You are on page 1of 9

Available online at www.sciencedirect.

com

ScienceDirect

The complexity of learning, memory and neural


processes in an evolutionary ecological context
Hans M Smid1 and Louise EM Vet1,2

The ability to learn and form memories is widespread among multiple, spaced experiences before information is stored
insects, but there exists considerable natural variation between as LTM. Nevertheless, some animals form LTM after a
species and populations in these traits. Variation manifests single experience suggesting that they may have evolved
itself in the way information is stored in different memory forms. to rely on the value of the learned information more
This review focuses on ecological factors such as readily than other animals, rather their having evolved
environmental information, spatial aspects of foraging behavior superior learning and memory abilities [7]. Several factors
and resource distribution that drive the evolution of this natural have been described that play a role in learning and
variation and discusses the role of different genes and neural memory including the high energetic costs of memory
networks. We conclude that at the level of individual, formation [8,9], the effects of age, physiological state,
population or species, insect learning and memory cannot be longevity, stress and the number of lifetime experiences
described as good or bad. Rather, we argue that insects evolve [10–13,14].
tailor-made learning and memory types; they gate learned
information into memories with high or low persistence. This This review focuses on aspects of variation in the impor-
way, they are prepared to learn and form memory to optimally tance and reliability of the learned information. An animal
deal with the specific ecologies of their foraging environments. must continuously be able to adjust its behavior according
to previous experiences, but only if that experience is still
Addresses relevant at the time of memory retrieval. Thus, insects
1
Laboratory of Entomology, Wageningen University, Wageningen, The evolve preparedness to learn and form memories in a
Netherlands
2 manner that is adaptive in the context of their specific
Department of Terrestrial Ecology, Netherlands Institute of Ecology
(NIOO-KNAW), Wageningen, The Netherlands ecology. The term prepared learning [15] is used in this
review to describe preparedness to learn (for instance to
Corresponding author: Vet, Louise EM (l.vet@nioo.knaw.nl) learn odors more easily than colors) but also preparedness
to gate the learned information into specific forms of
Current Opinion in Insect Science 2016, 15:61–69
memory that differ in stability (for instance in MTM or
in LTM). Below we will describe recent advances in
This review comes from a themed issue on Behavioural ecology
studies that focus on ecological factors driving natural
Edited by Lars Chittka and Deborah M Gordon variation in prepared learning.
For a complete overview see the Issue and the Editorial
Available online 16th April 2016 Effects of environmental variation and spatial
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cois.2016.03.008 foraging behavior
2214-5745/# 2016 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Environmental variation has been proposed as one of the
major factors driving the evolution of variation in learning
and memory retention [7,16–18]. More variable environ-
ments promote short lasting memories, which can be
updated continuously. If environmental variation is
low, learned information remains relevant for a long time,
The use of previous experience to optimize behavior in which favors long lasting memories.
an adaptive manner is obviously of great benefit to all
animals, including insects [1,2]. However, this does not Spatial foraging behavior of the insect influences variation
mean that insects should learn and remember information in the perceived environment. An insect that migrates
from all possible experiences they encounter. Studies on after learning area-specific information does not benefit
diverse insect species have revealed the daunting com- from LTM formation because the learned information
plexity of different forms of memory each with different becomes obsolete after migration. This could drive the
stabilities, including short-term memory, mid-term mem- evolution of differences in prepared learning, because
ory, anesthesia-resistant memory and long-term memory formation of LTM would be more beneficial to insects
(STM, MTM, ARM and LTM), for example [3–6]. In that stay longer in a certain area than insects that tend to
Figure 1 we provide a basic description of these memory forage over longer distances to areas with very different
forms and their abbreviations. Why does learning not types of resources. Such variation in prepared learning
always result in the formation of LTM? To ensure the may have evolved within natural populations of Drosophi-
reliability of learned information, most animals require la melanogaster flies (both in larvae and in adults), where so

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Insect Science 2016, 15:61–69


62 Behavioural ecology

Figure 1

(a) preference of naive wasp learning changed preference: memory

(b) oviposition experience oviposition experience oviposition experience +


+ cold shock protein synthesis inhibitor
Memory retention

Memory retention

Memory retention
1h 1day 5days 1h 1day 5days 1h 1day 5days
Time Time Time

(c) slow consolidation fast consolidation, ARM consolidation


with ARM and LTM STM followed by LTM only, rapid decline
Memory retention

Memory retention

Memory retention

total total total

STM ARM LTM STM LTM STM ARM


1h 1day 5days 1h 1day 5days 1h 1day 5days
Time Time Time
Current Opinion in Insect Science

