You are on page 1of 8

788379

article-commentary2018
QHRXXX10.1177/1049732318788379Qualitative Health ResearchCarminati

Commentary
Qualitative Health Research

Generalizability in Qualitative
1­–8
© The Author(s) 2018
Article reuse guidelines:
Research: A Tale of Two Traditions sagepub.com/journals-permissions
DOI: 10.1177/1049732318788379
https://doi.org/10.1177/1049732318788379
journals.sagepub.com/home/qhr

Lara Carminati1

Abstract
Generalizability in qualitative research has been a controversial topic given that interpretivist scholars have resisted the
dominant role and mandate of the positivist tradition within social sciences. Aiming to find universal laws, the positivist
paradigm has made generalizability a crucial criterion for evaluating the rigor of quantitative research. This positivist
echo has led generalizability to acquire a quantitative meaning, inappropriate for describing qualitative studies. The
purpose of qualitative research has, thus, been directed toward providing in-depth explanations and meanings rather
than generalizing findings. Through a critical review of empirical and theoretical studies, this commentary seeks to
show that in qualitative domains, generalizability is possible provided that, first, generalizability is the main objective
of the study; second, due precautions concerning the philosophy and terminology selected are taken. Hence, this
commentary contributes to the literature on qualitative research by making suggestions for more consistent and
unanimous procedures to adopt in qualitative inquiries.

Keywords
qualitative and quantitative research; positivist and interpretivist tradition; probabilistic generalizability; theoretical
generalizability; transferability; critical review; the UK

In one word, to draw the rule from experience, one must Trying to define the thorny and illusive concept of gen-
generalize: This is a necessity that imposes itself on the most eralizability may represent a challenge to researchers
circumspect observer. because of the ambiguity and polyhedral meanings associ-
ated with it (Polit & Beck, 2010). Simply put, the act of
―Henri Poincaré generalizing involves forming general and broad statements
from specific cases (Schwandt, 2001), which also means
Between the 1970s and the early 1990s, advancements in that inferences about what cannot be observed are made on
computer technology facilitated the processing of large the basis of what can be observed (Polit & Beck, 2010).
data sets in quantitative studies (Morse, 2008; Ormston, Defined in this way, the notion of generalizability has
Spencer, Barnard, & Snape, 2014). This fact, together neither a quantitative nor a qualitative dimension per se, but
with the increased interest of evidence-based decision it has, instead, a neutral and impartial connotation.
making in medical and health fields in the 1980s, and a Nevertheless, the dominant role of the positivist tradi-
widespread diffusion of the systematic review methodol- tion in social sciences has led generalizability to acquire
ogy in scientific research (Bradt, 2009), has made gener- a more quantitative nature (Kerlinger & Lee, 2000). Thus,
alizability of findings, already a valued standard for generalizability has become almost inherently associated
decades, a pivotal criterion for evaluating the excellence with quantitative research and its focus on finding univer-
of quantitative research (Polit & Beck, 2010). sal laws and statistical generalizations (Delmar, 2010).
Since then, researchers in the qualitative tradition have Consequently, due to this positivist echo, the application
begun to pay greater attention to discussions on the feasibil- of the same concept of generalizability to qualitative per-
ity of generalization in qualitative studies (Flyvbjerg, 2001). spectives, grounded in an interpretivist paradigm, has
However, this feasibility of generalizing findings could be
seen as a “phantom,” as an “illusion,” for qualitative schol-
ars (Groleau, Zelkowitz, & Cabral, 2009, p. 417). Indeed, 1
University of Surrey, Guildford, United Kingdom
without due precautions and clarifications, the nature of
Corresponding Author:
interpretivist paradigm, relying on a different logic to the Lara Carminati, Surrey Business School, University of Surrey,
positivist paradigm, could make generalizability almost an 388 Stag Hill, Guildford GU2 7XH, UK.
unachievable aim in qualitative research (Morse, 2008). Email: l.carminati@surrey.ac.uk
2 Qualitative Health Research 00(0)

