories . . . are structured by age" brook 1993), the present research entails a completely
(p. 377). new analysis of the data from study 1.

This content downloaded from 128.6.218.72 on Tue, 29 Sep 2015 01:13:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
NOSTALGIAAND CONSUMER TASTES 247

Stimulus Objects country of origin (U.S.A./other English-speaking/other


non-English-speaking). With respect to age, Davis
The questionnaire used in both studies employed a (1979) argues for a positive relationship with nostalgia
randomly ordered set of 62 movies that won Academy proneness (p. 64). Common sense might also suggest
Awards for Best Picture from 1927 to 1988 (Levy 1987). the possibility of a relationship between nostalgia
This list of movies appears in Table 1 (with dates shown proneness and gender. Therefore, in this research, both
in parentheses and with abbreviations as used in the gender and age were considered as potential correlates
figures capitalized). On the questionnaire, the 62 dated of the nostalgia scale and were tested accordingly.
films were accompanied by a nine-point numerical scale Meanwhile, country of origin was used primarily as a
intended to capture the respondent's degree of liking screening variable in a manner intended to ensure the
for each Oscar-winning movie from intense disliking homogeneity of the samples in this respect.
(1) to intense liking (9). Respondents unfamiliar with
a film were encouraged to mark the middle part of the
liking scale. This treatment of unfamiliarity as a neutral Samples
response helps to ensure that correlations of preferences Study 1. The age-homogeneous sample used in
among films are driven by associations among tastes study 1 included 226 respondents drawn from two in-
for familiar objects and not by distortions due to un- troductory classes in marketing strategy at a large grad-
familiarity. For purposes of reducing scale-response uate school of business. To encourage familiarity with
biases, because interest focused on preferences among the objects used to assess movie-related tastes, only those
the movies compared with one another, each respon- 170 respondents who named the United States as their
dent's movie ratings were normalized to sum to zero country of origin were retained in the sample. After the
(by subtracting that respondent's overall mean rating elimination of three age-atypical outliers (with ages of
across movies from each movie-preference score). 38, 39, and 52 years, respectively), the 167 remaining
respondents (72 women, 95 men) fell into the range of
Nostalgia Proneness ages between 21 and 34 years old (X = 26.4, SD = 2.44,
On the basis of the definition proposed earlier and median = 26). Analyses performed on only those 133
the conceptual considerations discussed by, among respondents from 24 to 28 years of age produced vir-
others, Davis (1979), Fodor (1950), Lowenthal (1985), tually identical results.
and Nawas and Platt (1965), 20 statements were de- Study 2. The age-heterogeneous sample used in
signed to represent the phenomenon of nostalgia study 2 was collected from members of the previously
proneness. These statements-ordered randomly with mentioned marketing classes during a subsequent se-
10 worded as high and 10 as low in nostalgia prone-
mester. Each student was asked to gather data from two
ness-appear in the Appendix. On the questionnaire, respondents, subject to the constraints (1) that the re-
the 20 items were accompanied by nine-point numerical spondents must not be present or former students of
scales defined for the respondent as indicating degree the school (to avoid biases due to participation in a
of agreement ranging from strong disagreement (1) to small circle of acquaintances), (2) that they must be
strong agreement (9). These raw numerical scores were native, English-speaking Americans (to ensure general
normalized for each respondent separately by subtract- familiarity with the movies used in the study), (3) that
ing their mean (so as to reduce the effects of scale- they must be more than 18 years old and should differ
response biases), were reversed in direction where ap- in age by at least 20-30 years (to encourage a wide spec-
propriate (for scale numbers 2, 3, 5, 8, 9, 12, 14, 15, trum of ages covered by this study), and (4) that they
18, and 20), and were standardized across respondents must not be apprised of any speculations concerning
(to a mean of 0.0 and an SD of 1.0). the purpose of the study (although the students them-
In study 1, a series of exploratory maximum-likeli-
selves were not informed about its rationale until after
hood factor analyses produced a set of items that showed the data had been collected). This procedure produced
an acceptable chi-square fit to a single-factor model. a set of 189 questionnaires. Of these, 33 were eliminated
Scores for this factor were then regarded as a measure because of missing data or a failure to indicate "U.S.A."
of nostalgia proneness-hereafter referred to as the as the country of origin. This left a sample of 156 re-
nostalgia scale-for use in subsequent parts of the anal- spondents (94 women, 62 men), who ranged in age from
ysis. However, because the derivation of this scale in- 21 to 85 years old (X = 41.7, SD = 15.9, 37 < median
troduced search bias into -the first study, a confirmatory < 38).
maximum-likelihood factor analysis was then per-
formed on these same items in study 2 to assess the
generalizability of the results from study 1. Analyses
Analyses of the relationships in question were con-
Demographics ducted primarily by means of principal components (to
At the conclusion of the questionnaire, respondents obtain spatial representations of preference patterns for
indicated their gender (male/female), age (in years), and movies), maximum-likelihood factor analysis (to assess

This content downloaded from 128.6.218.72 on Tue, 29 Sep 2015 01:13:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
TABLE 1

