You are on page 1of 17

The SAGE

Handbook of
Social Research
Methods

Edited by
Pertti Alasuutari,
Leonard Bickman,
Julia Brannen
Editorial arrangement and Introduction © Pertti Chapter 19 © Andrea Doucet and Natasha Mauthner 2008
Alasuutari, Leonard Bickman, Julia Brannen 2008 Chapter 20 © Joanna Bornat 2008
Chapter 2 © Alan Bryman 2008 Chapter 21 © Janet Smithson 2008
Chapter 3 © Marja Alastalo 2008 Chapter 22 © Suzanne E. Graham, Judith D. Singer and
Chapter 4 © Martyn Hammersley 2008 John B. Willett 2008
Chapter 5 © Karen Armstrong 2008 Chapter 23 © Rick H. Hoyle 2008
Chapter 6 © Pekka Sulkunen 2008 Chapter 24 © Stephen G. West and Felix Thoemmes 2008
Chapter 7 © Ann Nilsen 2008 Chapter 25 © Charles Antaki 2008
Chapter 8 © Celia B. Fisher and Andrea E. Anushko 2008 Chapter 26 © Matti Hyvärinen 2008
Chapter 9 © Howard S. Bloom 2008 Chapter 27 © Kathy Charmaz 2008
Chapter 10 © Thomas D. Cook and Vivian C. Wong 2008 Chapter 28 © Lindsay Prior 2008
Chapter 11 © Ken Kelley and Scott E. Maxwell 2008 Chapter 29 © Christian Heath and Paul Luff 2008
Chapter 12 © Giampietro Gobo 2008 Chapter 30 © Janet Heaton 2008
Chapter 13 © Linda Mabry 2008 Chapter 31 © Angela Dale, Jo Wathan and Vanessa
Chapter 14 © Jane Elliott, Janet Holland and Rachel Higgins 2008
Thomson 2008 Chapter 32 © Erika A. Patall and Harris Cooper 2008
Chapter 15 © David de Vaus 2008 Chapter 33 © Jane Fielding and Nigel Fielding 2008
Chapter 16 © James A. Bovaird and Susan E. Chapter 34 © Ann Cronin, Victoria D. Alexander, Jane
Embretson 2008 Fielding, Jo Moran-Ellis and Hilary Thomas 2008
Chapter 17 © Susan A. Speer 2008 Chapter 35 © Manfred Max Bergman 2008
Chapter 18 © Edith de Leeuw 2008 Chapter 36 © Amir Marvasti 2008

First published 2008

Apart from any fair dealing for the purposes of research or private
study, or criticism or review, as permitted under the Copyright,
Designs and Patents Act, 1988, this publication may be reproduced,
stored or transmitted in any form, or by any means, only with the
prior permission in writing of the publishers, or in the case of
reprographic reproduction, in accordance with the terms of licences
issued by the Copyright Licensing Agency. Enquiries concerning
reproduction outside those terms should be sent to the publishers.

SAGE Publications Ltd


1 Oliver’s Yard
55 City Road
London EC1Y 1SP

SAGE Publications Inc.


2455 Teller Road
Thousand Oaks, California 91320

SAGE Publications India Pvt Ltd


B 1/I 1 Mohan Cooperative Industrial Area
Mathura Road
New Delhi 110 044

SAGE Publications Asia-Pacific Pte Ltd


33 Pekin Street #02-01
Far East Square
Singapore 048763

Library of Congress Control Number: 2007929185

British Library Cataloguing in Publication data

A catalogue record for this book is available from


the British Library

ISBN 978-1-4129-1992-0

Typeset by CEPHA Imaging Pvt. Ltd., Bangalore, India


Printed in Great Britain by The Cromwell Press Ltd, Trowbridge, Wiltshire
Printed on paper from sustainable resources
8
Research Ethics in Social Science
Celia B. Fisher and Andrea E. Anushko

Unparalleled growth in the social and behav- and Africa among other developing countries
ioral sciences in the last half of the twentieth (e.g. Council for International Organizations
century has and will continue to make signif- of Medical Sciences, 2002; Indian Council of
icant contributions to society’s understanding Medical Research, 2000; National Consensus
of persons as individuals, as members of Conference on Bioethics and Health Research
familial and non-familial social groups, and in Uganda, 1997; National Research Council,
participants within cultural, social, economic 2003; Thailand Ministry of Public Health
and political macrosystems. Increased public Ethics Committee, 1995; World Medical
recognition of the value of social research has Association, 2000).
been accompanied by heightened sensitivity
to the obligation to conduct social science
responsibly. The formidable task of insuring A BRIEF HISTORY OF RESEARCH
ethical competence in social research depends ETHICS RULES AND REGULATIONS
upon sensitive and informed planning by
ethically informed scientists and careful Biomedical research ethics have a long history
review by nationally mandated or indepen- formally beginning with the Nuremberg Code
dent Institutional Review Boards (IRBs) or (1946), the international response to the
Research Ethics Committees (REC). The atrocities committed by the Nazi medical
broad language of national and international experimentation. However because the acts
regulations and the diversity of expertise and committed by the Nazi scientists seemed
wide latitude in decision-making given to so far removed from standard medical and
IRBs is often intimidating to social scientists social research, the Nuremberg Code had
who are required to apply for IRB approval little influence on medical or social science
as a condition of conducting their research. research (Steinbock et al., 2005). Biomedical
Social scientists are additionally challenged research ethics continued to evolve slowly in
because of the historical and biomedical bias the United States and abroad (Declaration of
in the language and scope of regulations Helsinki, 1964). In the United States it was not
governing IRBs in the United States and RECs until the 1970s, when revelations of subjects’
in Europe, Latin America, India, Thailand, abuse in the now infamous Tuskegee Syphilis
96 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF SOCIAL RESEARCH METHODS