become rather controversial (Polit & Beck, 2010). As a Generalizability in the Positivist,
result, generalizability in qualitative studies has often Quantitative Tradition
been misunderstood and disregarded in favor of the more
common purpose of providing in-depth understanding of The positivist tradition in social sciences has strongly
the specific topics under research (Ayres, Kavanagh, & polarized the notion of generalizability and influenced
Knafl, 2003). the terminology in scientific research (Flyvbjerg, 2001;
However, dismissing a priori the ability to generalize Ormston et al., 2014). Since the Enlightenment, natural
from qualitative studies on the bases of biased premises sciences, which are nomothetic by essence (Thomae,
due to “quantitative contamination” can significantly 1999), have indisputably dominated Western culture,
limit the strength and beauty of qualitative research, espe- drawing attention to instrumental rationality and the
cially in the eyes of early-career researchers. A clear con- focus on generalizable and context-independent theories
ceptualization of the meanings associated with the (Falk, Rocha, & Warnick, 2009). In an effort to emulate
concept of generalizability in qualitative research is natural science (Flyvbjerg, 2001), the positivist tradition
needed, taking into account both methodological and in social sciences, which traces back to philosophers such
philosophical boundaries. as Comte, Hume, and Bacon, has argued and acknowl-
Therefore, this commentary aims to show that even in edged the possibility of reaching one, monolithic truth
qualitative domains, generalizability is possible provided (Lee & Baskerville, 2003). Positivism assumes that “sci-
two paramount conditions exist. First, generalizability is the entific knowledge is the paradigm of valid knowledge”
main objective of the study. Second, due precautions related (Larrain, 1979, p. 197), and, as such, authentic knowl-
to the terminology selected are taken into consideration. edge can be verified through empirical evidence, which is
Concerning these due precautions, on one hand, if research- in turn interpreted through reason and logic (Macionis &
ers want to preserve the term generalizability in qualitative Gerber, 2010).
enquiries, the philosophical traditions underpinning the Consequently, the ultimate aim of quantitative research
research have to be clearly specified. In so doing, the kind defined by the positivist tradition is to produce laws able
of generalizability that can be attained, namely, analytical/ to explain and govern every observed phenomenon and to
theoretical, and the type of knowledge that can be pursued determine a universal knowledge (Delmar, 2010) that
in qualitative research would be straightforward and holds true, and is invariable in, all places and at all times
unequivocal. On the other hand, if new terms were to be (Falk et al., 2009). In this sense, as Morse (2008) has
introduced, a suitable vocabulary should be unanimously underlined, generalizability is at the heart of usefulness.
shared by all qualitative scholars to unmistakably describe Hence, by assuming an impartial and objective research-
quality in qualitative enquiries. Hence, this commentary er’s involvement (Winter, 2000) to limit any possible
contributes to the literature on qualitative research forms of influence in the research process (Davies &
approaches by making suggestions on those paramount Dodd, 2002), quantitative experimental methods have
conditions that may lead to more consistent and unanimous been employed to test hypotheses and infer generalizable
procedures to describe and elaborate qualitative research. conclusions (Dingwall, Murphy, Watson, Greatbatch, &
The commentary unfolds in four main parts. In the first Parker, 1998; Hoepfl, 1997).
section, “Generalizability in the Positivist, Quantitative These generalizable conclusions stem from a research
Tradition,” after providing a brief definition of the term, process based on methodological rigor. Rigor in the posi-
the value of generalizability within the positivist tradition tivist tradition refers to the soundness, exactitude, and
is analyzed. The second section “Generalizability in the accuracy of a study with regard to its planning, data col-
Interpretivist, Qualitative Tradition” investigates the nature lection, analysis, and reporting (Marquart, 2017). The
of generalizability in qualitative approaches in light of assessment of objective scores, such as reliable and valid
interpretivist perspectives. In the third section, “Grounded measurements, are indicators of such exactitude, so that
Theory, a Theory-Generating Research Method,” an exam- results from one study can be replicated in other studies
ple of a theory-generating research method, that is, and then generalized (Claydon, 2015).
grounded theory, within the interpretivist tradition is out- Results in quantitative research can be generalized
lined. The fourth section “Due Precautions in Qualitative through probabilistic generalization (Polit & Beck, 2010),
Investigations” tackles two main precautions researchers which is based on randomly selected samples representa-
should take when conducting qualitative investigations. tive of the population (Lee & Baskerville, 2003). Although
This part specifically proposes the adoption of the notion some authors have claimed that convenience sampling bias
of contextual knowledge as phronesis and the diffusion of could hamper the validity of findings in many studies
a terminology for qualitative research universally approved (Gheondea-Eladi, 2014), statistical generalizability remains
by the scientific community. The section “Implications and a crucial tenet and a prerogative in quantitative approaches
Conclusion” closes the commentary. (Winter, 2000). This probabilistic generalization is also
Carminati 3