Memory retention can be determined by various behavioral choice assays [34,69], such as depicted in (a), or by measuring conditioned reflexes
[48] at different intervals between conditioning and memory retention tests, resulting in a graph as shown in (b) (left panel). This memory is
composed of at least three different memory types, uncovered by specific inhibitors are used. Short term memory (STM) can be erased by
anesthesia such as cold shock (cooling insects on ice, b mid panel). Long term memory (LTM) requires the production of new proteins and can be
inhibited by translation and/or transcription inhibitors (b, right panel). Memory that is not affected by both treatments is anesthesia-resistant
memory (ARM). Note that the acronym ARM is used specifically in fly literature, and mid term memory (MTM) or intermediate term memory (ITM) is
used as equivalent to ARM in other species. MTM in flies refers to a late phase of STM [70]. STM, ARM and LTM have further been subdivided in
early and late forms by other specific inhibitors, see, for example, [4–6]. From the combined results of control, cold shock and protein synthesis
inhibitor experiments, memory type graphs can be prepared as shown in (c) (left panel). Here the memory type is shown where first STM is
formed (orange), followed by ARM (blue) and LTM (green), with the sum of all memories (observed without inhibiting treatments) depicted as
yellow. Variations exist, for instance in the speed of consolidation of LTM and consolidation with or without intermediate ARM (c, left versus mid
panel) or in the consolidation of ARM only (c, right panel) [37]. Also, the persistence of memory forms over time can vary between species.

called rover flies tend to forage for food over longer phism on a gene encoding a cGMP dependent protein
distances, whereas sitter flies tend to aggregate and exploit kinase (PKG), called foraging ( f or). Rover flies have a
a local food source extensively [19]. This behavior has higher PKG activity level [20] and experience a much
been shown to result from a single nucleotide polymor- more variable environment than sitter flies due to their

Current Opinion in Insect Science 2016, 15:61–69 www.sciencedirect.com


Olfactory basis of plant–pollinator interactions Smid and Vet 63

explorative behavioral lifestyle. The for gene has been lists [32]. This was again supported by a direct comparison
implicated in memory formation in several studies, for of the generalist aphid Myzus persicae that can feed on over
instance in visual orientation memory in flies [21], and 40 different plant species, and a closely related subspecies
there is accumulating evidence that this gene plays an M. persicae nicotianae, which is a specialist on tobacco [33].
important role in natural variation in prepared learning. The generalist but not the specialist aphids change their
The more explorative rover type flies are prepared to learn preference for the host plant odors on which they were
fast but remember the learned information for a shorter reared, to odors of a plant on which they subsequently had
period than sitter flies, which can, due to their less experience. Moreover, this difference in preference
explorative behavior benefit more from long lasting mem- learning was correlated with expression levels of the
ories. It was found that in adult sitter flies, aversive foraging gene in winged morphs, where the specialist
memory retention was higher 24 hours after spaced con- had lower expression levels than the generalist.
ditioning, but lower 15 min after single trial conditioning
compared to rover flies [22]. A similar pattern was de- Also in parasitic wasps, profound differences in learning
scribed for fly larvae after appetitive conditioning [23]. If between closely related generalist and specialist species
rover flies were sequentially conditioned to associate two have been described, as for example between two para-
different odors with an aversive stimulus, they showed sitic wasps that parasitize fly pupa, Nasonia vitripennis and
strong memory retention for the last experienced odor but N. giraulti [34,52]. N. vitripennis is a generalist species that
poor memory for the first, whereas sitter flies remembered parasitizes a wide range of different fly species, occurring
both odors equally well [24]. The foraging allele even in manure, carcasses and bird nests, whereas N. giraulti is a
determines whether or not flies use social learning specialist, which parasitizes on flies of the genus proto-
[25,26]. Both in an olfactory and a spatial learning task, calliphora, which occur in bird nests. In this comparison,
sitter flies perform better when conditioned and tested in a the generalist formed LTM after a single host encounter,
group than alone, whereas this difference was not present whereas the specialist formed ARM; only after multiple
for rover flies. Again this may be an adaptation to the more oviposition experiences spaced in time it formed LTM.
aggregated behavior of sitter flies, versus the more explor-
ative, solitary behavior of rover flies. Using specific PKG Effects of resource distribution patterns
inhibitors or activators, these differences could be corre- The variability in the distribution patterns of resources
lated to PKG activity levels [25]. can also drive the evolution of learning and memory as
shown in the case of parasitic wasps that attack butterfly
Other examples of specific adaptations of prepared learn- hosts [35]. The large cabbage white butterfly Pieris
ing to foraging behavior were described recently in brassicae lays clusters of over 100 eggs, and carefully
locusts [27] and butterflies [17]. In the desert locust selects clusters of host plants of the same species to
Schistocerca gregaria, individuals normally forage in a soli- guarantee sufficient food resources to its offspring. In
tary manner and reject toxic food. They also are capable contrast, the small cabbage white butterfly (P. rapae) lays
of aversive as well as appetitive taste learning when in the single eggs on different, isolated, plant species. This
solitary phase. Under crowded conditions, locusts become results in profound differences in host distribution for
gregarious, and in this phase they migrate over long parasitic wasps. In this type of tritrophic relations, para-
distances. The increased risk for predation in the gregari- sitic wasps learn to remember the host plant odors on
ous phase is compensated for by accepting toxic food to which they encounter their hosts, but the reliability of the
acquire unpalatability to deter predators. Gregarious association between host plant and host insect varies as a
locusts are incapable of aversive taste learning, whereas consequence of the oviposition behavior of the host.
they do show normal appetitive learning [28]. Remark- Finding a caterpillar of P. brassicae constitutes a much
ably, the gregarious phase correlates to higher PKG more significant reward than of P. rapae, because due to
expression in the brain [29], again linking the foraging the gregarious behavior of P. brassicae it predicts the
gene with variation in prepared learning. In the small presence of many additional caterpillars on the same host
cabbage white butterfly, Pieris rapae, a correlation be- plant species. Indeed, Cotesia glomerata wasps, which can
tween explorative behavior and learning was also demon- parasitize both species, show a profound difference in
strated. In this species, individuals with smaller wings and memory formation upon encountering a caterpillar of
thoraxes perform better in optimizing their foraging effi- these two butterfly species. When C. glomerata wasps
ciency through experience in complex habitats compared were given a single oviposition experience with either
to individuals with larger wings and thoraxes, which can P. rapae or P. brassicae as a host, their 4hr memory
forage over larger distances [30]. retention level with either species was high and did
not differ significantly, however, after 24 hours memory
Another aspect of foraging behavior that can drive the retention was lower with P. rapae [35]. Using cold shock
evolution of learning and memory is the level of resource and transcription inhibitors (see Figure 1) it was found
specialization [31]. Generalists have been shown to that 4hr memory was ARM with P. rapae as a reward,
change their innate preference more easily than specia- whereas it was LTM using P. brassicae. Interestingly, this

