You are on page 1of 22

This article was downloaded by: [2a09:8044:418b::6] On: 27 March 2024, At: 12:39

Publisher: Institute for Operations Research and the Management Sciences (INFORMS)
INFORMS is located in Maryland, USA

Information Systems Research


Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://pubsonline.informs.org

Human vs. Automated Sales Agents: How and Why


Customer Responses Shift Across Sales Stages
Martin Adam, Konstantin Roethke, Alexander Benlian

To cite this article:


Martin Adam, Konstantin Roethke, Alexander Benlian (2023) Human vs. Automated Sales Agents: How and Why Customer
Responses Shift Across Sales Stages. Information Systems Research 34(3):1148-1168. https://doi.org/10.1287/isre.2022.1171

Full terms and conditions of use: https://pubsonline.informs.org/Publications/Librarians-Portal/PubsOnLine-Terms-and-


Conditions

This article may be used only for the purposes of research, teaching, and/or private study. Commercial use
or systematic downloading (by robots or other automatic processes) is prohibited without explicit Publisher
approval, unless otherwise noted. For more information, contact permissions@informs.org.

The Publisher does not warrant or guarantee the article’s accuracy, completeness, merchantability, fitness
for a particular purpose, or non-infringement. Descriptions of, or references to, products or publications, or
inclusion of an advertisement in this article, neither constitutes nor implies a guarantee, endorsement, or
support of claims made of that product, publication, or service.

Copyright © 2022 The Author(s)

Please scroll down for article—it is on subsequent pages

With 12,500 members from nearly 90 countries, INFORMS is the largest international association of operations research (O.R.)
and analytics professionals and students. INFORMS provides unique networking and learning opportunities for individual
professionals, and organizations of all types and sizes, to better understand and use O.R. and analytics tools and methods to
transform strategic visions and achieve better outcomes.
For more information on INFORMS, its publications, membership, or meetings visit http://www.informs.org
INFORMATION SYSTEMS RESEARCH
Vol. 34, No. 3, September 2023, pp. 1148–1168
https://pubsonline.informs.org/journal/isre ISSN 1047-7047 (print), ISSN 1526-5536 (online)

Human vs. Automated Sales Agents: How and Why Customer


Responses Shift Across Sales Stages
Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

Martin Adam,a,* Konstantin Roethke,a Alexander Benliana


a
Information Systems and E-Services, Technical University of Darmstadt, 64293 Darmstadt, Germany
*Corresponding author
Contact: adam@ise.tu-darmstadt.de, https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9369-7203 (MA); roethke@ise.tu-darmstadt.de,
https://orcid.org/0000-0003-3921-6952 (KR); benlian@ise.tu-darmstadt.de, https://orcid.org/0000-0002-7294-3097 (AB)

Received: July 17, 2020 Abstract. Customers in sales processes increasingly encounter automated sales agents
Revised: May 19, 2021; March 27, 2022; (ASAs) that complement or replace human sales agents (HSAs). Yet, little is known about
August 11, 2022 whether, how, and why customers respond to ASAs in contrast to HSAs across successive
Accepted: September 2, 2022 decision stages of the same sales process. Even less is known about customer responses to
Published Online in Articles in Advance: HSA-ASA combinations, where both agents assume distinct roles and focus on comple-
November 10, 2022
mentary tasks that are traditionally performed by only one single sales agent. Against this
backdrop, this paper explores the influence of increasingly common sales representative
https://doi.org/10.1287/isre.2022.1171 (rep) types (i.e., ASA, HSA, and HSA-ASA) on customer decisions across sales stages.
Drawing on information processing theory and the literature on hedonic-utilitarian deci-
Copyright: © 2022 The Author(s)
sion making, we investigate customer responses to text-based ASAs from vendor compa-
nies in two common early stages of email sales processes (i.e., sales initiation stages) when
customers successively decide whether to indicate their initial interest in an offer and then,
whether to provide their contact information. Specifically, we conducted two complemen-
tary multi-decision experiments, namely (1) a randomized field experiment in a high-
stakes sales initiation setting (n  325) and (2) a subsequent randomized online experiment
to complement the real-world insights (n  408). Our core findings reveal reversing effect
patterns of sales rep types across stages: although customers are more likely to indicate
their initial interest to HSAs (versus ASAs) because of HSAs’ higher levels of social pres-
ence, they are less likely to provide contact information to HSAs because of HSAs’ lower
levels of performance expectancy and effort expectancy. We also show that HSA-ASA
combinations can be reasonable options for single ASAs, yet contextual features of the sales
setting may affect differential customer responses to HSA-ASA combinations (versus
ASAs) in each sales stage. Taken together, we uncover shifting effect patterns in customer
responses to sales rep types across successive sales stages and shed light on the consecutive
underlying mechanisms that explain these shifts. These findings have significant implica-
tions for vendor companies seeking to allocate HSAs and/or ASAs effectively across vari-
ous decision stages in sales processes and beyond.

History: Wonseok Oh, Senior Editor and Khim Yong Goh; Associate Editor.
Open Access Statement: This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-
NoDerivatives 4.0 International License. You are free to download this work and share with others,
but cannot change in any way or use commercially without permission, and you must attribute this
work as “Information Systems Research. Copyright © 2022 The Author(s).” https://doi.org/10.1287/
isre.2022.1171, used under a Creative Commons Attribution License: https://creativecommons.org/
licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.”
Funding: This work was supported by the Center for Responsible Digitality (ZEVEDI) and the German
Research Foundation (DFG) [Grant 471168026].
Supplemental Material: The online appendix is available at https://doi.org/10.1287/isre.2022.1171.

Keywords: sales agent • email • information processing • hedonic-utilitarian decision making • decision stages • sales funnel •
randomized field experiment

1. Introduction called email sales initiation stages), customers increas-


Customers in various sales settings increasingly encoun- ingly encounter text-based ASAs that undertake cus-
ter automated sales agents1 (ASAs) that emulate human- tomer outreach via email. For example, ASAs, such as
human interactions and complement or replace human 7Targets’s Assistant (2022), Conversica (2022), Pega’s Email
sales agents (HSAs). Particularly in the early stages of Bot (2022), and Salesforce’s Einstein Assistant (2022), can
vendor companies’ email sales processes (hereinafter initiate email communications with customers for sales

1148
Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s) 1149

offers, elicit interest, and finally, collect contact informa- Surprisingly, although customers are increasingly
tion to schedule sales appointment meetings for the dealing with ASAs, research has devoted only limited
specification and finalization of sales offers via more per- attention to examining differential customer responses to
sonal communication channels (e.g., live chats or phone sales representative (rep) types (i.e., ASA, HSA, and HSA-
communication). The deployment of ASAs in the email ASA), particularly across key email sales initiation stages
sales initiation stages is particularly consequential for (i.e., customers’ initial interest indication and contact infor-
Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

vendor companies because email is one of the most effec- mation provision). From our review of the information sys-
tive business communication channels in technology- tems literature on recommendation agents and related
mediated sales, often outranking websites and chats research, we highlight three salient issues.
(e.g., The Relevancy Group 2017, Econsultancy 2019). First, prior studies on customers’ acceptance of HSAs
Moreover, the largest number of customers are influ- and ASAs have mainly taken a monolithic view of sales
enced in sales initiation stages, making it hard to com- processes, treating them as single-stage phenomena. For
pensate for early mistakes in the later stages of the example, previous research has mainly investigated the
sales process. As such, it comes as no surprise that degree to which customers accept advice and purchase
more than 50% of enterprises engage in some form products from HSAs versus ASAs (e.g., Castelo et al. 2019,
of email communication automation and consider Luo et al. 2019b). Yet, customers in various sales settings
ASAs an explicit means through which to communi- need to make subsequent decisions and thus, pass through
cate with customers (e.g., Gartner 2017, Statista 2021). multiple decision stages in which customers prioritize and
Given these reports and developments, deploying process information differently (e.g., Bettman et al. 1998,
ASAs in sales processes can be considered one of the Huang et al. 2019). Especially in the early stages of email
most relevant technological trends for vendor com- sales processes (e.g., Peppers and Rogers 2004, Wood-
panies at the customer interface. pecker 2022), customers are likely to change priorities
The increasing deployment of ASAs is closely con- across decision stages and related decisions, as they first
nected to the ongoing transformation of business proc- decide (1) whether to indicate their initial interest in a sales
esses because of the automation of routine processes offer before deciding (2) whether to provide contact infor-
and the resulting complementation or substitution of mation for further sales interactions. Given these changes
HSAs with ASAs (e.g., Daugherty and Wilson 2018, Rai in priorities, customers may respond differently to HSAs
et al. 2019). In contrast to HSAs, ASAs promise unpre- than ASAs across consecutive decision stages. Thus, it
cedented utilitarian value (i.e., functional benefits, goal is theoretically intriguing to move beyond customer re-
achievement) through unique and scalable perform- sponses in single-stage sales processes and the implicit
ance and efficiency gains in communication channels assumption of uniform and stable effects of sales rep types.
for both customers and vendor companies, such as In doing so, we may reveal shifting customer responses to
through cost savings, 24/7 availability, and reduced sales rep types across successive decision stages and dem-
response times (e.g., Puntoni et al. 2021, Schanke et al. onstrate that HSAs and ASAs have distinct strengths in dif-
2021). At the same time, customers may anticipate ferent decision stages.
drawbacks in interactions with automated agents (e.g., Second and relatedly, previous studies on customer
Dietvorst et al. 2015, 2018; Srinivasan and Sarial-Abi acceptance of HSAs and ASAs have mainly looked at the
2021). For instance, customers may perceive that ASAs direct effects of sales rep types on single-decision out-
provide less hedonic value (i.e., affective benefits, expe- comes, such as whether customers accept a product rec-
riential pleasure) than HSAs, such as by displaying a ommendation or a sales offer from HSAs versus ASAs
lack of depth, compassion, and warmth (e.g., Castelo (e.g., Castelo et al. 2019, Longoni and Cian 2022). A few
et al. 2019, Longoni and Cian 2022). Given these asso- papers have also examined isolated effect mechanisms
ciations, customers may respond differently to ASAs (e.g., trust, eeriness) that explain single-stage decisions
versus HSAs depending on whether they prioritize (e.g., Castelo et al. 2019, Mende et al. 2019). Yet, previous
hedonic or utilitarian value at different sales stages. As research has largely neglected to explicate the distinct
such, it may even be better for customers to encounter a mechanisms underlying the potentially changing direct
combination of an ASA and an HSA, where both agents effects of sales rep types across consecutive customer
assume distinct roles and focus on complementary decisions. Indeed, customers may differentially prioritize
tasks that are traditionally performed by only one sin- hedonic and utilitarian attributes of sales reps and thus,
gle sales agent (e.g., Exceed.ai 2019, Paschen et al. 2020). make different hedonic-utilitarian attribute trade-offs
In one prevalent combinatorial type that emphasizes across different sales stages, which may explain shifting
relative strengths, the HSA is presented as being in customer responses to HSAs and ASAs. The implications
charge of customer communication, while the ASA is of potentially changing explanatory mechanisms are
presented as being responsible for sales offer specifica- important: without knowing why ASAs (versus HSAs)
tions in the later stages of the sales process (hereafter may be superior in influencing customer responses at a
referred to as HSA-ASA). certain stage, vendor companies may assume they are
Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
1150 Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s)

more appreciated and thus, more effective in situations focused on the effects of sales rep types in single-stage
where the exact opposite is perhaps true. sales processes (e.g., Castelo et al. 2019, Luo et al.
Third and lastly, previous research has mainly investi- 2019b), we shed light on their disparate effects across
gated customer acceptance of different types of ASAs multiple consecutive decision stages. Our results reveal
(e.g., Qiu and Benbasat 2009, Li and Karahanna 2015) or whether and how customer responses to HSAs versus
responses to HSAs versus ASAs (e.g., Burton et al. 2019, ASAs shift, highlighting that the effects of sales rep
Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