termed external validity (Gheondea-Eladi, 2014). External Consequently, the researcher is an agentic instrument
validity, together with internal validity, namely, the degree for collecting and analyzing data and an essential tool for
to which it is possible to infer causality between two vari- actively constructing concepts (Hallberg, 2013). Thus, by
ables (Bryman, Becker, & Sempik, 2008; Joppe, 2000), and using either theoretical or nonprobabilistic samples
reliability, which describes the extent to which results are (Gheondea-Eladi, 2014), qualitative studies aim to
consistent over time and accurately represent the total pop- explore meanings and processes of people’s everyday
ulation (Bryman et al., 2008; Joppe, 2000), is a paramount lives to gain an in-depth understanding of situations and
criterion for evaluating quality in quantitative studies actions (Groleau et al., 2009; Hallberg, 2013). Because of
(Gheondea-Eladi, 2014; Polit & Beck, 2010). this stress on values and subjective meanings, qualitative
As a result, generalizability has become associated research has often been criticized as being relativistic,
with the notion of external validity, which represents only soft, or unscientific compared with quantitative enquiries
a specific aspect of the original term, namely, probabilistic (Ormston et al., 2014).
generalizability (Flyvbjerg, 2001; Lee & Baskerville, To avoid this subordinated position, the same stan-
2003). Hence, generalizability has been pulled toward the dards used to judge quality in quantitative research have
positivist approaches, and their quantitative influence has been transposed and employed to evaluate quality in
been instilled. It is no surprise that when the term general- qualitative studies (Delmar, 2010), so that generalizabil-
izability is mentioned, statistical generalization is the first ity has become a rather pivotal matter even in an area
notion to come into one’s mind, as if synonymous with it. where it is beset by indistinctness (Delmar, 2010;
Golafshani, 2003). In fact, this osmotic transfer has raised
numerous issues related to its fundamental inadequacy in
Generalizability in the Interpretivist, depicting the true nature of qualitative research processes
Qualitative Tradition and aims (Kitto, Chesters, & Grbich, 2008). Winter
The highlighted importance of the word generalizability (2000) has stated that “qualitative research sets itself up
in quantitative studies has led many qualitative scholars for failure when it attempts to follow established proce-
to question the possibility of generalizing results from dures of quantitative research” (pp. 11). Indeed, by doing
qualitative research (Davies & Dodd, 2002) and, conse- so, interpretivist social sciences have accepted terms that
quently, ponder on how to judge its quality (Hallberg, are by nature self-defeating (Flyvbjerg, 2001).
2013). As Ormston et al. (2014) have underlined, qualita- As a result, the importance of generalizability of find-
tive approaches in social sciences have started to emerge ings in the qualitative field has been frequently dismissed,
as recent phenomena alongside the widespread diffusion considered unattainable or irrelevant (Kitto et al., 2008)
of sophisticated statistical methods, which were framed and not the purpose of quality enquiries (Morse, 1999).
within positivist principles to imitate natural sciences Researchers have, thus, preferred to invert their focus to
methodology. the other fundamental aim of qualitative studies, which is
Nonetheless, the philosophical tradition behind qualita- to engage in research able to produce thorough descrip-
tive research, drawing from authors such as Weber, Kant, tions as well as a deep, rich, and contextualized under-
and Dilthey is the interpretivist paradigm (Bryman et al., standing of human experience (Polit & Beck, 2010).
2008; Morse, Barrett, Mayan, Olson, & Spiers, 2002) and, However, (a) if the purpose of a study and its research
in an antipodal way to the positivist perspective, is idio- questions aim to build a new theory to bridge a gap in the
graphic in nature (Thomae, 1999). Interpretivism priori- literature (Gheondea-Eladi, 2014) or (b) if due precau-
tizes the understanding of human behaviour over the tions of the conceptualization of generalizability are
prediction and generalization of causes and effects respected (Hallberg, 2013), then discussing generaliz-
(Macionis & Gerber, 2010). ability of findings even in qualitative research makes
As such, the strength of qualitative inquiries defined sense (Delmar, 2010; Krefting, 1991; Morse, 1999; Polit
by the interpretivist tradition is the understanding of how & Beck, 2010; Winter, 2000). The following two sections
individuals, through their narratives, perceive and experi- expand on these two paramount conditions to clarify gen-
ence their lives, constructing meanings within their social eralization in the interpretivist paradigm.
and cultural contexts (Groleau et al., 2009; Mishler,
2000). In this sense, interpretivist research emphasizes Grounded Theory, a Theory-
the hermeneutics and perception of the social world, and
Generating Research Method
the interactions between individuals and the surrounding
context (Ormston et al., 2014). It follows that findings are With regard to the first point of building a new theory,
intrinsically linked to the research context and that the within the many different qualitative methods (Krefting,
interfacial distance between the researcher and the 1991), theory-generating procedures seek to generalize
researched is minimized (Davies & Dodd, 2002). findings (Eisenhardt, 1989; Glaser & Strauss, 1967;
4 Qualitative Health Research 00(0)