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Insect Science 2016, 15:61–69


64 Behavioural ecology

clear-cut effect of host species on memory formation was LTM are mutually exclusive in flies; the formation of
also found for the egg parasitoid Trichogramma evanescens, ARM prevents LTM consolidation and vice versa [41].
which can also parasitize both P. brassicae and P. rapae
[36]. The formation of ARM and not LTM was described The dependency of LTM and ARM was also investigated
previously for a closely related parasitic wasp, Cotesia by bidirectional artificial selection over nine generations
rubecula, which only parasitizes P. rapae [37]. This further in C. glomerata [42]. As explained above, this species forms
strengthens the link between foraging behavior of the LTM for plant odors after a single oviposition experience
host and the wasp’s specific learning and memory char- when oviposition on P. brassicae is used as a reward. In this
acteristics. C. rubecula only forms LTM after three ovipo- experiment, it was determined whether genetic diversity
sition experiences spaced in time. Similarly, the that allows selection for wasps that formed ARM instead
associative learning of plant odors by another Cotesia of LTM exists in this species. Wasps were given a single
species, C. marginiventris, using two different host cater- experience on a plant with oviposition into P. brassicae as a
pillar species as a reward, showed that these wasps were reward and were subsequently tested for memory for the
more prepared to learn the association with an optimal learned plant odor in a two-choice wind tunnel set-up.
host species, Spodoptera exigua than with a suboptimal Wasps were selected for propagation of a next generation,
host species, Trichoplusia ni, in which a high percentage of when they showed ARM-specific memory dynamics [37],
mortality occurs [38]. However, in Nasonia parasitic namely memory retention at 4 hours but not 24 hours
wasps, which parasitize fly pupae, such an effect was after conditioning. In other words, the reliability of the
absent [39], even though the different fly hosts were learned association was limited to 4 hours, whereas for the
profoundly different in host suitability. Thus, parasitic control line it was reliable also at 24 hours. This resulted
wasps have evolved different forms of prepared learning, in a wasp strain where single trial learning induced STM
resulting in tailor-made memories adapted to the specific but not ARM or LTM, whereas in the control line STM
ecology or suitability of their host species. was directly followed by LTM. Both lines showed normal
LTM after 3 oviposition experiences spaced in time.
Evolutionary aspects of prepared learning From these experiments, it can be concluded that in C.
How such a difference in prepared learning may evolve glomerata the ‘switch’ that gates LTM or ARM formation
was recently demonstrated by Dunlap and Stephens after a single oviposition experience is completely deter-
[15], using experimental evolution with different selec- mined by the host species used as a reward (Figure 2).
tion regimes over 40 generations. Flies experienced two Genetic variation that is present in the population allows
food dishes, of which one dish contained an aversive taste for selection of strains that do not express LTM after a
stimulus. Each dish was tagged with a different odor and single oviposition experience on P. brassicae, but ARM is
color combination. Flies were allowed to oviposit on two only formed when P. rapae is used for oviposition [35].
new dishes with food tagged with a color and odor, but
without the aversive taste stimulus. Eggs were collected Interestingly, these studies on C. glomerata using single or
for the subsequent generation from those food dishes spaced experiences with P. brassicae, resulted in consoli-
where either the color or the odor matched the experi- dation of LTM within 4 hours. However, in C. rubecula,
enced situation (so where flies avoided the substrate that spaced experiences with P. rapae resulted in LTM con-
had the aversive stimulus during the conditioning phase), solidation that progresses at a much slower rate [37]. First,
whereas the other modality was combined randomly. ARM is formed which in the course of 3 days is replaced
This way, only color or odor reliably predicted oviposition by LTM, as shown in Figure 1c. Hence, more complex
opportunities, and it was found that this selection regime memory dynamics, such as the speed of consolidation, can
resulted in congruent adaptation in prepared learning; a be part of prepared learning.
selection regime where color cues were reliable resulted
in improved color learning, but decreased odor learning These studies show that there exists considerable, herita-
and vice versa. Thus, genetic diversity within a popula- ble variation within a population that allows rapid adapta-
tion allows for relatively fast adaptations in prepared tions in prepared learning, not only in the sense of what is
learning. learned (for instance odor or color) but also how the
learning information is gated into specific forms of memo-
Differences in prepared learning resulting in different ry. As a consequence, results from highly inbred lines may
gating of information into ARM and LTM could also be yield strain specific results. Indeed, a comparison of four
selected for, and again an inverse correlation in prepared different strains of N. vitripennis, including homozygous as
learning was found. Lagasse et al. [40] selected flies well as genetically diverse populations [43], and a panel of
bidirectionally for improved ARM or improved LTM, different isofemale lines of D. melanogaster [44] showed
and found a functional and evolutionary trade-off be- considerable differences in memory formation. Likewise,
tween these two memory forms; flies with improved variation in learning was found between a sexual and
ARM had reduced LTM and vice versa. Interestingly, asexual population of the parasitic wasp Venturia canescens
this finding correlates well with reports that ARM and [45], between individuals of different bumblebee colonies

