You are on page 1of 12

Computers in Human Behavior 122 (2021) 106839

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Computers in Human Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

Literature review

Fear of missing out and social networking sites use and abuse: A
meta-analysis
Giulia Fioravanti a, Silvia Casale a, *, Sara Bocci Benucci a, Alfonso Prostamo a, Andrea Falone b,
Valdo Ricca b, Francesco Rotella c
a
Department of Health Sciences, Psychology Unit, Via di San Salvi 12, 50135, University of Florence, Italy
b
Department of Health Sciences, Psychiatry Unit, Largo Brambilla 3, 50134, University of Florence, Italy
c
Psychiatry Unit, AOU Careggi Hospital, Largo Brambilla 3, 50134, Florence, Italy

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: A growing body of research has examined the potential effects of the Fear of Missing Out (FoMO) on Social
Fear of missing out Networking Site (SNS) use and Problematic SNS use (PSNSU). The aim of the current meta-analysis is to sum­
Meta-analysis marize findings on the relationship between FoMO levels and (i) SNS use and (ii) PSNSU. Furthermore, we meta-
Problematic social networking sites use
analyzed results on the associations between FoMO and some individual characteristics. The sample included 33
Social media addiction
Social networking site use
independent samples with a total of 21,473 participants. The results of the random-effects meta-analysis show a
positive correlation between FoMO and SNS use and between FoMO and PSNSU, with effect sizes indicating
robust associations. Age, sex, and geographic area of the samples did not moderate the associations. FoMO was
positively correlated with depression, anxiety, and neuroticism and negatively correlated with consciousness.
These results give robustness to the construct validity of FoMO itself, as this concept was introduced to explain
why some people might be especially attracted to social media. Moreover, concerns that others might be having
rewarding experiences that one is absent from seem to be a trigger for a compulsive use of social platforms,
driven by the need to get in touch with others, or as tool to develop social competence.

1. Introduction and anxieties that people have in relation to being out of touch with the
satisfying events and experiences taking place across their extended
As social networking sites (SNSs) such as Instagram and Facebook social circles. As such, FoMO has recently received great scientific
are used worldwide on a daily basis by millions of people, growing attention in the field of social media use, because SNSs are considered an
scientific interest has emerged towards psycho-social factors driving ideal tool for fulfilling the “desire to stay continually connected with
high SNS engagement. Studies on social media use and abuse are often what others are doing” (Przybylski et al., 2013, p. 1841). In fact, initial
characterized by a confirmatory approach in which empirical studies support for the involvement of FoMO in social media engagement has
have focused their attention on those psychosocial risk factors (e.g., low been recently given with convergent results on the positive association
self-esteem, impulsivity) that have already been demonstrated to play a between FoMO and high SNS use (see supplementary material Table S1).
role in the development or maintenance of substance addictions (Bil­ However, empirical findings on the strength of the association have
lieux, Maurage, Lopez-Fernandez, Kuss, & Griffiths, 2015). Conversely, been mixed. For instance, Beyens, Frison, and Eggermont (2016)
although various recent evidence has supported its role in a variety of observed a moderate correlation (r = 0.50), whereas Chai et al. (2019)
negative outcomes (e.g., Scott & Woods, 2018), the concept of the fear of found a weak association (r = 0.0.21). This might at least in part be due
missing out (FoMO; Przybylski, Murayama, DeHaan, & Gladwell, 2013) to the fact that FoMO seems to vary depending on sociodemographic
was first introduced to describe a possible specific pathway towards high characteristics. In fact, young people tend towards higher levels of
social media engagement. FoMO is defined as “a pervasive apprehension FoMO (Casale & Fioravanti, 2020; Elhai et al., 2018; Przybylski et al.,
that others might be having rewarding experiences from which one is 2013; Stead & Bibby, 2017), whereas conflicting results about sex dif­
absent” (Przybylski et al., 2013, p. 1841). It reflects concerns, worries, ferences have been reported. Some studies show that men have high

* Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: giulia.fioravanti@unifi.it (G. Fioravanti), silvia.casale@unifi.it (S. Casale), sara.bocci@stud.unifi.it (S.B. Benucci), alfonso.prostamo@stud.unifi.
it (A. Prostamo), valdo.ricca@unifi.it (V. Ricca), francesco.rotella@unifi.it (F. Rotella).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2021.106839
Received 4 November 2020; Received in revised form 30 March 2021; Accepted 19 April 2021
Available online 30 April 2021
0747-5632/© 2021 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
G. Fioravanti et al. Computers in Human Behavior 122 (2021) 106839