Hallberg, 2013; Ormston et al., 2014). An example of It is simply a different kind of generalizability compared
these theory-generating procedures is grounded theory with that of quantitative research (Polit & Beck, 2010). It
(Corbin & Strauss, 2008; Eisenhardt, 1989; Glaser & is an analytic, or theoretical, form of generalization.
Strauss, 1967). Although the aim of this commentary is
far beyond providing an exhaustive description of Due Precautions in Qualitative
grounded theory, some key features of this theory-gener-
ating method are now discussed to provide evidence and Investigations
strength to the argument that generalizability is achiev- Given the fact that research undertaken in the interpretiv-
able also in qualitative research. This is feasible since the ist tradition can aim for theoretical generalizability, how
purpose of grounded theory is to inductively construct a are researchers able to avoid confusion when using such
general theory that is able to answer the research question an ambiguous and misused term? What can be done to
in the absence or incompleteness of alternative existing properly describe qualitative studies, releasing them from
frameworks (Gheondea-Eladi, 2014). the pervasive influence of positivist perspectives?
Starting from thick descriptions and then going
beyond through to high levels of abstraction (Goulding,
2002), grounded theory develops an iterative process
First Precaution
based on four main steps, namely, coding, conceptualiza- The first due precaution is related to the fact that if
tion, classification, and categorization, to build a sub- researchers want to preserve the term generalizability
stantiated theory (Gheondea-Eladi, 2014). After this first within the qualitative enquiries, then the interpretivist
theoretical result, the research is replicated in other set- paradigm should be clearly specified at the beginning of
tings so that several other substantiated theories can be a study (Hallberg, 2013). Stating the philosophical tradi-
yielded (Goulding, 2002). Indeed, as Firestone (1993) tion underpinning a research determines the kind of gen-
has noted, replication in different conditions reinforces eralizability, that is, analytical/theoretical, that can be
generalizability in qualitative investigations, since con- obtained and pursued and prevents misunderstandings
sistency of results when conditions vary indicates that (Hallberg, 2013). In this sense, by recognizing and
findings are robust. This also explains the reason why acknowledging the existence of this other specific form
extreme cases, those that due to their extreme character- of generalization, the quantitative meaning can be bal-
istics accentuate the dynamics being investigated facili- anced out and the originally neutral connotation of the
tating the emergence of the aspects of interest (Eisenhardt, broad term restored. Restoring the neutral connotation of
1989), are so critical for enhancing qualitative research the term generalizability emphasizes the view that gener-
(Polit &Beck, 2010). alizability in social sciences can never be certain (Polit
When every observation has been incorporated and no & Beck, 2010).
unexplained variance has been left out, then saturation is Because of the focus on and importance of the context,
reached (Dingwall et al., 1998). Saturation implies that interactions, and hermeneutics, the aim of the interpretiv-
sufficient and redundant information for all aspects of the ist tradition is to predict a theoretical understanding of the
phenomenon under research has been gathered (Morse topic under examination (Yin, 1994). As such, results are
et al., 2002), and thus, no further themes or concepts can not reached through statistical procedures or other means
emerge (Glaser & Strauss, 1967). At this point, a formal, of quantifications (Corbin & Strauss, 2008), and general-
final theory can be elaborated (Corbin & Strauss, 2008). ization is interpreted as generalization toward a theory
The formulation of a new theory is possible due to the rather than toward the population (Polit & Beck, 2010).
action of abstracting the “general” from the “particular” This is why the notion of sample size in relation to
in terms of similarities and differences in individuals and generalizability is rather controversial in qualitative
circumstances (Delmar, 2010). From this perspective, the investigations (Malterud, Siersma, & Guassora, 2016).
power of generalization of the final theory not only On one hand, sample sizes may be too large to permit a
depends on the researcher’s thorough immersion and deep and detailed analysis; on the other hand, they may
engagement with the data (Polit & Beck, 2010) but also be too small to support claims of having achieved either
on the intensity of within-case and across-case analyses theoretical saturation or information redundancy (Boddy,
(Ayres et al., 2003). As Morse (1999) has argued, “if 2016). In addition, data saturation and information redun-
qualitative research was considered not generalizable, dancy are elusive concepts, and no clear explanations of
then it would be of little use, insignificant and hardly how they should be understood or implemented to justify
worth doing” (p. 5). Hence, there would be a high risk of the number of participants exist (Morse, 1995). Although,
falling into an endemic relativism (Davies & Dodd, recently, some guidelines have been proposed to deter-
2002). Nevertheless, as grounded theory has shown, gen- mine “information power” (Malterud et al., 2016), defin-
eralizability of findings in qualitative studies is possible: ing the adequate sample size in qualitative research is
Carminati 5