Current Opinion in Insect Science 2016, 15:61–69 www.sciencedirect.com


Olfactory basis of plant–pollinator interactions Smid and Vet 65

Figure 2

low memory persistance high

memory

Memory

Memory

Memory

Memory
total total total

dynamics STM

Time
STM ARM

Time
STM ARM LTM

Time
STM LTM

Time

memory gating
mechanisms
examples of
(foraging gene,
ecological factors
dopaminergic neurons)
driving evolutionary
adaptations in STM
STM ARM STM
prepared learning: STM ARM LTM LTM

environmental variation: high low


foraging behavior: rover sitter
host behavior: solitary gregarious

tendency to gate prepared learning


information towards short
or long lasting memories
(left or right lanes)
information
Current Opinion in Insect Science

Tailor-made memory can be described by the learning and memory type, which comprises the dynamics of memory formation and retention in
insects induced by one or more types of conditioning. Learned information can be gated into different forms of memory, here represented as
different lanes on the memory highway. Left lanes refer to short lasting memory forms, STM and ARM, whereas the right lanes refer to long lasting
memory forms, such as the combination of STM, followed by ARM and LTM (slow consolidation), or LTM directly after STM (fast consolidation).
Prepared learning in this example is shown as the tendency to use the left or right lanes for gating of information. Examples of factors described
in this review that drive the evolution of prepared learning toward specific memory gating patterns are environmental variation, foraging behavior
and host behavior (leading to specific resource distribution patterns). The upper graphs indicate four possible examples of memory dynamics.
Note that a single experience generally results in STM and ARM, whereas LTM requires multiple experiences spaced in time, but some insects
already form LTM after a single experience [7]. Repeated experiences, spaced in time, increase the reliability of information and thereby also drive
the gating of information toward the right lanes.

[46,47], and between genetically different worker bees which induces ARM, and on P. brassicae, which induces
[48]. LTM. Furthermore, brains of C. glomerata and C. rubecula
were analyzed after 3 spaced oviposition experiences,
Mechanisms underlying natural variation in which induces LTM in both cases. In Nasonia, a compari-
learning and memory son was made between N. vitripennis, which forms LTM
Recently, considerable progress has been made to identify and N. giraulti, which forms ARM, both after a single
genes [49] and neural networks [50,51] involved in learn- oviposition experience. As expected, given the very large
ing and memory formation. However, the specific mecha- number of genes involved in learning and memory [49],
nisms underlying natural variation in prepared learning are many genes were differentially expressed in LTM in both
still unknown because most researchers that investigate Cotesia and Nasonia but there were also some genes that
learning and memory mechanisms do not consider natural were differentially expressed in opposing directions be-
variation. At the gene level, natural variation in prepared tween the ARM and LTM memory forms. These latter
learning in Nasonia and Cotesia parasitic wasps offered genes may be interesting in the context of the natural
opportunities to identify differentially expressed genes variation in prepared learning and the observed trade-offs
because of the clear-cut dichotomy between ARM and between ARM and LTM. One of the genes detected in
transcription dependent LTM [36,37,52]. In Cotesia, dif- Cotesia was the protein kinase Cd (PKCd) gene; one tran-
ferentially expressed genes were analyzed between brains script was downregulated in ARM but upregulated in
of wasps after a single oviposition experience on P. rapae, LTM memory forms. The orthologous Drosophila gene

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Insect Science 2016, 15:61–69


66 Behavioural ecology

which is involved in the regulation of LTM formation is and initially also in Drosophila [62]. However, in Drosoph-
called PKC98E and is involved in hyperphosphorylation of ila this view changed as octopamine signaling is only
the transcription factor cAMP responsive element binding involved in appetitive learning, leading to short lasting
(CREB) protein [53]. Another gene that was detected in memory formation, whereas dopamine signaling is in-
this comparison was the radish gene, a GTPase activating volved in learning leading to long lasting appetitive
gene specifically required for ARM in Drosophila [54]. This memory [14,63,64]. It is unclear to what extent this
gene was downregulated in LTM memory forms in Cotesia, discrepancy in reward pathways between species is
possibly to inhibit ARM formation. Finally, the PKG gene caused by differences in methodology [65]. Given the
foraging was upregulated in the ARM memory form in the supposed role of octopamine rather than dopamine in
Nasonia comparison. This is particularly interesting be- appetitive conditioning of honeybees, direct comparisons
cause the activity of this gene clearly correlates with many of the morphology of octopaminergic neurons have been
examples of natural variation described above. This gene performed between closely related species of parasitic
acts within the nitric oxide (NO) — cGMP — PKG signal- wasps of the genera Cotesia [66] and Nasonia [67], but no
ing pathway, which is known for its role in LTM formation differences in the number of octopaminergic neurons and
[21,55]. A phosphodiesterase involved in regulation of their projections could be linked to the species-specific
intracellular cGMP levels was downregulated in LTM differences in learning and memory. Possibly, dopami-
forms, both after a single and after 3 spaced oviposition nergic rather than octopaminergic neurons play a role in
experiences in Cotesia. these species-specific differences.