levels of FoMO, but only in a young sample (Przybylski et al., 2013). likely to develop PSNSU, which in turn causes them to “phub” (phone +
Other studies have found that women have higher levels of FoMO than snubbing) their offline interactions (Franchina, Vanden Abeele, Van
men (Casale, Rugai, & Fioravanti, 2018; Elhai et al., 2018; Stead & Rooij, Lo Coco, & De Marez, 2018). Similarly, Wegmann, Oberst, Stodt,
Bibby, 2017), whereas other findings indicate no sex differences (Casale and Brand (2017) found that FoMO mediates the association between
& Fioravanti, 2020). depression and Internet-communication disorder.
We thus want to provide a meta-analytic summary on the association A different line of research has hypothesized that FoMO might act as
between FoMO and social media use/abuse, thereby assuming that this an additional factor beyond personality traits or psychiatric disorders
association is moderated by sociodemographic characteristics. rather than be caused by them. Blackwell, Leaman, Tramposch,
Osborne, and Liss (2017) examined the effects of extraversion, neurot­
1.1. FoMO, social networking site use and problematic social networking icism, attachment styles, and levels of FoMO on PSNS. FoMO was the
site use only significant predictor of PSNSU, but a positive correlation between
neuroticism and FoMO was found, thus suggesting that FoMO might be
The relationship between FoMO and SNSs engagement has been at least partly derived from a more stable tendency to experience
proposed within the self-determination theory (SDT, Deci & Ryan, 1985; negative emotions. The latter result is in accordance with Stead and
Ryan & Deci, 2000) framework. SDT supports three basic psychological Bibby’s work (2017), which finds emotional stability to be negatively
needs that must be satisfied to foster well-being and health: a need for correlated with FoMO among young adults. Such a study also revealed a
competence (i.e., to feel effective, skillful, and able to face life chal­ moderate negative association between conscientiousness and FoMO.
lenges), a need for autonomy (i.e., to feel that one causes, identifies with, FoMO was found to be negatively associated with both general mood
and endorses one’s own behavior), and a need for relatedness (i.e., to and overall life satisfaction (Blachnio & Przepiòrka, 2018; Przybylski
feel close to and accepted by others and gain social acceptance). Przy­ et al., 2013).
bylski et al. (2013) found that individuals showing less satisfaction in As already mentioned, the magnitude of the association between
regard to these three basic psychological needs also report higher levels FoMO and impaired control over SNSs use seems to vary depending on a
of FoMO, which in turn leads to an increased behavioral engagement in number of factors. When sex-related differences were not considered,
SNS use. the empirical findings on the association between FoMO levels and PSNS
The existence of unmet needs was also the theoretical starting were generally consistent, showing a moderately positive association.
premise of Beyens and colleagues’ study (2016). These authors found For example, Blackwell et al. (2017) found a moderate correlation (r =
that adolescents’ need to belong and need for popularity were related 0.56) for a southeastern U.S. sample of young adults. Similarly, a
with increased FoMO, which, in turn, was associated with increased moderate correlation (r = 0.43) was found by Tunc-Aksan and Akbay
Facebook use. Wang et al. (2018) found that FoMO mediated the asso­ (2019) in a sample of Turkish adolescents. However, evidence from
ciation between the unmet need to belong and authentic other studies (e.g., Casale et al., 2018) seems to suggest that the
self-presentation on SNSs (i.e., authentically sharing one’s own feelings, magnitude of the correlation might depend on sex (i.e., r = 0.47 and r =
thoughts, and life events on SNSs) among adolescents with a low level of 0.26 among men and women, respectively). Moreover, no cross-cultural
perceived social support. studies have examined the possible effect of geographical location on the
Przybylski et al. (2013) also pointed out the potential links of FoMO relation between FoMO and PSNSU, even though the strength of the
with psychological health and well-being. Again, the authors speculated association may differ between countries. In fact, it has been highlighted
that FoMO could play an important role in linking individual variability that Internet behaviors can be better understood when the sociocultural
in multiple factors, such as psychological need satisfaction, overall and digital environment in which the problematic behaviors have
mood, and general life satisfaction, to social media engagement. evolved and take place are taken into consideration (Kuss, Kristensen, &
Researchers have also been increasingly interested in the possibility Lopez-Fernandez, 2021). For instance, Stead and Bibby (2017) found a
that FoMO levels might drive not only the level of engagement with weak, non-significant association between FoMO and PSNSU for a
SNSs, but also the development of a compulsive (i.e., unregulated) use of sample of undergraduates from the United Kingdom, whereas the
SNSs. Problematic SNS use (PSNSU) has been defined as a lack of self- overwhelming majority of the literature studies tend to report positive
regulation in one’s own use of SNSs that lead to negative conse­ associations.
quences in everyday life (Andreassen & Pallesen, 2014). Several scholars
(e.g., Kuss & Griffiths, 2011) argue that SNS use may be addictive 1.2. The present study
because some individuals experience symptoms similar to those expe­
rienced by those who suffer from other forms of addiction, and women Research on the FoMO construct has rapidly expanded in recent
are more likely to exhibit PSNSU (Su, Han, Yu, Wu, & Potenza, 2020). years, mainly - albeit not exclusively - in social media use research.
Mood modification, salience, tolerance, withdrawal symptoms, conflict, Relative to many other factors that have been a-priori linked to PSNS (e.
and relapse appear to be present in those who use SNSs excessively g., low self-esteem, impulsivity) because of their relevance in other
(Müller et al., 2016). Those who are concerned that they might miss an behavioral addictions, FoMO has been introduced first to explain social
opportunity for social interaction and/or rewarding experiences media engagement, thus warranting scientific attention in this field. Our
happening across their friend networks are more likely to show a defi­ primary aim was to summarize the evidence on the association between
cient self-regulation in their own use of SNSs because of the need to stay FoMO and social media use/abuse, notwithstanding the fact that FoMO
continually connected to what their friends are doing and to alleviate has been considered as a mediating or rather a moderating variable.
anxious feelings of being socially excluded (see Table S1). However, as FoMO has been theoretically conceptualized as a mediating
In keeping with Przybylski et al. (2013), some researchers have or moderating factor by most studies, we decided to include, as sec­
speculated that FoMO might act as a mediator variable in the relation­ ondary outcomes, the meta-analysis of the correlations between FoMO
ship between individual differences (e.g., personality trait factors or and all the other psychological variables assessed in the included studies
predisposing psychopathology) and PSNSU. Alt and Boniel-Nissim (e.g., personality traits). Providing evidence of an association between
(2018a) evidenced that those with high levels of neuroticism were FoMO and specific personality traits (e.g., neuroticism) might lend
more likely to experience the fear of missing out and then tended to initial support to the line of research that suggests that FoMO needs to be
compulsively use SNSs to fulfil their psychological need for social considered in the context of other psychological mechanisms, as a
connection which, in turn, interfered with their lives. Oberst, Wegmann, consequence or as a moderating variable.
Stodt, Brand, and Chamarro (2017) found that adolescents with strong Finally, as reviewed above, there are some debated issues that a
experiences of FoMO due to depression and anxiety symptoms are more meta-analysis on this topic could help clarify, such as (i) the possible

2
G. Fioravanti et al. Computers in Human Behavior 122 (2021) 106839

effect of geographic location on the association between FoMO and SNS/ openness, agreeableness, consciousness, extraversion, neuroticism, life
PSNS, that is, whether the findings can be generalized across countries; satisfaction, and fear of negative evaluation) and grouping by outcome.
(ii) the sex-related differences about the magnitude of the correlations Even in this case, a separate analysis for each outcome was performed. A
between FoMO and SNS/PSNS; (iii) if the association between FoMO meta-regression was performed to assess the influence of sex and age in
and SNS/PSNS differs according to the age of the users. For these rea­ the primary analysis results. Furthermore, an analysis grouped by area
sons, sample characteristics, including proportion of females, was performed to estimate the influence of geographic area on primary
geographic location of the sample, and mean age of participants have outcomes. All analyses were conducted by applying a random effects
been considered as potential moderators. model and a 95% confidence interval. Heterogeneity was computed by
using I2 statistics. To assess publication bias, funnel plot interpretation
2. Methods and Egger’s (Egger, Davey Smith, Schneider, & Minder, 1997) regression
intercept were used. All analyses were performed using software pro­
The reporting of this systematic review and meta-analysis follows gram Review Manager (RevMan) [Computer program], Version 5.3,
PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta- Copenhagen: The Nordic Cochrane Centre, The Cochrane Collaboration,
Analyses) guidelines provided by Liberati et al. (2015). The identi­ 2014.
fication of relevant abstracts, selection of studies and subsequent data
extraction were performed independently by two of the authors (A.P. 3. Results
and S.B.B.), and conflicts were resolved by a third investigator (G.F.).
3.1. Results of the literature search
2.1. Literature search
The study flow summary is reported in Fig. 1. Of the 112 records
Medline, Scopus, Web of Science, and Science Direct searches for identified through the database search, 33 fulfilled the inclusion criteria.
eligible studies published up to December 3, 2019 were performed using Of these, we excluded from our quantitative analysis the study con­
the following terms: “FoMO AND (problematic social network use OR so­ ducted by Wegmann et al. (2017), as these authors assess FoMO with a
cial network use OR internet OR social media use OR problematic social modified bifactorial construct, whereas all of the other studies included
media use OR social media addiction OR social networking use OR social use the original FoMO monofactorial construct. In two cases, we had to
networking addiction OR problematic internet use OR internet addiction OR determine whether to extract more effect sizes of the same outcome from
Generalized Problematic Internet Use OR Facebook use OR problematic the same study: (i) from Buglass, Binder, Betts, and Underwood (2017),
Facebook use OR Facebook addiction OR Instagram use OR problematic we discarded longitudinal data and considered only cross-sectional data
Instagram use OR Instagram addiction).” We include terms such as in accordance with our protocol; and (ii) from Dhir, Yossatorn, Kaur, and
“Internet addiction” and “Internet use” due to evidence that some re­ Chen (2018), we extracted only the effect size derived from Study A,
searchers include the assessment of SNS use/abuse with the assessment which was conducted before Study B, to limit any distortions due to the
of Internet use/abuse (e.g., Alt & Boniel-Nissim, 2018a). PROSPERO participants’ familiarity with the test. Therefore, the number of full text
REGISTRATION NUMBER: CRD42020184991. articles was 32. Since one paper reported data deriving from two inde­
pendent studies, a total of 33 studies were included in the meta-analysis.
2.2. Inclusion criteria and study selection Of these, 13 analyze the relationship between FoMO and SNS use, 14
analyze the relationship between FoMO and PSNS and the remaining six
All cross-sectional studies assessing both the fear of missing out studies examine both relationships.
(FoMO) and social network site (SNS) use or problematic social network The characteristics of the studies included in the meta-analysis are
site use (PSNS) were included. Only articles written in English were summarized in Table 1. In each study, FoMO was measured with the
included. Articles lacking quantitative data were excluded. For the ar­ Fear of Missing Out Scale (Przybylski et al., 2013) in its original English
ticles included for eligibility, the full text was analyzed. form or with other versions in non-original languages either validated
(the Italian, Turkish or Spanish versions) or non-validated (the Chinese
2.3. Data extraction and quality assessment version). On the other hand, SNS use or PSNS was assessed with different
scales or items depending on the study (Table 2), all with good psy­
The following information for each study was collected when chometric characteristics. Of the 33 studies, 23 included data on the
available: first author, year of publication, journal, area, population association between FoMO and secondary outcomes (depression n = 3;
type, sample size, mean age, female percentage, mean and standard anxiety n = 3; fear of negative evaluations n = 3; neuroticism n = 3;
deviation of FoMO, SNS use, PSNS, depression, anxiety, openness, extraversion n = 3; agreeableness n = 2; openness n = 2; life satisfaction
agreeableness, life satisfaction, extraversion, fear of negative evalua­ n = 2; and consciousness n = 2). The total sample consisted of 21.473
tion, neuroticism, consciousness, assessment measures for each domain participants (F = 44.65%, mean age = 22.70 ± .38, and range: 13–41
and correlation values. Primary outcomes were the correlations between years old).
FoMO and SNS use and between FoMO and PSNS. Secondary outcomes
were correlations between FoMO and the other selected domains. In 3.2. Associations between FoMO and SNS use
cases of missing data, authors of the included studies were contacted for
additional information. In cases of two or more effect sizes of an Meta-analytic results of the random-effects model applied to a total
outcome extractable from one study, we extracted all of the effect sizes of 19 independent samples show a positive correlation between FoMO
only when independence between effect sizes was assumed. and SNS use (Fisher’s Z = 0.322; SE = 0.049; 95% CI .226, .418; Z =
6.584; p < .001). The results are summarized in Fig. 2. Concerning the
2.4. Statistical analysis risk of publication bias, the funnel plot (Fig. 3) shows data symmetry,
denoting a low risk of publication bias; this result was confirmed by
The primary analysis was performed including all of the effect sizes Egger’s test (Egger et al., 1997), which yielded a statistically nonsig­
assessed by converting correlations between FoMO and PSNS and FoMO nificant p-value of .52.
and SNS use extracted by primary studies in Fisher’s Z values. A separate
analysis for each outcome was performed. The secondary analysis was 3.3. Associations between FoMO and problematic SNS use
carried out including all of Fisher’s Z values indicating a correlation
between FoMO and one of the secondary domains (depression, anxiety, Meta-analytic results of the random-effects model applied to a total