Castelo et al. 2019). Recent research has started to shed types are not uniform across decision stages and are
light on the hitherto “missing middle” in which HSAs more complex than previously noted. Second, depart-
and ASAs are not presented in isolation but in combina- ing from studies that mainly looked at the direct effects
tion; that is, humans are an integral part of a sales rep of sales rep types (e.g., Burton et al. 2019, Longoni and
configuration (Daugherty and Wilson 2018). For exam- Cian 2022), we shed light on key consecutive explana-
ple, initial studies on HSA-ASA combinations have tory mechanisms (i.e., social presence, performance ex-
investigated ASAs performing the same activities as pectancy, and effort expectancy) underlying the direct
HSAs (e.g., Xu et al. 2020) and ASAs acting as subordi- effects of sales rep types in different stages of email
nates to HSAs (e.g., McLeay et al. 2021, Longoni and sales initiations. As such, we unpack distinct explanatory
Cian 2022). Yet, we still know little about customer mechanisms that explain why customer responses shift
responses to HSA-ASA combinations having distinct across sales stages and reveal that the effects of sales
roles and performing complementary tasks, especially in rep types must be considered together with customers’
situations in which the HSA is in charge of customer hedonic-utilitarian attribute trade-offs during sales proc-
communication, while the ASA is responsible for sales esses. Third, our paper is one of the first to move beyond
offer specifications. This lack of research is puzzling treating customer responses to ASAs versus HSAs as
because both research and practice point toward the syn- two mutually exclusive and collectively exhaustive choice
ergistic value of employing HSAs and ASAs together, options (e.g., Burton et al. 2019, Castelo et al. 2019). Our
especially in distinct and complementary roles (e.g., Jain research shows that although promising HSA-ASA com-
et al. 2021, Fügener et al. 2022). As such, it is of scholarly binations (such as those in which HSAs and ASAs assume
interest to investigate customer responses to HSA-ASA distinct yet complementary roles) can be viable options
combinations relative to HSAs and ASAs in isolation. for single ASAs, contextual features of the sales settings
Against this backdrop, our paper aims to address the fol- are important boundary conditions (the when) that may
lowing research question: shape customer responses to HSA-ASA combinations in
How and why do customers differentially respond to sales each sales stage, and ultimately, the response shifts across
rep types (i.e., ASA, HSA, HSA-ASA) across critical sales stages. In doing so, we provide valuable insights into cus-
stages (e.g., email sales initiation stages)? tomer responses to increasingly relevant combinatorial
To answer our research question, we conducted a
sales rep types above and beyond HSAs and ASAs that
multi-method investigation (e.g., Mingers 2003, Venka-
act in strict isolation from one another. Taken together,
tesh et al. 2013) in the form of two complementary multi-
our findings suggest that vendors should carefully con-
decision experiments. Specifically, we investigated how
sider the priorities of their clients at various decision
and why customers respond to sales rep types (i.e., ASA,
stages of the sales process and employ the appropriate
HSA, and HSA-ASA) when making two crucial deci-
sales rep types.
sions in the email sales initiation stages (i.e., the initial
interest indication and contact information provision).
First, we partnered with the sales department of a lead- 2. Related Literature and
ing European pharmaceutical company to design and Theoretical Background
implement a randomized field experiment with real 2.1. Customer Responses to Automated
customers (n  325). The study reflected a high-stakes Sales Agents
field setting with serious consequences for both the Early research on recommendation agents has treated
pharmaceutical company and the customers in the ASAs as a specialized form of recommendation agent—
form of potentially high-volume sales transactions of up namely, one that specifically intends not only to recom-
to e20,000 per customer. Subsequently, we applied an mend products but also, to initiate and close sales (e.g.,
analogous design in a randomized online experiment Qiu and Benbasat 2009, Li and Karahanna 2015). This
(n  408) to complement the findings from our random- early research primarily analyzed customer acceptance
ized field experiment with an exploratory investigation of ASAs in isolation and without any reference groups
of the consecutive underlying mechanisms that explain (e.g., in contrast to HSAs), focusing on the design of
the observed direct effects of sales rep types across the their interface features, such as recommendation pre-
email sales initiation stages. sentation (e.g., set size, sorting cues) (e.g., Tam and Ho
Based on our insights, we make three main contribu- 2005, Komiak and Benbasat 2006) and avatar features
tions. First, in advancing prior work that has largely (e.g., anthropomorphism, explanation facility) (e.g.,
Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s) 1151

Wang and Benbasat 2007, Qiu and Benbasat 2009). research on the effects of sales rep types coincides with
Because of the increasing emergence of ASAs in sales the fact that ASAs increasingly permeate technology-
contexts (e.g., Mende et al. 2019, Luo et al. 2021, mediated communication channels (e.g., email, phone,
Schanke et al. 2021), recent research has started to chat), particularly because of their advances in conver-
make comparisons between HSAs and ASAs (e.g., Bur- sational capabilities (e.g., natural language processing
ton et al. 2019, Castelo et al. 2019), investigating cus- and understanding) (e.g., Benbya et al. 2021, Schanke
Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

tomer responses to HSAs versus ASAs and the related et al. 2021). In these channels, customers are even often
effects of these sales rep types on, for instance, per- unable to distinguish ASAs from HSAs (e.g., Luo et al.
ceived decision quality (Xu et al. 2017), consumption 2019b). Yet, despite this technological progress, evi-
(Mende et al. 2019), and sales (Luo et al. 2019b). dence that customers consistently appreciate ASAs
More recently, scholars’ interest in the deployment of over HSAs in sales processes is largely lacking. In fact,
HSA-ASA combinations has been increasing (e.g., customers have indicated that they prefer HSAs to
Daugherty and Wilson 2018, Jain et al. 2021). For exam- ASAs, particularly in hedonic-focused situations that
ple, studies on HSA-ASA combinations have inve- are, for example, highly subjective and emotion ori-
stigated HSAs performing the identical activities as ented (e.g., Castelo et al. 2019, Longoni and Cian 2022).
ASAs (Xu et al. 2020), HSAs acting as managers of Moreover, research has mostly neglected HSA-ASA
ASAs (e.g., Longoni et al. 2019, McLeay et al. 2021, combinations in which HSAs and ASAs perform com-
Longoni and Cian 2022), and the resulting effects of plementary tasks and are presented as one team to
these combinations on, for instance, recommendation customers, in contrast to studies on HSAs and ASAs
uncertainty (Xu et al. 2020) and feedback effectiveness performing identical tasks (e.g., Xu et al. 2020) or
(Luo et al. 2021). Indeed, researchers and practitioners being on different status levels (e.g., Longoni et al. 2019,
increasingly consider that the most effective configura- McLeay et al. 2021). Lastly, previous research on cus-
tions may not simply be “set-and-forget-it automations” tomer responses to sales rep types has mainly treated
that no longer include humans but rather, those that derive sales processes as a single-stage phenomenon but rarely
their power from combinations between humans and considered sales processes with multiple, successive
machines (e.g., Xu et al. 2014, 2020; Fügener et al. 2022). stages (e.g., Xu et al. 2017, Castelo et al. 2019), although
A previously largely under-researched combination is customers in the process of exploring, evaluating, and
where HSAs act in complementary partnership with purchasing an object of interest often face multiple deci-
ASAs, where an HSA and an ASA assume distinct roles sions and thus, go through different decision stages
that are traditionally performed by only one single sales (e.g., Choudhury and Karahanna 2008). Consequently,
agent (e.g., Paschen et al. 2020). In a prevalent type of potentially shifting customer responses to sales rep
HSA-ASA combinations, the HSA manages customer types along the decision stages in sales processes are
communication to elicit initial product interest and con- likely and important to consider, yet hitherto, they have
tact information in the very early stages of sales proc- been largely under-investigated. Against this backdrop,
esses, while the ASA is deployed to take care of more it is of scholarly interest to elucidate whether, how, and
analytical and operational tasks, such as specifying sales why the effects of sales rep types on customer res-
offers in the later sales stages. As such, these HSA-ASA ponses persist or change across consecutive decision
combinations comprise “two parties—one human, one stages of sales processes.
automated—each doing what they do best to close the
deal” (Exceed.ai 2019). Both research and practice antici- 2.2. Customer Information Processing and
pate orders-of-magnitude increases in productivity and Hedonic-Utilitarian Attribute Trade-Offs in
high synergy potentials through allocating HSAs and Sales Initiation Stages
ASAs to distinct yet complementary roles (e.g., Rai et al. According to information processing theory (e.g., Newell
2019, Jain et al. 2021, Fügener et al. 2022). Therefore, and Simon 1972, Simon 1978), humans process the infor-
HSA-ASA combinations have the potential to outper- mation they receive rather than merely responding to it.
form both single sales agents and teams of same-kind Information processing hereby mainly follows a linear
sales agents (e.g., two HSAs) and thus, may be particu- sequence: humans first perceive a large amount of infor-
larly appreciated by both customers and vendors. mation and process it at a superficial level. For deeper
Although existing studies on differential customer processing, they then prioritize the information and use
responses to sales rep types (i.e., ASA versus HSA ver- attention to separate relevant from irrelevant information.
sus HSA-ASA) provide an initial understanding of the Consequently, information processing is mainly about
effects of ASAs while accounting for the presence or trade-offs, in that humans can only deeply engage with
absence of HSAs, the differential effects of sales rep information when prioritizing and considering a small
types are still largely underexplored, resulting in “a amount of the available information.
surprising lack of empirical research investigating the The marketing and information systems literature
differences” (Xu et al. 2017, p. 852). The recent surge in (e.g., Bettman et al. 1998, Lambrecht et al. 2011, Huang
Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
1152 Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s)

et al. 2019) has drawn on information processing theory value (e.g., joy, excitement) to generate initial interest in
as a theoretical lens to postulate that as customers move the object before they look to signals for utilitarian attrib-
through different decision stages in the sales process, utes (e.g., shipping costs, price promotions) and related
they face different decisions and thus, have different utilitarian value (e.g., efficiency, task-technology fit) to
priorities, trading-off information in a way that is rele- inform their cost-benefit analysis of the object (e.g., Liu
vant for the respective decision in the decision stage. and Goodhue 2012, Huang et al. 2019).
Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