In another study, the genetic background underlying the Tailor-made memory described by learning
observed difference in memory formation between N. and memory types
vitripennis and N. giraulti was also investigated using Studies of natural variation in learning and memory refer
introgression techniques [28]. Since crosses between to individuals as good or bad learners. This generalization
these species produce fertile offspring after removing is misleading, because the significance of individual dif-
the endosymbiont Wolbachia by treatment with antibio- ferences in learning and memory strongly depends on the
tics, hybrids can be produced from isogenic strains [56]. types of learning and memory that are investigated as
Hybrids were tested for their memory and showed the N. illustrated with the differences in memory retention
giraulti phenotype, suggesting that the trait to form ARM between rover and sitter flies at different times after
after a single experience is dominant over LTM forma- learning [22]. In fact, a ‘bad’ learner may have developed
tion. Hereafter, hybrid females were backcrossed for preparedness to gate information in short lasting memory
several generations with males of N. vitripennis and only or to high levels of active forgetting, because of its specific
females with N. giraulti-like memory phenotypes were lifestyle or environment, and therefore learns in an adap-
used for the next generation. This way, the genes under- tive, optimized manner. We conclude that this natural
lying the N. giraulti-like memory phenotype were isolated variation in prepared learning should not be captured by
in a N. vitripennis background. After genotyping, two such simple terms as good or bad. We previously coined
chromosome regions could be identified that were linked the term ‘tailor-made memory’, referring to the observa-
to N. giraulti-like memory [28]. tion of species-specific or population-specific adaptations
[68] and by including the way how the acquired informa-
At the neuron level the apparent inverse correlation tion is stored in memory. The learning and memory type for
between ARM and LTM formation, or in more general a given population can be described by the total proper-
terms between short and long lasting memory, points to a ties and temporal dynamics of acquisition, consolidation,
mechanism that gates information either toward short waning, forgetting and retrieving of memory after a
lasting memory types, or more robust, long lasting mem- specific type of learning. Below we summarize the con-
ory forms (Figure 2). In Drosophila, two pairs of dopami- cept of prepared learning discussed in this paper. Varia-
nergic neurons determine whether memory is gated into tion in prepared learning occurs at different levels. Within
ARM or LTM, and these neurons are therefore candi- the behavioral repertoire of individual insects, there is
dates to play a role in the observed natural variation in this plasticity in the learning and memory type, for instance
form of prepared learning [41]. Such a gating mechanism depending on the host species of parasitic wasp involved
has so far only been found in Drosophila, and comparative [35]. At the population level, there is genetic variation in
studies of this mechanism on other species are compli- prepared learning associated with foraging behavior,
cated by fundamental differences in reward pathways linked to the foraging gene [22,24,25,26], or due to
between flies, honeybees and crickets. Until recently, genetic variation as for example in prepared learning of
signaling of aversive and appetitive reinforcement in visual and olfactory cues [15]. Finally, between species
insects was thought to be mediated in a dichotomous there exists variation due to species-specific adaptations
manner by dopaminergic and octopaminergic neurons of prepared learning to ecological needs, such as de-
respectively. This view was and is still supported by scribed between specialist and generalist species [33].
studies in crickets [57,58], in the Honeybee [59–61] Together, this variation allows for continuous fine-tuning

Current Opinion in Insect Science 2016, 15:61–69 www.sciencedirect.com


Olfactory basis of plant–pollinator interactions Smid and Vet 67

of prepared learning in order to adapt to changes in the dopaminergic neurons in Drosophila. Curr Biol 2015, 25:
751-758.
reliability and significance of information, supporting This study reveals the different dopaminergic neurons that play a role in
optimal decision making. acquisition of the STM trace induced by sweet taste, and LTM trace
induced by the nutritive value of the sugar reward.