3
G. Fioravanti et al. Computers in Human Behavior 122 (2021) 106839

Fig. 1. Flow chart for the study included in the meta-analysis.

of 20 independent samples indicate a positive correlation between 3.4. Associations between FoMO and secondary outcomes
FoMO and PSNS (Fisher’s z = 0.486; SE = 0.031; 95% CI .426, .547; Z =
15.695; p < .001). The results of the analysis are shown in Fig. 4. Con­ The results of the analysis are shown in Fig. 6. FoMO was positively
cerning the risk of publication bias, the funnel plot (Fig. 5) shows data correlated with depression (Fisher’s Z = 0.352; SE = 0.146; 95% CI .065,
symmetry, denoting a low risk of publication bias; this result was .639; Z = 2.404; p = .016), anxiety (Fisher’s Z = 0.693; SE = 0.022, 95%
confirmed by Egger’s test (Egger et al., 1997), which yielded a statisti­ CI .650, .737; Z = 31.240; p < .001), fear of negative evaluation (Fisher’s
cally nonsignificant p-value of .33. Z = 0.501; SE = 0.030; 95% CI .442, .560; Z = 16.654; p < .001) and
neuroticism (Fisher’s Z = 0.379; SE = 0.060; 95% CI .261, .496; Z =
6.322; p < .001). Otherwise, FoMO was negatively correlated with
conscientiousness (Fisher’s Z = − 0.276; SE = 0.072; 95% CI -.417,

4
G. Fioravanti et al. Computers in Human Behavior 122 (2021) 106839

Table 1 analysis was conducted on a total of 19 samples (Asia = 4; Europe + UK


Characteristics of the included studies. = 6; Latin America = 1; the Middle East = 1; the USA = 7) for the
First author Sample % of Mean SD of age National setting relation between FoMO and SNS use and on a total of 18 samples (Asia
(year) size women age of of = 1; Europe + UK = 9; Latin America = 1; the Middle East = 4; the USA
sample sample = 3) for the relation between FoMO and PSNS. The results of the analysis
Alt (2015) 296 85.30 25.4 7.1 Israel show that geographic area interfered with the correlation between
Alt (2018a) 216 49.00 – Israel FoMO and SNS use (Q-value(df) = 11.78(4); p = . 019). More in detail,
Alt (2018b) 359 52.60 48.38 5.36 Israel the strength of correlation between the two dimensions seems to be
Alt (2018c) 270 49.00 Israel
greater in the studies performed in the Middle East and in Europe + UK