Study (Heller, 1972; Jones, 1993) prompted


Ethics in the social sciences
U.S. Public Law 93-348 to call for the estab-
lishment of the National Commission for the Problems identified in social science research
Protection of Human Subjects of Biomedical did not produce the serious harms observed
and Behavioral Research. The National Com- in medical studies during the period of
mission published recommendations, known national and international biomedical research
as the Belmont Report (DHEW, 1978), regulations. Indeed, in the United States
that served as the basis for revised federal for example, prior to the National Com-
regulations published in the Federal Register mission’s report, social science researchers
in 1979 with continued revisions through rarely sought informed consent even when
2001 (DHHS, Code of Federal Regulations punishing stimuli were part of the research
Title 45-Part 46 Protection of Human Subjects design and the use of deception and invasion
45 CFR 46, 2001). At the same time the of privacy was common place (Sieber,
Council for International Organizations of 1992). This is somewhat surprising since the
Medical Sciences (CIOMS) in association American Psychological Association (APA)
with the World Health Organization (WHO) adapted its first ethics code covering research,
set out to develop guidelines that applied the teaching, and practice in 1953 (APA, 1953)
principles of the Declaration of Helsinki to the and the American Anthropological Associa-
conduct of biomedical research, particularly tion officially approved their Statement on
in developing countries. The final product Problems of Anthropological Research and
was the 1982, Proposed International Ethical Ethics in 1967 (Nolan, 2002). One reason
Guidelines for Biomedical Research Involv- for the lack of ethical awareness within
ing Human Subjects. Since 1982 two revisions social science research at that time might
have been made to the CIOMS guidelines: one have been the broad aspirational languages
in 1993 and the most recent in 2002. National of the codes. For example, it was not until
and international guidelines base research 1992 that specifically worded operational
ethics regulation on three general ethical standards of conduct for research, teaching
principles: (1) Beneficence: the obligation and professional practice were included
to maximize research benefits and minimize in the APA Ethics Code (Canter et al.,
research harms; (2) Respect: the responsi- 1994) and this model was then adapted by
bility to ensure that research participation other social sciences including the American
is informed, rationale, and voluntary; and Sociological Association, and the Canadian
(3) Justice: the obligation to ensure the fair Psychological Association (ASA, 1999; CPA,
distribution of research benefits and burdens 2000). The most recent revision of the APA
across populations. While the conceptual and Ethics Code (APA, 2003) includes a more
practical frameworks for research ethics in protective standard on deception research,
its present form are rooted in and largely prohibiting such research if it leads to pain or
dominated by Western culture (Ogundiran, substantial stress or discomfort and requiring
2004), these principles have retained their investigators to respect a participant’s request
fundamental value in guiding the ethical to withdraw data following debriefing (Fisher,
conduct of contemporary research in the West 2003a).
and increasingly in developing countries. In Recognizing the strong biomedical basis
Africa for example, the Pan African Bioethics for many of the previous guidelines gov-
Initiative (PABIN), was established in 2001 erning research, some countries have shifted
to foster the development of research ethics their focus to create statements of ethical
with special emphasis on the need to develop conduct specific to the social sciences.
the capacity for reviewing the ethics of Australia for instance, in revising their
research conducted in Africa by nationals and 1999 National Statement on Ethical Conduct
internationals (see:http://www.pabin.net/en/ in Research Involving Humans has drafted
index.asp). a new set of guidelines specifically for
RESEARCH ETHICS IN SOCIAL SCIENCE 97

social scientists while still building upon federal research funds to have written
the Nuremberg Code and the Declaration guidelines for the avoidance and institutional
of Helsinki, highlighting such principles as review of conflict of interest. These guidelines
research merit and integrity, justice, benef- must reflect state and local laws and
icence, and respect. Others have chosen to cover financial interests, gifts, nepotism,
order their principles according to the weight political participation, and other issues (see:
they should receive when in conflict specific http://grants.nih.gov/grants/policy/emprograms/
to the types of dilemmas social scientists often overview/ep-coi.htm).
face. For example, Canada prioritizes their Of relevance to investigators is the U.S.
four principles for social science researchers Public Health Service and National Science
in the order of: (1) Respect for the Dignity of Foundation (NSF) requirement that any
Persons; (2) Responsible Caring; (3) Integrity funding application must include a statement
in Relationships; and (4) Responsibility to on whether there are any significant financial
Society (CPA, 2000). interests that could directly and significantly
This chapter now turns to four specific areas affect the design, conduct or reporting of
of continued and emerging ethical concern in the research. Such interests can include
social research: conflicts of interest, informed consulting fees, honoraria, ownership or
consent, cultural equivalence, and the use of equity options, or intellectual property (e.g.
monetary incentives. The chapter concludes patents, copyrights, and royalties) where
with a call for ethical commitment, ethical such values exceed $10,000. Academic
awareness and active engagement in the institutional salaries and lectures sponsored
ongoing development of courses of action by non-profit or public entities are exempt
reflecting the highest ideals of responsible from this policy (see: http://www.nsf.gov/
social science. policies/conflicts.jsp, http://grants2. nih.gov/
grants/policy/nihgps_2001/nihgps_2001.pdf).
In addition, many IRBs in the United States
CONFLICT OF INTERESTS are requiring researchers to include a conflict
of interest statement in their informed
Social researchers should strive to estab- consents and journals are requiring a
lish relationships of trust with research statement describing the absence or existence
participants, the scientific community, and of a potential conflict of interest. For example,
the public. When conflicting professional, APA publications require authors to reveal
personal, financial, legal or other interests any possible conflict of interest (e.g. financial
impair the objectivity of data collection, interests in a test procedure, funding by
analysis or interpretation, such trust and the pharmaceutical companies) in the conduct
validity of the research is compromised. and reporting of research. According to the
Ethical steps to avoid potentially harmful or International Committee of Medical Journal
exploitative conflicts of interest are critical Editors (ICMJE) (2003) editors may use
to ensure that the objectivity of data analysis information disclosed in conflict of interest
and interpretation is led by data and not other and financial interest statements as a basis for
interests. Impairment of objectivity can harm editorial decisions. Prompted in large part by
participants, the public, institutions, funders, concerns about conflicts of interest stemming
and the integrity of social science as a field. from the relationship between pharmaceutical
Several national bodies and organizations companies and independent clinical research
have produced guidelines for conflict of organizations, India and other developing
interest decision-making relevant to the countries are beginning to call for adoption
conduct of social science research. For of international and establishment of national
example, in the United States the National regulations for research conflicts of interest
Institutes of Health Office of Extramural (Editorial, The Hindu, 2005; Pan African
Research requires every institution receiving Bioethics Initiative, 2001).
98 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF SOCIAL RESEARCH METHODS

Several professional codes of conduct, emerge when investigators sponsored by pri-


