You are on page 1of 12

THE DEVELOPMENT OF EVALUATIVE

RESPONSES TO MUSIC:
Infants Prefer To Listen To Consonance
Over Dissonance

Laurel J. Trainor
Becky M. Heinmiller
McMaster University

In Experiment 1, &month-old infants looked longer in order to listen to a set of consonant intervals
than to a set of dissonant intervals. In Experiment 2, infants preferred to listen to the original version of
a Mozart minuet than to a version altered to contain many dissonant intervals. Thus, although infants
do not yet have the musical-system-specific knowledge of scale structure that is involved in adults’
emotional reactions to music, infants are similar to adults in their evaluative reactions to consonance
and dissonance.

infant auditory perception music preference consonance dissonance

1NTRODUCTlON Recent empirical research suggests that music


may play an important role in state regulation
Considerable controversy surrounds the ques- and emotional communication between care-
tion of the origin, biological significance, and givers and infants (e.g., Trehub & Trainor, in
function of music (e.g., Dissanayake, 1992; press; Trainor, 1996; Trainor, Clark, Huntley,
Kogan, 1994; Lomax, 1968; Trehub & & Adams, 1997). From the newborn period,
Trainor, in press; Winner, 1982). However, the caregivers around the world sing to their
universality of music points to an important infants (e.g., Trehub, Unyk, & Trainor, 1993a,
role in emotional expression and communica- b; Trehub & Trainor, in press), believing that
tion that may have adaptive significance. music has the power to convey affect and alter

l Laurel J. Trainor, Department of Psychology, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada L8S 4Kl; email,
Ijt@mcmaster.ca.

INFANT BEHAVIOR & DEVELOPMENT 21 (l), 1998, pp. 77-88 ISSN 0163-6383
78 INFANT BEHAVIOR & DEVELOPMENT Vol. 21, No. 1, 1998

state, such as when lullabies promote sleep in 1990; Fernald, 1993; Werker, & McLeod,
a restless infant. Caregivers are exquisitely 1989).
sensitive to the perceptual capabilities of their Infants also pay attention to infant-directed
infants, singing to them more slowly, at a singing. When given a choice of listening to a
higher pitch, with exaggerated rhythm, and in recording of infant-directed singing versus a
a more loving or emotionally engaging man- recording of the same mother singing the same
ner than when singing alone (Trainor, et al., song alone, they prefer to listen to the infant-
1997; Trehub & Trainor, in press). Caregivers directed version (Trainor, 1996). Emotional
also render their speech to infants in a sing expression appears to be involved in this pref-
song manner (e.g., Fernald, 1991; Papousek, erence as the degree of infant preference is
1992). If infants understand the emotional correlated with the proportion of adult raters
messages of the music and speech directed who find the infant-directed version to be ren-
toward them, the evolutionary value of such dered in a more loving tone of voice than the
activity is obvious: Emotional communica- non-infant-directed version. Infants also show
tion is crucial to survival. differential behaviors when listening to a
Past research on the early development of recording of a mother singing with the inten-
musical perception has focused on infants’ tion of putting her baby to sleep, versus a
recording of the same mother singing the same
discrimination abilities (e.g., see Trehub,
song but with the intention of arousing and
Schellenberg, & Hill, in press; Trehub &
playing with her baby (Trainor & Rock, 1997).
Trainor, 1993; Trehub, Trainor, & Unyk, 1993,
While these studies indicate that infants
for reviews), showing that infants can detect
have preferences and attend to emotional
changes in pitch contour (i.e., whether the
information, they have not linked infants’
melody goes up or down) and pitch interval
preferences to aspects of musical structure
(pitch distance between two tones). Little
beyond performance or surface features such
research has examined whether infants have
as voice quality, pitch height, and tempo.
distinct evahative responses to musical forms
According to Meyer (19.56), emotions in
that convey different emotional messages to
music arise as a function of the relation
adults, but infants do show marked prefer-
between musical events (sounds) and learned
ences for many other kinds of stimuli. For
expectations generated through knowledge of
instance, breast-fed babies prefer to smell
the underlying structure of the musical system.
breast pads from their mother over those from Just as there are different languages, there are
another nursing mother (e.g., Cernoch & Por- different musical systems, each employing
ter, 1985) and a preference for sweet over different pitch interval relations or scales. This
acidic tastes is present in the newborn (Crook, structure is learned through simple exposure:
1987). In the visual domain, infants prefer to even musically untrained Western adults have
look at faces that are rated as attractive by implicit knowledge of Western musical pitch
adults over those rated as unattractive (e.g., structure (e.g., Trainor & Trehub, 1992). How-
Langlois, Ritter, Roggman, & Vaughn, 1991). ever, 6- to 8-month-old infants do not (Lynch,
Caregivers modify their speech to infants, Eilers, Oller, & Urbano, 1990; Trainor & Tre-
raising it in pitch, slowing it in tempo, elon- hub, 1992). Therefore, any emotional reac-
gating vowels, and adding slow pitch contours tions infants experience when listening to
with large frequency ranges (e.g., Fernald, music cannot be based on musical-system-
1991; Papousek, 1992). A number of studies specific pitch structure, as Meyer (1956) pro-
have shown that infants prefer to listen to such posed for adults.
infant-directed (“musical”) over adult-directed Before concluding that musical pitch struc-
speech, and show heightened emotional ture cannot generate emotional reactions in
responses to the former (e.g., Cooper & Aslin, infants, however, the precursors of sensitivity
Preference for Consonance 79