Bailey (2018) 296 67.00 – Costa Rica/
Colombia and lower in the Latin America. However, geographical area did not
Barry (2017) 113 45.10 15.27 1.02 U.S.A. moderate the correlation between FoMO and PSNS (Q-value (df) = 0.15
Beyens 402 57.00 16.41 1.43 Belgium (4); p = .997).
(2016)
The results of the meta-regression analysis of the correlation between
Blackwell 207 7.38 22.15 7.38 U.S.A.
(2017) FoMO and SNSs use adjusted for age and sex show that the effect was not
Błachnio 360 – 22.20 6.84 Poland related to covariates (Q = 2.25; df = 2; p = .324), and the goodness of fit
(2018) test (Tau2 = 0.044; I2 = 95.73% Q(df) = 210.94(9); p = .0000) dem­
Buglass (2017 506 47.00 20.70 9.1 U.K. onstrates that the model did not explain the variance between studies.
study 1)
Casale (2018) 573 54.60 22.39 2.82 Italy
Similar results were obtained from a meta-regression analysis of the
Casale (2020 239 61.10 23.02 2.64 Italy correlation between FoMO and PSNSU adjusted for age and sex. Tests of
study 2) the model (Q = 0.34; df = 2; p = .842) conclude that the effect size is not
Chai (2019) 1319 53.70 – China related to the covariates. Furthermore, the regression model did not
Dempsey 291 57.60 20,03 3.06 U.S.A.
explain variance between studies, as the goodness of fit test showed
(2019)
Dhir (2018 1554 45.50 14.54 1.73 Finland (Tau2 = 0.17; I2 = 92.91%; Q(df) = 127.01(9); p = .0000). A summary of
study 1) the meta-regression results is shown in Table 3.
Franchina 2663 57.10 14.87 1.67 Belgium
(2018) 3.6. Sensitivity analysis
Hadlington 630 50.00 41.41 14.18 U.K.
(2018)
Liu (2018a) 465 – 18.83 1.08 China Finally, consistent with the MOOSE guidelines (Stroup et al., 2000),
Oberst (2017) 1468 74.30 16.59 0.62 Latin-America a sensitivity analysis was performed based on study quality. Three
Pontes (2018) 511 64.60 – – criteria were used to define methodological quality based on literature
Przybylski 2079 49.90 43.21 11.49 U.K.
suggestions (Downes, Brennan, Williams, & Dean, 2016) and the infor­
(2013)
Roberts (2019 107 47.00 – U.S.A. mation available in the studied reports: (i) the use of a previously
study 1) published validated scale of the Fear of Missing Out Scale; (ii) reports of
Roberts (2019 458 45.00 20.35 0.94 U.S.A. any information on scale score reliability (e.g., Cronbach’s alpha or
study 2) test-retest) in the study sample; and (iii) reports of any information on
Rogers (2019) 97 62.00 19.81 2.55 U.S.A.
Salim (2017) 326 73.90 21.44 2.12 Indonesia
the factorial validity of the Fear of Missing Out Scale in the study sample.
Scott (2018) 101 66.00 – U.K. Overall, 32 studies satisfied two of the three criteria and were catego­
Sha (2019) 2299 13.00 30.33 9.8 Germany, rized as being of higher quality among the sample of studies included in
Austria & this meta-analysis. We then performed the analysis only for the relation
Switzerland
between FoMO and SNS use, as the only study that satisfied one of the
Stead (2017) 495 69.00 20.62 1.6 –
Tomczyk 717 47.00 13.00 5.59 Bosnia and three criteria was focused on this relation. For the relation between
(2018) Herzegovina FoMO and SNS use, the resulting Fisher’s Z and confidence interval were
Tunc-Aksan 296 45.90 – Turkey recorded as Fisher’s Z = 0.32 (SE = 0.05), 95% CI [.22, .42], Z = 6.35, p
(2019) < .001.
Wang (2018) 832 48.00 16.43 0.93 China
Wolniewicz 299 57.1 20.00 3.02 U.S.A.
(2018) 4. Discussion and conclusion
Yin (2015) 629 40.20 – Taiwan
The main purpose of the present meta-analytic study was to sum­
marize the evidence on the association between FoMO and social media
− 0.135; Z = 3.848; p < .001). No significant results emerged from
use/abuse, taking into consideration possible moderators. Our findings
correlation analyses of FoMO with agreeableness (Fisher’s Z = − 0.15;
indicate that FoMO is positively related to both SNSs use and PSNSU,
SE = 0.119; 95% CI -.382, .083, Z = − 1261; p = .207), openness
with effect sizes that can be considered moderate according to Cohen’s
(Fisher’s Z = 0.208; SE = 0.277; 95% CI -.336, .751; Z = 0.749; p =
criteria (1992), and large according to Hemphill’s criteria (2003).
.454), extraversion (Fisher’s Z = 0.053; SE = 0.027; 95% CI -.001, .107;
Moreover, the sensitivity analysis and analyses of publication bias
Z = 1.942; p = .052) and life satisfaction (Fisher’s Z = − 0.066; SE =
confirm that these results are robust. Interestingly, the relationship be­
0.021; 95% CI -.107, .726; Z = 3.219; p = .001).
tween FoMO and PSNSU was found to be stronger than the relationship
between FoMO and social networking site use. These results, as a whole,
3.5. Subgroup analysis and meta-regression give robustness to the construct validity of FoMO itself, as this concept
was introduced to explain why some people might be especially
To establish whether the geographic area moderates the correlations attracted to social media (Przybylski et al., 2013). In fact, our findings
between FoMO, on the one hand, and SNS engagement and PSNSU, on are consistent with Przybylski et al.’s speculations (2013) that PSNSU
the other, we performed a subgroup analysis. We recoded the moderator might arise as a result of an effort to mitigate emotional frustration. Put
to convert variable “country” extracted from the original paper into new differently, FoMO might not be only an important mechanism that ex­
variable “geographic area” to enable the formation of subgroups. We plains the widespread use of social media but also an important pre­
identified five macroareas (Europe + UK, Asia, Latin America, the cursor of impaired control with such media. Worries that others might
Middle East and the USA) corresponding to as many subgroups. The be having rewarding experiences that one is absent from seem to be a

5
G. Fioravanti et al. Computers in Human Behavior 122 (2021) 106839

Table 2
Psychometric characteristics of FoMo, SNS use and PSNSU measures of the included studies.
First Author Assessment FoMO/ Cronbach’ s α Assessment SNSs use Cronbach’s Assessment problematic Cronbach’s
(year) Language version α SNSs use α
Alt (2015) FoMOs/ENG .83 SME .84
Alt (2018a) FoMOs/ENG .82 SPIUT .80
Alt (2018b) FoMOs/ENG .80 SPIUT .83
Alt (2018c) FoMOs/ENG .81 SPIUT .80
Bailey (2018) FoMOs/ENG - short form .84 PEOUSM .85
Barry (2017) FoMOs/ENG .94 Social Media Survey - adolescent version *
Beyens (2016) FoMOs/ENG .84 The Facebook Intensity Scale .78 Perceived stress related to .82
Facebook use
Blackwell (2017) FoMOs/ENG .91 SME .85 BSMAS .88
Błachnio (2018) FoMOs/ENG .78 FIQ .87
Buglass (2017 FoMOs/ENG .88 One item on individual daily use of –
study 1) Facebook
Casale (2018) FoMOs/ITA .81 BSMAS Italian version .82
(preliminary version)
Casale (2020 FoMOs/ITA Factor 1: .79 BSMAS Italian version .83
study 2) Factor 2: .73
Chai (2019) FoMOs/Chinese .73 Qzone use intensity scale .85
translation
Dempsey (2019) FoMOs/ENG .87 Five items regarding how often they engage .87 BSMAS .87
in various Facebook features
Dhir (2018 study FoMOs/ENG - short form ** BSMAS - Compulsive Use **
1)
Franchina (2018) FoMOs/ENG - short form .56 Items on breadth of social media platform – PSMU .82
used (number of platforms used)
Hadlington FoMOs/ENG .91 RFDT .80
(2018)
Liu (2018a) FoMOs/Chinese .85 The Chinese social media .94
translation addiction scale
Oberst (2017) FoMOs/ES .82 SNI .78
Pontes (2018) FoMOs/ENG .91 BSMAS .86
Przybylski FoMOs/ENG .90 SME .82
(2013)
Roberts (2019 FoMOs/ENG .88 SMI .86
study 1)
Roberts (2019 FoMOs/ENG .86 SMI .93
study 2)
Rogers (2019) FoMOs/ENG .90 Social Media Use - frequency –
Salim (2017) FoMOs/ENG .79 Self-Presentation Tactics in Social Media .83
Scott (2018) FoMOs/ENG .89 Nighttime social media use .82
Sha (2019) FoMOs/ENG .76 SAS - Facebook Use .90
Disorder
Stead (2017) FoMOs/ENG .83 Spraggins’ modified .89
questionnaire
Tomczyk (2018) FoMOs/ENG .89 SMI .89 BSMAS .89
Tunc-Ansan FoMOs/TR .81 SMAS .74
(2019)
Wang (2018) FoMOs/ENG .80 Honest Self-presentation .74
Scale/Chinese version
Wolniewicz FoMOs/ENG .87 Smartphone Usage .71 SAS-SV .88
(2018)
Yin (2015) FoMOs/Chinese .84 Items of Continuance Intention .74
translation