including the APA Ethics Code standard on vate industry or organizations do not consider
conflict of interest (APA, 2002, Standard in advance the implications of data ownership
3.06), the British Sociological Association (Fried & Fisher, in press). Investigators
Code of Ethics (BSA, 2002, Standard 42), the working on independent projects funded
British Psychological Society Code of Ethics externally need to ensure that they maintain all
and Conduct (BPS, 2006, Standard 4.2), and access to and ownership of data as well as the
the Canadian Code of Ethics for Psychologists right to publish results without prior approval
(CPA, 2000, Standard III.31) are applicable or interference from the sponsor. Sponsors
for all social science researchers. As applied with financial interest in the outcome of the
to research they prohibit conflict of interests research if provided the opportunity may
if another personal, scientific, professional, deny investigators access to the final dataset,
financial or other interests or relationships attempt to dictate analytic strategies, stall
could reasonably be expected to impair dissemination of negative findings, or insist
objectivity, competence or effectiveness of on ghostwriting the scientific report. Failure
the psychologist to conduct the research, to anticipate the consequences of, acquiescing
if it would expose his or her organization to or naively signing a contract waiving
to harm, or if it would result in the harm these responsibilities can result in becoming
or exploitation of research participants or an accomplice to letting a financial agenda
research assistants. The ethics codes of other rather than the data drive research results.
social science organizations have similar In addition to resulting in a violation of
prohibitions against conflicts of interest (e.g. avoidance of unethical conflicts of interest,
AAA, 1998; ASA, 1999 in the U.S.). such decisions can result in other violations
within APA. For example, according to the
APA Ethics Code (APA, 2002, Standards
Examples of potentially harmful 1.01 Misuse of Psychologists Work and 5.01
conflicts of interest Avoidance of False or Deceptive Statements)
and the International Sociological Association
Examples of potential conflicts of interest can
Code of Ethics (ISA, 2001, Standard 3,
occur if: (a) a social scientist takes gifts from
Publication and Communication of data)
or has financial holdings in a company whose
investigators are prohibited from knowingly
product she or he is investigating; (b) the
making public statements that are false,
research is sponsored by a company or orga-
deceptive or fraudulent concerning their
nization that has a financial investment in the
research and are responsible for preventing
direction of results that might place pressure
or correcting false statements about their
on the investigator; (c) the investigator or his
work by others. For social scientists such
or her institution will hold the patent for the
public statements can include not only false
researched instrument; or (d) scientists are
statements in publications and professional
reviewing a grant application or manuscript
presentations, but product endorsements, false
submission from a competitor.
statements concerning conflict of interest
or delegation of research responsibilities on
Conflict of interest and industry grant applications, and expert testimony about
scientific data in legal proceedings.
sponsored research: Who owns
the data?
Conflicts between ethics and
In traditional academic contexts, social sci-
organizational demands
entists have a responsibility to report on
the results of their data, and to ensure that Social scientists who are employees or
the report accurately represents the findings. consultants to an organization face a slightly
Potentially unethical conflicts of interest can different set of ethical challenges. In such
RESEARCH ETHICS IN SOCIAL SCIENCE 99

contexts, the company or organization may INFORMED CONSENT


have a priori ownership of any data produced
by its employees. In such contexts the inves- The principle of respect reflects a moral
tigator’s role is to provide the organization concern for the autonomy and privacy rights
with the results and interpretation of data of those recruited for research participation.
collected from well-designed studies that In its most fundamental form, it embodies
were conducted to provide information for the moral necessity of obtaining consent
organizational decision-making. The choice to participate in research that is informed,
to make public the findings belongs to the rationale and voluntary. The informed require-
organization. Unethical conflicts of interest ment requires that prospective participants
can emerge in such settings. For example, are provided with all information about the
if the researcher agrees to a request by study that would be expected to influence
the company or organization to design a their willingness to participate. As embodied
study that will guarantee results are all in U.S. federal regulations and the APA Ethics
biased in a particular direction, falsify Code (APA, 2002, Standards 3.10 and 8.02;
results from previously collected data or DHHS, 2001) as well as the Canadian Code of
write a report that provides an incomplete Ethics (CPA, 2000, Standard 1.24) and the EU
summary of the data or that intentionally Code of Ethics for Socio-Economic Research
misinterprets study results. When entering (Dench, Iphofen, Huws, 2004, Standard 4.3)
into an employment or contractual agreement such information includes: (1) the purpose,
with a company or organization, social duration, and procedures; (2) the right
scientists should anticipate and educate the to decline or withdraw from participation;
company to the conflict of interest issues (3) consequences of declining or withdraw-
that may emerge and establish agreements ing; (4) risks and potential discomforts or
about data collection, interpretation, and adverse effects; (5) any prospective benefits
dissemination that permit the investigator to to participants or society; (6) extent and limits
act ethically. of confidentiality; (7) incentives for partic-
ipation; (8) who to contact with questions
regarding the research (usually the principal
investigator) and their research rights (usually
Conflicts of interest in social
the Chair of the IRB); and (9) an opportunity
research: Unchartered territory
to ask questions. Some forms of social
In summary, as industry and organizations research create consent challenges. Next we
increasingly recognize the value of social discuss informed consent within the context of
research for policy decisions and public three of these research methods: Qualitative,
relations, social scientists will increasingly archival, and deception research.
be confronted with conflict of interest
challenges. Not all conflicts of interest are
Qualitative research
unethical or avoidable. The ethical challenge
for social scientists is to be vigilant in The exploratory and open-ended nature of
identifying such conflicts, assure the public semi-structured interviews, participant obser-
that conflicts are eliminated when possible vation, or ethnographic work raises questions
and effectively managed when necessary. about whether truly informed consent for
As noted by the Office of Human Research such research can be obtained (Marshall,
Protections, ‘Openness and honesty are 1992). Several Codes, including the Aus-
indicators of integrity and responsibility, tralian National Statement on Ethical Conduct
characteristics that promote quality research in Human Research (National Health and
and can only strengthen the research process’ Medical Research Council (NHMRC), 2007).
(http://www.hhs.gov/ohrp/nhrpac/mtg12-00/ Set out specific guidelines for qualitative
finguid.htm). research (Standard 3.1). The movement to
100 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF SOCIAL RESEARCH METHODS

view social sciences as ‘hard science’and IRB be archived. Social science has a prestigious
unfamiliarity with qualitative research meth- history of archives (Young & Brooker, 2006).
ods has also posed challenges to anthropolo- The purpose of archived data is to provide
gists, sociologists, and other social scientists a rich set of data that can be used by
whose research often strays from the classical future investigators to examine empirical
scientific method because of unique research questions about populations that may not be
questions or the nature of their population anticipated when information is first collected.
(Marshall, 2003). Informed consent is also prob- Several organizations have begun to unite
lematic when working with immigrant popu- social science researchers and their data from
lations or in international settings for reasons around the world to create large and secure
ranging from language barriers and fear of accessible databases of archived information.
exploitation or deportation to authority to con- For instance, the Inter-University Consortium
sent resting with an individual other than the for Political and Social Research (ICPSR)
participants, e.g. in countries where women has over 500 college or university members
are not permitted to consent to research with- and has four major operations units, one of
out prior male permission (Marshall, 2003). which is data security and preservation. The
In studies where informed consent is Harvard-MIT data center also archives and
obtained, it is often difficult to ensure fully protects various social science data to allow
informed consent at the start of a project access for future generations of social science
because researchers may not be able to researchers. Participant identity is protected
anticipate the full extent of information that in these archives through a very detailed pro-
will emerge (Haverkamp, 2005). Risks to cess of individual de-identification. However,
privacy and confidentiality emerge when the the racial, ethnic, cultural, health, or other
information leads to unanticipated revelations demographic-based populations from which
regarding illegal behaviors (crimes, child or participants were recruited in most instances
domestic abuse, illegal immigration), health must remain identifiable for the research
problems (HIV status, genetic disorder) or questions to be meaningful.
other information that if revealed could Within the continuously changing social-
jeopardize participants’ legal or economic political context in which science and society
status (Fisher & Goodman, in press; Fisher & evolve, some investigators have begun to
Ragsdale, 2006). One way to address this question the validity of informed consent to
issue is to develop in advance a re-consent ongoing secondary analysis by unknown third
strategy for situations in which unanticipated parties with research questions that may be
and sensitive issues emerge during the course inconsistent with the consent understandings
of observation or discussion (Fisher, 2004; of those who initially agreed to participation
Haverkamp, 2005). The strategy can include and preservation. This becomes of particular
a set of criteria to help the interviewer: concern when secondary analysis of data from
(1) identify when unexpected information historically oppressed or disenfranchised
may lead to increased participant privacy and communities is requested (Young & Brooker,
confidentiality risk; (2) determine whether 2006) or if the circumstances under which the
the direction of the conversation is relevant original data was collected is questionable as
to the research question; (3) if not relevant, in the 1968 Yanomami research conducted
find ways to divert the discussion; or (4) if by Neel (http://members.aol.com/archaeodog/
relevant, alert the participant to the new nature darkness_in_el_dorado/documents/0081.htm).
of information and implement a mutually Requiring individual participants to recon-
negotiated re-consent procedure. sent to the use of archival data can be
both harmful and infeasible. First, it would
require that records linking responses to
Archival research
individually identifiable information is pre-
Similar, but more difficult issues emerge when served over decades, where confidentiality
consent is obtained for social research that will protections may be vulnerable over time.
RESEARCH ETHICS IN SOCIAL SCIENCE 101