ultimately a matter of the researcher’s experience and setting, a context-dependent knowledge would do justice
judgment of the quality of the data collected and the to the essence of qualitative enquiries and their conceptu-
research method employed (Boddy, 2016). alization of generalizability.
Thus, generalizability in qualitative studies is focused
on the researcher’s analysis and understanding of circum-
Second Precaution
stances rather than on the collection of representative data
(Delmar, 2010; Morse, 1999). In this sense, generalization The second precaution is related to the introduction of
entails inferring the potential extrapolations, or transfer- new terms. It refers to some qualitative scholars’ plea to
ability, of those results on the basis of both a theoretical select a novel and unanimously accepted vocabulary to
analysis of the aspects generating the outcomes and the better indicate the principles underpinning qualitative
effects of the context (Eisenhardt, 1989; Yin, 1994). approaches and evaluate their quality (Agar, 1986;
Such contextual effects are crucial in qualitative Golafshani, 2003; Seale, 1999). Quality is in fact a hall-
enquiries. Since they seek to understand phenomena mark of every piece of research (Hallberg, 2013), and
unfolding naturally in specific environments (Patton, “each paradigm should be judged by its own paradigm’s
2002), declaring the philosophical tradition underpin- terms” (Golafshani, 2003, p. 601). This calls into ques-
ning a study also has an impact on the type of knowledge tion the use of the term generalizability itself.
that can be acquired. Within the philosophical tradition Some researchers, echoing Shakespeare’s “What’s in a
of interpretivism, a theory that pertains only to the set- name?”1 (Romeo & Juliet, Act II, Scene II, v. 47), have
ting in which it was developed would not detract from its argued that the positivist terminology, such as the head-
scientific status nor would it prohibit researchers from ings reliability, validity, and rigor, could be still used in
extending that theory to additional settings (Lee & interpretivist qualitative research and just redefined in
Baskerville, 2003). On the contrary, what may work for terms of meaning (Guba & Lincoln, 1981; Morse et al.,
people in a certain circumstance is likely to work for 2002). For instance, while reliability could indicate the
other people placed in a similar situation (Hallberg, “negotiation of truths through a series of subjective
2013; Hyde, 2000). accounts,” validity could reside “with the representation
Therefore, since qualitative understanding relies on a of the actors, the purposes of the research and appropriate-
comprehensive knowledge of the case and its nuances in ness of the processes involved” (Winter, 2000, pp. 9–10).
each specific context (Hyde, 2000), talking about “situ- Nonetheless, some new labels have also been pro-
ated” knowledge rather than epistemological knowledge posed to cut ties with the positivist tradition and under-
would then be more appropriate (Davies & Dodd, 2002; line the independent identity of the interpretivist
Haraway, 1988). As such, as Flyvbjerg (2001) has sug- perspective (Agar, 1986). Indeed, qualitative research
gested, it would be interesting to introduce the Aristotelian often breaks with and challenges many of the tenets of
concept of phronesis, value-based, context-dependent so-called good quantitative research, as it is the case
knowledge. Words such as episteme (pure knowledge) with the notion of reliability and validity (Davies &
and techne (applied knowledge) have informed the cur- Dodd, 2002; Golafshani, 2003). Hence, a plethora of
rent language (i.e., epistemology, technic, technology). alternative concepts have arisen to mark the initially
While episteme indicates generalizable-across-settings moderate and then radical paradigm shift (Seale, 1999),
knowledge, usually called science, techne refers to practi- and an alternative terminology, able to express qualita-
cal, specific knowledge. However, occurrences of the tive connotations, has been supported. For example, the
word phronesis, which sits between episteme and techne, word trustworthiness could be considered as an alterna-
and encompasses both scientific and technical knowledge tive to the qualitative term for rigor (Davies & Dodd,
by adding values, experiences, context, and reflexivity, 2002; Morse et al., 2002). Trustworthiness has been pro-
do not exist in the present language (Flyvbjerg, 2001). posed and initially defined by Guba and Lincoln (1981)
It follows that the idea according to which true knowl- through four criteria: truth value (corresponding to
edge is only that which can be replicated in different quantitative internal validity), applicability (quantita-
places and times has to be rejected. Instead, context- tive external validity or generalizability), consistency
dependent knowledge, with its different modes of expres- (quantitative reliability), and neutrality (quantitative
sion, can offer a likewise true understanding, but in researcher’s objectivity and distance).
another form in terms of its replicability and applicability To relate this term even more closely to the interpretiv-
(Delmar, 2010). Thus, in the interpretivist tradition of ist tradition, the same authors have further modified the
social science, for which the constant interaction between four-point list of criteria (Lincoln & Guba, 1985). This
human agents and the surrounding context is essential, it modified and well-established model comprises concepts
would be more suitable to talk about phronetic knowl- such as credibility (prior truth value—quantitative internal
edge (Flyvbjerg, 2001). Because of the link with the validity), transferability (prior applicability—quantitative
6 Qualitative Health Research 00(0)

external validity or generalizability), dependability (prior Implications and Conclusion