Note: BSMAS= Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale (Andreassen, Torsheim, Brunborg, & Pallesen, 2012); BSMAS Italian version (Monacis, de Palo, Griffiths, &
Sinatra, 2017); FoMOs/Chinese translation (Chai et al., 2019); FoMOs/ENG = Fear of Missing Out Scale/English version (Przybylski et al., 2013); FoMOs/ES = Fear of
Missing Out/Spanish version (Gil, Del Valle, Oberst, & Chamarro, 2015); FoMOs/ITA= Fear of Missing Out Scale/Italian version (Casale & Fioravanti, 2020);
FoMOs/TR = Fear of Missing Out Scale/Turkish version (Can & Satici, 2019); FIQ= Facebook Intrusion Questionnaire (Elphinston & Noller, 2011); Honest
Self-presentation Scale/Chinese version (Niu, Bao, Zhou, Kong, & Sun, 2015); Items on breadth of social media platform used (Franchina et al., 2018); Items of
Continuance Intention (Bhattacherjee, Perols, & Sanford, 2015); Nighttime social media use (Woods & Scott, 2016); PEOUSM= Perceived Ease Of Use of Social Media
(Curran & Lennon, 2011); PSMU= Problematic Social Media Use (Franchina et al., 2018); Perceived stress related to Facebook use (Beyens et al., 2016); RFDT =
Responses to Failures with Digital Technology (Hadlington & Scase, 2018); Qzone use intensity scale (Niu et al., 2018); SAS - Facebook Use Disorder = Smartphone
Addiction scale - Facebook use disorder (Kwon, Kim, Cho, & Yang, 2013); SAS-SV= Smartphone addiction scale-short version (Kwon et al., 2013); Self-Presentation
Tactics in Social Media (Huang, 2014); SMAS= Social Media Addiction Scale (van den Eijnden, Lemmens, & Valkenburg, 2016); Smartphone Usage (Elhai, Levine,
Dvorak, & Hall, 2016); SME= Social Media Engagement questionnaire (Przybylski et al., 2013); SMI= Social Media Intensity (Ellison, Steinfield, & Lampe, 2007);
Social Media Survey - adolescent version (Barry, Sidoti, Briggs, Reiter, & Lindsey, 2017); SNI= Social Network Intensity (Salehan and Negahban (2013); Spraggins’
modified questionnaire (Spraggins, 2009); The Chinese social media addiction scale (Liu & Ma, 2018b); SPIUT= The Short Problematic Internet Use Test (Siciliano
et al., 2015); The Facebook Intensity Scale (Ellison et al., 2007).
* = the authors reported: "items developed for this study directly reflected the constructs of interest” (p. 10; Barry et al., 2017).
** = the authors reported: "sufficient construct validity in the context of the measurement.

6
G. Fioravanti et al. Computers in Human Behavior 122 (2021) 106839

Fig. 2. Forest plot for correlation between FoMO and Social Network Sites Use (95% CI).

Fig. 3. Symmetric funnel plot for the correlation between FoMO and SNS use.

trigger for a compulsive use of social platforms, driven by the need to get experience more worry about being socially excluded, thereby causing
in touch with others, or as tool to develop social competence. them to experience high levels of FoMO (Alt & Boniel-Nissim, 2018a;
According to Przybylski et al. (2013) and subsequent research, FoMO Blackwell et al., 2017; Stead & Bibby, 2017). Individuals with high
is driven by individual differences (whether they be unmet needs, low levels of conscientiousness are goal-oriented and inclined toward plan­
life satisfaction, or personality traits). In the current study, we ning and seeking high achievement. Such individuals might experience
meta-analytically examined the relations between FoMO and various lower FoMO levels because they attach less importance to social events
psychological dimensions assessed by the included studies. These results or tend to avoid social platforms as they view SNSs as a potential
should be interpreted with caution and should not be generalized distraction from more important tasks (Butt & Philipps, 2008; Stead &
because very few studies focused on the correlations between FoMO and Bibby, 2017).
PSNSU also included individual difference (i.e., personality traits, psy­ The relationship between FoMO and depression shows a moderate
chopathology, and life satisfaction). That said, neuroticism and consci­ effect size. As previously reported (e.g., Dempsey, O’Brien, Tiamiyu, &
entiousness were positively correlated with FoMO. As personality traits Elhai, 2019; Dhir et al., 2018; Oberst et al., 2017; Elhai, 2020), negative
are by definition stable variables, it could be argued that these results mood/affectivity could lead to experiences of FoMO. On the other hand,
support Przybylski et al.’s (2013) speculations that FoMO needs to be the fear of being socially excluded and the non-fulfillment of the need to
understood in the context of predisposing factors. Neurotic individuals stay continuously connected to others could lead to negative emotions
have a generally anxious predisposition and tend to worry. Conse­ and depressive symptoms. Finally, both the fear of negative evaluation
quently, vulnerable, insecure and emotionally unstable individuals may and anxiety showed the strongest relations to FoMO, as previously

7
G. Fioravanti et al. Computers in Human Behavior 122 (2021) 106839

Fig. 4. Forest plot for correlation between FoMO and PSNSU (95% CI).

Fig. 5. Symmetric funnel plot for the correlation between FoMO and PSNSU.

reported (e.g., Blackwell et al., 2017; Dempsey et al., 2019; Dhir et al., exacerbate compulsive SNS checking behaviors and SNS addiction
2018; Oberst et al., 2017; Wegmann et al., 2017). From a theoretical symptoms. On the other hand, being frequently exposed to other peo­
point of view, both the fear of missing out and the fear of being nega­ ple’s lives through SNSs could increase the apprehension surrounding
tively judged are emotional dimensions related to interpersonal re­ being excluded from rewarding experiences that others might have and
lationships (Casale, Fioravanti, Flett, & Hewitt, 2014; Casale & therefore enhance the fear of missing out. Indeed, one of the few lon­
Fioravanti, 2020). It is possible to assume that the large effect size found gitudinal studies in this field (Buglass et al., 2017) has shown that in­
could be the result of a common construct associated with/underlining dividuals engaging in increased levels of SNS use exhibit raised levels of
both variables, that is, social anxiety. However, it is also possible that FoMO. It has also been hypothesized that individuals with higher levels
individuals might be more likely to suffer from anxiety when they feel of FoMO substitute real social contact with online social contact when
that they do not belong or believe that they are missing out on important their levels of expired loneliness increase and that this “substitution”
shared experiences. Indeed, due to the cross-sectional design of the contributes to increasing levels of expired loneliness (Dossey, 2014).
selected studies, we cannot assume with certainty the direction of the Longitudinal studies are needed to clarify whether FoMO is an ante­
relationships studied. Although FoMO has mainly been conceptualized cedent or consequence of PSNS, but also for clarifying its relationship
as a psychological driver of SNS use, the relationship between FoMO and with unmet needs or loneliness.
SNS use could be bidirectional. As suggested by Przybylski et al. (2013) Finally, the subgroup analyses and meta-regression using age, sex,
themselves, a vicious cycle between FoMO and engagement in social and the geographical area of samples as moderators show that these
media use could exist. On the one hand, the rewarding experience variables do not moderate the relations between FoMO and SNS use and
originating from the fulfilment of a need to “socially monitor” other between FoMO and problematic SNS use. Findings from previous studies
people’s lives to alleviate anxieties of being socially excluded might were quite consistent in showing that young people tend towards higher

8
G. Fioravanti et al. Computers in Human Behavior 122 (2021) 106839

Fig. 6. Forest plot for correlation between FoMO and secondary outcomes (95% CI).

levels of both FoMO and PSNSU (Casale & Fioravanti, 2020; Elhai et al., Similarly, the percentage of women included in the samples did not
2018; Przybylski et al., 2013; Stead & Bibby, 2017). The absence of a significantly explain the between-study variability of effect sizes. This
moderating role of age could be explained by the scarce heterogeneity of finding could be attributed to conflicting results on sex differences in
the ages of participants (i.e., low variance) of the studies included in the FoMO levels. As a matter of fact, some studies have found that young
meta-analysis. The mean age of the total sample was 22.70 (SD = 9.38), men have high levels of FoMO (Przybylski et al., 2013), whereas some
and most of the studies were therefore conducted on adolescents or others show that women report higher levels of FoMO than men (Casale,
young adults. Of the 33 studies included, only four recruited participants Fioravanti, & Rugai, 2018; Elhai et al., 2018; Stead & Bibby, 2017).
had a mean age higher than 30 years (and less than 50 years). For this Other findings indicate no sex differences (Casale & Fioravanti, 2020).
reason, future studies should examine the FoMO phenomenon and its Regarding the moderating role of the geographical area of samples, the
relationship to the use and abuse of SNSs among adult samples. meta-regression shows that only the association between FoMO and SNS