Second, it would require locating individuals the moral ambiguity surrounding consent
after years or decades which in many cases for deception research when the investi-
would be impossible and the unavailability gator intentionally gives participants false
of segments of the initial population would information about the purpose and nature
compromise the validity of the sample. In of the study. In such contexts consent for
response to these challenges, the Council of deception research distorts the informed
National Psychological Associations for the consent process, because it leads prospective
Advancement of Ethnic Minority Interests participants to believe they have autonomy
(CNPAAEMI, 2000) has recommended that to decide about the type of experimental
social research archives consider setting procedures they will be exposed to, when in
up standing community (broadly defined) fact they do not.
advisory boards as a means of helping
archive administrators determine when newly
The deception debate
proposed analyses may violate the intent of
the informed consent. Debate on the ethical justification for decep-
tive research practices reflects a tension
between scientific validity and respect for
Deception research and the
participants’ right to make a truly informed
‘consent paradox’
participation decision (Fisher & Fyrberg,
In research using deceptive methods, the 1994). Arguments for deception emphasize
researcher intentionally misinforms partici- the methodological advantage of keeping par-
pants about the purpose of the study, the ticipants naïve about the purpose of the study
procedures, or the role of individuals with to ensure responses to experimental manipula-
whom the participant will be required to tions are spontaneous and unbiased (Milgram,
interact (Sieber, 1982). The use of deceptive 1964; Resnick & Schwartz, 1973; Smith &
techniques is not prohibited in any national Richardson, 1983). Arguments against decep-
research regulations and is explicitly permit- tion emphasize the violation of participant
ted with stipulations in professional ethics autonomy, the potential to create public
codes including the American Psychological distrust in social science research in general
Association (2002), American Sociological and the harm resulting from infliction of self-
Association (1999), Canadian Psychologi- knowledge that was unexpected, unwanted,
cal Association (2000), British Psycholog- shameful or distressful (Baumrind, 1964).
ical Society (2006), and the International Sociologists have been at the center of
Sociological Association (2001). Baumrind deception controversy and have members
(1979) distinguished between nonintentional who are stanch advocates and opponents
deception, in which failure to fully inform of the practice. Allen (1997) falls into the
cannot be avoided because of the complexity latter category, criticizing sociologists for
of the information, and intentional deception, befriending groups of interest without letting
which is the withholding of information in on that they were subjects of sociological
order to obtain participation that the subject research, misrepresenting the motives of
might otherwise decline. Simply not pro- their research, and adopting a false persona
viding participants with specific hypotheses to conduct research. Particularly disturbing
regarding the relationship among experimen- to Allen is the defense that personal time
tal variables does not in itself constitute and effort prevented the feasibility of other
deception. methods, thus in order to get the research done
Deception most obviously violates the deception was necessary.
principle of respect, by depriving prospective
participants the opportunity to make an
Ethical options
informed choice regarding the true nature
of their participation. What Fisher (2005) Bulmer (1982) concludes that completely
has termed the ‘consent paradox’ underscores disguising the intent of research can affect
102 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF SOCIAL RESEARCH METHODS

the quality of the data collected as well physical pain or severe emotional distress.
as exaggerate the unknown biases of the Third, the investigator must prove that
researcher. Instead he proposes such meth- the same hypotheses cannot be sufficiently
ods as retrospective participant observation explored and tested using non-deceptive
in which a sociologist uses retrospective designs. This standard thus prohibits the use
observations from previous experience when of deception research if inconvenience or
she was a total participant prior to any costs of performing non-deceptive research
research interest. He also supports the use are the only reasons for proposing such
of native as stranger, in which an already methods (Fisher, 2003a). In addition, the true
established member of the group is trained as nature of the deception must be revealed to
a sociologist. The covert outsider is another participants at the end of the study unless the
suggested method in which a legitimate role, debriefing might reasonably be expected to
such as a teacher in a prison, is taken on in bias future participant responses; or withhold
order to observe behavior and gain access to such information if the debriefing itself
an otherwise unreachable population (Bulmer, would cause participant harm (APA, 2002,
1982). Standard 8.08b).
According to U.S. federal guidance (OPRR, While the APA and other organizations’
1993), when considering the use of deception, ethics codes attempt to increase the ethical
investigators must first decide whether the rigor of decisions to use deception methodolo-
information to be withheld during consent gies, no guidance can erase the threat to partic-
would, if known, influence the individual’s ipant autonomy that such procedures reflect.
desire to participate in research. However how Neither, debriefing (even when believed to
to judge this prospectively is difficult. Some be valid by participants) nor the opportunity
have argued that responses from previous par- to withdraw their data, are a panacea for
ticipants during dehoaxing (revealing the true the ethical paradox of deception research.
nature of the study at the end of participation) Consent can only be obtained prospectively
can be used to document the benign effects (OPRR, 1993); subsequent procedures can
of different deceptive methodologies. This never be considered an adequate substitute.
approach raises its own (debriefing) paradox
(Fisher, 2005). Fisher and Fyrberg (1994)
found that introductory psychology students FAIR DISTRIBUTION OF THE BENEFITS
(the most commonly recruited participants for AND BURDENS OF RESEARCH
deception studies) were likely to believe that
the dehoaxing process was either simply a The principle of justice is concerned with
continued extension of the research or that the fair and equitable distribution of research
the debriefing information was itself untrue. benefits and burdens. In social research,
As a result, students reported they would benefits are defined by the usefulness of
be unlikely to reveal their true feelings to data generated to help understand micro and
experimenters during the dehoaxing process; macro social processes within and among
and some were concerned they would be different populations. The burdens of social
penalized if they were truthful. research include exposure to research risks
TheAPAEthics Code (APA, 2002) attempts and required time and effort associated
to balance the principles of beneficence, with participation. Justice in social research
non-maleficence, and respect. First, the use becomes a particular ethical challenge when
of deceptive methods must be justified by racial or ethnic minority, disadvantaged, or
the study’s prospective value in scientific, disenfranchised populations are recruited for
educational or applied areas. Second, even participation in research designs that fail to
if the research is determined to have value, include consideration of unique population
deception is prohibited if it is reasonably characteristics that may reduce the knowledge
expected that the procedures will cause any value of data generated or expose them to
RESEARCH ETHICS IN SOCIAL SCIENCE 103