Acknowledgements 15. Dunlap AS, Stephens DW: Experimental evolution of prepared


 learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2014, 111:11750-11755.
We thank Emma van der Woude for preparing the drawings in Flies that evolve in a situation where color, but not odor information is
Figures 1 and 2. Two anonymous reviewers provided valuable comments. reliable improve their ability for color learning and vice versa.
This study was supported by the NWO-ALW Open Competition grants 16. Mery F: Natural variation in learning and memory. Curr Opin
819.01.011 (to H.M.S.) and 2014.086 (to L.E.M.V). Neurobiol 2013, 23:52-56.
17. Snell-Rood EC: An overview of the evolutionary causes and
consequences of behavioural plasticity. Anim Behav 2013,
References and recommended reading 85:1004-1011.
Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review,
have been highlighted as: 18. Dunlap AS, Stephens DW: Components of change in the
evolution of learning and unlearned preference. Proc Biol Sci
 of special interest 2009, 276:3201-3208.
 of outstanding interest
19. Osborne KA, Robichon A, Burgess E, Butland S, Shaw RA,
Coulthard A, Pereira HS, Greenspan RJ, Sokolowski MB: Natural
1. Zrelec V, Zini M, Guarino S, Mermoud J, Oppliger J, Valtat A, behavior polymorphism due to a cGMP-dependent protein
 Zeender V, Kawecki TJ: Drosophila rely on learning while kinase of Drosophila. Science 1997, 277:834.
foraging under semi-natural conditions. Ecol Evol 2013, 3:
4139-4148. 20. Kaun KR, Sokolowski MB: cGMP-dependent protein kinase:
This study shows how D. melanogaster flies retain their memory after linking foraging to energy homeostasis. Genome 2009, 52:
conditioning when released in a semi natural situation. 1-7.
2. Kruidhof HM, Roberts AL, Magdaraog P, Muñoz D, Gols R, 21. Kuntz S, Poeck B, Sokolowski MB, Strauss R: The visual
Vet LEM, Hoffmeister TS, Harvey JA: Habitat complexity reduces orientation memory of Drosophila requires Foraging (PKG)
parasitoid foraging efficiency, but does not prevent upstream of Ignorant (RSK2) in ring neurons of the central
orientation towards learned host plant odours. Oecologia complex. Learn Mem 2012, 19:337-340.
2015:1-9.
22. Mery F, Belay AT, So AKC, Sokolowski MB, Kawecki TJ: Natural
3. Bouzaiane E, Trannoy S, Scheunemann L, Plaçais PY, Preat T: polymorphism affecting learning and memory in Drosophila.
Two independent mushroom body output circuits retrieve the Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2007, 104:13051-13055.
six discrete components of Drosophila aversive memory. Cell
Rep 2015, 11:1280-1292. 23. Kaun KR, Hendel T, Gerber B, Sokolowski MB: Natural variation
4. Schurmann D, Sommer C, Schinko APB, Greschista M, Smid H, in Drosophila larval reward learning and memory due to a
Steidle JLM: Demonstration of long-term memory in the cGMP-dependent protein kinase. Learn Mem 2007, 14:342-349.
parasitic wasp Nasonia vitripennis. Entomol Exp Appl 2012, 24. Reaume CJ, Sokolowski MB, Mery F: A natural genetic
143:199-206. polymorphism affects retroactive interference in Drosophila
5. Schurmann D, Kugel D, Steidle JL: Early memory in the melanogaster. Proc Biol Sci 2011, 278:91-98.
parasitoid wasp Nasonia vitripennis. J Comp Physiol A
25. Kohn NR, Reaume CJ, Moreno C, Burns JG, Sokolowski MB,
Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 2015, 201:375-383.
 Mery F: Social environment influences performance in a
6. Eisenhardt D, Kuhn C, Leboulle G: The PKA-CREB system cognitive task in natural variants of the foraging gene. PLoS
encoded by the honeybee genome. Insect Mol Biol 2006, ONE 2013, 8:e81272.
15:551-561. Both in an aversive olfactory conditioning test and in an aversive spatial
learning task it was shown that sitter but not rover flies perform better
7. Hoedjes KM, Kruidhof HM, Huigens ME, Dicke M, Vet LEM, when conditioned and tested in a group than alone.
Smid HM: Natural variation in learning rate and memory
dynamics in parasitoid wasps: opportunities for converging 26. Foucaud J, Philippe AS, Moreno C, Mery F: A genetic
ecology and neuroscience. Proc Biol Sci 2011, 278:889-897.  polymorphism affecting reliance on personal versus public
information in a spatial learning task in Drosophila
8. Plaçais PY, Preat T: To favor survival under food shortage, the melanogaster. Proc Biol Sci 2013, 280:20130588.
brain disables costly memory. Science 2013, 339:440-442. Both in an aversive olfactory conditioning test and in an aversive spatial
learning task it was shown that sitter but not rover flies perform better
9. Jaumann S, Scudelari R, Naug D: Energetic cost of learning and when conditioned and tested in a group than alone.
memory can cause cognitive impairment in honeybees. Biol
Lett 2013, 9:20130149. 27. Simões PM, Niven JE, Ott SR: Phenotypic transformation
affects associative learning in the desert locust. Curr Biol 2013,
10. Dunlap AS, McLinn CM, MacCormick HA, Scott ME, Kerr B: Why 23:2407-2412.
some memories do not last a lifetime: dynamic long-term
retrieval in changing environments. Behav Ecol 2009, 20: 28. Hoedjes KM, Smid HM, Vet LEM, Werren JH: Introgression study
1096-1105.  reveals two quantitative trait loci involved in interspecific
11. Sasakawa K, Uchijima K, Shibao H, Shimada M: Different variation in memory retention among Nasonia wasp species.
patterns of oviposition learning in two closely related Heredity 2014, 113:542-550.
ectoparasitoid wasps with contrasting reproductive Two Nasonia species with different memory types can be crossed, after
strategies. Naturwissenschaften 2013, 100:117-124. which their progeny can be backcrossed against N. vitripennis, while
selecting the N. giraulti phenotype of memory, using a memory test. After
12. Abram PK, Cusumano A, Peri E, Brodeur J, Boivin G, Colazza S: several generations, two QTL’s could be identified that underlie the N.
Thermal stress affects patch time allocation by preventing giraulti slow learning genotype.
forgetting in a parasitoid wasp. Behav Ecol 2015, 26:1326-1334.
29. Lucas C, Kornfein R, Chakaborty-Chatterjee M, Schonfeld J,
13. Farooqui T: Oxidative stress and age-related olfactory memory Geva N, Sokolowski MB, Ayali A: The locust foraging gene. Arch
impairment in the honeybee Apis mellifera. Front Genet 2014, Insect Biochem Physiol 2010, 74:52-66.
5:60.
30. Snell-Rood EC, Steck M: Experience drives the development of
14. Huetteroth W, Perisse E, Lin S, Klappenbach M, Burke C, movement-cognition correlations in a butterfly. Front Ecol Evol
 Waddell S: Sweet taste and nutrient value subdivide rewarding 2015, 3:21.