to system-specific pitch structure need to be wood, 1991). Hence, two simultaneous tones
examined. One dimension of perceptual pitch (or harmonics) separated by less than a critical
space, that of consonance/dissonance, appears band are not fully resolved by the ear and the
to play a role in virtually all musical systems beating (amplitude fluctuations) that arises
(Schellenberg & Trehub, 1994b). Further, from their interaction is perceived as rough-
infants discriminate consonant from dissonant ness or dissonance.
intervals (Schellenberg & Trainor, 1996; The perceived consonance of an interval is
Schellenberg & Trehub, 1996; Trainor, 1997) a function of the simplicity of the ratio of the
and adults rate one extreme of this dimension fundamental frequencies of its component
as sounding “pleasant” or “beautiful” (e.g., tones rather than the pitch distance between
van de Geer, Levelt & Plomp, 1962) and the them (Helmholtz, 1954). When the ratio of the
other extreme as sounding “unpleasant” (e.g., fundamental frequencies of two tones can be
ominous, solemn, dark; Wedin, 1972) and expressed with small integers, the tones have
suitable for lamentation (Rigg, 1937). Thus, many harmonics in common and relatively
the consonance/dissonance di-mension few that are less than a critical bandwidth
appears to be a logical place to begin to exam- apart. By contrast, when the fundamental fre-
ine the effect of musical pitch structure on quencies are related by larger-integer ratios,
infants’ evaluative reactions. the tones have fewer harmonics in common,
The interval between two tones specifies and more that fall within a critical band. For
the distance in pitch that separates them. In its example, the most consonant interval is the
most simple definition, consonant intervals are octave, with component fundamental frequen-
those that sound smooth or pleasant to adults, cies standing in a 1:2 ratio. In this case, all
while dissonant intervals are those that sound harmonics of the upper tone are also harmon-
rough or unpleasant (e.g., Plomp & Levelt, ics of the lower tone. The second most simple
1965; Schellenberg & Trehub, 1994b). While ratio, 2:3, forms a perfect fifth, which is per-
the perceived pleasantness of an interval can ceptually the second most consonant interval.
be affected by experience and the context in Two dissonant intervals are the tritone (ratio
which the interval is heard (such effects are 32:45) and the minor ninth (15:32), which
referred to as musical consonance, see have no low harmonics in common and many
Cazden, 1980), sensor-y comonance (also pairs that fall within a critical band. Pitch dis-
referred to as tonal, Plomp & Levelt, 1965, or tance does not play a direct role in this classi-
psychoacoustic, Bregman, 1990, consonance) fication. The two tones of the octave and
is perceived for intervals presented in isolation perfect fifth are separated by 12 and 7 semi-
and is thought to result from relatively periph- tones, respectively, whereas those of the minor
eral properties of the auditory system. ninth and tritone are separated by 13 and 6
Sensory dissonance arises when two tones semitones, respectively.
sounded simultaneously have non-identical Adults’ sensitivity to the consonanceldisso-
harmonics (a complex tone with a fundamen- nance dimension emerges from three types of
tal frequency or pitch of 100 Hz can have findings, spanning perceptual to aesthetic
energy at each harmonic: 100, 200, 300, Hz effects. First, consonant intervals are easier to
and so on) that are separated by less than a process than dissonant intervals. From at least
critical bandwidth (somewhat less than 3 as young as 6 months of age, this is true of
semitones, where a semitone is l/12 of an infants as well. It is easier for both age groups
octave on a log frequency scale) (Kameoka & to detect changes in simultaneous (both tones
Kuriyagawa, 1969; Plomp & Levelt, 1965). sounded at the same time) consonant over dis-
The critical bandwidth corresponds to the sonant intervals (Schellenberg & Trehub,
width of the auditory filter characteristic of the 1996; Trainor, 1997). Further, it is easier for
basilar membrane in the inner ear (Green- both age groups to detect changes in one note
80 INFANT BEHAVIOR & DEVELOPMENT Vol. 21, No. 1, 1998