9
G. Fioravanti et al. Computers in Human Behavior 122 (2021) 106839

Table 3
Summary of the results of meta-regressions with covariates: geographical area, age and gender between FoMO and SNS use and FoMO and PSNS.
FoMO - SNS use

Covariate: Geographical Area Covariates: Age and Gender

Geographical area n of studies Point estimate df (Q) p value Test of the Model
Asia 4 0.231 Q(df) p value
Europe + UK 6 0.404 2.25(2) 0.324
Latina America 1 − 0.050 Goodness of fit
Middle East 1 0.829 Tau2 I2 Q(df) p value
USA 7 0.283 0.044 95.73% 210.94(9) 0.000
Total Between 4 (11.78) 0.019
FoMO - PSNS
Covariate: Geographical Area Covariates: Age and Gender
Geographical area n of studies Point estimate df (Q) p value Test of the Model
Asia 1 0.427 Q(df) p value
Europe + UK 9 0.467 0.34(2) 0.842
Latina America 1 0.499 Goodness of fit
Middle East 4 0.461 Tau2 I2 Q(df) p value
USA 3 0.467 0.017 92.91% 127.01(9) 0.000
Total Between 4 (0.15) 0.997

use is moderated by geographical area. However, we were not able to Despite these limitations, the current meta-analysis clarifies that a
determine how much the effect size changes by geographical area. No strong association between FoMO and PSNSU does exist, although it
changes in the magnitude of the relationship between FoMO and PSNS does not seem to depend on geographical location, age or sex. From a
were found across different geographic areas. It thus seems that FoMO is clinical perspective, the data suggest that FoMO should be used as a
trans-culturally related to problematic social networking site use. relevant dimension in the evaluation and treatment of problematic SNS
Although the effect sizes obtained from the present meta-analysis use. Moreover, results from the current meta-analysis might inform
indicate robust associations between FoMO and SNS use and between related areas of study which examine very similar questions but do not
FoMO and PSNSU, some limitations should be considered. As mentioned use the FOMO scale. For instance, some authors provided evidence of an
previously, as only cross-sectional studies were included, we cannot interactive effect of the fear of being negatively evaluated and low social
make inferences about the causality or direction of the relations iden­ support on PSNSU (Casale et al., 2014), and some others have shown
tified. Related to this point is the fact that the small number of studies that higher perceived isolation is associated with higher PSNS among
focused on the individual characteristics associated with FoMO limits both young (Bonsaksen et al., 2021) and older adults (Meshi, Cotten, &
the statistical power of the analyses performed on secondary outcomes. Bender, 2020).
The generalizability of the current findings could also be compromised
by the convenience sampling methods used across studies. In this Funding
respect, future studies might want to use the machine learning (ML)
approach in this field. Such an approach has received a growing atten­ This research did not receive any specific grant from funding
tion in clinical psychology and psychiatry in recent years (see, for agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
example, Coutanche & Hallion, 2019; Dwyer, Falkai, & Koutsouleris,
2018), especially because it has the ability to quantify the generaliz­
Declaration of competing interest
ability of a “learned” pattern or algorithm to new, non-collected data.
This might be particularly advantageous in the present field, as the vast
The authors declare that they have no known competing financial
majority of the studies are generally conducted with non-clinical sam­
interests or personal relationships that could appear to influence the
ples. In fact, the use of ML in this field might provide accurate pre­
work reported in this paper.
dictions concerning FoMO and problematic social media in new data set
(e.g., treatment-seeking adults) beyond the dataset in which they were
Appendix A. Supplementary data
originally tested (e.g., undergraduates).
In terms of the stability of our findings, we acknowledge that FoMO
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.
is a psychological construct, i.e., an effort to represent reality rather than
org/10.1016/j.chb.2021.106839.
an objectively existing constituent of it. This implies that its ability to
describe and explain the human experience might be context- or
situation-dependent, and some authors (e.g., Casale & Flett, 2020) have References1
recently argued that FoMO levels might decrease if social experiences
* Alt, D. (2015). College students’ academic motivation, media engagement and fear of
are reduced or forbidden (e.g., during the COVID-19 pandemic) as FoMO missing out. Computers in Human Behavior, 49, 111–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
includes, by definition, the possibility for significant others to have chb.2015.02.057.
* Alt, D., & Boniel-Nissim, M. (2018a). Links between adolescents’ deep and surface
enjoyable or rewarding experiences. Although this seems psycholog­
learning approaches, problematic internet use, and fear of missing out (FoMO).
ically plausible, some authors (Boursier, Gioia, Musetti, & Schimmenti, Internet Interventions, 13, 30–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.invent.2018.05.002.
2020) have recently shown that people have reported that they spend * Alt, D., & Boniel-Nissim, M. (2018b). Using multidimensional scaling and PLS-SEM to
more hours/day on social media during the pandemic than before the assess the relationships between personality traits, problematic internet use, and fear
of missing out. Behaviour & Information Technology, 37(12), 1264–1276. https://doi.
pandemic, and the association with FoMO levels was similar in strength org/10.1080/0144929X.2018.1502353.
to that observed before the pandemic (Gioia, Fioravanti, Casale, & * Alt, D., & Boniel-Nissim, M. (2018c). Parent–adolescent communication and
Boursier, 2021). These conflicting positions might encourage studies problematic internet use: The mediating role of fear of missing out (FoMO). Journal
of Family Issues, 39(13), 3391–3409. https://doi.org/10.1177/0192513X18783493.
that clarify whether the strength of the association between FoMO and
PSNS is situation-dependent. In other words, it might be the case that
other factors (e.g., isolation) interact with FoMO levels to predict social 1
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies included in the meta-
media use and abuse depending on the circumstances.
analysis.