LOADINGS FOR 62 MOVIES ON THE FIRST TWO PRINCIPAL COMPONENTS IN THE PREFERENCE SPACE: STUDIES 1 AND 2

Study 1 Study 2

Movie title, date Dimension 1 Dimension 2 Dimension 1 Dimension 2

Oliver!, 1968 (OLIVER) -.30 -.32 -.05 -.28


Around the World in 80 Days, 1956 (AROUND) -.08 -.11 .02 -.05
The Sting, 1973 (STING) -.42 .27 -.43 .25
You Can't Take It with You, 1938 (CAN'T) .42 .01 .26 .12
Ben Hur, 1959 (BEN) .01 .16 -.14 .27
Lawrence of Arabia, 1962 (LAWRENCE) .02 -.08 .00 .28
Going My Way, 1944 (GOING) .39 -.04 .33 .20
The Last Emperor, 1987 (EMPEROR) -.26 -.22 -.10 -.13
Midnight Cowboy, 1969 (COWBOY) .09 .17 -.15 -.04
Amadeus, 1984 (AMADEUS) -.29 -.15 -.16 -.36
Hamlet, 1948 (HAMLET) .08 -.02 .22 -.06
West Side Story, 1961 (WEST) -.25 -.19 -.15 -.49
The Broadway Melody, 1929 (MELODY) .23 -.07 .14 .10
Mutiny on the Bounty, 1935 (MUTINY) .12 .24 .11 .32
Ordinary People, 1980 (ORDINARY) -.30 -.09 -.23 -.39
Gone with the Wind, 1939 (GONE) -.27 -.35 .06 -.22
My Fair Lady, 1964 (FAIR) -.23 -.50 .35 -.54
Seventh Heaven, 1928 (SEVENTH) .53 -.00 .26 .14
Grand Hotel, 1932 (HOTEL) .46 -.03 .44 .01
It Happened One Night, 1934 (HAPPENED) .40 -.20 .39 .05
The Godfather, Part iI, 1974 (GODFATHER2) -.24 .52 -.53 .30
Gentleman's Agreement, 1947 (AGREEMENT) .46 .10 .31 .14
Terms of Endearment, 1983 (ENDEARMENT) -.53 -.10 -.32 -.47
Out of Africa, 1985 (OUT) -.46 -.48 -.08 -.52
Marty, 1955 (MARTY) .41 .31 .16 .08
The Bridge on the River Kwai, 1957 (BRIDGE) .00 .42 .29 .53
How Green Was My Valley, 1941 (GREEN) .50 -.31 .58 .27
A Man for All Seasons, 1966 (MAN) .37 -.06 .43 .06
The French Connection, 1971 (FRENCH) -.22 .50 -.46 .43
The Deer Hunter, 1978 (DEER) -.21 .69 -.57 .43
The Greatest Show on Earth, 1952 (SHOW) .33 .28 -.10 .37
Gandhi, 1982 (GANDHI) -.30 -.24 -.11 -.17
The Life of Emile Zola, 1937 (ZOLA) .45 .00 .34 .14
One Flew Over the Cuckoo's Nest, 1975 (FLEW) -.22 .44 -.34 -.12
Kramer vs. Kramer, 1979 (KRAMER) -.47 -.06 -.35 -.48
Gigi, 1958 (GIGI) -.00 -.44 .42 -.34
Tom Jones, 1963 (JONES) .22 .11 .19 -.12
From Here to Eternity, 1953 (ETERNITY) -.06 -.16 18 -.05
All About Eve, 1950 (EVE) .24 -.28 .23 -.24
The Godfather, 1972 (GODFATHER1) -.19 .64 -.58 .22
The Lost Weekend, 1945 (WEEKEND) .59 -.00 .35 .20
Mrs. Miniver, 1942 (MINIVER) .54 -.15 .50 .15
Cimarron, 1931 (CIMARRON) .61 .04 .12 .39
Cavalcade, 1933 (CAVALCADE) .68 .04 .00 .35
In the Heat of the Night, 1967 (HEAT) .23 .07 -.17 .17
Platoon, 1986 (PLATOON) -.34 .41 -.54 .23
Casablanca, 1943 (CASABLANCA) -.09 -.16 -.11 .02
The Best Years of Our Lives, 1946 (YEARS) .36 -.03 .35 -.10
Rain Man, 1988 (RAIN) -.32 -.15 -.46 -.39
Wings, 1927 (WINGS) .47 -.00 .17 .16
An American in Paris, 1951 (AMERICAN) -.15 -.46 .39 -.39
Chariots of Fire, 1981 (CHARIOTS) -.24 -.26 .18 -.18
The Great Ziegfeld, 1936 (ZIEGFELD) .31 .05 .31 .08
On the Waterfront, 1954 (WATERFRONT) .11 .07 .07 .01
All Quiet on the Western Front, 1930 (FRONT) .23 .06 .35 .42
The Sound of Music, 1965 (SOUND) -.25 -.47 -.06 -.40
Patton, 1970 (PATTON) .02 .51 -.06 .50
Rocky, 1976 (ROCKY) -.41 .40 -.61 .17
The Apartment, 1960 (APARTMENT) .49 .00 .07 .04
All the King's Men, 1949 (MEN) .32 -.12 .10 .15
Rebecca, 1940 (REBECCA) .36 -.31 .43 -.34
Annie Hall, 1977 (ANNIE) -.09 -.09 -.26 -.32

NOTE.-Abbreviations are indicated with capital letters in parentheses.

This content downloaded from 128.6.218.72 on Tue, 29 Sep 2015 01:13:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
NOSTALGIA AND CONSUMER TASTES 249

TABLE 2
FACTOR ANALYSES
LOADINGSAND z-VALUES FOR EXPLORATORYAND CONFIRMATORYMAXIMUM-LIKELIHOOD
OF EIGHTNOSTALGIAITEMS IN STUDIES 1 AND 2

Study 1 (exploratory) Study 2 (confirmatory)