of a melody with prominent consonant inter- The measure of infants’ preference was rela-
vals between successive tones than in a mel- tive looking time to consonant versus disso-
ody with prominent dissonant intervals (e.g., nant intervals. There is much evidence that
Schellenberg & Trehub, 1994a, 1996; Cohen, looking time is a good measure of affective
Thorpe, & Trehub, 1987; Trainor & Trehub, response as well as attentional preference. In
1993a, b). Second, consonance is a dimension the visual domain, infants, children, and adults
of the perceived similarity of intervals (Levelt, all look longer at faces rated independently by
van de Geer, & Plomp, 1966). Again, this has adults as attractive than those rated as unat-
been shown to be true of infants as well tractive (e.g., see Langlois, Roggman, &
(Demany & Armand, 1984; Schellenberg & Rieser-Danner, 1990). At the same time,
Trainor, 1996). Given a consonant interval, infants show more positive affect to the
infants find it easier to detect a change to a “attractive” faces. In the auditory domain,
dissonant interval than to another consonant infants both look longer at and show more
interval, even when the change in pitch dis- positive affect for infant-directed over adult-
tance is greater in the latter case (Schellenberg directed speech (Werker & McLeod, 1989).
& Trainor, 1996). Thus, two consonant inter- Thus, we used the relative amount of time
vals sound more similar than do a consonant infants looked at visual displays in order to lis-
and a dissonant interval. ten to the consonant versus dissonant intervals
The third type of finding is of most interest as the measure of infants’ preference.
here: as described above, adults find conso-
nant intervals more pleasant-sounding than
EXPERIMENT 1
dissonant intervals (e.g., see Schellenberg &
Trehub, 1994b for a review). Crowder,
Method
Reznick, and Rosenkrantz (199 1) reported that
although infants showed no preference for a
major over a minor chord (the former being Participants
somewhat more consonant than the latter),
infants preferred to listen to a highly conso- Twelve infants (6 male and 6 female),
nant (C, E, G4 C, [130.8, 329.6, 392.0, 261.6 between 6 months 0 days and 6 months 28
Hz]) over a highly dissonant (C, C#, F#4 B, days (M = 6 months 11 days) were tested. All
[130.8, 277.2, 349.2, 493.9 Hz]) chord. How- were born within 2 weeks of term, weighed at
ever, these chords differ in several ways (e.g., least 2500 g at birth, and were healthy at the
the range of the second chord is larger; the time of testing. A further 3 were excluded, 1
first chord contains 3 different pitch classes due to equipment failure, 1 for failing to turn
whereas the second chord contains 4). Zentner his or her head, and 1 due to fussing. A t-test
(1996) also reported that infants preferred to revealed that the performance of males and
listen to consonant over dissonant intervals. females did not differ.
However, interval size and pitch distance were
completely confounded in this study (the con-
Apparatus
sonant intervals were predominantly 3 or 4
semitones whereas the dissonant intervals The stimulus materials were created on a
were 1 semitone in size). It is possible that Macintosh Quadra 950 computer using Night-
infants simply prefer larger over smaller inter- ingale software (Camporo Icuity Products)
vals. and a Korg WFD synthesizer. Digital record-
The present study examined preferences for ings were made with a Macintosh Quadra 950
consonant versus dissonant intervals that were computer running Digidesign software.
matched for average interval size and trans- During the experiment, the digital sound
posed to a number of different pitch levels. files were played by a Macintosh IIci com-
Preference for Consonance 81