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Insect Science 2016, 15:61–69


68 Behavioural ecology

31. Bernays EA: Neural limitations in phytophagous insects: olfactory memory formation in Drosophila. Genetics 2015,
implications for diet breadth and evolution of host affiliation. 199:1173-1182.
Ann Rev Entomol 2001, 46:703-727.
50. Owald D, Lin S, Waddell S: Light, heat, action: neural control of
32. Vet LEM, Dicke M: Ecology of infochemical use by natural fruit fly behaviour. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2015,
enemies in a tritrophic context. Ann Rev Entomol 1992, 37: 370:20140211.
141-172.
51. Aso Y, Sitaraman D, Ichinose T, Kaun KR, Vogt K, Belliart-
33. Tapia DH, Silva AX, Ballesteros GI, Figueroa CC, Niemeyer HM,  Guérin G, Plaçais PY, Robie AA, Yamagata N et al.: Mushroom
Ramı́rez CC: Differences in learning and memory of host plant body output neurons encode valence and guide memory-
features between specialist and generalist phytophagous based action selection in Drosophila. Elife 2014, 3:e04580.
insects. Anim Behav 2015, 106:1-10. A highly influential study that shows how experience-dependent prefer-
ence levels for odors is encoded in a set of 34 MB output neurons.
34. Hoedjes KM, Steidle JL, Werren JH, Vet LEM, Smid HM: High-
throughput olfactory conditioning and memory retention test 52. Hoedjes KM, Smid HM: Natural variation in long-term memory
show variation in Nasonia parasitic wasps. Genes Brain Behav formation among Nasonia parasitic wasp species. Behav
2012, 11:879-887. Process 2014, 105:40-45.
35. Kruidhof HM, Pashalidou FG, Fatouros NE, Figueroa IA, Vet LEM, 53. Zhang J, Little CJ, Tremmel DM, Yin JC, Wesley CS: Notch-
 Smid HM, Huigens ME: Reward value determines memory inducible hyperphosphorylated CREB and its ultradian
consolidation in parasitic wasps. PLoS ONE 2012, 7:e39615. oscillation in long-term memory formation. J Neurosci 2013,
Using a preferred host as reward results in LTM consolidation, whereas 33:12825-12834.
using a less optimal host as reward results in ARM consolidation.
54. Folkers E, Waddell S, Quinn WG: The Drosophila radish gene
36. Kruidhof HM, de Rijk M, Hoffmann D, Harvey JA, Vet LEM, Soler R: encodes a protein required for anesthesia-resistant memory.
Effect of belowground herbivory on parasitoid associative Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006, 103:17496-17500.
learning of plant odours. Oikos 2012, 122:1094-1100.
55. Hou Q, Jiang H, Zhang X, Guo C, Huang B, Wang P, Wang T, Wu K,
37. Smid HM, Wang G, Bukovinszky T, Steidle JLM, Bleeker MAK, Li J et al.: Nitric oxide metabolism controlled by formaldehyde
van Loon JJA, Vet LEM: Species-specific acquisition and dehydrogenase (fdh, homolog of mammalian GSNOR) plays a
consolidation of long-term memory in parasitic wasps. Proc R crucial role in visual pattern memory in Drosophila. Nitric Oxide
Soc B 2007, 274:1539-1546. 2011, 24:17-24.
38. Harris CM, Ruberson JR, Meagher R, Tumlinson JH: Host 56. Werren JH, Loehlin DW: The parasitoid wasp Nasonia: an
suitability affects odor association in Cotesia marginiventris: emerging model system with haploid male genetics. Cold
implications in generalist parasitoid host-finding. J Chem Ecol Spring Harb Protoc 2009, 2009 pdb.emo134.
2012, 38:340-347.
57. Matsumoto Y, Matsumoto CS, Wakuda R, Ichihara S, Mizunami M:
39. Hoedjes KM, Kralemann LE, van Vugt JJ, Vet LEM, Smid HM:  Roles of octopamine and dopamine in appetitive and aversive
Unravelling reward value: the effect of host value on memory memory acquisition studied in olfactory conditioning of
retention in Nasonia parasitic wasps. Anim Behav 2014, 96:1-7. maxillary palpi extension response in crickets. Front Behav
40. Lagasse F, Moreno C, Preat T, Mery F: Functional and Neurosci 2015, 9:230.
evolutionary trade-offs co-occur between two consolidated Two studies that confirm previous evidence on the role of dopamine in
memory phases in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Biol Sci aversive and octopamine in appetitive conditioning of crickets.
2012, 279:4015-4023. 58. Awata H, Watanabe T, Hamanaka Y, Mito T, Noji S, Mizunami M:
41. Plaçais PY, Trannoy S, Isabel G, Aso Y, Siwanowicz I, Belliart-  Knockout crickets for the study of learning and memory:
Guérin G, Vernier P, Birman S, Tanimoto H, Preat T: Slow dopamine receptor Dop1 mediates aversive but not appetitive
oscillations in two pairs of dopaminergic neurons gate long- reinforcement in crickets. Sci Rep 2015, 5:1-9.
term memory formation in Drosophila. Nat Neurosci 2012, Two studies that confirm previous evidence on the role of dopamine in
15:592-599. aversive and octopamine in appetitive conditioning of crickets.