One such trade-off is the consideration of the hedonic In our paper, we presume shifting customer responses
and utilitarian attributes of an object (e.g., good, serv- to sales rep types (i.e., ASA, HSA, and HSA-ASA) in
ice) (e.g., Dhar and Wertenbroch 2000, Khan et al. 2005). common and salient customer decisions in the email
Hedonic attributes of an object (e.g., an appealing de- sales initiation stages because of customers’ hedonic-
sign of a laptop) signal hedonic value in the form of utilitarian attribute trade-offs. In these sales settings,
affective benefits that reflect customers’ perceptual and vendor companies typically organize the email sales ini-
experiential pleasure, focusing on how the object is tiation using a series of email exchanges, in which cus-
rewarding in itself; utilitarian attributes (e.g., reliable tomers face two main customer decisions (e.g., Peppers
performance of the same laptop) signal utilitarian value and Rogers 2004, Exceed.ai 2019, Woodpecker 2022).
in the form of cognitive benefits that reflect customers’ Customers successively decide (1) whether to indicate
functional and instrumental (i.e., goal-related) delibera- their initial interest in a new sales offer in the first step
tions, assessing the object as a means to an end (e.g., (i.e., an initial interest indication) and (2) whether to pro-
Botti and McGill 2011). Although customer decision vide contact information to schedule a sales appointment
making can comprise the consideration and processing meeting via more personal communication channels
of both hedonic and utilitarian attributes, customers (e.g., phone, live chat) in the second step (i.e., contact
often base a decision on the importance of either he- information provision).2 Given these two salient deci-
donic or utilitarian attributes in the respective decision sions, customers have two different guiding questions
stage and thus, make hedonic-utilitarian attribute and priorities that influence their hedonic-utilitarian
trade-offs in their information processing (e.g., Khan attribute trade-offs, whereby customers care about both
et al. 2005, Bhargave et al. 2015). Moreover, customers the sales object itself and the sales rep making the sales
in a mental state of discovering and exploring an object offer. In their decision about whether to indicate their
of interest do not randomly display hedonic-utilitarian initial interest, customers most often focus on whether
attribute trade-offs in the decision stages but rather fol- the email signals hedonic attributes to assess the he-
low systematic trade-off patterns, in that customers usu- donic value (e.g., warmth) to be initially interested in
ally first prioritize hedonic attributes to formulate initial building a trusting sales relationship. Subsequently, in
preferences before they subsequently consider utilitarian their decision about whether to provide contact infor-
attributes to engage in more granular cost-benefit assess- mation, customers usually care about whether the same
ments (e.g., Dhar and Wertenbroch 2000, Lambrecht and sales offer signals utilitarian attributes to assess the util-
Tucker 2013). For example, in e-commerce, customers itarian value (e.g., efficiency) that justify the investment
tend to look first to signals for hedonic attributes (e.g., of further time and effort in subsequent interactions
word of mouth, popularity cues) and associated hedonic (e.g., Dhar and Wertenbroch 2000, Dimoka et al. 2012).

Figure 1. Customer Information Processing in the Email Sales Initiation Stages


Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s) 1153

Figure 1 provides an overview of customer information 1, (c) and (d)) and of performance expectancy as well as
processing in the email sales initiation stages, highlight- effort expectancy as crucial customer beliefs regarding
ing customer decisions, guiding questions, and key utilitarian attributes that are factored into customers’ cost-
hedonic-utilitarian attribute considerations. benefit calculus driving contact information provi-
sion (Hypotheses 2, (c) and (d)).
3. Research Model and Hypotheses
Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

Development 3.1. The Effects of Sales Rep Type on Initial


Building on customer information processing and Interest Indication
hedonic-utilitarian trade-offs in email sales initiation In the first stage of an email sales initiation, customers
stages, we argue that the sales rep type (ASA versus usually receive an email from a sales rep and are asked
HSA versus HSA-ASA) influences customer decision to indicate their initial interest in a sales offer by answer-
outcomes.3 The gist of our theorizing is that sales rep ing the email. At this very first encounter, customers
types signal different hedonic and utilitarian attrib- usually ask, “Do I want to begin a business relationship
utes that customers appreciate based on the respective with the sales rep?” To answer this question, customers
hedonic-utilitarian attribute trade-off in the given deci- in a mental state of discovering an unexpected object
sion stage, causing shifting customer responses across tend to follow a systematic trade-off pattern in their
stages. More specifically, as depicted in our research information processing by first looking for hedonic
model in Figure 2, we argue that sales rep types have attributes that indicate hedonic value (e.g., affective
differential effects on the initial interest indication (Hy- benefits, experiential pleasure) to formulate initial
potheses 1, (a) and (b)) and contact information provi- preferences before they consider utilitarian attributes
sion (Hypotheses 2, (a) and (b)). For each stage, we that indicate utilitarian value (e.g., cognitive benefits,
hypothesize about the effects of HSAs relative to ASAs goal achievement) to engage in more granular cost-
(Hypotheses 1(a) and 2(a)), hence focusing on the com- benefit analyses (e.g., Dhar and Wertenbroch 2000,
parative effects of human versus automated single sales Bleier and Eisenbeiss 2015). For instance, research has
agents (e.g., Rai et al. 2019, Jain et al. 2021). Addition- shown that customers must first deem the sales initia-
ally, we hypothesize about the effects of HSA-ASAs rel- tion to be trustworthy before they are open to consid-
ative to ASAs (Hypotheses 1(b) and 2(b)), highlighting ering the sales offer and thus, proceeding toward
the potential of human-machine combinations in com- indicating their initial interest (e.g., De Wulf et al.
parison with full automation (e.g., Daugherty and 2001, Liu and Goodhue 2012).
Wilson 2018). Moreover, we develop our mediation We argue that customers respond more favorably to
hypotheses that shed light on the role of social pres- HSAs than to ASAs in the first encounter of a sales ini-
ence as a crucial customer perception of hedonic tiation and thus, are more likely to indicate their initial
attributes driving initial interest indication (Hypotheses interest to an HSA (versus an ASA). This is because

Figure 2. Research Model


Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
1154 Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s)

customers are more likely to perceive more hedonic for more personal interactions with customers than an
attributes (e.g., personal attention, empathy) that signal ASA alone, which is not only expected to signal less
the hedonic value in an HSA (versus an ASA), which hedonic value than an HSA does but also has to cover
customers appreciate in business relationships (e.g., Xu both roles. Given that customers face a primarily
et al. 2014, Tan et al. 2019). Indeed, the relationship mar- hedonic-focused decision when expressing their initial
keting and social psychology literatures (e.g., Snyder interest in a sales initiation encounter and therefore pri-
Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

and Fromkin 1980, De Wulf et al. 2001) indicate that oritize primarily hedonic attributes, the HSA's special-
customers tend to discount standardized and cold serv- ization in the role of a personal contact person makes
ices and instead, appreciate personalized and warm the HSA-ASA combination particularly appealing.
interactions in the very early stages of sales processes. In Previous research supports this argument. For instance,
these decision stages, customers usually tend to watch Longoni et al. (2019) demonstrated that patients accepted
out for social cues as hedonic attributes that signal this medical advice from a recommendation system less often
desired personalized care—cues that are particularly when the recommendation system derived the medical
rare and thus, sought after, particularly in text-based, advice alone relative to a human doctor deriving the medi-
technology-enabled communication (e.g., Gefen and cal advice together with a recommendation system. Thus,
Straub 2003, Tan et al. 2019). We assert that one of the we predict that customers will be more likely to indicate
few available social cues is the type of sales rep. their initial interest when experiencing an HSA-ASA com-
Accordingly, we expect that customers interpret ASAs bination (versus an ASA alone).
as capable of operating in a standardized and repetitive
Hypothesis 1 (b). Customers are more likely to indicate
manner that treats every case similarly, with little to no
their initial interest in email sales initiations when encoun-
marginal costs for the vendor company, whereas they
tering an HSA-ASA combination rather than an ASA.
are likely to view HSAs as a hedonic attribute signal of
the vendor company’s investment to meet the unique Why exactly should customers respond differently to
needs of individual customers (e.g., Leung et al. 2018, HSAs (and HSA-ASAs) versus ASAs and thus, be more
Castelo et al. 2019). This is in line with previous studies likely to indicate their interest? As elaborated, custom-
pointing toward higher acceptance of HSAs (versus ers who follow systematic trade-off patterns in their
ASAs) in relational and personal settings (e.g., Castelo information processing usually base their decision on
et al. 2019, Longoni and Cian 2022). Given the argu- hedonic attributes in very early stages of decision mak-
ments and empirical evidence, we propose that custom- ing to formulate initial preferences. Thus, any signal of
ers are more likely to indicate their initial interest to a human and warm touch that allows the customer to
HSAs (versus ASAs). infer some sense of personal commitment, investment,
and relationship should increase customers’ likelihood
Hypothesis 1 (a). Customers are more likely to indicate
of indicating an initial interest. Consequently, we
their initial interest in email sales initiations when encoun-
expect that social presence (Short et al. 1976, Gefen
tering an HSA rather than an ASA.
and Straub 2003)—the perceived sense of personal
How do customers respond to an HSA-ASA combina- and warm interactions with another human being—
tion relative to an ASA alone in terms of initial interest will be a crucial customer perception that explains
indication in email sales initiations? For HSA-ASA combi- why customers are more or less likely to indicate their
nations, one widely used practice in email sales initiations initial interest. In general, online environments, such
is to introduce two collaborating sales agents (i.e., an as email communications, are considered to lack social
HSA and an ASA) to the customer in the email who are presence (e.g., Hassanein and Head 2007). Yet, adding
collectively in charge of addressing customer needs (e.g., digital social cues through text and pictures can convey
Exceed.ai 2019, Paschen et al. 2020), whereby the HSA social presence, which in turn, can lead to desirable cus-
is responsible for the customer communication and the tomer responses, such as trust and purchase intentions
ASA is responsible for the preparation and specification of (e.g., Cyr et al. 2009, Qiu and Benbasat 2009). We argue
the sales offer, which is presented to the customer in later that just such a digital social cue is the type of sales rep.
stages of the sales process (Online Appendix A presents As such, we hypothesize that customers perceive that
examples). As such, both sales agents assume complemen- social presence is largely missing in the presence of a
tary roles and thus, can signal advantages because of their “cold and cost-efficient” ASA but is more salient in the
specializations (i.e., HSA: hedonic value; ASA: utilitarian presence of a “warm and costly” HSA (e.g., Pavlou and
value) (e.g., Castelo et al. 2019, Longoni and Cian 2022). Gefen 2004, Dimoka et al. 2012). Consequently, custom-
We argue that customers are more likely to indicate ers are likely to feel a more personal connection when
their initial interest to an HSA-ASA (versus an ASA). they recognize the presence of an HSA (versus an
The indication of an HSA specializing in the task of ASA), and thus, they will be more willing to invest fur-
communication evokes the perception that the HSA ther time and effort in the interaction. This is in line
can fully focus on providing hedonic value, allowing with previous research indicating that customers are
Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s) 1155

more likely to accept recommendations for chocolate delivery tend to focus on utilitarian rather than he-
bars—a product related to hedonic consumption—the donic value, such as effort minimization, efficiency,
more the recommender (i.e., human, machine, or and performance, which ASAs (as opposed to HSAs) in
machine-human hybrid) is associated with assessing particular provide (e.g., Huang and Rust 2018, Castelo
and providing hedonic value (e.g., Longoni and Cian et al. 2019). Accordingly, customers in this stage tend to
2022). Thus, the more a sales rep evokes social presence, process information following a trade-off pattern in
Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