puter with an Audiomedia II sound card (for [261X&392.0 Hz]) and two octaves (C&Z,
16-bit sound production). The sound was fed [261X5-523.3 Hz] and E,-E, [329X+659.3
through a Denon amplifier (PMA-480R) to Hz]). The dissonant set consisted of two tri-
two audiological loudspeakers (GSI) located tones (Bb3-E, [246.9-329.6 Hz] and F4-B4
in a large sound-attenuating booth (Industrial [349.2493.9 Hz]) and two minor ninths (Bb,-
Acoustics Co). The loudspeakers were located B, [246.9-493.9 Hz] and E, to F, [329.6-
inside the sound-attenuating booth, one on 698.5 Hz]). These intervals are similar in size:
each side of the infant, who sat on his or her the tritone is one semitone smaller than the
parent’s lap across from the experimenter. A perfect fifth and the minor ninth is one semi-
chamber containing a toy and lights was tone larger than the octave. With octave equiv-
located under each loudspeaker. The front of alence, both the consonant and dissonant sets
each chamber was smoked Plexiglas, such that contain 4 different notes, A C E G and Bb B E
the toy was only visible when the lights were F, respectively. Further, the range is identical
illuminated. The lights and a button box were (19 semitones) in both cases. In terms of pitch
height, infants would be expected to prefer the
connected to a Strawberry Tree I/O card in the
dissonant set, if anything, as it is slightly
computer through a custom-built interface
higher (1 semitone), and has a slightly higher
box.
average pitch. The two sets differ in that the
notes of the consonant set are all members of
Stimuli one key, that of C major, whereas two keys are
needed to encompass the four notes of the dis-
During consonant trials, a set of four con- sonant set. However, adults tend to hear the
sonant intervals was repeated in random order, consonant set as going between two keys, C
with the constraint that no interval could be major and A minor. In any case, infants of this
presented twice in a row. During dissonant tri- age do not appear to have knowledge of key
als, a set of dissonant intervals was presented structure (Trainor & Trehub, 1992), so it is
similarly. All tones comprising the intervals unlikely that this difference could influence
were created with piano timbre. The conso- their preferences. Each interval was repeated
nant set of intervals consisted of two perfect in a rhythmic pattern (onset-to-onsets of 600,
fifths (A,-E, [220.0-329.6 Hz] and C, to G, 300, 300, 600 ms; see Figure 1). Rhythmic

consonant set
etc

261.6 220.0 329.6 220.0 261.6

dissonant set
etc

246.9 349.2 246.9 349.2 329.6

FIGURE1
Sample excerpts based on the consonant (upper panel) and dissonant (lower panel) sets from Experi-
ment 1. Numbers under the notes indicate the fundamental frequencies.
82 INFANT BEHAVIOR & DEVELOPMENT Vol. 21, No. 1, 1998