10
G. Fioravanti et al. Computers in Human Behavior 122 (2021) 106839

Andreassen, C. S., & Pallesen, S. (2014). Social network site addiction – an overview. Downes, M. J., Brennan, M. L., Williams, H. C., & Dean, R. S. (2016). Development of a
Current Pharmaceutical Design, 20, 4053–4061. https://doi.org/10.2174/ critical appraisal tool to assess the quality of cross-sectional studies (AXIS). British
13816128113199990616 Medical Journal, 6(12). https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2016-011458
Andreassen, C. S., Torsheim, T., Brunborg, G. S., & Pallesen, S. (2012). Development of a Dwyer, D. B., Falkai, P., & Koutsouleris, N. (2018). Machine learning for clinical psychology
Facebook addiction scale. Psychological Reports, 110(2), 501–517. https://doi.org/ and clinical neuroscience. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
10.2466/02.09.18.PR0.110.2.501-517 Egger, M., Davey Smith, G., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. (1997). Bias in meta-analysis
* Bailey, A. A., Bonifield, C. M., & Arias, A. (2018). Social media use by young Latin detected by a simple, graphical test. British Medical Journal, 315, 629–634. https://
American consumers: An exploration. Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services, 43, doi.org/10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629
10–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jretconser.2018.02.003. van den Eijnden, R. J. J. M., Lemmens, J. S., & Valkenburg, P. M. (2016). The social
* Barry, C. T., Sidoti, C. L., Briggs, S. M., Reiter, S. R., & Lindsey, R. A. (2017). Adolescent media disorder scale. Computers in Human Behavior, 61, 478–487. https://doi.org/
social media use and mental health from adolescent and parent perspectives. Journal 10.1016/j.chb.2016.03.038
of Adolescence, 61, 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2017.08.005. Elhai, J. D., Levine, J. C., Alghraibeh, A. M., Alafnan, A. A., Aldraiweesh, A. A., &
* Beyens, I., Frison, E., & Eggermont, S. (2016). “I don’t want to miss a thing”: Hall, B. J. (2018). Fear of missing out: Testing relationships with negative affectivity,
Adolescents’ fear of missing out and its relationship to adolescents’ social needs, online social engagement, and problematic smartphone use. Computers in Human
Facebook use, and Facebook related stress. Computers in Human Behavior, 64, 1–8. Behavior, 89, 289–298. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.08.020
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.083. Elhai, J. D., Levine, J. C., Dvorak, R. D., & Hall, B. J. (2016). Fear of missing out, need for
Bhattacherjee, A., Perols, J., & Sanford, C. (2015). Information technology continuance: touch, anxiety and depression are related to problematic smartphone use. Computers
A theoretic extension and empirical test. Journal of Computer Information Systems, 49 in Human Behavior, 63, 509–516. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.079
(1), 17–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/08874417.2008.11645302. Elhai, J. D., Rozgonjuk, D., Liua, T., & Yanga, H. (2020). Fear of missing out predicts
Billieux, J., Maurage, P., Lopez-Fernandez, O., Kuss, D., & Griffiths, M. (2015). Can repeated measurements of greater negative affect using experience sampling
disordered mobile phone use Be considered a behavioral addiction? An update on methodology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 262, 298–303. https://doi.org/10.1016/
current evidence and a comprehensive model for future research. Current Addiction j.jad.2019.11.026
Reports, 2, 156–162. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40429-015-0054-y Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of Facebook ‘‘friends:’’
* Blackwell, D., Leaman, C., Tramposch, R., Osborne, C., & Liss, M. (2017). Extraversion, Social capital and college students use of online social network sites. Journal of
neuroticism, attachment style and fear of missing out as predictors of social media Computer-Mediated Communication, 12, 1143–1168. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-
use and addiction. Personality and Individual Differences, 116, 69–72. https://doi.org/ 6101.2007.00367.x
10.1016/j.paid.2017.04.039. Elphinston, R. A., & Noller, P. (2011). Time to face it! Facebook intrusion and the
Bonsaksen, T., Schoultz, M., Thygesen, H., Ruffolo, M., Price, D., Leung, J., et al. (2021). implications for romantic jealousy and relationship satisfaction. Cyberpsychology,
Loneliness and its associated factors nine months after the COVID-19 outbreak: A Behavior, and Social Networking, 14, 631–635. https://doi.org/10.1089/
cross-national study. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public cyber.2010.0318
Health, 18(6), 2841. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph18062841 * Franchina, V., Vanden Abeele, M., Van Rooij, A. J., Lo Coco, G., & De Marez, L. (2018).
Boursier, V., Gioia, F., Musetti, A., & Schimmenti, A. (2020). Facing loneliness and Fear of missing out as a predictor of problematic social media use and phubbing
anxiety during the covid-19 isolation: The role of excessive social media use in a behavior among flemish adolescents. International Journal of Environmental Research
sample of Italian adults. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11, 586222. https://10.3389/ and Public Health, 15(10), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15102319.
fpsyt.2020.586222. Gil, F., Del Valle, G., Oberst, U., & Chamarro, A. (2015). Nuevas tecnologías - ¿ nuevas
* Buglass, S. L., Binder, J. F., Betts, L. R., & Underwood, J. D. M. (2017). Motivators of patologías ? El smartphone y el fear of missing out. Aloma, 33, 77–83.
online vulnerability: The impact of social network site use and FOMO. Computers in Gioia, F., Fioravanti, G., Casale, S., & Boursier, V. (2021). The effects of the fear of
Human Behavior, 66, 248–255. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.09.055. missing out on people’s social networking sites use during the COVID-19 pandemic:
Butt, S., & Philipps, J. G. (2008). Personality and self-reported mobile phone use. The mediating role of online relational closeness and individuals’ online
Computers in Human Behavior, 24, 346–360. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. communication attitude. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 620442. https://doi.org/
chb.2007.01.019. 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.620442
* Błachnio, A., & Przepiórka, A. (2018). Facebook intrusion, fear of missing out, * Hadlington, L., & Scase, M. O. (2018). End-user frustrations and failures in digital
narcissism, and life satisfaction: A cross-sectional study. Psychiatry Research, 259, technology: Exploring the role of fear of missing out, internet addiction and
514–519. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.11.012. personality. Helyon, 4, 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2018.e00872.
Can, G., & Satici, S. A. (2019). Adaptation of fear of missing out scale (FoMOs): Turkish Hemphill, J. F. (2003). Interpreting the magnitudes of correlation coefficients. American
version validity and reliability study. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 32(3), 1–7. Psychologist, 58(1), 78–79. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.58.1.78
https://doi.org/10.1186/s41155-019-0117-4. Huang, H. (2014). Self-presentation Tactics in social media. International conference on
* Casale, S., & Fioravanti, G. (2020). Factor structure and psychometric properties of the social science (ICSS 2014). Chengdu, Sichuan: Atlantis Press.
Italian version of the fear of missing out scale in emerging adults and adolescents. Kuss, D. J., & Griffiths, M. D. (2011). Online social networking and addiction – a review
Addictive Behaviors, 102, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.106179. of the psychological literature. International Journal of Environmental Research and
Casale, S., Fioravanti, G., Flett, G. L., & Hewitt, P. L. (2014). From socially prescribed Public Health, 8(9), 3528–3552. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph8093528
perfectionism to problematic use of internet communicative services: The mediating Kuss, D., Kristensen, A. M., & Lopez-Fernandez, O. (2021). Internet addictions outside of
roles of perceived social support and the fear of negative evaluation. Addictive Europe: A systematic literature review. Computers in Human Behavior, 115. https://
Behaviors, 39(12), 1816–1822. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.06.006 doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106621
Casale, S., & Flett, G. (2020). Interpersonally-based fears during the COVID-19 pandemic: Kwon, M., Kim, D. J., Cho, H., & Yang, S. (2013). The smartphone addiction scale:
Reflections on the fear of missing out and the fear of not mattering constructs. Development and validation of a short version for adolescents. PloS One, 8(12), 1–7.
Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 17, 88–93. https://doi.org/10.36131/CN20200211. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0083558.
* Casale, S., Rugai, L., & Fioravanti, G. (2018). Exploring the role of positive Liberati, A., Altman, D. G., Tetzlaff, J., Mulrow, C., Gotzsche, P. C., Ioannidis, J. P. A.,
metacognitions in explaining the association between the fear of missing out and et al. (2015). PRISMA statement per il reporting di revisioni sistematiche e meta-
social media addiction. Addictive Behaviors, 85, 83–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. analisi degli studi che valutano gli interventi sanitari: Spiegazione ed elaborazione.
addbeh.2018.05.020. Evidence, 7(6), 1–36. https://doi.org/10.4470/E1000115
* Chai, H.-Y., Niu, G.-F., Lian, S.-L., Chu, X.-W., Liu, S., & Sun, X.-J. (2019). Why social Liu, C., & Ma, J. (2018a). Social support through online social networking sites and
network site use fails to promote well-being? The roles of social overload and fear of addiction among college students: The mediating roles of fear of missing out and
missing out. Computers in Human Behavior, 100, 85–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. problematic smartphone use. Current Psychology, 1–8. https://doi.org/10.1007/
chb.2019.05.005. s12144-018-0075-5
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112(1), 155–159. https://doi. Liu, C., & Ma, J. (2018b). Development and validation of the Chinese social media
org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155. addiction scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 134(11), 55–59. https://doi.
Coutanche, M. N., & Hallion, L. S. (2019). Machine learning for clinical psychology and org/10.1016/j.paid.2018.05.046.
clinical neuroscience. In A. G. C. Wright, & M. N. Hallquist (Eds.), The cambridge Meshi, D., Cotton, S., & Bender, A. R. (2020). Problematic social media use and perceived
handbook of research methods in clinical psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge social isolation in older adults: A cross-sectional study. Gerontology, 66, 160–168.
University Press. https://doi: 10.1159/000502577.
Curran, J. M., & Lennon, R. (2011). Participating in the conversation: Exploring usage of Monacis, L., de Palo, V., Griffiths, M. D., & Sinatra, M. (2017). Social networking
social media networking sites. Academy of Marketing Studies Journal, 15(1), 21–38. addiction, attachment style, and validation of the Italian version of the Bergen social
Deci, E., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human media addiction scale. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 6, 178–186. https://doi.org/
behavior. New York: Plenum Press. 10.1556/2006.6.2017.023
* Dempsey, A. E., O’Brien, K. D., Tiamiyu, M. F., & Elhai, J. D. (2019). Fear of missing out Müller, K. W., Dreier, M., Beutel, M. E., Duven, E., Giralt, S., & Wolfling, K. (2016).
(FoMO) and rumination mediate relations between social anxiety and problematic A hidden type of internet addiction? Intense and addictive use of social networking
Facebook use. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 9, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. sites in adolescents. Computers in Human Behavior, 55, 172–177. https://doi.org/
abrep.2018.100150. 10.1016/j.chb.2015.09.0070747-5632
* Dhir, A., Yossatorn, Y., Kaur, P., & Chen, S. (2018). Online social media fatigue and Niu, G., Bao, N., Zhou, Z., Kong, F. C., & Sun, X. (2015). The effect of self-presentation in
psychological wellbeing—a study of compulsive use, fear of missing out, fatigue, online social network sites on self-esteem: The mediating role of social support.
anxiety and depression. International Journal of Information Management, 40, Journal of Psychological Science, 38(4), 939–945.
141–152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijinfomgt.2018.01.012. Niu, G. F., Luo, Y. J., Sun, X. J., Zhou, Z. K., Yu, F., & Yang, S. L. (2018). Qzone use and
Dossey, L. (2014). FoMO, digital dementia, and our dangerous experiment. Explore, 10 depression among Chinese adolescents: A moderated mediation model. Journal of
(2), 69–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.explore.2013.12.008. Affective Disorders, 231, 58–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2018.01.013