the more likely a customer is to indicate their initial favor of utilitarian rather than hedonic attributes. Liter-
interest. As such, customers should be more responsive ature on the phenomenon of automation (e.g., Daugh-
(i.e., indicate higher interest) to HSAs and HSA-ASA erty and Wilson 2018, Rai et al. 2019, Jain et al. 2021)
combinations than to ASAs alone because of their particularly indicates the operational benefits of ASAs
higher levels of social presence. over HSAs, which allow for vast efficiency gains for
customers and thus, for minimizing customer effort
Hypothesis 1 (c). Social presence mediates the effect of sales
and maximizing customer convenience and performance.
rep type on initial interest indications, such that a customer is
Moreover, ASAs are less likely to make careless mistakes
more likely to indicate their initial interest to an HSA rather
or have bad days in operational tasks than HSAs (e.g.,
than an ASA because of higher levels of social presence.
Huang and Rust 2018, Schanke et al. 2021). Therefore, we
Hypothesis 1 (d). Social presence mediates the effect of suggest that customers associate less utilitarian value
sales rep type on initial interest indications, such that a cus- with an HSA than an ASA when scheduling a sales
tomer is more likely to indicate their initial interest to an appointment meeting, thus being less likely to provide
HSA-ASA combination rather than an ASA because of contact information to an HSA (versus an ASA).
higher levels of social presence. This argument is in line with previous research (e.g.,
Castelo et al. 2019, Logg et al. 2019) indicating that algo-
3.2. The Effects of Sales Rep Type on Contact rithms (versus humans) performing analytical, objec-
Information Provision tive, and automatable tasks are not only superior in
After their initial interest has been indicated, in the efficiency and accuracy but are also often preferred in
follow-up email communication, customers usually these areas by users. In the same vein, users typically
need to decide whether they are willing to agree to turn to automated agents for their operations-oriented
schedule a sales appointment, resulting in the provision capabilities to increase efficiency and performance,
of their contact information to switch the communica- such as setting an alarm with Siri, turning on the lights
tion channel from emails to cue-richer communication with Alexa, or getting directions from Google. In sum,
(e.g., live chat or phone) with the same sales rep (e.g., we propose that customers are less likely to provide
Peppers and Rogers 2004, Woodpecker 2022). Such an contact information to an HSA (versus an ASA).
appointment for an initial sales meeting and related
Hypothesis 2 (a). Customers are less likely to provide
contact information provision are non-binding and
contact information in email sales initiations when encoun-
allow the customer both to get to know the sales rep
tering an HSA rather than an ASA.
better and to more easily scout possible sales offer spec-
ifications (e.g., Xiao and Benbasat 2007), all before the How do customers respond to an HSA-ASA combi-
customer engages in more serious negotiations and nation relative to an ASA alone in terms of contact
faces the binding decision on whether to complete the information provision? When comparing HSA-ASA
purchase. In this non-binding situation, customers may combinations with ASAs, we argue that a customer is
implicitly ask themselves, “Do I want to provide con- less likely to provide contact information when the
tact information to this sales rep?” The decision to sales rep is an HSA-ASA combination (versus an ASA
engage in the scheduling of a sales appointment builds alone). This is because customers at this decision stage
on the mental processing of utilitarian attributes that tend to prioritize utilitarian over hedonic attributes and
goes beyond the hedonic attributes perceived when thus, believe that HSA-ASAs (versus ASAs) are less
deciding to indicate their initial interest. Accordingly, adept in providing utilitarian value that is, for example,
at this subsequent decision stage, customers typically associated with scheduling a sales appointment. Even if
look for signals that justify the time and effort that must the HSA in the HSA-ASA combination is completely
be invested in making a sales appointment with a sales devoted to the task of customer communication and
rep (e.g., Bettman et al. 1998, Lambrecht et al. 2011). thus, to scheduling sales appointments, the HSA (ver-
We argue that customers have a lower likelihood of sus an ASA only) is still subject to various human flaws
providing their contact information to schedule a sales in performance, such as natural response delays, lim-
appointment when they encounter an HSA (versus an ited time resources, and proneness to mistakes (e.g., Huang
ASA). Our rationale is that customers faced with the organ- and Rust 2018, Schanke et al. 2021). Thus, having the HSA
izational and operational tasks of contact information specializing in the role of the communicator does not make
Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
1156 Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s)

the HSA-ASA combination more appealing than an ASA these differences in performance expectancy and effort
alone when scheduling a sales appointment. Based on the expectancy, customers are less likely to provide their
logic, we propose that a customer has a lower likelihood of contact information and thus, engage in the task of
providing contact information when encountering an scheduling a sales appointment meeting in the presence
HSA-ASA combination (relative to an ASA alone). of an HSA (versus an ASA).
This is in line with previous information systems
Hypothesis 2 (b). Customers are less likely to provide
Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

research demonstrating that performance expectancy


contact information in email sales initiations when encoun-
tering an HSA-ASA combination rather than an ASA. and effort expectancy are two of the strongest predic-
tors of users accepting technologies, particularly in util-
Why exactly should customers be less likely to pro- itarian contexts (e.g., Venkatesh et al. 2003, Cenfetelli
vide contact information when encountering an HSA et al. 2008). Relatedly, customers were found to be more
(and also, an HSA-ASA combination) rather than an likely to accept product recommendations in utilitarian-
ASA alone? As mentioned earlier, customers at this focused decisions the more the recommender (i.e., human,
later stage of the email sales initiation face a utilitarian- machine, or machine-human hybrid) was considered to be
focused decision and typically follow an information able to assess and provide utilitarian value (Longoni and
trade-off that prioritizes utilitarian over hedonic attr- Cian 2022). Thus, the more a sales rep evokes performance
ibutes to engage in more granular cost-benefit ass- expectancy and effort expectancy, the more likely a cus-
essments. Accordingly, any signal of efficiency and tomer is to provide contact information. Therefore, custom-
seamless goal achievement allows the customer to infer ers should be less responsive (i.e., at providing contact
some sense of performance maximization and effort information) to HSAs and HSA-ASA combinations than to
minimization, which increases customers’ likelihood of ASAs because of their lower levels of performance expect-
engaging in the cumbersome task of scheduling a sales ancy and effort expectancy.
appointment. Consequently, we expect that perform-
ance expectancy, a customer’s belief that the interaction Hypothesis 2 (c). Performance expectancy and effort
with the sales rep will help to attain gains in perform- expectancy mediate the effect of sales rep type on contact
ance (Venkatesh et al. 2003), and effort expectancy, a information provision such that a customer is less likely to
customer’s belief that the interaction with a sales rep is provide contact information to an HSA rather than an ASA
free of effort (Venkatesh et al. 2003), are two crucial because of lower levels of performance expectancy and lower
beliefs that explain why customers are more or less levels of effort expectancy.
likely to provide contact information when scheduling Hypothesis 2 (d). Performance expectancy and effort
a sales appointment. ASAs—particularly contemporary expectancy mediate the effect of sales rep type on contact
ones (e.g., Luo et al. 2019b, Schanke et al. 2021)—are information provision such that a customer is less likely to
known for their operational benefits over HSAs, which
provide contact information to an HSA-ASA combination
allow for vast efficiency gains for customers and thus,
rather than an ASA because of lower levels of performance
for maximizing performance (e.g., Daugherty and Wil-
expectancy and lower levels of effort expectancy.
son 2018, Rai et al. 2019, Jain et al. 2021). Accordingly,
ASAs executing operational tasks are not limited by
well-known human idiosyncrasies, such as making 4. Research Methodology
careless mistakes, having bad or sick days, and becom- We employed a multi-method approach (Mingers 2003,
ing frustrated or tired (e.g., Huang and Rust 2018). Venkatesh et al. 2013) and used the complementary
Moreover, ASAs are much more flexible and easier to properties of a randomized field experiment and a
use for operational routines, can better focus on the cus- follow-up randomized online experiment to test our
tomers’ organizational demands, and require less commu- hypotheses. The first study addressed the direct effect
nication with the customer to move on in the sales process. hypotheses (i.e., Hypotheses 1, (a) and (b) and 2, (a) and
For instance, whereas an ASA can make the sales appoint- (b)) and was a randomized field experiment (Study 1)
ment meeting at any time proposed by the customer and based on a sales initiation via email communication
may even talk to several customers at the same time, HSAs of a real company to examine the main effects of our
in charge of and even specialized in communication (i.e., research model in a setting with high ecological validity
in the HSA-ASA combination) are restricted to their and generalizability. Subsequently, we conducted a
schedule and thus, have limits (e.g., time and length of randomized online experiment (Study 2) to investigate
calls). Furthermore, interacting with ASAs does not the direct effect hypotheses in a setting with high inter-
require several exchanges and confirmations regarding nal validity. In addition, we aimed to explore the medi-
the scheduling of the sales appointment meeting. Addi- ating mechanisms underlying the effects of the sales
tionally, the emergence of missed phone calls by the rep type on the initial interest indication (Hypotheses 1,
sales rep and the potential necessity to reschedule a call (c) and (d)) and contact information provision (Hypoth-
are less likely with an ASA (versus an HSA). Given eses 2, (c) and (d)), respectively.
Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s) 1157

The remainder of this paper is structured as follows. Recently, Pharma tested deploying ASAs at various
In Section 5, we describe the research context and stages in their sales processes. The customer sales and
experimental design of the field experiment (Study 1), service centers were at the forefront of these business
followed by the experimental procedures and results. transformations, so Pharma could assign any type of
In Section 6, we present the refined experimental design sales rep (i.e., ASA, HSA, or HSA-ASA) to its email
and measured variables for the follow-up online experi- sales initiation stages. In collaboration with Pharma, we
designed and executed a randomized field experiment,
Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