patterns were separated by 600 ms. Longer set was always played from one side and the
notes were 500 ms and shorter notes were 200 dissonant set from the other. The initial set
ms. Intervals were presented at approximately (consonant or dissonant) and the initial side of
60 dB(A) at the location of the infant’s head. presentation was counterbalanced across
infants. The parent and experimenter listened
to masking music through headphones, and
Procedure
were not aware of which set was being pre-
A preference procedure was modified from sented on which side for each infant.
that of Fernald (1985), whereby infants con-
trolled the amount of time that each of the
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
consonant and dissonant versions were
sounded through their looking behavior. Each
infant was tested individually, sitting on their For each infant, the looking time to the 10
parent’s lap facing the experimenter. When the consonant set trials was divided by the total
infant was looking forward (i.e., at the experi- looking time across all 20 trials. The propor-
menter), the experimenter pressed a button on tion of looking time to the consonant set (M =
the box connected to the computer to begin the .60, SD = .12) was significantly greater than
first trial. This caused the lights to flash in the the expected chance value of 0.5, t( 11) = 3.06,
chamber on one side of the infant, revealing a p < ,005 (Figure 2). Eleven of the 12 infants
toy. When the infant turned to look at the illu- looked longer to produce the consonant set.
minated toy, the experimenter pressed a sec- An ANOVA with proportion looking time to
ond button that caused the lights to remain on the consonant set as the dependent variable
and the set (consonant or dissonant) for that revealed no significant difference across the
trial to begin playing. The music (and lights) first 10 versus second 10 trials, as well as no
remained on until the infant looked away for significant effects of which set (consonant or
at least two seconds (all controlled by com- dissonant) or which side (left or right)
puter). Twenty trials of the consonant and dis- occurred first. Absolute looking times did
sonant sets alternated, as did side of pre- decline significantly from the first to the sec-
sentation, so that for each infant the consonant ond half of the procedure, t(l1) = 3.87, p <

consonant version
dissonant version

fmt 10 trials second 10 trials


FIGURE 2
Mean looking times (and standard errors) to the consonant and dissonant sets across the first and sec-
ond halves of Experiment 1.
Preference for Consonance a3

.002 (mean looking times per trial = 9.2s and with piano timbre. The consonant version was
5.4 s for the first and second halves, respec- played as written. In the dissonant version, all
tively). Thus, although significant habituation Gs were changed to Gbs and all Ds to Dbs.
occurred from the first to the second half, the This had the effect of creating many dissonant
proportion of looking time to produce the con- intervals, including many tritones and minor
sonant version remained similar. ninths. For each subsequent trial of each ver-
These findings indicate that not only do sion, the music began where it had left off in
infants discriminate consonant from dissonant the previous trial. The tempo (120 quarter
intervals, but, like adults, they prefer to listen notes per min.) and intensity (approximately
to consonant intervals. In Experiment 2, the 60 dB(A)) were identical across the two ver-
question of whether this preference for conso- sions.
nance would generalize to a more naturalistic With the dissonant modification, the piece
context was tested. Specifically, we modified a is no longer clearly in a key, which would
Mozart minuet to produce versions with pre- have consequences for adults’ perception in
dominantly consonant or predominantly disso- terms of musical consonance. As defined
nant intervals. In both versions, however, the above, the perception of musical consonance
sense of the phrasing and general structure of depends on the context in relation to one’s
the piece remained intact as the rhythmic, knowledge of the musical structure of the
durational, and pitch contour information was idiom, in this case, Western musical structure.
preserved. Adults would likely find the violations of key
structure in the dissonant version to sound par-
EXPERIMENT 2 ticularly bad. Infants, however, have not yet
learned about key structure (Lynch et al.,
1990; Trainor & Trehub, 1992), so it is
Method
unlikely that the lack of a well-defined key per
se would affect their perception of the disso-
Participants nant version. By contrast, infants would be
expected to perceive the abundant sensory dis-
The participants were 16 infants (7 female,
sonance in the modified version.
9 male) between 5 months 21 days and 6
months 25 days (M = 6 months 10 days) who
met the same criteria as those of Experiment 1. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
A further 5 infants were excluded, 1 for failing
to turn his or her head and 4 because of fuss- The results mirrored those of Experiment 1
ing. As in Experiment 1, there was no differ- (Figure 4). Infants listened significantly longer
ence in performance between males and to the consonant versions, t(l5) = 2.98, p <
females. .005 (mean proportion looking time to the
consonant version = .57, SD = .092). Twelve
Apparatus of the 16 infants looked longer to produce the
consonant version. Again there were no signif-
The apparatus was identical to that of icant effects of initial version (consonant or
Experiment 1. dissonant), initial side of presentation, or pro-
portion of looking time to the consonant ver-
sion across the first and second 10 trials,
Stimuli
although absolute looking times decreased
The stimulus was a simple minuet in C significantly from the first 10 to the second 10
major by Mozart, K. # If (see Figure 3). Two trials, t(l5) = 3.62, p < .OOl (mean looking
30-second versions were digitally generated times per trial = 9.7 s and 7.4 s, respectively).
a4 INFANT BEHAVIOR is DEVELOPMENT vol. 21, NO. 1, 1998

Minuet (original version)

FIGURE 3
The original consonant (upper panel) and dissonant (lower panel) versions of the Mozart minuet used
in Experiment 2.
Preference for Consonance 85

q consonant version
l-
q dissonant version

10

0
fmt 10 trials second 10 trials

FIGURE 4
Mean looking times (and standard errors) to the consonant and dissonant versions across the first and
second halves of Experiment 2.