consistency—quantitative reliability), and confirmability
(prior neutrality—quantitative researcher’s objectivity The aim of this commentary has been to show that qual-
and distance; Lincoln & Guba, 1985). In subsequent semi- itative studies, besides providing an in-depth under-
nal works, Guba and Lincoln (1989, 1994) also introduced standing of contextualized human experience, may also
the notion of authenticity, to represent a range of multiple pursue theoretical generalizability or, more pertinent
realities and truths in line with the interpretivist perspec- for qualitative approaches, transferability, as outlined
tive. In line with this alternative terminology, transferabil- by Lincoln and Guba (1985). This purpose can be
ity should be used and preferred in qualitative research achieved depending on two circumstances: First, gener-
instead of theoretical generalizability. alization of results has to be the aim of the research and
However, even if the original aim of the changes in the implemented method; second, terminological precau-
terminology was to bring clarity and a universally tions have to be taken.
acknowledged terminology in the field, these guidelines Regarding terminological precautions, on one hand,
still lack shared agreement between academics (Morse if researchers want to make use of the term generaliz-
et al., 2002). A bewildering proliferation of new terms has ability, a first precaution would suggest that the philo-
hindered the selection of a vocabulary unanimously sophical roots underpinning a study are briefly declared
accepted by scholars (Seale, 1999). The absence of a rec- at the beginning of that study (Hallberg, 2013). Given
ognized canon, through which to appropriately evaluate the variety of philosophical approaches, providing
the quality and trustworthiness of the study may be one of details on the author’s specific philosophical orientation
the reasons for the skepticism surrounding generalizabil- would help clarify the kind of generalizability, that is,
ity in qualitative research (Delmar, 2010). analytical/theoretical, that can be drawn from the find-
Nevertheless, while this burgeoning terminology ings, avoiding potential future misinterpretations. This
reflects the difficulties and contradictions that qualita- may also have an impact on the specific knowledge that
tive methodologists have met in developing an over- can be acquired in social sciences (Flyvbjerg, 2001).
arching system for specifying quality (Seale, 1999), it Because this major category of academic disciplines
also represents an essential first step toward defining the deals with the unpredictable and ever-changing human
autonomous status of qualitative research (Morse, subject, the importance of the context, especially in the
2008). These criteria are in fact fundamental to judge interpretivist tradition, is paramount to defying the
the rigor or, better, trustworthiness and attentiveness of dynamics surrounding the role of the researcher, the par-
qualitative inquiries because they are a watershed ticipants, the corpus of data, the analysis, and the gen-
between “good” and “bad” quality research (Davies & eral interpretations of their emerging interrelationships.
Dodd, 2002). Trustworthiness is built through the trans- As such, a contextual knowledge, which could be called
parency, reflexivity, and accuracy of the research prac- phronetic knowledge, is as fundamental as a more posi-
tice itself, and the credibility and genuineness of a tivist epistemological knowledge.
qualitative study depends mainly on the researcher’s On the other hand, the second precaution concerns the
ability and effort to unfold and explain interactions use of a new terminology, symbol of a paradigm shift,
(Golafshani, 2003; Thorne & Darbyshire, 2005). more suitable and consistent with the interpretivist philo-
Hence, a thorough and exhaustive explanation of these sophical tradition of qualitative enquiries. To be efficient
processes should be able to provide high-quality qualita- and effective, the community of scholars should unani-
tive works (Golafshani, 2003). In this sense, by reflecting mously agree on and adopt an adequate, recognized, and
valid and rich descriptions of sufficient depth, the research unambiguous vocabulary to accurately describe their
findings warrant a degree of generalizability (Thorne & findings, their own role in the study, and the research pro-
Darbyshire, 2005), or, following the well-established cri- cess as a whole. Although variety and creativity also play
teria (Golafshani, 2003; Shenton, 2004) proposed by an important role in qualitative research, a shared termi-
Lincoln and Guba (1985), transferability. Transferability nological canon may facilitate the evaluation of the excel-
becomes possible thanks to the agentic role of the reader. lence and trustworthiness of qualitative studies.
As final recipient and arbiter, and thanks to the thick In so doing, the commentary has contributed to the lit-
details of the phenomena under investigation provided by erature on qualitative research approaches by making
the author (Delmar, 2010), the reader can justify the suggestions for more consistent and common procedures
extrapolation and application of the findings to other set- in qualitative inquiries. Indeed, introducing precise
tings and situations, thus making transferability infer- guidelines may enhance the clarity of qualitative investi-
ences (Shenton, 2004). In so doing, a final and solid gations and give more justice to the importance and
knowledge is confirmed and approved by the research beauty of such research. This would also spur researchers
community (Delmar, 2010). who have just started their research journey traditions not
Carminati 7