Factor Factor
Nostalgia item loading z-Value loading z-Value

(1) They don't make 'em like they used to .49 6.0 .54 6.2
(4) Things used to be better in the good old days .47 5.8 .50 5.8
(7) Products are getting shoddier and shoddier .52 6.5 .54 6.3
(9) Technological change will insure a brighter future [reversed] .76 10.3 .60 7.0
(14) History involves a steady improvement in human welfare
[reversed] .48 5.9 .43 4.8
(17) We are experiencing a decline in the quality of life .50 6.2 .61 7.2
(18) Steady growth in GNP has brought increased human happiness
[reversed] .58 7.4 .34 3.8
(20) Modern business constantly builds a better tomorrow
[reversed] .62 7.9 .45 5.1

the unidimensionality of the nostalgia scale), and or- reliability coefficient of only 0.43 (Bentler 1989, p. 114;
dinary least squares regressions (to insert attribute-, Marsh, Balla, and McDonald 1988, p. 393; Tucker and
gender-, age-, and nostalgia-related vectors into the Lewis 1973, p. 5).
preference spaces). Specifically, in the latter connection, Accordingly, a stepwise search was conducted to
relative scores for the correlate of interest (e.g., an at- eliminate additional nostalgia items until the maxi-
tribute or differential preferences associated with gen- mum-likelihood factor analysis no longer rejected the
der, age, or nostalgia) were regressed on spatial coor- null hypothesis of fit for the single-factor model. This
dinates (across movies in the preference space) with the occurred for an analysis involving the eight nostalgia
direction of the resulting vector obtained by plotting items whose factor loadings and z-values appear in Ta-
an arrow from the origin through the coordinates given ble 2. Here, the smallest z-value is 5.8, indicating that
by the relevant regression coefficients and with the all factor loadings are significant at well beyond p
length of the vector equal to its correlational fit (Carroll < .000001. For this set of items, the single-factor model
1972; Holbrook and Holloway 1984). Those objects that was not rejected at p < .10: X2 = 27.8 (df = 20, p = .11)
project farther toward the head (tail) of a vector tend with a Tucker-Lewis reliability coefficient of 0.96. The
to be higher (lower) on the attribute of interest or stron- construct reliability of the factor and coefficient alpha
ger (weaker) in terms of relative preferences associated for the corresponding summative index were both 0.78.
with the relevant general customer characteristic (gen- The nostalgia scale was therefore represented in the re-
der, age, nostalgia, etc.). Further tests were conducted maining analyses by factor scores for the eight items in
to see whether fits could be increased by including extra Table 2.
quadratic terms to represent an "ideal point" model;
however, such refinements did not significantly improve Gender, Age, and the Nostalgia Scale
over the performance of the vector models and will not
be discussed further here. On the basis of the nostalgia scale just described,
women appeared to be marginally more nostalgia-prone
than men (r = -0.15, p = .05). However, no association
RESULTS: STUDY 1 appeared between the nostalgia scale and age (r = -0.04,
The Nostalgia Scale NS). Further, shared variance with nostalgia proneness
was extremely low for both gender (r2 = 0.02) and age
To perform confirmatory maximum-likelihood factor (r2 = 0.001). Hence, it seems safe to conclude that no
analysis on the nostalgia items, it was necessary to omit confounding occurred between nostalgia proneness and
one item so as to avoid perfect linear dependence among these other demographic variables in this study.
the normalized scores. For this purpose, item 2 was
omitted because it had the smallest item-total correla- Preference Space for Movies
tion and the weakest loading on the first principal com-
ponent. Confirmatory maximum-likelihood factor Principal components analysis of preference scores
analysis on the remaining 19 items produced rather (normalized within each of the 167 respondents) for
disappointing results for a single-factor model-x2 the 62 movies described earlier was used to construct
= 571.4 (df = 152, p < .00001) and a Tucker-Lewis a spatial representation of the intercorrelations among

This content downloaded from 128.6.218.72 on Tue, 29 Sep 2015 01:13:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
250 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

film preferences as a criterion for showing how patterns The Bridge on the River Kwai, The French Connection,
of cinematic tastes are related to gender, age, and nos- The Deer Hunter, The Godfather, Platoon, Wings, All
talgia proneness. This analysis produced an elbow in Quiet on the Western Front, Patton, and Rocky; those
eigenvalues after the second (or fourth) component, as scored as musical (+ 1) included Oliver!, Amadeus, West
follows: 6.99, 4.81, 3.53, 3.38, 2.25, 2.11, 1.89, and so Side Story, The Broadway Melody, My Fair Lady, Gigi,
on. Because only the first two dimensions appeared An American in Paris, and The Sound of Music. Vectors
clearly interpretable, further analyses were based on representing date and musical (vs. violent) content ap-
these first two principal components. pear in Figure 1 (with their lengths equal to their cor-
Table 1 presents loadings on these two principal relational fits).
components for the 62 movies (shown in the random- First, across the 62 movies, the date vector moved
ized order in which they appeared on the questionnaire). strongly to the left (r = -0.79, p < .0001) and only
However, a more vivid representation of the preference slightly upward (r = 0.14, NS) with an overall multiple
space for movies appears in Figure 1, where the posi- regression fit of R = 0.81 (F(2,59) = 54.6, p < .0001).
tions of the films are plotted according to their loadings This suggests a strong relation between the horizontal
on the first principal component (the horizontal di- axis and the date when a movie appeared and thereby
mension) and the second principal component (the supports the interpretation of this horizontal dimension
vertical dimension), respectively. Here we observe a suggested earlier.
tendency for the more recent films (Terms of Endear- Second, across the 62 movies, the musical vector hugs
ment, Kramer vs. Kramer, Out of Africa) to lie toward the vertical axis (r = -0.70, p < .0001) with a very
the left at the negative end of the horizontal axis and slight drift to the left (r = -0.01, NS) and an overall
for the earlier films (Cavalcade, Cimarron, The Lost multiple regression fit of R = 0.70 (F(2,59) = 28.8, p
Weekend) to lie farther to the right at the positive end. < .0001). The musicals tend to project toward the head
Meanwhile, as represented by the vertical axis, the sec- of this vector near the bottom of the space, while the
ond principal component appears to distinguish be- war movies and other violent films tend to project to-
tween more tender or musical movies at the bottom in ward the vector's tail at the top of the space. Once more,
the negative direction (My Fair Lady, The Sound of the close association of this musical vector with the ver-
Music, An American in Paris, Gigi) as opposed to more tical dimension supports the interpretation proposed
violent or military films at the top in the positive di- earlier.
rection (The Deer Hunter, The Godfather, The God-
father, Part II, Patton). Gender-, Age-, and Nostalgia-related
Preference Vectors
Attribute Vectors
Vectors representing the differential preferences as-
It would, of course, be possible to question this in- sociated with gender, age, and nostalgia proneness ap-
terpretation of the preference space for movies on the pear in Figure 1 with lengths proportional to their cor-
grounds that not all of the films included occupy the relational fits.
spatial positions that logic might dictate. For example, The gender-related vector adheres closely to the ver-
one might notice that a very old film like Wings (1927) tical axis, with a regression fit of R = 0.94 (F(2,59)
and a recent motion picture like Rain Man (1988) are = 239.8, p < .000 1). It appears that men tend relatively
not positioned as far to the right and left of the hori- to prefer the more tough-minded and violent movies
zontal dimension as one might expect. Similarly, a mu- located toward the top of the preference space. By con-
sical like The Broadway Melody and a violent movie trast, women seem to feel a stronger relative preference
like Ben Hur do not lie at the low or high extremes of for the more tenderhearted and musical films at the
the vertical dimension. Here, however, the point is not bottom of the spatial representation.
to argue that the spatial representation is perfect but For this sample of respondents from the same age
rather that it is consistent with the interpretation sug- cohort, the age-related vector showed only a weak and
gested. Accordingly, the aforementioned interpretation statistically insignificant fit to the preference space for
was tested by inserting attribute vectors to represent (1) movies: R = 0. 18 (F(2,59) = 1.0, NS). This finding does
the date of a movie's release (as indicated by the year not invite an interpretation other than to note that such
in which it won its Oscar for Best Picture) and (2) nonsignificance would be expected because of the age-
whether the content of a film is primarily violent, neu- homogeneous nature of the sample.
tral, or musical (coded -1/0/+1 according to whether The nostalgia vector points downward and slightly
it features themes related to war, guns, or physical to the left in the preference space for movies, with a
aggression, as opposed to singing, dancing, or instru- moderately strong overall fit of R = 0.51 (F(2,59)
mental performances). In the latter connection-using = 10.2, p = .0002). Here, it appears that those higher
a coding scheme explained and justified elsewhere in nostalgia proneness tend relatively to prefer more
(Holbrook 1993)-films scored as violent (-1) were Ben musical or tenderhearted films (OUT, FAIR, SOUND,
Hur, Lawrence of Arabia, The Godfather, Part II, AMERICAN, GIGI). By contrast, those lower in nos-