Thus, infants’ preference for consonant responses to an aspect of musical pitch struc-
over dissonant intervals remained even in a ture early in life. It also indicates that the con-
musical context with coherent rhythmic, dura- sonance/dissonance dimension is very basic to
tional, and pitch contour structure. This find- musical processing.
ing implies that consonance is highly salient to It remains an open question whether 6-
infants. month-old infants’ preference for consonance
is a direct consequence of auditory system
structure or whether it is learned early in life
GENERAL DISCUSSION through exposure to sounds in the natural
environment. Western music, particularly chil-
Infants preferred to listen to consonant over dren’s music, contains predominantly conso-
dissonant intervals. Given the relation nant intervals. Thus, infants have had
between looking times and affective responses considerably more exposure to consonant than
(e.g., Langlois et al., 1990; Werker & to dissonant intervals. Zajonc and his col-
McLeod, 1989), it can be concluded that leagues (e.g., Zajonc, Crandall, Kail, & Swap,
infants, like adults, have an affective prefer- 1974) have demonstrated that adults’ affective
ence for consonance. Because 6-month-old ratings of stimuli increase with exposure. It is
infants do not yet have knowledge of musical possible that infants’ preference for conso-
scale structure (Lynch et al., 1990; Trainor & nance is also an effect of exposure.
Trehub, 1992), emotional reactions to music Although the prevailing view in musical
cannot arise in an identical manner in infants aesthetics is that a moderate degree of devia-
and adults (Meyer, 1956). Thus it is particu- tion from expectancies based on prototypical-
larly interesting for the study of the origins of ity is preferred over complete predictability or
emotional responses to music that infants a high degree of unpredictability (e.g., Ber-
show similar affective reactions to those of lyne, 1974), both musically untrained listeners
adults to consonance and dissonance. It indi- and undergraduate music majors prefer sim-
cates first of all that infants have affective ple, highly prototypical chord sequences over
86 INFANT BEHAVIOR & DEVELOPMENT Vol. 21, No. 1, 1998