greater risk or financial burden (Fisher, 1999; social, economic, and political forces contin-
Trimble & Fisher, 2006). uously shape and redefine these definitions
for both individuals and society at large
(Chan & Hume, 1995; Zuckerman, 1990).
Population generalizability Investigators need to consider and explicitly
describe the theoretical, empirical, and social
The constantly changing demographic U.S.
frameworks driving the definitions of race,
and international landscapes pose the risk
ethnicity, or culture used to select participant
that research findings from one participant
populations, to insure the scientific validity
population will be inappropriately generalized
of the research question and to allow their
to other populations. This can occur in at least
research findings to be evaluated within the
two ways. First, injustices may occur when
context of continuously changing scientific
populations are intentionally or unintention-
and societal conceptions of these definitions
ally excluded from recruitment, but results of
(Fisher et al., 2002).
the study are inappropriately generalized to
Within group differences are also an
apply to their social or psychological charac-
important factor to consider when identifying
teristics and circumstances. This becomes par-
population characteristics relevant to the
ticularly problematic for social science when
study questions. Investigators often ignore
the descriptions of ethnic/racial characteris-
the scientific implications of variation among
tics are vaguely described in journal articles.
populations described under broad panethnic
Typical descriptions that provide inadequate
labels. For example, failure to identify the
knowledge for assessing the relevance of the
national origins of participants categorized
data to ethnic minority populations in the
as ‘Hispanic’ (e.g. Mexico, Puerto Rico,
United States, for example are: ‘the majority
Guatemala, Chile) can produce overgener-
of participants were non-Hispanic white’;
alizations that dilute or obscure moderating
or ‘eighty-percent of participants were non-
effects on social behavior resulting from
Hispanic white; the remaining 20 percent were
national origin, immigration history, religion,
African American and Hispanic’ (Fisher &
and tradition. In addition, within even these
Brennan, 1992).
more nationally defined categories, research
participants may vary greatly in their identifi-
cation with the ethnic group of family origin or
Defining race, ethnicity, and culture
with the degree to which they are acculturated
When participants’ race, ethnicity, or culture to majority culture (Fisher et al., 1997).
are described in greater detail there is often
an absence of definition of what these
Cultural equivalence of assessment
terms mean or how decisions to identify
measures
participants by ‘race’ (physical similarities
assumed to reflect phenotypic expressions Investigators need to heed a second risk
of shared genotypes), ‘ethnicity’ (assumed of producing research injustice: failure to
cultural, linguistic, religious, and historical recognize when a measure of a social
similarities), or ‘culture’ (group ways of construct established in one population when
thinking and living based upon shared knowl- applied to another ethnic/cultural group may
edge, consciousness, skills, values, expressive not yield similar psychometric properties
forms, social institutions, and behaviors that nor reflect a social phenomenon that has
allow individuals to survive in the contexts similar behavioral or psychological patterns
within which they live) reflects assumptions of relationships (Hoagwood & Jensen, 1997;
about the underlying causal mechanisms Laosa, 1990). The use of such measures
driving similarities or differences found risks the over- or under-identification of
among populations (Fisher et al., 1997). socially meaningful characteristics, compro-
Further, there is often little recognition that mising the scientific benefits of the research
104 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF SOCIAL RESEARCH METHODS

and potentially resulting in harmful social and inconvenience of research as long as no


labeling or maladaptive self-conceptions of ‘undue inducements’are offered to lure people
members of the racial or ethnic group studied into participating and incentives are not
(Canino & Guarnaccia, 1997; Fisher et al., included as a ‘benefit’ in risk-benefit analyses
2002; Knight & Hill, 1998). Thus, whenever (APA, 2002, Standard 8.06; BPS, 2006,
possible, investigators should select surveys, Standard 3.3.4; CPA, 2000, Standard 1.14;
interview techniques or instruments that have NHMRC, 2007, Standard 2.2.9; National
been standardized on members of the research Advisory Council on Drug Abuse, 2000;
participants’ racial or ethnic group. When OHRP, 1993). The science establishment
such measures have not yet been developed thus recognizes that some inducement is
or sufficiently evaluated, investigators can necessary to insure sufficient sample size
evaluate the cultural validity of the measure and that it is possible for investigators to
by evaluating item equivalence and other distinguish between ‘due’and ‘undue’induce-
psychometric properties. ments (Dickert & Grady, 1991; Macklin,
1999). Selecting non-coercive incentives is
critical to insuring the voluntary nature
Moving away from comparative and of participation and that research burdens
deficit approaches are not born unequally by economically
disadvantaged populations. Cash payments or
Injustices in research can also occur when
other incentives may be considered coercive if
social research involving ethnic minority pop-
they: (1) prompt participants to lie or conceal
ulations focuses only on population deficits
information that would disqualify them from
rather than a more comprehensive analysis of
the research or; (2) lure into participating
both population vulnerabilities and strengths.
those who would otherwise choose not to
This ‘deficit’ investigative approach often
expose themselves to research risks (Macklin,
appears alongside another potential bias in
1999). The extent to which these criteria are
social research design: the assumption that
met will vary across research populations.
ethnic minority social constructs can only be
understood when compared to non-minority
standards (Fisher et al., 2002; Heath, 1997). To Types of payments
provide fair and equitable research knowledge
Ethical decisions about the use of cash
benefits, social scientists need to apply the
incentives to secure and retain participa-
same principles of scientific inquiry to all
tion in surveys on illegal and dangerous
populations studied (e.g. EU Code of Ethics
behaviors must include consideration of
for Socio-Economic Research, Standard 2.5,
how monetary inducements will affect the
Dench et al., 2004). Cultural bias in social
quality of data as well as the equitable
science has also been identified in developing
distribution of the benefits and burdens of
countries. In India for example, the People’s
research participation. Monetary incentives
Science Movement (PSM) has drawn atten-
are often used for participant recruitment.
tion to the internalization of local cultural
Payments to research participants can be
gender biases by scientists in developing
ethically justified as: (1) reimbursement for
countries (Varma, 1999).
legitimate travel or other expenses accrued
because of research participation; (2) fair
compensation for time and inconvenience
DUE AND UNDUE RESEARCH involved in research participation; (3) appre-
INCENTIVES FOR DIVERSE ciation payments (e.g. in the form of cash,
SOCIOECONOMIC POPULATIONS coupons, or gifts); and (4) incentive payments
that offer money or the equivalence beyond
National guidelines and organizational ethics those limited to reimbursement, compensa-
codes permit compensation for effort, time, tion, or appreciation (Wendler et al., 2002).
RESEARCH ETHICS IN SOCIAL SCIENCE 105