This content downloaded from 128.6.218.72 on Tue, 29 Sep 2015 01:13:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
NOSTALGIA AND CONSUMER TASTES 251

FIGURE 1
PREFERENCESPACE FOR MOVIESWITHATTRIBUTE-,GENDER, AGE-, AND NOSTALGIA-RELATED
VECTORS: STUDY 1
Dim. 2
Dt Men (R=0.94)
DEER

GODFATHER1

GODFATHER2
FRENCH
U
PTO
PATTON

FLEW -
ROCKYPLATOON BR1IGE

MARTY
STING SHOW
(R=O81) ~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~MUTINY
te|Age )
(R=0.81 (R=0. 18)
I EN COWBOY

JONES AGREEMENT

!'l
WATERfRONT HEAT
FRONTZIEGFELD CAVALCADE
CIMARRON

CAN'T APARTMENT
-ZOLA------SEVENTH---------WEEKEND--- -;--------------------------------------ZOLA SEVENTH--WEEKEND--------
HAMLET YEARS WINGS Dim- 1
GOING HOTEL

ENDEARMENT ~ORDINARY
ORDINARY ANNIE LA RENCE MLD A
ENDEARMENT AROUND/ IME
AMADEUS |
ETERNITY MINIVER
RAIN CASABLANCA I
WEST I | HAPPENED
EMPEROR
GANDHI
CHARIOTS
EVE
OLIVER REBECCA GREEN
GONE

AMERIA IGI?I

OUT SOUND AMERICA


FAIR
Nostalgia
(R=0.51) 4

Musicat (R=0.70)

talgia proneness display a relative preference for more mensions is supported by well-fitting attribute vectors
violent movies (DEER, GODFATHER 1, PATTON, based on independent observations of time period and
GODFATHER2, FRENCH). movie content. Fifth, these dimensions are related in a
reasonable way to differential preferences associated
with gender and nostalgia proneness such that women
Implications and those high in nostalgia proneness tend relatively to
The results from study 1 go some way toward sup- prefer more tenderhearted and musical films, while men
porting the general perspective pursued in the present and those low in nostalgia proneness tend to show a
research. First, a reduced set of eight nostalgia-related differential preference for motion pictures that depict
items appears to possess satisfactory reliability and uni- violence and warfare.
dimensionality as an index of nostalgia proneness. Sec- However, these five implications of study 1 leave cer-
ond, this nostalgia scale shows little or no redundancy tain questions unanswered. First, because the reduced
with gender or age. Third, preferences toward motion set of eight items derived in this first study may depend
pictures appear to hang together in a manner partially in part on search bias, it remains to be seen whether
accounted for by two dimensions that make sense in- they will show satisfactory reliability and unidimen-
tuitively. Fourth, the intuitive interpretation of the di- sionality when tested on a new sample. Second, because