to dismiss qualitative research as less scientific, but, Bryman, A., Becker, S., & Sempik, J. (2008). Quality cri-
instead, to better appreciate the inherent differences teria for quantitative, qualitative and mixed meth-
between two opposite philosophical approaches. ods research: A view from social policy. International
From a more practical point of view, appreciating such Journal of Social Research Methodology, 11, 261–276.
doi:10.1080/13645570701401644
differences would recognize the massive contribution of
Clark, W. G., & Aldis Wright, W. Eds. (1998). The complete
qualitative research to the understanding of real-world
works of William Shakespeare (Vol. 1). Nelson Doubleday.
problems and phenomena. This is particularly evident, Inc.: Garden City.
for instance, when considering decision-making issues in Claydon, L. S. (2015). Rigour in quantitative research. Nursing
the crucial field of medicine and health, as well as in busi- Standard, 29(47), 43–48. doi:10.7748/ns.29.47.43.e8820
ness, marketing, and ethics problems. Qualitative research Corbin, J., & Strauss, A. (2008). Basics of qualitative
explores individuals’ perceptions and feelings about research (3rd ed.): Procedures and techniques for devel-
those processes and dynamics underpinning decision oping grounded theory. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
making, accessing areas not amenable to quantitative doi:10.4135/9781452230153
research (Pope & Mays, 1995). In this sense, they are also Davies, D., & Dodd, J. (2002). Qualitative research and the
prerequisite and, thus, complementary to good quantita- question of rigor. Qualitative Health Research, 12, 279–
289. doi:10.1177/104973230201200211
tive research (Pope & Mays, 1995). As Albert Einstein
Delmar, C. (2010). “Generalizability” as recognition:
has famously stated, “Everybody is a genius. But if you
Reflections on a foundational problem in qualitative
judge a fish by its ability to climb a tree, it will live its research. Qualitative Studies, 1, 115–128.
whole life believing that it is stupid.” Dingwall, R., Murphy, E., Watson, P., Greatbatch, D., &
Parker, S. (1998). Catching goldfish: Quality in qualitative
Acknowledgment research. Journal of Health Services Research and Policy,
The author would like to thank Professor MariaLaura Di 3, 167–172. doi:10.1177/135581969800300308
Domenico for her constructive and helpful comments on earlier Eisenhardt, K. (1989). Building theories from case study
versions of this manuscript and Claire Booth for proofreading research. Academy of Management Review, 14, 532–550.
this manuscript. doi:10.5465/amr.1989.4308385
Falk, T. M., Rocha, S., & Warnick, B. R. (2009). Social science
Declaration of Conflicting Interests and its discontents: An essay review of Ben Flyvbjerg’s
Making social science matter. Education Review, 12, 1–16.
The author declared no potential conflicts of interest with Firestone, W. A. (1993). Alternative arguments for generalizing
respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this
from data as applied to qualitative research. Educational
article.
Researcher, 22, 16–23. doi:10.2307/1177100
Flyvbjerg, B. (2001). Making social science matter: Why
Funding social inquiry fails and how it can succeed again.
The author received no financial support for the research, Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/
authorship, and/or publication of this article. cbo9780511810503
Gheondea-Eladi, A. (2014). Is qualitative research generaliz-
Note able? Journal of Community Positive Practices, 14, 114–
124.
1. “What’s in a name? That which we call a rose by any other
Glaser, B., & Strauss, A. (1967). The discovery grounded the-
name would smell as sweet,” William Shakespeare—
ory: Strategies for qualitative inquiry. Chicago: Alden.
Romeo and Juliet, Act II, Scene II, v. 47 (Clark & Aldis
doi:10.1097/00006199-196807000-00014
Wright, 1998).
Golafshani, N. (2003). Understanding reliability and validity in
qualitative research. The Qualitative Report, 8, 597–606.
References Goulding, C. (2002). Grounded theory: A practical guide for
Agar, M. H. (1986). Speaking of ethnography (Vol. 2). SAGE. management, business and market researchers. SAGE.
doi:10.4135/9781412985895 doi: 10.4135/9781849209236
Ayres, L., Kavanagh, K., & Knafl, K. (2003). Within- Groleau, D., Zelkowitz, P., & Cabral, I. (2009). Enhancing
case and across-case approaches to qualitative data generalizability: Moving from an intimate to a politi-
analysis. Qualitative Health Research, 13, 871–883. cal voice. Qualitative Health Research, 19, 416–426.
doi:10.4135/9781412985895 doi:10.1177/1049732308329851
Boddy, C. R. (2016). Sample size for qualitative research. Guba, E. G., & Lincoln, Y. S. (1981). Effective evaluation:
Qualitative Market Research: An International Journal, Improving the usefulness of evaluation results through
19, 426–432. doi:10.1108/qmr-06-2016-0053 responsive and naturalistic approaches. San Francisco,
Bradt, D. A. (2009). Evidence-based decision-making (part 1): CA: Jossey-Bass.
Origins and evolution in the health sciences. Prehospital Guba, E. G., & Lincoln, Y. S. (1989). Fourth generation evalu-
and Disaster Medicine, 24, 298–305. ation. Newbury Park, CA: SAGE.
8 Qualitative Health Research 00(0)