more complex sequences, even though they Cooper, R. P., & Aslin, R. N. (1990). Preference for
rate the latter as more interesting (Smith & infant-directed speech in the first month after
Melara, 1990). If infants’ preference for con- birth. Child Development, 61, 1584-1595.
sonance over dissonance is learned and is a Crook, C. K. (1987). Taste and olfaction. In I? Sala-
Patek & L. B. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of infant
function of familiarity, familiarity may play a
perception (Vol. 1, pp. 237-264). New York:
similar role in both infants’ and adults’ emo-
Academic Press.
tional reactions to music. On the other hand,
Crowder, R. G., Reznick, J. S., & Rosenkrantz, S.
preference for consonance may be a conse- L. (1991). Perception of the major/minor dis-
quence of the physiological structure of the tinction: V. Preferences among infants. Bulletin
auditory system. Adults and infants may pre- of the Psychonomic Society, 29, 187-188.
fer the musically familiar because it also tends Demany, L., & Armand, E (1984). The perceptual
to be the more consonant. An innate prefer- reality of tone chroma in early infancy. Journal
ence for consonance would also account for of the Acoustical Society of America, 76, 57-66.
the predominance of consonant intervals Dissanayake, E. (1992). Homo aestheticus: Where
art comes from and why. New York: Free Press.
across musical systems. Whatever the basis of
Femald, A. (1985). Four-month-old infants prefer
infants’ preference for consonance, the crucial
to listen to motherese. Infant Behavior and
result is that even though infants do not yet
Development, 8, 181-195.
have the musical-system-specific knowledge
Femald, A. (1991). Prosody in speech to children:
of scale structure that is involved in adults’ Prelinguistic and linguistic functions. Annals of
emotional reactions to music, infants are simi- Child Development, 8, 43-80.
lar to adults in their evaluative reactions to Fernald, A. (1993). Approval and disapproval:
consonance and dissonance. Infant responsiveness to vocal affect in familiar
and unfamiliar languages. Child Development,
Acknowledgment: This research was sup- 64,657-674.
ported by a grant from Sciences
the Natural Greenwood, D. D. (1991). Critical bandwidth and
and Engineering Research Council of Canada. consonance in relation to cochlear frequency-
position coordinates, Hearing Research 54,
We thank David Hill for help generating the
164-208.
stimuli and Glenn Schellenberg for comments
Helmholtz, H. L. F. von. (1954). On the sensations
on an earlier draft. of tone as a physiological basis for the theory of
melody (2nd ed.). A. J. Ellis (Ed. and Trans.).
New York: Dover. (Original work published
REFERENCES 1885.)
Kameoka, A., and Kuriyagawa, M. (1969). Conso-
Berlyne, D. E. (Ed.). (1974). Studies in the new nance theory: Part I. Consonance of dyads.
experimental aesthetics: Steps toward an objec- Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,
tive psychology of aesthetic appreciation. New 45,1451-1459.
York: Wiley. Kogan, H. (1994). On aesthetics and its origins:
Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory Scene Analysis. Some psychobiological and evolutionary con-
Cambridge, MA: Bradford. siderations. Social Research, 61, 139-165.
Cazden, N. (1980). The definition of consonance Langlois, J. H., Ritter, J. M., Roggman, L. A., &
and dissonance. International Review of the Vaughn, L. S. (1991). Facial diversity and infant
Aesthetics and Sociology of Music, 2, 23-l 68. preferences for attractive faces. Developmental
Cemoch, J. M., & Porter, R. H. (1985). Recognition Psychology, 27, 79-84.
of maternal auxiliary odors by infants. Child Langlois, J. H., Roggman, L. A., Rieser-Danner, L.
Development, 56, 1593-159s. A. (1990). Infants’ differential social responses
Cohen, A. J., Thorpe, L. A., & Trehub, S. E. (1987). to attractive and unattractive faces. Develop-
Infants’ perception of musical relations in short mental Psychology, 26, 153-159.
transposed tone sequences. Canadian Journal of Levelt, W. J. M., van de Geer, J. P., and Plomp, R.
Psychology, 41, 33-47. (1966). Triadic comparisons of musical inter-
Preference for Consonance 87