This content downloaded from 128.6.218.72 on Tue, 29 Sep 2015 01:13:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
252 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

the age-homogeneous nature of the first sample left little dimensions appeared clearly interpretable. For these
scope for statistical association between age and the reasons and to preserve comparability with the spatial
nostalgia scale, the apparent independence of these two representation described in the first study, further anal-
constructs requires further assessment on a second yses were based on these first two principal components.
sample of respondents who show a greater range in ages. Table 1 presents the loadings for the 62 movies on
Third and fourth, the apparently reasonable spatial these two principal components. Again, as shown in
representation derived for the 62 films needs checking Figure 2, interpretation is facilitated by plotting the
for reliability and validity. Fifth, the conclusion that loadings of the movies on the first (horizontal) and sec-
gender and nostalgia proneness are meaningfully related ond (vertical) dimensions. This spatial representation
to movie preferences awaits further exploration on a again suggests a tendency for more recent films (Rocky,
sample of respondents who do show heterogeneity with The Godfather, The Deer Hunter, Platoon) to load
respect to age. The analyses conducted in study 2 were heavily in the negative direction to the left on dimension
intended to address these questions posed by study 1. 1 and for earlier films (How Green Was My Valley, Mrs.
Miniver, Grand Hotel, Rebecca) to load heavily in the
positive direction to the right. Meanwhile, dimension
RESULTS: STUDY 2 2 again appears to distinguish vertically between more
The Nostalgia Scale tender or musical movies in the negative direction at
the bottom (My Fair Lady, Out of Africa, Kramervs.
A confirmatory maximum-likelihood factor analysis Kramer, WestSide Story, Terms of Endearment)and
based on the eight nostalgia items selected in the first more violent or military films in the positive direction
study produced the factor loadings and z-values shown at the top (The Bridge on the River Kwai, Patton, The
on the right-hand side of Table 2. For study 2, the Deer Hunter, The FrenchConnection,All Quiet on the
smallest z-value is 3.8, indicating that all loadings are WesternFront).
statistically significant at p < .0001 or better. The test A central question concerns the degree to which the
of fit suggests that the single-factor model would be re- spatial representation from this second study matches
jected at the .05 but not at the .01 level of statistical that derived earlier from the first set of data. On the
significance: X2 = 32.1 (df = 20, p = .04). Further, the basis of the similar interpretations offered for the hor-
Tucker-Lewis reliability coefficient appears reassuring izontal and vertical axes, it appears that the correspon-
at 0.90 (somewhat better than the value of about 0.86 dence is quite close. This impression was confirmed by
obtained by Marsh et al. [ 1988] for "well-defined" data). regressing the spatial positions on each dimension of
The construct reliability of the factor and coefficient each space on those for the two dimensions of the other
alpha for the corresponding summative index were both spatial representation to determine the overall shared
0.73. Accordingly, factor scores on this nostalgia scale variance in spatial positions between the two preference
were used as a measure of nostalgia proneness for the spaces. Here, the root mean squared multiple correla-
remainder of the analyses presented here. tion between the preference spaces from studies 1 and
2 (the same in either direction) was 0.85. This indicates
Gender, Age, and the Nostalgia Scale a good fit between the spatial representations found in
the two studies and therefore a satisfactory degree of
As in the first study, women scored somewhat higher reliability for conclusions based on interpretations of
than men on the nostalgia scale (r = -0.21, p = .01). the horizontal and vertical dimensions.
Again, however, no significant correlation appeared be-
tween age and nostalgia (r =-0.1 1, NS). Further, shared AttributeVectors
variance between nostalgia and gender (r2 = 0.04) or This point concerning the reliability of the interpre-
age (r2 = 0.01) was small enough in both cases to in- tation was further supported by evidence for its validity
dicate that no serious confounding or redundancy ap- in the form of attribute vectors for date and musical
peared between these three general consumer charac- content (using the measures described earlier). The rel-
teristics. Most importantly, this statistical independence evant attribute vectors appear in Figure 2.
suggests that nostalgia proneness and age are separate First, the date vector points strongly to the left
constructs, even when the latter varies across a wide (r = -0.7 1, p < .0001) and somewhat downward
range of heterogeneity. (r =-0.35, p < .005) with an overall multiple regression
fit of R = 0.79 (F(2,59) = 49.1, p < .0001). The more
PreferenceSpace for Movies recent films tend to project toward the head of this vec-
tor (RAIN, KRAMER, ENDEARMENT), while the
A principal components analysis of relative liking older movies tend to project toward its tail (GREEN,
scores for the 62 movies across 156 respondents pro- BRIDGE, FRONT, MINIVER). Hence, the strong as-
duced an elbow in eigenvalues after the second com- sociation between the horizontal axis and the date-re-
ponent, according to the following sequence: 5.94, 5.03, lated attribute vector again supports the interpretation
3.95, 3.32, 2.84, 2.59, 2.03, and so forth. The first two of this horizontal dimension suggested earlier.

This content downloaded from 128.6.218.72 on Tue, 29 Sep 2015 01:13:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
NOSTALGIA AND CONSUMER TASTES 253

FIGURE2
PREFERENCESPACE FOR MOVIESWITHATTRIBUTE-,GENDER-, AGE-, AND NOSTALGIA-RELATED
VECTORS: STUDY 2
Dim. 2
Men (R=0.92) T t

\ \\ | BRIDGE
PATTON

DEER FRENCH FRONT

\ \ \ ? CIMARRON
SHOW I
CAVALCADE
MUTINY
GODFATHER2
BEN LAWRENCE GREEN
STING
PLATOON
GODFATHER1 GOING

ROCKY HEAT \:\


I ~~~~~~~WEEKEND
I MEN WINGS AGREEMENT MINIVER (R=D.86)

MELODY Age
MARTY ZIEGFELD MAN

APARTMENT HAPPENED
CASABLANCA \ WATERFRONT HOTEL

COWBO A OUND ETERNITY


~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~D
1
HAMLET
YEARS
FLEW JONES
EMPEROR i

/ C~~~~ANDHI i \\
CHARIOTS

GON
EVE
/ ~~~~~~~~
~~~OLIVER
| \

/ ~~ANNIE ~
~~~~ ~ \\
|
\GI GIGI
\\ \
REBECCA
AMADEUS

RAI N ORDINARY AMERICAN


Date
Date SOUND

(R=0.79)
ENDEARMENT
KRAMER WEST \
OUT
FAIR

Nostalgia
(R=0.58)