ment (Study 2), concluding with the results. Finally, we


discuss the implications of the findings and draw con- unbeknownst to the participants under study (i.e., cus-
clusions in Section 7. tomers). The customers in our sample were observed
while making real-life decisions with serious conse-
5. Study 1: Randomized Field Experiment quences for both them and Pharma. Thus, the findings
5.1. Company Background and Context of this study are subject neither to the reporting biases
We partnered with a leading European pharmaceutical and demand effects inherent in survey and laboratory
company, Pharma,4 to conduct a randomized field ex- research nor to issues of self-selection, as we were able
periment. Pharma makes more than e10 billion in sales to observe subjects even when they did not respond to
every year, mainly through sales to other businesses; it the emails at all.
sells purification and monitoring technologies (among
other products) as well as related supplies and services 5.2. Experimental Design
To investigate our hypotheses, we randomly assigned
for laboratory applications to its customers. These cus-
customers to one of three experimental conditions in a
tomers are from different geographic regions as well as
between-subjects design, where each customer received
from the pharmaceutical and related technological sec-
emails from one of three different sales reps (i.e., ASA
tors. Thus, the optimal purification and monitoring
Only, HSA Only, and HSA-ASA). In the baseline condi-
technology and related supplies and services for each
tion, ASA Only, the sales rep was a single sales agent
customer highly depend on customer-specific require-
that represented an automated identity, whereas in the
ments (e.g., the chemicals used, daily purification
HSA Only condition, the sales rep was a single sales
volume needs, monitoring requirements, necessary certi-
agent who represented a human identity. In the HSA-
fications), so that sales reps usually help customers in
ASA condition, the sales rep consisted of a team of two
specifying and selecting the best sales offers to optimally
sales agents, where the HSA was presented as being in
fulfill the customers’ specific needs. charge of the customer communication and the ASA
As a result of our cooperation with Pharma, we was presented as being responsible for the sales offer
gained access to existing customers of the organization specifications.
that had acquired purification and monitoring technol- In the emails to the customers, the sales reps pre-
ogy at some point between 2006 and 2018. At least one sented identical sales offers (e.g., content, presentation)
year after the sale, these customers usually require and saliently indicated their identity several times (i.e.,
further and/or different supplies (e.g., chemicals) as in the introduction, farewell message, and signature).
well as services (e.g., repair and maintenance) for their Online Appendix A presents examples of the sent emails.
purification and monitoring technologies, presenting To rule out any confounding effects because of previous
additional sales opportunities for Pharma. These sales interactions between the sales reps and the customers, we
usually range between e5,000 and e20,000 without con- ascertained that no type of sales agent had previous direct
sidering other potential sales, including cross-selling and (e.g., email, call, meeting) or indirect touch points (e.g.,
after sales. However, the required supplies and services Twitter, LinkedIn) with the contacted customers and
rely on open standards and thus, can easily be specified that Pharma had no commercial contact with the cus-
and provided by competitors, hence not representing tomers since its sales of the purification and monitor-
safe sales for Pharma. Moreover, presenting such sales ing technology.
offers to customers usually resembles first-time vendor- To ascertain the validity of the conditions, we conducted
customer encounters (and not encounters in long-lasting two workshops and one pilot study with Pharma. Thus,
vendor-customer relationships) because (1) a long time we ensured that the procedure and sent emails in-
period (i.e., at least a year) has elapsed since the sale cluded content that was representative of the usual
of the purification and monitoring technology, usually Pharma sales emails. Moreover, each email contained
without any vendor-customer interaction in between, the same content, except for the features related to the
and (2) there are high fluctuations on both the sales agent type of sales rep. Consistent with previous research
and customer sides (e.g., job rotation, promotion, turn- (Senecal and Nantel 2004), we ran a pretest to ascertain
over). For more information on the legal and ethical that customers could identify the respective sales rep
situation of sales initiations via email communication type as intended and differentiate it from any other rele-
particularly in Europe, see Online Appendix I. vant sales rep type (see Online Appendix B). Consistent
Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
1158 Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s)

with previous research (Senecal and Nantel 2004), we also representatives not in charge anymore), 367 (46.7%) did
ran a pretest with participants from Amazon Mechanical not respond, and 325 (41.3%) responded. Of the 325
Turk to ascertain that customers could identify the re- who responded, 87 (26.8%) indicated an initial interest
spective sales rep as intended and that they could differ- in the sales offer, and 238 (73.2%) indicated that they
entiate it from any other relevant sales rep (see Online had no initial interest in the sales offer. Of the 87 who
Appendix B for details). Finally, we made sure that no indicated their initial interest, 54 (62.1%) provided
Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

effects were subject to time-related confounds by verify- their contact information. More information on the
ing that all emails were sent between 10 a.m. and 11 a.m. descriptive statistics and further analyses are pro-
during usual working hours, when most customers vided in Online Appendix C.
would be at their workplace.
5.4. Analysis and Results
5.3. Experimental Procedure and Sample 5.4.1. Model-Free Results. We first report the model-
Description free results based on the mean comparisons between
The experimental procedure comprised four steps, the three sales reps. Consistent with previous research
allowing us to directly examine how different sales on multiple decisions in sales processes (e.g., Huang
rep types influenced customer decisions in early sales et al. 2019), we calculated two (customer) conversion
initiations via email communication. rates as our dependent variables for (1) initial interest
1. Pharma sent each customer an email with a general indication (i.e., Conversion Rate 1) and (2) contact infor-
sales offer from one of our three sales reps. In this email, mation provision (i.e., Conversion Rate 2) (see Table 1 for
the sales rep requested that the customer respond by the definitions of our dependent variables).
stating whether he or she was interested in a personal Figure 3 shows the two conversion rates across the
sales offer. three sales rep types. HSA Only had a significantly
2. Each customer could indicate initial interest by higher Conversion Rate 1 than ASA Only, in that Conver-
answering the email directly or by clicking one of two sion Rate 1 was 63.6% higher when comparing HSA Only
implemented buttons (i.e., “request a sales offer” or with ASA Only (MHSA Only  0.36 versus MASA Only 
“not interested”) within the email. 0.22; F  4.66, p < 0.05). However, there was no sig-
3. Customers who expressed an initial interest re- nificant difference between HSA-ASA and ASA Only
ceived another email from the same sales rep type. This regarding Conversion Rate 1 (MHSA-ASA  0.22 versus
time, they were required to provide both their phone MASA Only  0.22; F  0.02, p > 0.1). Thus, we find initial
number and time slots when they would be available support for Hypothesis 1(a) but not for Hypothesis 1(b).
for a sales appointment meeting via phone with the Regarding Conversion Rate 2, HSA Only had a sig-
respective sales rep to specify the sales offer to meet the nificantly lower Conversion Rate 2 than ASA Only.
customers’ needs. Specifically, Conversion Rate 2 was 37.7% lower when
4. Each customer could provide the requested con- comparing HSA Only with ASA Only (MHSA Only  0.48
tact information by answering the email directly. versus MASA Only  0.77; F  5.45, p < 0.05). Yet, there was
At this point, our data-gathering process stopped. no significant difference between ASA Only and HSA-
We applied stratified random sampling to a pool of ASA regarding Conversion Rate 2 (MHSA-ASA  0.72 versus
Pharma’s customers, where we ensured that each con- MASA Only  0.77; F  0.16, p > 0.1). Thus, we find initial
dition had the same number of customers and similar support for Hypothesis 2(a) but not for Hypothesis 2(b).
characteristics regarding gender and education. We Online Appendix D provides more descriptive and stat-
recorded all variables via email responses. A total of istical details of the model-free comparisons.
786 customers from different geographic regions and
sectors in Germany were contacted. Of these, 94 (12.0%) 5.4.2. Main Effects. We proceed by applying econo-
were dead ends (i.e., mail delivery failures or customer metric models to control for the identified effects of

Table 1. Dependent Variable Conversion Rate Definitions


n
xk
We measured the conversion rate in the form of a point estimator, P: P(Conversion Rate)  k1
n

Conversion rate Definition

Conversion Rate 1 n denotes the total number of customers in the respective condition who responded to our first email
(excluding “dead ends” and “no responses”), and xk is a binary variable that equals 1 when the
customer indicated initial interest (i.e., “interest”) and 0 when not (i.e., “no interest”)
Conversion Rate 2 n denotes the total number of customers in the respective condition who indicated initial interest,
and xk is a binary variable that equals 1 when the customer provided the contact information
(i.e., “information”) and 0 when not responding or declining the request (i.e., “no information”)
Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s) 1159

Figure 3. (Color online) Conversion Rates (Study 1)


Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

sales rep types on the two conversions not being driven free results. Customers had a significantly higher Conver-
by other covariates (i.e., gender, education, type of sion Likelihood 1 relative to ASA Only when encountering
organization, and public equity). Because we have ran- HSA Only but not when the sales rep was an HSA-ASA,
domized field experimental data to identify direct ceteris paribus. As such, relative to encountering ASA
effects, our modeling analyses of conversion likelihood Only, customers were 94.6% more likely to indicate an
are straightforward. We develop three models (i.e., a initial interest when encountering HSA Only (βHSA Only
linear probability model (LPM), a probit model, and a  0.67, Exp(βHSA Only)  1.95, p < 0.05). Yet, customers
logit model) that are commonly employed when inves- were not significantly more or less likely to indicate
tigating the effects on binary outcome variables (e.g., their initial interest when encountering an HSA-ASA
Huang et al. 2019, Luo et al. 2019b). We use R’s general- (βHSA-ASA  −0.05, Exp(βHSA-ASA)  0.95, p > 0.1). Accord-
ized linear model function to implement and estimate ingly, the results further support Hypothesis 1(a) but not
the logistic regression models (R Core Team 2022). For Hypothesis 1(b).
our main model (the logit model), we also provide the Similar to the results presented above, for the contact
odds ratios for a better interpretation of the results. In information provision decision, the regression analyses
the logit model, the unobserved conversion likelihood support the model-free results. The analyses reveal that
is a logit function of the randomized conditions: customers had a significantly lower Conversion Likeli-
hood 2 when encountering HSA Only relative to ASA
Exp(Ui )
Conversion Likelihoodi  Only but not when encountering HSA-ASA, ceteris par-
Exp(Ui ) + 1 ibus. As such, relative to encountering ASA Only, the
Ui  α + β1 × (HSA Onlyi ) + β2 customers were 80.9% less likely to provide contact
× (HSA–ASAi ) + ΓControlsi + εi , information when encountering HSA Only (βHSA Only 
−1.65, Exp(βHSA Only)  0.19, p < 0.05). Yet, customers
where Ui denotes the latent utility of participant i to were not significantly more or less likely to provide
proceed to the next stage, and the dependent variable contact information when encountering HSA-ASA
of the conversion is whether customer i decided to pro- (βHSA-ASA  −0.44, Exp(βHSA-ASA)  0.65, p > 0.1). Thus,
ceed to the next stage. The key variables are the three the results further support Hypothesis 2(a) but not
conditions in the experiment, which are the two dummy Hypothesis 2(b).
variables (i.e., HSA Only and HSA-ASA), with ASA In Online Appendices D and E, we report supple-
Only as the comparison baseline. Thus, the three regres- mentary analyses regarding Study 1 that provide addi-
sion analyses (LPM, probit, and logit) presented in tional findings as well as further support the robustness
Table 2 present the effects of HSA Only and HSA-ASA of our main findings. More specifically, (1) we provide
to ASA Only, where Conversion Likelihood 1 refers to the analyses and interpretations for supplementary sales
initial interest indication and Conversion Likelihood 2 rep types (i.e., ASA-HSA and HSA-HSA) that were
refers to contact information provision. tested during the field experiment in addition to the
For the initial interest indication decision, the three main sales rep types; (2) we accounted for nonrespond-
regression analyses consistently corroborated the model- ing customers by treating nonresponding customers as
Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
1160 Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s)

Table 2. Customer Conversion Likelihoods (Study 1)

Conversion Likelihood 1 Conversion Likelihood 2


(initial interest indication) ASA Only as baseline (contact information provision) ASA Only as baseline

LPM Probit Logit LPM Probit Logit

Sales Rep Type


Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

HSA Only 0.14** 0.40** 0.67** −0.32** −1.02*** −1.65**


(0.06) (0.19) (0.31) (0.12) (0.38) (0.67)
HSA-ASA −0.01 −0.03 −0.05 −0.07 −0.30 −0.44
(0.06) (0.19) (0.33) (0.14) (0.43) (0.74)
Control variables Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes
Intercept 0.22*** −0.76*** −1.24*** 0.89*** 1.22** 2.00**
(0.04) (0.14) (0.24) (0.16) (0.50) (0.85)
N 325 325 325 87 87 87
Log likelihood — −185.20 −185.20 — −48.90 −48.93
Adjusted/ 0.02 0.03 0.03 0.13 0.25 0.25
Nagelkerke R2
Omnibus model 3.76** 370.39** 370.39** 3.06*** 97.79*** 97.85***
χ2/F test
Note. Standard errors are in parentheses.
**p < 0.05; ***p < 0.01.