Payments across research populations dif- published studies are related to time and
fering in financial need create a tension level of activity. In addition they found no
between fair compensation for the time and evidence that participants of these studies
inconvenience of research participation and were being enticed with large monetary
coercion. inducements.
Ideally monetary incentives for research
participation should strengthen generalizabil-
Payment for participation in illicit
ity by providing a balanced representation of
individuals from all economic levels appro-
drug use research
priate to the research question (Giuffrida & Cash payment for participation in illicit drug
Togerson, 1997; Kamb et al., 1998). However, use research can create an ethical paradox if
individuals from different economic circum- it is used by participants to purchase illegal
stances can have different responses to cash drugs, encourages them to maintain their drug
inducement as fair or coercive (Levine, 1986). habits to continue earning research money, or
Payments that are unnecessarily low can leads them to provide answers to experimental
reduce the generalizability of data through questions that distort evaluation of the social
under-recruitment of economically disadvan- correlates and consequences of drug use
taged populations. Payments that are too (Fisher, 2003b; Koocher, 1991; McGrady &
high raise different concerns. For example, Bux, 1999; Shaner et al., 1995). On the
large financial incentives can jeopardize the other hand, for those who have difficulty
voluntary nature of participation, under- obtaining and holding jobs, the money may be
mine altruistic motivations for engaging in ethically justified as a legal means of obtaining
research, tempt prospective participants to payment for unskilled labor. Policies aimed
provide false information to become eligible at addressing this problem include spreading
for study participation, or lie in response out the payment of full compensation over
to experimental questions to comply with a period of time, using food coupons or
investigator expectations (Attkisson et al., vouchers for other health-related products,
1996; Fisher, 2003b; Saunders et al., 1999). making payments to third parties on behalf
Grady (2001) argues that arbitrary or large of the participant, or withholding payment if
sums of money to entice participants is poor a participant is intoxicated or in withdrawal
practice, while modest payments help to (Fisher, 2004; Gorelick et al., 1999). Such
minimize possible undue inducement. She alternatives raise their own ethical quandaries.
proposes that the informed consent process First, there is no evidence that any substitute
in which participants are reminded of their for non-cash incentives deters participants
freedom to refuse participation or withdraw with illicit drug habits from using the
their consent without repercussions is ade- monetary value of the incentives to purchase
quate protection against potential coercion drugs. For example, informal observations by
(Grady, 2001). social scientists working in the field suggest
Based on an analysis of compensation that if need be vouchers are easily sold by
practices of a representative sample of participants for cash. Furthermore, a decision
biomedical and psychosocial research con- not to pay substance abusers can reinforce
ducted in 1997 and 1998, Latterman and economic inequities between drug abusing
Merz (2001) reported research payments and non-abusing populations or deny them the
on average of $9.50/hour plus $12.00 for right to apply their own value system to life
each additional task (U.S. dollars); larger risk decisions (Fisher, 1999).
compensation was related to longer partic-
ipatory time, repeated interaction with the
Ensuring fairness
researcher, invasive tasks, and the number
of tasks. From their small study these Social scientists are challenged to determine
researchers concluded that payments in payments that are perceived by all participants
106 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF SOCIAL RESEARCH METHODS

as equally attractive and legitimate for REFERENCES


the time and effort contributed. To ensure
fairness, some institutions adopt a standard Allen, C. (1997). Spies like us: When Sociologists deceive
compensation rate for all research partic- their subjects. Lingua franca, 7, 31–39.
ipation. Others have defined fair financial American Anthropological Association. (1998). Code
inducements as the amount of money a of ethics of the American Anthropological Associ-
normal, healthy volunteer would lose in work ation. Retrieved March 10, 2006 from http://www.
and travel time or by fair market value for aaanet.org/committees/ethics/ethicscode.pdf.
American Psychological Association. (1953).Ethical
the work involved (Dickert & Grady, 1991;
standards of psychologists. Washington, DC:
Winslade & Douard, 1992). Obtaining the
American Psychological Association.
opinions of community representatives prior American Psychological Association. (2002). Ethical
to research initiation provides another means principles of psychologists and code of conduct.
of establishing fair and non-coercive research American Psychologist, 57, 1060–1073.
payments (Fisher, 2003b). American Sociological Association. (1999). Code of
ethics. Washington, DC: American Sociological
Association. Retrieved March 10, 2006 from
DOING GOOD SCIENCE WELL http://www.asanet.org/galleries/default-file/Code%
20of%20Ethics.pdf.
The conduct of responsible social science Attkisson, C. C., Rosenblatt, A., & Hoagwood, K. (1996).
Research ethics and human subjects protection in
depends upon investigators’ commitment and
child mental health services research and adolescent
lifelong efforts to act ethically. However, and parent studies. In K. Hoagwood, P. Jenson, &
a desire to do the right thing must be C. B. Fisher (eds.), Ethical issues in research with
accompanied by familiarity with national children and adolescents with mental disorders
and international regulations, ethics codes, (pp. 43–58). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
and laws essential to the identification and Associates, Inc.
resolution of ethics-in-science challenges Baumrind, D. (1964). Some thoughts on ethics
(Fisher, 2003a). Ethical commitment and of research: after reading Milgram’s ‘Behavioral
consciousness in turn are necessary but not study of obedience.’ American Psychologist, 26,
sufficient to anticipate and rightly address the 887–896.
array of ethical challenges that will emerge Baumrind, D. (1979). IRBs and social science research:
The cost of deception. IRB: A Review of Human
when social scientists work in diverse contexts
Subjects Research, 1, 1–4.
with diverse populations. Doing good science British Psychological Society (2006). http://www.bps.
well requires flexibility and sensitivity to org.uk/downloadfile.cfm?file_uuid=5084A882-1143-
the research context, the scientist’s fiduciary DFD0-7E6C-F1938A65C242&ext=pdf.
responsibilities, and participant expectations British Sociological Association. (2002). http://www.
unique to each study. The evaluation of risks britsoc.co.uk/user_doc/Statement%20of%20Ethical
and benefits, the construction of informed %20Practice.pdf.
consent procedures, and the development Bulmer, M. (1982). When is disguise justified?
of confidentiality and disclosure policies Alternatives to covert participant observation. Quali-
need to reflect a ‘goodness of fit’ between tative Sociology, 5, 251–264.
study goals and participant characteristics Canadian Psychological Association. (2000). Canadian
(Fisher, 2002, 2003c; Fisher & Goodman, code of ethics for psychologists, 3rd edition.
Retrieved March 10, 2006 from http://www.
in press; Fisher & Masty, 2006; Fisher &
cpa.ca/cpasite/userfiles/Documents/Canadian%20
Ragsdale, 2006). Framing the responsible
Code%20of%20Ethics%20for%20Psycho.pdf.
conduct of social science as a process that Canino, G., & Guarnaccia, P. (1997). Methodological
draws upon investigators’ dual commitment challenges in the assessment of Hispanic children
to scientific validity and participant protection and adolescents.Applied Developmental Science, 7,
will nourish activities that reflect the high- 13–26.
est ideals of science and merit participant Canter, M. B., Bennett, B. E., Jones, S. E., & Nagy, T. F.
trust. (1994). Ethics for psychologists: A commentary on
RESEARCH ETHICS IN SOCIAL SCIENCE 107