Musical (R=0.75)

Second, the musical vector lies close to the vertical with a regression fit of R = 0.92 (F(2,59) = 167.2, p
axis (r = -0.66, p < .0001) with a drift toward the right < .0001). It appears that men again tend relatively to
(r = 0.34, p = .006) and an overall multiple regression prefer the more violent movies at the top of the pref-
fit of R = 0.75 (F(2,59) = 37.2,p < .0001). The musicals erence space, whereas women again show a relative
fall toward the head of this vector near the bottom right liking for the more tenderhearted and musical films
of the space (FAIR, AMERICAN, GIGI, WEST, at the bottom.
SOUND), while the war movies and other violent films This time, by virtue of the age-heterogeneous nature
lie at the vector's tail near the top left of the space of the sample, the age-related vector does show a strong
(DEER, FRENCH, GODFATHER2, PLATOON, and highly significant fit with the spatial representation:
GODFATHER1). Hence, the direction and fit of this R = 0.86 (F(2,59) = 81.4, p < .0001). As shown in
musical vector once more support the earlier interpre- Figure 2, this age vector lies close to the horizontal axis
tation. and suggests that older respondents express relative
preferences for the movies of earlier vintage located to-
Gender-, Age-, and Nostalgia-related ward the right of the space (GREEN, MINIVER,
Preference Vectors HOTEL), whereas younger respondents tend relatively
As further shown in Figure 2, the gender-related to prefer the more recent films positioned toward the
vector again adheres closely to the vertical axis, left side (ROCKY, GODFATHER1, DEER).

This content downloaded from 128.6.218.72 on Tue, 29 Sep 2015 01:13:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
254 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Finally,the nostalgiavector again shows a close cor- past and the sort of wistful sentimentality that causes
respondence with the vertical axis generally and the some people to shed a tear or to overflow with gushing
musicalvector in particularand achieves a moderately emotions when they reminisce about products from the
strong degree of fit: R = 0.58 (F(2,59) = 15.0,p <.0001). good old days. Further, this study has subsumed a va-
This again indicates that those higher in nostalgia riety of consumption-related experiences under the
proneness tend relatively to prefer musicals and ten- general heading of nostalgia. These would include not
derhearted films (FAIR, AMERICAN, REBECCA, only a liking for older products (e.g., film classics) but
GIGI, OUT). Conversely, those lower in nostal- also an attachment to sentimental offerings (e.g., movie
gia proneness again show a relative preference for musicals). Here, we may have neglected complications
violent and militaristic movies (DEER, FRENCH, that might arise in the form of "mixed cases." For ex-
GODFATHER2,PATTON).Hence, the findingsfrom ample, it may be nostalgic to like Rebecca better than
study2 supportthe implicationfrom study 1 that those The Deer Hunter; but it would be even more nostalgic
higherin nostalgiapronenessdisplaya differentialpref- to prefer the Hitchcock original in black-and-white and
erence for motion pictures with more sentiment and to abhor a colorized version. Moreover, nostalgia as-
less violence or more music and less warfare. sociated with a particular era may be highly selective
in its focus. For example, one could fondly recall Marty
from 1955 while disliking Around the World in 80 Days
DISCUSSION from 1956. Also, one's nostalgic feelings could be con-
Limitations flated with certain negative aspects-as when remi-
niscing with ambivalence about an old romantic co-
As is common in researchon consumerbehavior,the medy that was well acted but tedious or a thriller that
findings reported in this study are subject to certain was exciting but conducive to bad dreams. Again, all
important limitations and caveats. For example, dif- these issues await further exploration in future con-
ferentresultsmightbe obtainedwith differentproducts sumer research on nostalgia.
(e.g., household nondurables), with different movies
(box-officehits or art films), with a differentmeasure Conclusions
of nostalgiaproneness(depth interviewsor some other Subject to these limitations, the findings appear to
standardizedtest), with more narrowlydefinedage co- offer clear support for the importance of nostalgia as a
horts (young adults, senior citizens, or baby boomers), phenomenon that affects the preference patterns of
with respondentsfrom differentsocioeconomic strata consumers who do (study 2) or do not (study 1) vary
(the homeless or blue-collar workers), with a sample widely in age but who differ (both studies) in their levels
more representativeof the U.S. population (a national of nostalgia proneness. Further, in connection with age,
probabilitysample), or with people from differentcul- nostalgia proneness, and time-related taste preferences
tures(developingcountriesor ethnic minorities).These (in this case, for movies), the research suggests answers
possibilities deserve investigation in future consumer to five important questions addressed by the convergent
researchon age, nostalgiaproneness, and related phe- results for studies 1 and 2.
nomena. First, an eight-item nostalgia scale based on an ex-
Other important questions that must await further ploratory maximum-likelihood factor analysis con-
research concern comparisons of the nostalgia scale ducted in study 1 shows an acceptable fit when validated
proposedhere with two alternativemeasuresthat have on an independent sample in study 2. This robustness
come to light subsequentto collecting the data used in of the results for the nostalgia scale-based on confir-
these studies.Specifically,relevantmeasuresdeveloped matory factor analysis of the eight statements listed in
by two environmentalpsychologistsassess "antiquari- Table 2-suggests its potential usefulness in further re-
anism" (McKechnie 1974) and "appreciationof direct search comparing it to possible alternative measures of
experiencewith the past" (Taylor and Konrad 1980). nostalgia proneness.
Questions therefore arise concerning how these mea- Second, although both age (as a chronological vari-
sures relate to the present nostalgia scale in terms of able) and nostalgia proneness (as an individual char-
such issues as convergent/discriminantvalidity, con- acteristic) are logically connected to nostalgia-related
currentvalidity with respectto consumer preferences, preferences, the evidence suggests that they are not con-
and predictivevalidity in connection with future con- founded in such a way as to be redundant. Thus,
sumption behavior. whether the sample is homogeneous or heterogeneous
We should also acknowledgethat the treatment of with respect to age, only trivial variance is shared be-
nostalgia pursued here (in connection with age and tween age and nostalgia proneness. This suggests that
nostalgiaproneness)may have been too coarsegrained the two measures represent different constructs and that
to capturecertaincomplexitiesand subtletiesthat might an individual propensity toward nostalgia proneness
operate at a more fine-tuned level of detail. Thus, as operates independently of the aging process.
noted earlier,our definition of nostalgiadoes not dis- Third, preference spaces for a set of 62 Oscar-winning
tinguishbetweena dry-eyedliking for objects from the movies are reliable between the two studies and appear