vals. British Journal of Mathematical and Sta- infant-directed singing. Infant Behavior and
tistical Psychology, 19, 163-179. Development, 20, 383-396.
Lomax, A. (1968). Folk song style and culture. Trainor, L. J., & Rock, A. M. L. (1997, April). Dis-
Washington, DC: American Association for the tinctive messages in infant-directed playsongs
Advancement of Science. and lullabies. Poster presented at the Society for
Lynch, M. P., Eilers, R. E., Oller, D. K., & Urbano, Research in Child Development, Washington,
R. C. (1990). Innateness, experience, and music D.C.
perception. Psychological Science, 1,272-276. Trainor, L. J., & Trehub, S. E. (1992). A compari-
Meyer, R. B. (1956). Emotion and meaning in son of infants’ and adults’ sensitivity to Western
music. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. musical structure. Journal of Experimental Psy-
Papousek, M. (1992). Early ontogeny of vocal com- chology: Human Perception and Performance,
munication in parent-infant interactions. In H. 18,394-402.
Papousek, U. Jurgens, & M. Papousek (Eds.), Trainor, L. J., & Trehub, S. E. (1993a). Musical
Nonverbal Vocal Communication (pp. 230- context effects in infants and adults: Key dis-
261). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. tance. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Plomp, R. & Level& W. J. M. (1965). Tonal conso- Human Perception and Perfomtance, 19, 615-
nance and critical bandwidth. Journal of the 626.
Acoustical Society of America, 38,518-560. Trainor, L. J., & Trehub, S. E. (1993b). What medi-
Rigg, M. G. (1937). Musical expression: An inves- ates infants’ and adults’ superior processing of
tigation of the theories of Erich Sorantin. Jour- the major over the augmented triad? Music Per-
nal of Experimental Psychology, 21,223-229. ception, 11, 185-196.
Schellenberg, E. G., & Trainor, L. J. (1996). Sen- Trehub, S. E., Schellenberg, E. G., & Hill, D. S. (in
sory consonance and the perceptual similarity of press). The origins of music perception and cog-
complex-tone harmonic intervals: Tests of adult nition: A developmental perspective. In I.
and infant listeners. Journal of the Acoustical Delitge & J. Sloboda (Eds.), Music perception
Society of America, 100, 3321-3328. and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Schellenberg, E. G., & Trehub, S. E. (1994a). Fre- Trehub, S. E., & Trainor, L. J. (in press). Singing to
quency ratios and the discrimination of pure infants: Lullabies and play songs. In C. Rovee-
tone sequences. Perception & Psychophysics, Collier (Ed.), Advances in infancy research.
56,472-478. Greenwich, CT Ablex.
Schellenberg, E. G., & Trehub, S. E. (1994b). Fre- Trehub, S. E., & Trainor, L. J. (1993). Listening
quency ratios and the perception of tone pat- strategies in infancy: The roots of language and
terns. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 191- musical development. In S. McAdams & E. Big-
201. and (Eds.), Cognitive aspects of human audition
Schellenberg, E. G., and Trehub, S. E. (1996). Nat- (pp. 278-327). London: Oxford University
ural musical intervals: Evidence from infant lis- Press.
teners, Psychological Science, 7, 272-277. Trehub, S. E., Trainor, L. J., & Unyk, A. M. (1993).
Smith, J. D., & Melara, R. J. (1990). Aesthetic pref- Music and speech processing in the first year of
erence and syntactic prototypicality in music: life. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in Child
‘Tis the gift to be simple. Cognition, 34, 279- Development and Behavior (Vol. 24, pp. l-35).
298. New York: Academic Press.
Trainor, L. J. (1997). The effect of frequency ratio Trehub, S. E., Unyk, A. M., & Trainor, L. J.
on infants’ and adults’ discrimination of simul- (1993a). Adults identify infant-directed music
taneous intervals. Journal of Experimental Psy- across cultures. Znfant Behavior and Develop-
chology: Human Perception and Performance, ment, 16, 193-211.
23, 1427-38. Trehub, S. E., Unyk, A. M., & Trainor, L. J.
Trainor, L. J. (1996). Infant preferences for infant- (1993b). Maternal singing in cross-cultural per-
directed versus non-infant-directed play songs spective. Infant Behavior and Development, 16,
and lullabies. Infant Behavior and Development, 285-295.
19,83-92. van de Geer, J. l?, Levelt, W. J. M., & Plomp, R.
Trainor, L. J., Clark, E. D., Huntley, A., & Adams, (1962). The connotation of musical intervals.
B. (1997). The acoustic basis of preferences for Acta Psychol. 20, 308-3 19.
88 INFANT BEHAVIOR & DEVELOPMENT vol. 21, NO. 1, 1998

Wedin, L. (1972). A multidimensional study of per- Zajonc, R. B., Crandall, R., Kail, R. V., Jr., & Swap,
ceptual-emotional qualities in music. Scandina- W. (1974). Effect of extreme exposure frequen-
vian Journal of Psychology, 13, 241-257. cies on different affective ratings of stimuli.
Werker, J. F., & McLeod, P J. (1989). Infant prefer- Perceptual and Motor Skills, 38, 667-678.
ence for both male and female infant-directed Zentner, M. R. (1996). Infant’s preferences for con-
talk: A developmental study of attentional and sonance vs. dissonance and for major vs. minor
affective responsiveness. Canadian Journal of in music. Infant Behavior and Development, 19,
Psychology, 43, 230-246. 836 (Abstract).
Winner, E. (1982). Invented worlds: The psychol-
ogy of the arts. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Uni-
versity Press.
05 February 1997; Revised 28 May 1997m

You might also like