not interested customers for Conversion Likelihood 1, 6.2. Experimental Procedures


revealing similar effect patterns; and (3) we used nested Following established guidelines for experimental de-
models, finding similar effects and further demonstrat- signs (e.g., Aguinis and Bradley 2014), we used a
ing the robustness of our results. scenario-based vignette for our randomized online ex-
periment to simulate realistic decision making. In this
6. Study 2: Randomized vein, we instructed participants to imagine that they
Online Experiment were going to receive a sales offer via email from a
6.1. Purpose and Methods well-known company that provides products and serv-
The randomized field experiment in Study 1 was ices for purification and monitoring technologies. Thus,
characterized by high ecological validity thanks to the we created a scenario in which participants had no pre-
real-life context in which contemporary ASAs are prac- vious contact and hence, no purchase history with not
tically applied. Yet, the Study was constrained in terms only the sales rep but also the vendor company. We
of internal validity, and we were unable to assess the randomly assigned participants to one of three condi-
underlying mechanisms that explain the direct effects tions that differed in terms of the sales rep type; these
of sales rep types on customers’ initial interest indica- were identical to the three conditions in Study 1 (i.e.,
tion and contact information provision. We intended to ASA, HSA, and HSA-ASA).
address these limitations in Study 2 by corroborating To collect sample data comparable with Study 1, we
the high external validity of the first Study within a con- reached out to a European market research firm and
trolled, randomized online experiment with high inter- recruited participants who worked in sales settings (i.e.,
nal validity. Additionally, we aimed to explore the key relationship management, negotiation, buying products
explanatory mechanisms underlying the direct effects and services). We incentivized participation in two sub-
of sales rep types in each of the two stages. sequent randomized online experiments about email
Against this backdrop, we designed Study 2 analo- sales initiations with e5 remuneration. Similar to the
gously to Study 1 regarding the experimental proce- timing in Study 1, we ensured that all links for the
dures, the design and timing of the outgoing emails, the experiment were sent out at about 10 a.m. for the first
manipulations of the sales rep types, and the dependent part of the experiment (i.e., first decision stage) and
variables. We created vignettes and related emails again at 10 a.m. the day after (i.e., second decision stage).
according to the characteristics and context of Study 1. In the experiment, participants went through an experi-
Study 2 mainly differs from Study 1 regarding some mental procedure similar to the one in Study 1, in that
aspects of the experimental procedures, the sample, participants made a decision about (1) whether they
and the measured variables (i.e., the mediators, con- would indicate their interest after the first email and (2)
trols, and attention checks). The following sections will whether they would provide contact information for
detail these differences. Online Appendix F provides scheduling a sales appointment meeting after the sec-
more details as well as supplementary analyses and ond email. In contrast to the experimental procedure in
robustness checks for the findings in Study 2. Study 1, however, after the participants provided their
Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s) 1161

answers, their participation did not end. Instead, we mediation analyses as well as Curiosity (Real et al. 2000,
instructed the participants to fill out a questionnaire, Lee and Chen 2011) and Moral Awareness (Reynolds
including the measures for our mediators, controls, and 2006) to rule out alternative explanations (e.g., see Online
attention checks. Moreover, for the second decision Appendix I for ethical issues on deploying ASAs in
stage, we also invited all participants who decided not Europe). We mainly used seven-point Likert-type scales
to indicate their interest in the offer in the first stage to ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (7).
Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

pretend that they had indicated their interest (e.g., Luo Finally, we included two attention checks.
et al. 2019a). The reason for including interest-indicating From a total of 514 participants who were assigned to
and noninterest-indicating participants in the second one of the three sales reps in the first decision stage, 455
stage was to analyze the hypothesized effects of sales (88.5%) participants also participated in the second deci-
rep type on contact information provision independ- sion stage. Of those 455, we removed 47 participants
ently from customers’ information processing, priori- (10.3%) who failed at least one of our attention checks.
ties, and thus, decisions in the first stage, thereby Thus, the final sample comprised 408 participants for
offering a more robust explanation for contact informa- the subsequent analyses. Online Appendix F provides
tion provision and allowing for a more formal quantifi- details on the descriptive statistics, the measurement
cation of the hypothesized effects of sales rep types in items, the constructs’ psychometric properties, and the
the second stage (e.g., Morgan and Winship 2015). results of a confirmatory factor analysis that demon-
strates the convergent and discriminant validity of our
6.3. Dependent Variables, Mediators, Controls, constructs. Online Appendix G demonstrates that com-
and Sample Description mon method bias did not confound our results.
For our dependent variables, Conversion Rate 1 (i.e., ini-
tial interest indication) and Conversion Rate 2 (i.e., con- 6.4. Results
tact information provision), we used the same binary 6.4.1. Model-Free Results. We first report the model-
variables as in Study 1, except that we asked partici- free results based on mean comparisons between the
pants to answer a binary question (yes or no) for each three sales rep types (see Figure 4). The results for Con-
decision instead of tracking real email interactions. We version Rate 1 indicate that HSA Only and HSA-ASA
also accounted for and measured sample characteristics had a significantly higher Conversion Rate 1 than ASA
and controls (i.e., Age, Gender, Education, Company Type, Only, respectively. Specifically, Conversion Rate 1 was
Technology Sector, Technology Use at Work, Computer 108.1% higher when comparing HSA Only with ASA
Programming Experience, and Prior Knowledge about Only (MHSA Only  0.38 versus MASA Only  0.18; F 
Automated Agents) to check for the robustness of our 14.99, p < 0.01). Likewise, HSA-ASA had an 83.3%
findings. Additionally, we used and adapted estab- higher Conversion Rate 1 than ASA Only (MHSA-ASA 
lished scales to measure Social Presence (Gefen and 0.33 versus MASA Only  0.18; F  8.67, p < 0.01). Thus, in
Straub 2003) and Performance Expectancy as well as Effort contrast to Study 1, we find initial support for Hypothe-
Expectancy (Venkatesh et al. 2003) to conduct the sis 1(a) and for Hypothesis 1(b).

Figure 4. (Color online) Conversion Rates (Study 2)


Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
1162 Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s)

Regarding Conversion Rate 2, HSA Only and HSA-ASA likely to provide contact information when encounter-
had a significantly lower Conversion Rate 2 than ASA ing HSA Only (βHSA Only  −0.93, Exp(βHSA Only)  0.40,
Only, respectively. Specifically, Conversion Rate 2 was p < 0.01) and 45.8% less likely when encountering HSA-
21.5% lower when comparing HSA Only with ASA Only ASA (βHSA-ASA  −0.61, Exp(βHSA-ASA)  0.54, p < 0.05).
(MHSA Only  0.62 versus MASA Only  0.79; F  9.78, p < As such, we find support for Hypotheses 2(a) and (b).
0.01). Likewise, HSA-ASA had a 15.2% lower Conversion
Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

Rate 2 than ASA Only (MHSA-ASA  0.67 versus MASA Only 6.4.3. Mediation Analyses. As elaborated in our theo-
 0.79; F  5.28, p < 0.05). Thus and in contrast to Study retical background and hypotheses development, cus-
1, we find support for Hypothesis 2(a) and for Hypothe- tomers face different decisions in the email sales initiation
sis 2(b). See Online Appendix H for more descriptive stages and thus, make different hedonic-utilitarian trade-
and statistical details of the model-free comparisons. offs that drive the respective decision outcome. Whereas
customers tend to focus on hedonic attributes to assess
6.4.2. Main Effects. We proceed to conduct a set of the hedonic value for the decision on their initial interest
regression analyses (LPM, probit, and logit) similar to indication, customers care rather more about utilitarian
those in Study 1 to ensure that the model-free findings attributes to assess the utilitarian value for the decision
of the effects of sales rep types are not driven by covari- on contact information provision. Specifically, we hy-
ates. Table 3 presents the details of the results. pothesized that social presence (SP) would mediate the
For the decision on the initial interest indication, the effects of sales rep types on initial interest indication (Hy-
regression analyses consistently corroborate the model- potheses 1(c) and (d)) and that performance expectancy
free results and reveal that customers had a signifi- (PE) and effort expectancy (EE) would mediate the effects
cantly higher Conversion Likelihood 1 relative to ASA of sales rep types on contact information provision (Hy-
Only when encountering HSA Only or HSA-ASA, cete- potheses 2(c) and (d)).
ris paribus. Specifically, in contrast to encountering To test our mediation hypotheses, we performed
ASA Only, customers were 194.8% more likely to indi- mediation analyses using the bootstrapping mediation
cate an initial interest when encountering HSA Only technique with 10,000 samples and 95% confidence
(βHSA Only  1.08, Exp(βHSA Only)  2.95, p < 0.01) and intervals (95% CIs) based on PROCESS (Hayes 2022,
143.3% more likely when encountering HSA-ASA model 4). We hereby used ASA Only as the baseline.
(βHSA-ASA  0.89, Exp(βHSA-ASA)  2.43, p < 0.01). Thus, We first entered HSA Only and HSA-ASA as the inde-
the results further support Hypotheses 1(a) and (b). pendent variables and Conversion Likelihood 1 (Conver-
For the decision on contact information provis- sion Likelihood 2) as the dependent variable along with
ion, the regression analyses consistently support the the control variables; this is similar to our logit models
model-free results. Customers had a significantly lower in Table 3. We then added Social Presence, Performance
Conversion Likelihood 2 when encountering HSA Only or Expectancy, and Effort Expectancy as mediators as well as
HSA-ASA, ceteris paribus. In particular, in contrast to Curiosity and Moral Awareness as covariates. The results
encountering ASA Only, the customers were 60.5% less in Table 4 reveal that the direct effect of both HSA Only

Table 3. Customer Conversion Likelihoods (Study 2)

Conversion Likelihood 1 Conversion Likelihood 2


(initial interest indication) ASA Only as baseline (contact information provision) ASA Only as baseline

LPM Probit Logit LPM Probit Logit

Sales Rep Type


HSA Only 0.21*** 0.64*** 1.08*** −0.19*** −0.55*** −0.93***
(0.05) (0.17) (0.29) (0.06) (0.17) (0.28)
HSA-ASA 0.16*** 0.52*** 0.89*** −0.12** −0.36** −0.61**
(0.05) (0.16) (0.28) (0.05) (0.16) (0.28)
Control variables Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes
Intercept 0.16 −0.98*** −1.65*** 0.78*** 0.78** 1.30**
(0.12) (0.38) (0.64) (0.13) (0.37) (0.63)
N 408 408 408 408 408 408
Log likelihood — −231.05 −231.09 — −236.83 −236.73
Adjusted/ 0.03 0.09 0.09 0.02 0.07 0.07
Nagelkerke R2
Omnibus model 1.95** 471.57** 471.65** 2.29** 465.26** 465.40**
χ2/F test
Note. Standard errors are in parentheses.
*p < 0.1; **p < 0.05; ***p < 0.01.
Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s) 1163

and HSA-ASA on Conversion Likelihood 1 is significantly The results are supported even when accounting for
mediated via Social Presence (HSA Only: indirect effect Curiosity and Moral Awareness in the mediation models,
SP  0.27; 95% CI  [0.10, 0.58]; HSA-ASA: indirect eff- helping to extend the generalizability of our findings
ect SP  0.27; 95% CI  [0.11, 0.54]), whereas Performance and provide additional support for Social Presence, Per-
Expectancy and Effort Expectancy do not significantly formance Expectancy, and Effort Expectancy as crucial
mediate the direct effects (HSA Only: indirect effect effect mechanisms in the respective decision stages.
Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