This content downloaded from 128.6.218.72 on Tue, 29 Sep 2015 01:13:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
NOSTALGIAAND CONSUMER TASTES 255

to represent valid aspects of nostalgia-related taste pref- preferences-namely, factors related to age (a demo-
erences. Thus, one dimension reflects the era in which graphic variable) and those linked to nostalgia prone-
a movie was produced (older vs. newer), while a second ness (a psychographic characteristic). In essence, two
captures the difference between tenderhearted and vi- studies have shown in different ways that-for samples
olent films (musicals vs. war stories). In terms of face both homogeneous and heterogeneous with respect to
validity, these taste clusters appear to be plausibly re- age-the effects of age and nostalgia proneness appear
lated to factors associated with age and nostalgia prone- to operate independently in shaping consumer prefer-
ness. ences. Specifically, consumers higher in nostalgia
Fourth, the interpretation of the spatial dimensions proneness tend relatively to like more tenderhearted or
just suggested receives convincing support from attri- musical films in both studies; whereas, when age varies,
bute vectors constructed in both studies 1 and 2 to rep- older consumers tend to favor earlier movies. More
resent the date when a movie won its Oscar and the generally, therefore, it appears that nostalgia (age and
musical (vs. violent) nature of its content. Although nostalgia proneness working together) does play a role
different dimensions would of course be needed to de- in shaping consumption preferences by influencing
scribe the structures of preferences in other product patterns of consumer tastes.
categories, these findings for movies do support the face
validity of a connection between consumer tastes and APPENDIX
nostalgia in the area of popular entertainment.
Fifth, the spatial representations just described appear
Twenty Statements Representing Nostalgia
to reflect strong gender-, age-, and nostalgia-related Proneness
preference patterns consistent with a general orientation 1. They don't make 'em like they used to.
toward the role of nostalgia in the consumption expe- 2. Newer is almost always better [reversed].
rience. Specifically, women express relative preferences 3. In the future, people will have even better lives [re-
for musical movies, whereas men tend relatively to pre- versed].
fer more violent films. This finding may reflect an im- 4. Things used to be better in the good old days.
portant gender-related difference in cultural tastes 5. I believe in the constant march of progress [re-
(Joyrich 1990; Morley 1986; Press 1991, pp. 144, 159), versed].
but it is not directly relevant to the key aspects of nos- 6. Yesterday, all my troubles seemed so far away.
talgia (age and nostalgia proneness) emphasized here. 7. Products are getting shoddier and shoddier.
With respect to age-a variable held constant and 8. Compared to our parents, we've got it good [re-
therefore not a meaningful source of variation in study versed].
1-older versus younger respondents tend relatively to 9. Technological change will insure a brighter future
prefer earlier as opposed to more recent films as indi- [reversed].
cated by the findings in study 2. Meanwhile, with respect 10. When I was younger, I was happier than I am today.
to nostalgia proneness, those higher versus lower in 11. Today's new movie stars could learn from the old
nostalgia express a relative liking for tender or musical pros.
as opposed to violent or military movies in both studies. 12. I must admit it's getting better, better all the time
Further, and quite important, the directions of the sig- [reversed].
nificant age and nostalgia vectors in study 2 are basically 13. The truly great sports heroes are long dead and
orthogonal in the preference space-one horizontal, the gone.
other vertical-indicating that, even when age varies 14. History involves a steady improvement in human
over a wide range in the sample of interest, the effect welfare [reversed].
of nostalgia proneness works independently of age. 15. Today's standard of living is the highest ever at-
Thus, in general, older respondents tend relatively to tained [reversed].
prefer earlier films, whereas those higher in nostalgia 16. Sometimes, I wish I could return to the womb.
proneness show differential preferences for tender mu- 17. We are experiencing a decline in the quality of life.
sicals. Clearly, both phenomena are nostalgic. But the 18. Steady growth in GNP has brought increased hu-
former refers to temporally related aspects of age, the man happiness [reversed].
latter to sentimental aspects of nostalgia proneness as 19. Compared to the classics, today's music is mostly
a psychographic variable. Hence, one cannot capture trash.
consumption phenomena related to nostalgia by look- 20. Modern business constantly builds a better tomor-
ing at either age or nostalgia proneness in isolation. row [reversed].
Both constructs are needed to obtain a full picture of
the nostalgic patterns in consumer preferences. [Received March 1992. Revised February 1993.]