the APA ethics code. Washington, DC: American Fisher, C. B. (2004). Informed consent and clini-
Psychological Association. cal research involving children and adolescents:
Chan, K. S., & Hume, S. (1995). Racialization and Implications of the revised APA ethics code and
panethnicity: From Asians in America to Asian HIPAA. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent
Americans. In W. D. Hawley & A. W. Jackson (Eds.), Psychology, 33, 833–840.
Toward a common destiny: Improving race and ethnic Fisher, C. B. (2005). Deception research involving
relations in America (pp. 205–236). San Francisco: children: Ethical practices and paradoxes. Ethics &
Jossey-Bass. Behavior, 15, 271–287.
Council for International Organizations of Medical Fisher, C. B., & Brennan, M. (1992). Application
Sciences. (2002). International guidelines for eth- and ethics in developmental psychology. In
ical review of epidemiological studies Geneva: D. L. Featherman, R. M. Lerner, & M. Perlmutter
Council for International Organizations of Medical (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior
Sciences. Retrieved May 11, 2007 from http://www. (pp. 189–219). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
cioms.ch/frame_guidelines_nov_2002.htm. Associates.
Council of National Psychological Associations for Fisher, C. B., & Fyrberg, D. (1994). College students
the Advancement of Ethic Minority Interests. weigh the costs and benefits of deceptive research.
(2000).Guidelines for research in ethnic minority American Psychologist, 49, 417–426.
communities. Washington, DC: American Psycholog- Fisher, C. B., & Goodman, S. J. (in press). Goodness-
ical Association. of-fit ethics for non-intervention research involving
Dench, S., Iphofen, R., & Huws, U. (2004). IES Report dangerous and illegal behavior. In D. Buchanan,
412. An EU code of Ethics for Socio-economic C. B. Fisher, & L. Gable (Eds.), Ethical & legal
Research. Brighton, UK: The Institute for Employment issues in research with high risk populations:
Studies. Addressing threats of suicide, child abuse, and
Department of Health, Education, & Welfare (DHEW). violence. Washington, DC: APA Press.
(1978). The Belmont report: Ethical principles and
Fisher, C. B., Hoagwood, K., Boyce, C., Buster, T.,
guidelines for the protection of human subjects of
Frank, D. A., Grisso, T., Levine, R. J., Macklin, R.,
research. Washington DC: US Government Printing
Spencer, M. B., Takanishi, R., Trimble, J. E., &
Office.
Zayas, L. H. (2002). Research ethics for mental health
Department of Health and Human Services. (2001). Title
science involving ethnic minority children and youth.
45 Public Welfare, Part 46, Code of federal regu-
American Psychologist, 57, 1024–1040.
lations, Protections of human subjects.Washington,
Fisher, C. B., Jackson, J., & Villarruel, F. (1997). The study
DC: Government Printing Office.
of African American and Latin American children
Dickert, N., & Grady, C. (1991). What’s the price
and youth. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of
of a research subject: Approaches to the payment
child psychology (Vol. I, 5th ed., pp. 1145–1207).
for research participation. New England Journal of
New York: Wiley.
Medicine, 341, 198–203.
Fisher, C. B. (1999). Relational ethics and research Fisher, C. B., & Masty, J. K. (2006). A goodness-of-
with vulnerable populations. Reports on research fit ethic for informed consent to pediatric cancer
involving persons with mental disorders that may research. In R. T. Brown (Ed.), Comprehensive
affect decision-making capacity (Vol. II, pp. 29–49). handbook of childhood cancer and sickle cell
Commissioned Papers by the National Bioethics disease: A biopsychosocial approach (pp. 205–217).
Advisory Commission, Rockville, MD. Retrieved New York: Oxford University Press.
March 21, 2006 from http://www.georgetown. Fisher, C. B., & Ragsdale, K. (2006). A goodness-of-
edu/research/nrcbl/nbac/pubs.html. fit ethics for multicultural research. In J. Trimble
Fisher, C. B. (2002). A goodness-of-fit ethic for informed and C. B. Fisher (Eds.),The handbook of ethical
consent. Fordham Urban Law Journal, 30, 159–171. research with ethnocultural populations and com-
Fisher, C. B. (2003a). Decoding the ethics code: munities (pp. 3–26). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
A practical guide for psychologists. Thousand Oaks, Publications.
CA: Sage Publications. Fried, A. F., & Fisher, C. B. (in press). The ethics
Fisher, C. B. (2003b). Adolescent and parent perspec- of informed consent for research in clinical and
tives on ethical issues in youth drug use and suicide abnormal psychology. In D. McKay (Ed.), Handbook
survey research. Ethics & Behavior, 13, 302–331. of research methods in abnormal and clinical
Fisher, C. B. (2003c). A goodness-of-fit ethic for psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
child assent to nonbeneficial research. The American Giuffrida, A., & Togerson, D. J. (1997).Should we pay the
Journal of Bioethics, 3, 27–28. patient? Review of financial incentives to enhance
108 THE SAGE HANDBOOK OF SOCIAL RESEARCH METHODS