42. van den Berg M, Duivenvoorde L, Wang GH, Tribuhl S, 59. Hammer M: The neural basis of associative reward learning in
Bukovinszky T, Vet LEM, Dicke M, Smid HM: Natural variation in honeybees. Trends Neurosci 1997, 20:245-252.
learning and memory dynamics studied by artificial selection
60. Vergoz V, Roussel E, Sandoz JC, Giurfa M: Aversive learning in
on learning rate in parasitic wasps. Anim Behav 2011, 81:
honeybees revealed by the olfactory conditioning of the sting
325-333.
extension reflex. PLoS ONE 2007, 2:e288.
43. Koppik M, Hoffmeister TS, Brunkhorst S, Kieß M, Thiel A:
61. Klappenbach M, Kaczer L, Locatelli F: Dopamine interferes with
Intraspecific variability in associative learning in the parasitic
appetitive long-term memory formation in honey bees.
wasp Nasonia vitripennis. Anim Cogn 2015, 18:593-604.
Neurobiol Learn Mem 2013, 106:230-237.
44. Versace E, Reisenberger J: Large-scale assessment of
olfactory preferences and learning in Drosophila 62. Schwaerzel M, Monastirioti M, Scholz H, Friggi Grelin F, Birman S,
melanogaster: behavioral and genetic components. PeerJ Heisenberg M: Dopamine and octopamine differentiate
2015, 3:e1214. between aversive and appetitive olfactory memories in
Drosophila. J Neurosci 2003, 23:10495-10502.
45. Thiel A, Schlake S, Kosior D: Omnia tempus habent: habitat-
specific differences in olfactory learning and decision making 63. Burke CJ, Huetteroth W, Owald D, Perisse E, Krashes MJ, Das G,
in parasitic wasps. Anim Cogn 2013, 16:223-232. Gohl D, Silies M, Certel S, Waddell S: Layered reward signalling
through octopamine and dopamine in Drosophila. Nature 2012,
46. Ings TC, Raine NE, Chittka L: A population comparison of the 492:433-437.
strength and persistence of innate colour preference and
learning speed in the bumblebee Bombus terrestris. Behav 64. Liu C, Plaçais PY, Yamagata N, Pfeiffer BD, Aso Y, Friedrich AB,
Ecol Sociobiol 2009, 63:1207-1218. Siwanowicz I, Rubin GM, Preat T, Tanimoto H: A subset of
dopamine neurons signals reward for odour memory in
47. Raine NE, Chittka L: No trade-off between learning speed and Drosophila. Nature 2012, 488:512-516.
associative flexibility in bumblebees: a reversal learning test
with multiple colonies. PLoS ONE 2012, 7:e45096. 65. Søvik E, Perry CJ, Barron AB: Insect reward systems:
comparing flies and bees. In Advances in Insect Physiology, vol.
48. Junca P, Carcaud J, Moulin S, Garnery L, Sandoz JC: Genotypic 48. Edited by Zayed A, Kent CF. Academic Press; 2015:189-226.
influence on aversive conditioning in honeybees, using a novel
thermal reinforcement procedure. PLOS ONE 2014, 9:e97333. 66. Bleeker MAK, Van der Zee B, Smid HM: Octopamine-like
immunoreactivity in the brain and suboesophageal ganglion of
49. Walkinshaw E, Gai Y, Farkas C, Richter D, Nicholas E, Keleman K, two parasitic wasps, Cotesia glomerata and Cotesia rubecula.
Davis RL: Identification of genes that promote or inhibit Anim Biol 2006, 56:247-257.

Current Opinion in Insect Science 2016, 15:61–69 www.sciencedirect.com


Olfactory basis of plant–pollinator interactions Smid and Vet 69

67. Haverkamp A, Smid HM: Octopamine-like immunoreactive 69. Pitman JL, DasGupta S, Krashes MJ, Leung B, Perrat PN,
neurons in the brain and subesophageal ganglion of the Waddell S: There are many ways to train a fly. Fly 2009, 3:
parasitic wasps Nasonia vitripennis and N. giraulti. Cell Tissue 3-9.
Res 2014, 358:313-329.
70. Margulies C, Tully T, Dubnau J: Deconstructing memory in
68. Smid HM, Vet LEM: Learning in insects: from behaviour to Drosophila. Curr Biol 2005, 15:R700-R713.
brain. Anim Biol 2006, 56:121-124.

www.sciencedirect.com Current Opinion in Insect Science 2016, 15:61–69

You might also like