Summary REFERENCES
This research has moved some distance toward dem- Belk, Russell W. (1990), "The Role of Possessions in Con-
onstrating two key aspects of nostalgic consumption structing and Maintaining a Sense of Past," in Advances

This content downloaded from 128.6.218.72 on Tue, 29 Sep 2015 01:13:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
256 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

in ConsutmerResearch, Vol. 17, ed. Marvin E. Goldberg Strategy, ed. John A. Howard, Englewood Cliffs, NJ:
et al., Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, Prentice-Hall).
669-676. and Douglas V. Holloway (1984), "Marketing Strategy
(1991), "Possessions and the Sense of Past," in High- and the Structure of Aggregate, Segment-specific, and
ways and Buyways, ed. Russell W. Belk, Provo, UT: As- Differential Preferences," Jouirnal of Marketing, 48
sociation for Consumer Research, 114-130. (Winter), 62-67.
, Melanie Wallendorf, John Sherry, and Morris B. and Robert M. Schindler (1989), "Some Exploratory
Holbrook (1991), "Collecting in a Consumer Culture," Findings on the Development of Musical Tastes," Journal
in Highways and Buyways, ed. Russell W. Belk, Provo, of Consiumer Research, 16 (June), 119-124.
UT: Association for Consumer Research, 178-215. and Robert M. Schindler (1991), "Echoes of the Dear
, Melanie Wallendorf, John Sherry, Morris Holbrook, Departed Past: Some Work in Progress on Nostalgia,"
and Scott Roberts (1988), "Collectors and Collecting," in Advances in ConsutmerResearch, Vol. 18, ed. Rebecca
in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 15, ed. Michael H. Holman and Michael R. Solomon, Provo, UT: As-
J. Houston, Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Re- sociation for Consumer Research, 330-333.
search, 548-553. Joyrich, Lynne (1990), "Critical and Textual Hypermascu-
Bentler, Peter M. (1989), EQS. StructutralEquations Program linity," in Logics of Television: Essays in CultutralCrit-
Manual, Los Angeles: BMDP Statistical Software. icism, ed. Patricia Mellencamp, Bloomington: Indiana
Campbell, Colin (1987), The Romantic Ethic and the Spirit University Press, 156-172.
of Modern Consuimerism, New York: Blackwell. Levy, Emanuel (1987), And the Winner Is The History
Carroll, J. Douglas (1972), "Individual Differences and Mul- and Politics of the Oscar Awards, New York: Ungar.
tidimensional Scaling," in Multidimensional Scaling: Lowenthal, David (1985), The Past Is a Foreign Couintry,
Theory and Applications in the Behavioral Sciences, Vol. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
1, ed. Roger N. Shepard et al., New York: Seminar, 105- Marsh, Herbert W., John R. Balla, and Roderick P. McDonald
155. (1988), "Goodness-of-Fit Indexes in Confirmatory Factor
Csikszentmihalyi, Mihaly and Eugene Rochberg-Halton Analysis: The Effect of Sample Size," Psychological Bl!l-
(1981), The Meaning of Things: Domestic Symbols and letin, 103 (May), 391-410.
the Self, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. McCracken, Grant (1988), Cuiltutre and Consumption: New
Davis, Fred (1979), Yearning for Yesterday: A Sociology of Approaches to the Symbolic Character of Consumer
Nostalgia, New York: Free Press. Goods and Activities, Bloomington: Indiana University
Fodor, Nandor (1950), "Varieties of Nostalgia," The Psycho- Press.
analytic Review: An Educational American Journal of McKechnie, George E. (1974), ERI Manual: Environmental
Psychoanalysis, 37, 25-38. Response Inventory, Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychol-
Havlena, William J. and Susan L. Holak (1991), " 'The Good ogists.
Old Days': Observations on Nostalgia and Its Role in Power and
Morley, David (1986), FcamilyTelevision: Cutltutral
Consumer Behavior," in Advances in Consuimer Re- Domestic Leisulre, London: Routledge.
search, Vol. 18, ed. Rebecca H. Holman and Michael R. Nawas, M. Mike and Jerome J. Platt (1965), "A Future-ori-
Solomon, Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Re- ented Theory of Nostalgia," Joulrnal of Individiual Psy-
search, 323-329. chology, 21 (May), 51-57.
Hirsch, Alan R. (1992), "Nostalgia: A Neuropsychiatric Un- Press, Andrea L. (1991), Women Watching Television: Gen-
derstanding," in Advances in ConsutmerResearch, Vol. der, Class, and Generation in the American Television
19, ed. John F. Sherry, Jr., and Brian Sternthal, Provo, Experience, Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania
UT: Association for Consumer Research, 390-395. Press.
Hobsbawm, Eric (1983), "Introduction: Inventing Tradition," Schuman, Howard and Jacqueline Scott (1989), "Generations
in The Invention of Tradition, ed. Eric Hobsbawm and and Collective Memories," American Sociological Re-
Terence Ranger, Cambridge: Cambridge University view, 54 (June), 359-381.
Press, 1-14. Stern, Barbara B. (1992), "Nostalgia in Advertising Text:
Holak, Susan L. and William J. Havlena (1992). "Nostalgia: Romancing the Past," in Advances in Consutmer Re-
An Exploratory Study of Themes and Emotions in the search, Vol. 19, ed. John F. Sherry, Jr., and Brian Stern-
Nostalgic Experience," in Advances in Consumer Re- thal, Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research,
search, Vol. 19, ed. John F. Sherry, Jr., and Brian Stern- 388-389.
thal, Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, Stipp, Horst (1990), "Musical Demographics: The Strong
380-387. Impact of Age on Music Preferences Affects All Kinds
Holbrook, Morris B. (1989), "'These Foolish Things,' 'The of Business," American Demographics, 12 (August), 48-
Dear Departed Past,' and the Songs of David Frishberg: 49.
A Commentary and Critique," Newsletter of the Asso- Taylor, S. Martin and Victor A. Konrad (1980), "Scaling Dis-
ciation for Consumer Research (June), 1-8. positions toward the Past," Environment and Behavior,
(1993), "Nostalgia Proneness and Consumer Tastes," 12 (September), 283-307.
working paper, Graduate School of Business, Columbia Tucker, Ledyard R. and Charles Lewis (1973), "A Reliability
University, New York, NY (forthcoming as an abbrevi- Coefficient for Maximum Likelihood Factor Analysis,
ated appendix in Consumer Behavior in Marketing Psychometrika, 38 (March), 1-10.

This content downloaded from 128.6.218.72 on Tue, 29 Sep 2015 01:13:43 UTC
All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like