PE  −0.06; 95% CI  [−0.23, 0.02]; indirect effect EE  Moreover, the significant direct effects of sales rep types
0.07; 95% CI  [−0.02, 0.24]; HSA-ASA: indirect effect in the base models become insignificant in the media-
PE  −0.07; 95% CI  [−0.24, 0.03]; indirect effect EE  tion models, indicating that no crucial mediator has
−0.05; 95% CI  [−0.05, 0.19]). Moreover, we find that been omitted in the analyses (e.g., Zhao et al. 2010).
the direct effect of HSA Only and HSA-ASA on Con- In Online Appendices H and I, we report a variety of
version Likelihood 2 is significantly mediated via Per- supplementary analyses regarding Study 2 that pro-
formance Expectancy and Effort Expectancy (HSA Only: vide additional insights and corroborate our main find-
indirect effect PE  −0.47; 95% CI  [−1.07, −0.16]; indi- ings. In particular, (1) we provide analyses and results
rect effect EE  −0.43; 95% CI  [−1.01, −0.07]; HSA- of the effects of supplementary sales rep types (i.e.,
ASA: indirect effect PE  −0.26; 95% CI  [−0.67, −0.02]; ASA-HSA and HSA-HSA), (2) we used nested models
indirect effect EE  −0.35; 95% CI  [−0.83, −0.03]) but to further demonstrate the robustness of our results,
not via Social Presence (HSA Only: indirect effect SP  and (3) we present analyses and a discussion of ethical
−0.21; 95% CI  [−0.59, 0.03]; HSA-ASA: indirect effect and legal applications of sales rep types that do not dis-
SP  −0.27; 95% CI  [−0.73, 0.03]). Overall, these close their identity.
results support our mediation hypotheses (Hypotheses
1(c) and (d), and 2(c) and (d)), in that they demonstrate 7. Discussion
both positive indirect effects of HSA Only and HSA- How and why do customers respond to different sales
ASA (versus ASA Only) on Conversion Likelihood 1 via rep types (i.e., ASAs, HSAs, and HSA-ASAs) across
Social Presence and negative indirect effects of HSA Only sales stages? Our paper offers notable findings that sup-
and HSA-ASA (versus ASA Only) on Conversion Likeli- port our assertion that sales rep types have differential
hood 2 via Performance Expectancy and Effort Expectancy. effects on customer decisions across consecutive sales

Table 4. Mediation Analyses (Study 2)

Notes. N  408. Effects of Curiosity and Moral Awareness. Decision 1: Curiosity: 0.54 (0.15)***; 95% CI  [0.25, 0.83]; Moral Awareness: 0.20 (0.14) n.s.;
95% CI  [−0.08, 0.47]. Decision 2: Curiosity: 0.55 (0.18)**; 95% CI  [0.21, 0.90]; Moral Awareness: −0.42 (0.15)**; 95% CI  [−0.72, −0.12]. The
analyses are based on bootstrapping with 10,000 samples and 95% CIs (Hayes 2022, model 4), taking ASA Only as baseline. ΔR2 denotes the
increase in explanatory power of each of the respective models when including the added terms. All coefficients are displayed on a log-odds
metric to account for the binary dependent variables.
n.s., not significant; **p < 0.05; ***p < 0.01
Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
1164 Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s)

stages. Specifically, in two studies about email sales ini- initial interest to HSAs versus ASAs because of HSAs’
tiations, we consistently find support that (1) customers higher levels of social presence, whereas they are less
are about 95% (Study 1) and 195% (Study 2) more likely likely to provide contact information because of HSAs’
to indicate their initial interest to HSAs (versus ASAs), lower levels of performance expectancy and effort ex-
whereas (2) they are about 81% (Study 1) and 60% pectancy. Explicitly revealing these hitherto missing
(Study 2) less likely to provide contact information to explanatory mechanisms is important for understand-
Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

HSAs (versus ASAs). We reveal that customers are ing why sales rep types may operate in a more or less
more likely to indicate their initial interest to HSAs effective manner across sales stages.
(versus ASAs) because of HSAs’ higher levels of social Third, we go beyond the study of customer accept-
presence, yet they are less likely to subsequently provide ance of ASAs in comparison with HSAs (e.g., Luo et al.
contact information because of lower levels of perform- 2019b) and in isolation from other sales rep types (e.g.,
ance expectancy and effort expectancy associated with Li and Karahanna 2015). To date, there has been little
HSAs (versus ASAs). Although we find that the shifting research on the “missing middle,” where HSAs and
effect patterns for HSA-ASAs (versus ASAs) across ASAs are deployed together rather than alone (e.g.,
the email sales initiation stages resemble those of HSAs Daugherty and Wilson 2018). Against this backdrop,
(versus ASAs) in the online experiment, the results in we shed light on the effects of HSA-ASAs, where both
the field experiment diverged. More precisely, HSA- agents specialize in complementary activities that each
ASAs and ASAs exhibited no different effects on initial one is considered to do best. The findings of our two
interest indications and contact information provision. studies reveal that we cannot unequivocally conclude
Overall, these findings significantly advance the field of whether customers respond differently to HSA-ASAs
study on customer responses to human as opposed to versus ASAs. We surmise that these diverging results
computerized sales agents. can be explained by contextual features in the field set-
ting (e.g., Hong et al. 2014). Specifically, in conversa-
7.1. Contributions to Research and Practice tions with customers in the field, we learned that they
Our article extends the research in three important paid only limited attention to the profound recognition
ways. First, we advance prior work by suggesting that and assessment of the HSA-ASA because of the con-
customer responses to HSAs versus ASAs can shift straints of their daily work environments (e.g., lack of
across sales stages. Previous research on customer accep- time and cognitive resources to process the subtleties of
tance of HSAs versus ASAs embraced a fairly mono- their emails). As such, customers tended to focus on the
lithic view of sales processes, implicitly assuming that more outstanding and salient sales agent in the HSA-
the effects of sales rep types on decision outcomes are ASA (i.e., the ASA), which probably overshadowed the
uniform and stable for the entire sales process (e.g., Cas- nuances of the combination. This observation is in line
telo et al. 2019, Longoni and Cian 2022). Yet, our find- with previous research on hedonic-utilitarian decision
ings show that customer responses to sales rep types making in that the saliency of attributes can influence
cannot simply be extrapolated from one stage to an- the trade-off between hedonic and utilitarian attributes
other. In particular, we demonstrate whether and how (e.g., Dhar and Wertenbroch 2000). The dependence on
customer responses to HSAs versus ASAs can shift and such salient attributes demonstrates that although pro-
even reverse across subsequent stages. Our study thus mising HSA-ASA combinations (such as those in which
paints a more nuanced picture of customer responses to HSAs and ASAs assume distinct yet complementary
sales rep types across sales stages. roles) can be viable options for single ASAs, customer
Second and relatedly, we advance our understanding responses to HSA-ASA combinations (versus ASAs) are
of the effect mechanisms that explain customers’ shift- not simple and context-agnostic. Instead, contextual fea-
ing responses to HSAs and ASAs across sales stages. tures of the sales setting can crucially affect the customer
Whereas previous studies have extensively investiga- responses to HSA-ASA combinations in each sales stage,
ted the direct effects of different sales agent types (e.g., and eventually, the response shifts across stages. Expos-
Castelo et al. 2019, Longoni and Cian 2022), the ques- ing such diverging customer responses and thus, vary-
tion of why these agents affect decision outcomes ing response shifts across studies represent a useful
across sales stages has remained largely unanswered. contribution because it serves to identify potential boun-
By proposing shifting customer priorities in the consid- dary conditions (the when) of our theorizing.
eration of sales reps’ hedonic and utilitarian attributes Our insights also provide valuable practical lessons
across sales stages, we provide a window into the for vendor companies that consider presenting ASAs in
mechanisms that explain why the effects of sales rep several sales stages to improve customer responses and
types can differ across stages. More specifically, we ultimately, increase sales. Specifically, the findings on
reveal that customers are more likely to indicate their how and why the effects of sales rep types persist or
Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s) 1165

change across sales stages are important because with- versus HSAs across early decision stages—can gener-
out proper forethought, vendor companies may assume alize to lower-stakes sales settings with less complex
that customers will consistently appreciate ASAs in sit- processes and products, future research should exam-
uations where the exact opposite is true. The findings of ine the boundary conditions of our findings across dif-
our high-stakes field experiment are particularly useful ferent processes and product settings. More research is
for vendor companies because they build on real cus- also required to test the generalizability of our findings
to different domains (e.g., finance, e-government, and
Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

tomer responses. Our insights may also extend to other


sales stages and contexts where ASAs and HSA-ASA healthcare) and cultural and geographic contexts (e.g.,
combinations are increasingly emerging and consecu- the United States and China). Similarly, although our
tive customer decisions are most common, such as in research focused on sales initiation via emails, a chan-
credit approval or online investment processes (e.g., nel with growing relevance and broad applicability, we
robo-advisory services). encourage future researchers to examine the effects of
Our research demonstrates that employing ASAs— sales rep types on customer responses via alternative
either separately or jointly with HSAs—can be more or sales channels (e.g., chat or social media). Future re-
less beneficial in sales processes depending on the spe- search is also warranted to explore other sales-setting
cific stage of customer decision making in which they are specificities, such as sales initiator (e.g., vendor-initiated
deployed. In this vein, vendor companies need to con- versus customer-initiated), artifact design (e.g., text- ver-
sider not only that different sales rep types bring distinct sus voice-based ASAs), and conversation content (e.g.,
attributes to the table but also that customers’ apprecia- sentiment, topic, communication style).
tion of these attributes shifts across the stages of the sales Second, although we found that customers responded
process. Specifically, customers are more likely to indi- differently to HSA-ASAs than to ASAs in the online
cate their initial interest when encountering an HSA, experiment, we did not find these differential effects in
which emphasizes customers’ preference for rather per- the field experiment. As conjectured, the results in the
sonal human-human relationships and thus, for hedonic field suggest that customers are primarily affected by the
value during the initial sales contact. Yet, once customers contextual features in the field and the mere presence of
are asked to provide their contact information to sched- an ASA in the HSA-ASA combination, while the nuances
ule a business appointment, they seem to prioritize utili- of the combination were largely ignored. We encourage
tarian value regarding performance and effort when future research to investigate whether and how boundary
communicating with ASAs. Overall, our findings inform conditions influence customer responses in each stage as
vendor companies about customer responses to ASAs, as well as the resulting emergence and direction of response
one and the same ASA can either help or hurt customer shifts across sales stages. Moreover, it will be interesting
responses depending on what stage customers have to investigate whether and how customer responses
reached in the sales process and what their hedonic- to ASAs and HSA-ASA combinations will change once
utilitarian attribute trade-offs are. Given these differ- ASAs have further developed their capabilities and/or cus-
ential customer responses, vendor companies are well tomers have tested, experienced, and acquainted them-
advised to consider whether they need to emphasize selves more with ASAs. In the same vein, ethical concerns
hedonic value (i.e., social presence) or utilitarian value will increase in relevance, such as the truthful disclosure
(i.e., performance and effort) when deploying different of the sales rep identity (see Online Appendix I for an
types of sales reps in their sales processes. extended discussion). Taken together, we highlight the
importance of conducting additional field studies, such
7.2. Strengths, Limitations, and Directions for as those based on longitudinal and qualitative research
Future Research designs, to enhance our understanding of the reasons for
The present manuscript has various strengths, including customer responses to ASAs and HSA-ASA combinations.
the complementary features of a high-stakes randomized Third, we mainly focused on two salient consecutive
field experiment and a controlled online experiment, in- decisions in the early stages of email sales processes,
sights from objective customer behaviors, and the investi- namely the initial interest indication and contact informa-
gation of two highly consequential but under-researched tion provision, which are highly consequential for later
consecutive customer decisions and their underlying me- stages of the sales process. Future studies could explore
chanisms. Yet, like any paper, our research has limitations customer decisions after the email sales initiation (e.g.,
that provide fertile ground for future research. evaluation and purchase decisions) and beyond the sales
First, we conducted our research in a sales setting closure (e.g., customer complaints and product returns).
with high transaction volumes and quite complex Accordingly, researchers could investigate other depen-
products. Although we believe that the core patterns dent variables (e.g., brand perceptions, revisits, and
of findings—shifting customer responses to ASAs repurchases), mediators (i.e., customer engagement
Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
1166 Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s)