patient compliance. British Medical Journal, 315, Koocher, G. P. (1991). Questionable methods in
703–707. alcoholism research. Journal of Consulting and
Gorelick, D. A., Pickens, R. W., & Bonkovsky, F. O. Clinical Psychology, 59, 246–248.
(1999). Clinical research in substance abuse: Laosa, L. M. (1990). Population generalizeability, cul-
Human subjects issues. In H. A. Pincus, J. A. tural sensitivity, and ethical dilemmas. In C. B. Fisher &
Lieberman, & S. Ferris (Eds.), Ethics in psychiatric W. W. Tryon (Eds.), Ethics in applied developmental
research (pp. 177–192). Washington, DC: American psychology: Emerging issues in an emerging field
Psychiatric Association. (pp. 227–252). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
Grady, C. (2001). Money for research participation: Does Latterman, J., & Merz, J. F. (2001). How much are
it jeopardize informed consent? American Journal of subjects paid to participate in research? The American
Bioethics, 1, 40–44. Journal of Bioethics, 1, 45–46.
Haverkamp, B. E. (2005). Ethical perspectives on Levine, R. (1986). Ethics and regulation of clin-
qualitative research in applied psychology. Journal of ical research (2nd ed.). Baltimore: Urban &
Counseling Psychology, 52, 146–155. Schwarzenberg.
Heath, S. B. (1997). Culture: Contested realm in research Macklin, R. (1999). Moral progress and ethnical
on children and youth. Applied Developmental universalism. In R.Macklin (Ed.), Against relativism:
Science, 1, 113–123. Cultural diversity and the search for ethical universal
Heller, J. (1972). Syphilis victims in the U.S. study went in medicine (pp. 249–274). New York: Oxford
untreated for 40 years. New York Times, 26 July University Press.
1972, 1, 8. Marshall, P. L. (1992). Research ethics in applied
Hoagwood, K., & Jensen, P. S. (1997). Developmental anthropology. IRB: A Review of Human Subjects
psychopathology and the notion of culture: Introduc- Research, 14, 1–5.
tion to the special section on ‘The fusion of cultural Marshall, P. L. (2003). Human Subjects Protections,
horizons: Cultural influences on the assessment Institutional Review Boards, and Cultural Anthro-
of psychopathology in children and adolescents.’ pological Research. Anthropological Quarterly, 76,
Applied Developmental Science, 1, 108–112. 269–285.
Indian Council of Medical Research. (2000). Ethical McGrady, B. S., & Bux, D. A. (1999). Ethical issues in
guidelines on biomedical research involving human informed consent with substance abusers. Journal of
subjects New Delhi: Indian Council of Medical Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 186–193.
Research. Retrieved May 11, 2007 from http://www. Milgram, S. (1964). Issues in the study of obedience:
icmr.nic.in/ethical.pdf. A reply to Baumrind. American Psychologist, 19,
International Committee of Medical Journal Editors. 848–852.
(2003). Uniform requirements for manuscripts sub- National Advisory Council on Drug Abuse. (2000).
mitted to biomedical journals: Writing and editing for Recommended guidelines for the administra-
biomedical publication. Retrieved April 2, 2004 from tion of drugs to human subjects. DA-01-002.
http://www.icmje.org/#conflicts. NIDA-CAMCODA. Retrieved January 11, 2004
International Sociological Association (2001). http:// from http://grants.nih.gov/grants/guide/noticefiles/
www.isa-sociology.org/about/isa_code_of_ethics.htm. NOT-DA-01-002.html.
Jones, J. H. (1993). Bad blood: The Tuskegee syphilis National Consensus Conference on Bioethics and
experiment, new and expanded ed. New York: Free Health Research in Uganda. (1997). Guidelines for
Press. the Conduct of Health Research Involving Human
Kamb, M. L., Rhodes, F., Hoxworth, T., Rogers, J., Subjects in Uganda. Kampala, Uganda.
Lentz, A., Kent, C., MacGowen, R., & Peterman, T. A. National Health and Medical Research Council
(1998). What about money? Effects of small monetary (NHMRC). (2007). National Statement on Ethical
incentives on enrollment, retention, and motivation to Conduct in Human Research. http://www.nhmrc.
change behavior in an HIV/STD prevention counseling gov.an/publications/synopses/_files/e72.pdf.
intervention. Sexually Transmitted Infection, 74, National Research Council. (2003). Protecting partici-
253–255. pants and facilitating social and behavior sciences
Knight, G. P., & Hill, N. E. (1998). Measurement equiv- research. In C. F. Citro, D. R. Ilgen, & C. B. Marret
alence in research involving minority adolescents. (Eds.). Washington, D.C.: The National Academies
In V. C. McLoyd & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Studying minor- Press.
ity adolescents: Conceptual, methodological,and Nolan, R. W. (2002). Anthropology in practice: building
theoretical issues (pp. 183–211). Mahwah, NJ: a career outside the academy (directions in applied
Erlbaum. anthropology). Boulder: Lynne Rienner.
RESEARCH ETHICS IN SOCIAL SCIENCE 109

Nuremberg Code. (1946). Journal of the American of medical ethics. Thailand: Ministry of Public
Medical Association, 132, 1090. Health.
Office for Protection From Research Risks, Department The Hindu. (2005). Editorial: A dangerous conflict of
of Health and Human Service, National Institutes interest. The Hindu. Retrieved May 11, 2007 from
of Health. (1993). Protecting human research http://www.hindu.com/2005/11/30/stories/2005113
subjects: Institutional review board guidebook. 002301000.htm.
Washington, DC: Government Printing Office. Trimble, J. E., & Fisher, C. B. (2006). The handbook
Ogundiran, T. O. (2004). Enhancing the African bioethics of ethical research with ethnocultural population
initiative. BMC Medical Education, 4, 21. and communities. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Pan African Bioethics Initiative (PABIN). (2001). Terms Publications.
of Reference. http://www.pabin.net/enindex.asp. Varma, R. (1999). Women and people’s science
Resnick, J. H., & Schwartz, T. (1973). Ethical standards movements in India. Technology & Society:
as an independent variable in psychological research. Historical, Societal, and Professional Perspectives
American Psychologist, 28, 134–139. Proceedings, 1999 International Symposium, 29-21,
Saunders, C. A., Thompson, P. D., & Weijer, C. (1999). 378–382.
What’s the price of a research subject? New England Wendler, D., Rackoff, J. E., Emanuel, E. J., &
Journal of Medicine, 341, 1550–1552. Grady, C. (2002). The ethics of paying for children’s
Shaner, A., Eckman, T. A., & Roberst, L. J. (1995). participation in research. Journal of Pediatrics,
Disability income, cocaine use, and repeated hos- 141(2), 166–171.
pitalization among schizophrenic cocaine abusers: Winslade, W. J., & Douard, J. W. (1992). Ethical issues
A government-sponsored revolving door? New in psychiatric research. In L. K. G. Hsu & M. Hersen
England Journal of Medicine, 333, 777–783. (Eds.), Research in psychiatry: Issues, strategies, and
Sieber, J. E. (1982). Ethical dilemmas in social research. methods (pp. 57–70). New York: Plenum.
In J. E. Sieber (Ed.), The ethics of social research: World Medical Association. (2000). Declaration of
Surveys and experiments (pp. 1–30). New York: Helsinki: Ethical principles for medical research
Springer-Verlag. involving human subjects. Edinburgh: World
Sieber, J. E. (1992). Planning ethically responsible Medical Association. Retrieved May 11, 2007 from
research: A guide for students and internal review http://www.wma.net/e/policy/pdf/17c.pdf.
boards. Thousand Oaks: CA. Sage Publications. Young, C. H., & Brooker, M. (2006). Safeguarding sacred
Smith, S. S., & Richardson, D. (1983). Amelioration lives: The ethical use of archival data for the study of
of deception and harm in psychological research. diverse lives. In J. E. Trimble & C. B. Fisher (Eds.),
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, The handbook of ethical research with ethnocultural
1075–1082. populations and communities. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Steinbock, B., Arras, J. D., & London, A. J. (2005). Sage Publications.
Ethical issues in modern medicine. New York, NY: Zuckerman, M. (1990). Some dubious premises in
McGraw-Hill Higher Education. research and theory on racial differences: Scientific,
Thailand Ministry of Public Health Ethics Committee. social and ethical issues. American Psychologist, 45,
(1995). Rule of the medical council on the observance 1297–1303.

You might also like