11
G. Fioravanti et al. Computers in Human Behavior 122 (2021) 106839

* Oberst, U., Wegmann, E., Stodt, B., Brand, M., & Chamarro, A. (2017). Negative Spraggins, A. (2009). Problematic use of online social networking sites for college students
consequences from heavy social networking in adolescents: The mediating role of prevalence, predictors, and association with well-being. Retrieved from https://ufdc.ufl.
fear of missing out. Journal of Adolescence, 55, 51–60 https://10.1016/j.adol edu/UFE0024085/00001.
escence.2016.12.008. * Stead, H., & Bibby, P. A. (2017). Personality, fear of missing out and problematic
* Pontes, H. M., Taylor, M., & Stavropoulos, V. (2018). Beyond “Facebook addiction”: internet use and their relationship to subjective well-being. Computers in Human
The role of cognitive-related factors and psychiatric distress in social networking site Behavior, 76, 534–540. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.08.016.
addiction. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 21(4), 1–16. https://doi. Stroup, D. F., Berlin, J. A., Morton, S. C., Olkin, I., Williamson, G. D., Rennie, D., et al.
org/10.1089/cyber.2017.0609. (2000). Meta-analysis of observational studies in epidemiology: A proposal for
* Przybylski, A. K., Murayama, K., DeHaan, C. R., & Gladwell, V. (2013). Motivational, reporting. The Journal of the American Medical Association, 283(15), 2008–2012.
emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing out. Computers in Human https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.283.15.2008
Behavior, 29, 1841–1848 https://10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.014. Su, W., Han, X., Yu, H., Wu, Y., & Potenza, M. N. (2020). Do men become addicted to
* Roberts, J. A., & David, M. E. (2019). The social media party: Fear of missing out internet gaming and women to social media? A meta-analysis examining gender-
(FoMO), social media intensity, connection, and well-being. International Journal of related differences in specific internet addiction. Computers in Human Behavior, 113,
Human-Computer Interaction, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 106480. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2020.106480
10447318.2019.1646517. * Tomczyk, L., & Selmanagic-Lizde, E. (2018). Fear of Missing Out (FOMO) among youth
* Rogers, A. P., & Barber, L. K. (2019). Addressing FoMO and tele-pressure among in Bosnia and Herzegovina — scale and selected mechanisms. Children and Youth
university students: Could a technology intervention help with social media use and Services Review, 88, 541–549. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2018.03.048.
sleep disruption?. Computers in Human Behavior, 93, 192–199. https://doi.org/ * Tunc-Aksan, A., & Akbay, S. E. (2019). Smartphone addiction, fear of missing out, and
10.1016/j.chb.2018.12.016. perceived competence as predictors of social media addiction of adolescents.
Ryan, R., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic European Journal of Educational Research, 8(2), 559–566. https://doi.org/10.12973/
motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55(1), 68–78. eu-jer.8.2.559.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68 * Wang, P., Wang, X., Nie, J., Zeng, P., Liu, K., Wang, J., et al. (2018). Envy and
Salehan, M., & Negahban, A. (2013). Social networking on smartphones: When mobile problematic smartphone use: The mediating role of FOMO and the moderating role
phones become addictive. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(6), 2632–2639. https:// of student-student relationship. Personality and Individual Differences, 146, 136–142.
doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.07.003 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.04.013.
* Salim, F., Rahardjo, W., Tanaya, T., & Qurani, R. (2017). Are self-presentation of Wegmann, E., Oberst, U., Stodt, B., & Brand, M. (2017). Online-specific fear of missing
Instagram users influenced by friendship-contingent self-esteem and fear of missing out and Internet-use expectancies contribute to symptoms of Internet-
out?. Makara Hubs-Asia, 21(2), 70–82. https://doi.org/10.7454/mssh.v21i2.3502. communication disorder. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 5, 33–42. https://doi.org/
* Scott, H., & Woods, H. C. (2018). Fear of missing out and sleep: Cognitive behavioural 10.1016/j.abrep. 2017.04.001
factors in adolescents’ nighttime social media use. Journal of Adolescence, 68, 61–65. * Wolniewicz, C. A., Tiamiyu, M. F., Weeks, J. W., & Elhai, J. D. (2018). Problematic
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2018.07.009. smartphone use and relations with negative affect, fear of missing out, and fear of
* Sha, P., Sariyska, R., Riedl, R., Lachmann, B., & Montag, C. (2019). Linking internet negative and positive evaluation. Psychiatry Research, 262, 618–623. https://doi.
communication and smartphone use disorder by taking a closer look at the Facebook org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.09.058.
and WhatsApp applications. Addictive Behaviors Reports, 9, 1–9. https://doi.org/ Woods, H. C., & Scott, H. (2016). #Sleepyteens: Social media use in adolescence is
10.1016/j.abrep.2018.100148. associated with poor sleep quality, anxiety, depression and low self-esteem. Journal
Siciliano, V., Bastiani, L., Mezzasalma, L., Thanki, D., Curzio, O., & Molinaro, S. (2015). of Adolescence, 51, 41–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.05.008
Validation of a new short problematic internet use test in nationally representative * Yin, F. S., Liu, M. L., & Lin, C. P. (2015). Forecasting the continuance intention of social
sample of adolescents. Computers in Human Behavior, 45, 177–184. https://doi.org/ networking sites: Assessing privacy risk and usefulness of technology. Technological
10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.097. Forecasting and Social Change, 99, 267–272. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
techfore.2015.07.019.

12

You might also like