Guba, E. G., & Lincoln, Y. S. (1994). Competing paradigms Morse, J. M. (2008). “It’s only a qualitative study!”
in qualitative research. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln Considering the qualitative foundations of social sciences.
(Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (pp. 105–117). Qualitative Health Research, 18, 147–148. doi:10.1177/
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 1049732307310262
Hallberg, L. (2013). Quality criteria and generalization of Morse, J. M., Barrett, M., Mayan, M., Olson, K., &
results from qualitative studies. International Journal of Spiers, J. (2002). Verification strategies for estab-
Qualitative Studies on Health and Well-Being, 8, Article 1. lishing reliability and validity in qualitative research.
doi:10.3402/qhw.v8i0.20647 International Journal of Qualitative Methods, 1, 13–22.
Haraway, D. (1988). Situated knowledge: The science ques- doi:10.1177/160940690200100202
tion in feminism and the privilege of partial perspective. Ormston, R., Spencer, L., Barnard, M., & Snape, D. (2014). The
Feminist Studies, 14, 575–599. doi:10.2307/3178066 foundations of qualitative research. In J. Ritchie, J. Lewis,
Hoepfl, M. C. (1997). Choosing qualitative research: A primer C. McNaughton Nicholls & R. Ormston (Eds.), Qualitative
for technology education researchers. Journal of Technology Research Practice: A Guide for Social Science Students
Innovations, 9, 47–63. doi:10.21061/jte.v9i1.a.4 and Researchers, 2 (pp. 1-23). SAGE.
Hyde, K. F. (2000). Recognising deductive processes in qualita- Patton, M. Q. (2002). Qualitative evaluation and research
tive research. Qualitative Market Research: An International methods (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Journal, 3, 82–90. doi:10.1108/13522750010322089 Polit, D. F., & Beck, C. T. (2010). Generalization in quanti-
Joppe, M. (2000). The research process. Retrieved from https:// tative and qualitative research: Myths and strategies.
www.uoguelph.ca/hftm/research-process International Journal of Nursing Studies, 47, 1451–1458.
Kerlinger, F. N., & Lee, H. B. (2000). Foundations of behav- doi:10.1016/j.ijnurstu.2010.06.004
ioral research (4th ed.). Fort Worth, TX: Harcourt College Pope, C., & Mays, N. (1995). Qualitative research: Reaching
Publishers. the parts other methods cannot reach: An introduction to
Kitto, S. C., Chesters, J., & Grbich, C. (2008). Quality in qualita- qualitative methods in health and health services research.
tive research. Medical Journal of Australia, 188, 243–246. British Medical Journal, 311(6996), 42–45. doi:10.1136/
Krefting, L. (1991). Rigor in qualitative research: The assess- bmj.311.6996.42
ment of trustworthiness. American Journal of Occupational Schwandt, T. A. (2001). Dictionary of qualitative inquiry (2nd
Therapy, 45(3), 214-222. doi:10.5014/ajot.45.3.214 ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
Larrain, J. (1979). The concept of ideology. London: Hutchinson. Seale, C. (1999). Quality in qualitative research. Qualitative
Lee, A. S., & Baskerville, R. L. (2003). Generalizing generaliz- Inquiry, 5, 465–478. doi:10.1177/107780049900500402
ability in information systems research. Information Systems Shenton, A. K. (2004). Strategies for ensuring trustworthiness
Research, 14, 221–243. doi:10.1287/isre.14.3.221.16560 in qualitative research projects. Education for Information,
Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Beverly 22, 63–75.
Hills, CA: SAGE. doi:10.1177/144078338702300329 Thomae, H. (1999). The nomothetic-idiographic issue: Some
Macionis, J., & Gerber, L. (2010). Sociology (7th ed.). Toronto: roots and recent trends. International Journal of Group
Pearson. Tensions, 28, 187–215. doi:10.1023/a:1021891506378
Malterud, K., Siersma, V. D., & Guassora, A. D. (2016). Sample Thorne, S., & Darbyshire, P. (2005). Land mines in the field:
size in qualitative interview studies. Qualitative Health A modest proposal for improving the craft of qualitative
Research, 26, 1753–1760. doi:10.1177/1049732315617444 health research. Qualitative Health Research, 15, 1105–
Marquart, F. (2017). Methodological rigor in quantitative research. 1113. doi:10.1177/1049732305278502
The International Encyclopedia of Communication Research
Winter, G. (2000). A comparative discussion of the notion of
Methods, 1-9. doi:10.1002/9781118901731.iecrm0221
“validity” in qualitative and quantitative research. The
Mishler, E. G. (2000). Validation in inquiry-guided research:
Qualitative Report, 4(3), 1–14.
The role of exemplars in narrative studies. In B. M. Brizuela,
Yin, R. K. (1994). Case study research: Designs and methods.
J. P. Stewart, R. G. Carrillo, & J. G. Berger (Eds.), Acts of
Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.
inquiry in qualitative research (pp. 119–146). Cambridge,
MA: Harvard Educational Review.
Morse, J. M. (1995). The significance of saturation. Qualitative Author Biography
Health Research, 5, 147–149. doi:10.1177/10497323950050 Lara Carminati is a PhD candidate in Management,
0201 Organisational Behaviour, at Surrey Business School. Her PhD
Morse, J. M. (1999). Qualitative generalizability. Qualitative project deals with healthcare professionals’ identity conflict in
Health Research, 9, 5–6. doi:10.1177/104973299129121622 decision making in End-of-Life situations.

You might also like