and behavioral uncertainty), and moderators (e.g., cus- Athene Young Investigator program at Technical University
tomer traits, such as social anxiety or need for control) of Darmstadt.
that may be more pertinent in other decision stages and
product settings. In this vein, future research may also Endnotes
1
investigate how to shape the hedonic and utilitarian Consistent with previous information systems research on recom-
attributes of the sales rep type to improve customer mendation agents (e.g., Xiao and Benbasat 2007, Qiu and Benbasat
2009, Li and Karahanna 2015), we use the term sales agent to refer to a
Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

responses, such as emphasizing social cues (e.g., an- human-like (either a human or technology-based) entity that aims to ini-
thropomorphic features or avatar representations) to tiate and close sales in addition to providing product recommendations.
increase customers’ perceptions of social presence in 2
Although vendor companies usually demarcate the interaction
hedonic-focused decisions or utilitarian cues (e.g., fast with customers across two email exchanges to not overwhelm cus-
delivery speed or high service quality) to evoke per- tomers with too much information in a single email and lose cus-
formance and effort expectancies in utilitarian-focused tomer interest by writing too many emails before moving to a more
personal communication channel (e.g., Peppers and Rogers 2004, Simon
decisions. Moreover, it would be interesting to see 2015, Woodpecker 2022), the actual number of email exchanges and
whether it is beneficial or detrimental for vendor com- related salient customer decision stages and decisions can vary depend-
panies to keep the sales rep type consistent throughout ing on, for example, the content of the sales offer, the number of follow-
the email sales initiation stages (which was the focus of ups by the sales agent, and specific customer attributes (e.g., Bhargave
et al. 2015, Zhang et al. 2017).
our paper) or to dynamically alternate between sales
3
rep types across stages. In this way, vendor companies Please note that in addition to the three main sales rep types (i.e.,
ASA, HSA, and HSA-ASA), we explored three supplementary sales
may explore what factors customers value most when rep types (i.e., ASA-HSA, HSA-HSA, and no identity). With these
making their respective decisions and thus, determine additional sales rep types, we account for specificities that result from
the best sales rep types for each stage, adhering to the the differences in the order and assumed tasks of each sales agent in
principle of “deploying the right sales rep at the right time” possible HSA-ASA combinations, for the number of sales agents oper-
ating as sales reps (i.e., one versus two sales agents), and for the dis-
to best meet customer needs. In this regard, more in-depth
play of the identity overall (i.e., disclosure of sales rep identity versus
research is also warranted to investigate differences in nondisclosure of sales rep identity). To keep the presentation of our
customer responses to varying combinations of HSAs research succinct and to ascertain adequate theoretical depth and dis-
and ASAs (e.g., in complementary or substitutive peer cussion of our findings, we focus on customer responses to the three
roles and in subordinate or superordinate roles). Finally, main sales rep types in the main manuscript and provide our insights
into customer responses to the other sales rep types in Online Appen-
researchers may go beyond the sales context, as it is likely
dices A–I.
that responses shift across stages in entirely different 4
We used pseudonyms for the names of the vendor company and
decision contexts, such as responses of employees to its sales agents because they wished to remain anonymous.
human versus automated managers in organizational
settings (e.g., Benlian et al. 2022) or patients responding
to human versus automated physicians/assistants in References
medical care (e.g., Longoni et al. 2019). 7Targets (2022) What AI assistants can do? Retrieved February 10,
2022, https://7targets.ai/ai-sales-assistant.html.
In conclusion, we believe that there is much to learn Aguinis H, Bradley KJ (2014) Best practice recommendations for
about the applicability of human and/or automated designing and implementing experimental vignette methodol-
agents in sales processes and beyond, a topic that is ogy studies. Organ. Res. Methods. 17(4):351–371.
being increasingly discussed across industries and Benbya H, Pachidi S, Jarvenpaa S (2021) Special issue editorial: Arti-
geographies. We have only scratched the surface of ficial intelligence in organizations: Implications for information
systems research. J. Assoc. Inform. Systems, 10.17705/1jais.00662.
the myriad challenges and opportunities faced by cus- Benlian A, Wiener M, Cram WA, Krasnova H, Maedche A, Möhlmann
tomers and vendors. More collaborative work between M, Recker J, Remus U (2022) Algorithmic management. Bus.
academics and practitioners is needed to help decision Inform. Systems Engrg., ePub ahead of print August 1, https://doi.
makers leverage and extend our insights. We offer this org/10.1007/s12599-022-00764-w.
Bettman JR, Luce MF, Payne JW (1998) Constructive consumer
work as an important step toward building the foundation
choice processes. J. Consumer Res. 25(3):187–217.
for future research and practice in this arena. Bhargave R, Chakravarti A, Guha A (2015) Two-stage decisions
increase preference for hedonic options. Organ. Behav. Human
Acknowledgments Decision Processes 130:123–135.
The authors thank the senior editor, the associate editor, and Bleier A, Eisenbeiss M (2015) Personalized online advertising effec-
three anonymous reviewers for a most constructive and devel- tiveness: The interplay of what, when, and where. Marketing
Sci. 34(5):669–688.
opmental review process. Moreover, the authors gratefully
Botti S, McGill AL (2011) The locus of choice: Personal causality and
acknowledge the funding support by the German Research
satisfaction with hedonic and utilitarian decisions. J. Consumer
Foundation (DFG) as part of the project “Accountable Artifi- Res. 37(6):1065–1078.
cial Intelligence-based Systems: A Multi-Perspective Analysis” Burton JW, Stein M-K, Jensen TB (2019) A systematic review of
(project number 471168026) and the Center for Responsible algorithm aversion in augmented decision making. J. Behav.
Digitality (ZEVEDI). Open Access funding enabled by the Decision Making 33(2):220–239.
Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s) 1167

Castelo N, Bos MW, Lehmann DR (2019) Task-dependent algorithm Komiak SY, Benbasat I (2006) The effects of personalization and
aversion. J. Marketing Res. 56(5):809–825. familiarity on trust and adoption of recommendation agents.
Cenfetelli RT, Benbasat I, Al-Natour S (2008) Addressing the what MIS Quart. 30(4):941–960.
and how of online services: Positioning supporting-services Lambrecht A, Tucker C (2013) When does retargeting work? Informa-
functionality and service quality for business-to-consumer suc- tion specificity in online advertising. J. Marketing Res. 50(5):561–576.
cess. Inform. Systems Res. 19(2):161–181. Lambrecht A, Seim K, Tucker C (2011) Stuck in the adoption funnel:
Choudhury V, Karahanna E (2008) The relative advantage of electronic The effect of interruptions in the adoption process on usage.
channels: A multidimensional view. MIS Quart. 32(1):179–200. Marketing Sci. 30(2):355–367.
Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

Conversica (2022) Conversica sales AI assistants will engage 100% Lee Y, Chen AN (2011) Usability design and psychological ownership
of your leads. Retrieved March 2022, https://www.conversica. of a virtual world. J. Management Inform. Systems 28(3):269–308.
com/ai-assistants/sales-marketing/. Leung E, Paolacci G, Puntoni S (2018) Man vs. machine: Resisting
Cyr D, Head M, Larios H, Pan B (2009) Exploring human images in automation in identity-based consumer behavior. J. Marketing
website design: A multi-method approach. MIS Quart. 33(3): Res. 55(6):818–831.
539–566. Li SS, Karahanna E (2015) Online recommendation systems in a
Daugherty PR, Wilson HJ (2018) Human + Machine: Reimagining B2C e-commerce context: A review and future directions. J.
Work in the Age of AI (Harvard Business Press, Boston). Assoc. Inform. Systems 16(2):72–107.
De Wulf K, Odekerken-Schröder G, Iacobucci D (2001) Investments Liu BQ, Goodhue DL (2012) Two worlds of trust for potential
in consumer relationships: A cross-country and cross-industry e-commerce users: Humans as cognitive misers. Inform. Systems
exploration. J. Marketing 65(4):33–50. Res. 23(4):1246–1262.
Dhar R, Wertenbroch K (2000) Consumer choice between hedonic Logg JM, Minson JA, Moore DA (2019) Algorithm appreciation:
and utilitarian goods. J. Marketing Res. 37(1):60–71. People prefer algorithmic to human judgment. Organ. Behav.
Dietvorst BJ, Simmons JP, Massey C (2015) Algorithm aversion: Peo- Human Decision Processes 151:90–103.
ple erroneously avoid algorithms after seeing them err. J. Longoni C, Cian L (2022) Artificial intelligence in utilitarian vs.
Experiment. Psych. General 144(1):114–126. hedonic contexts: The “word-of-machine” effect. J. Marketing
Dietvorst BJ, Simmons JP, Massey C (2018) Overcoming algorithm 86(1):91–108.
aversion: People will use imperfect algorithms if they can (even Longoni C, Bonezzi A, Morewedge CK (2019) Resistance to medical
slightly) modify them. Management Sci. 64(3):1155–1170. artificial intelligence. J. Consumer Res. 46(4):629–650.
Dimoka A, Hong Y, Pavlou PA (2012) On product uncertainty in Luo X, Lu X, Li J (2019a) When and how to leverage e-commerce
online markets: Theory and evidence. MIS Quart. 36(2):395–426. cart targeting: The relative and moderated effects of scarcity
Econsultancy (2019) 2019 email marketing industry census. Retrieved and price incentives with a two-stage field experiment and
May 10, 2020, https://content.adestra.com/hubfs/2019_Reports/ causal forest optimization. Inform. Systems Res. 30(4):1203–1227.
2019%20Email%20Marketing%20Census.pdf. Luo X, Qin MS, Fang Z, Qu Z (2021) Artificial intelligence coaches
Exceed.ai (2019) Hand in hand: The power AI-driven autonomous for sales agents: Caveats and solutions. J. Marketing 85(2):14–32.
reps bring to your human sales team. Retrieved March 10, Luo X, Tong S, Fang Z, Qu Z (2019b) Frontiers: Machines vs.
2022, https://exceed.ai/blog/hand-in-hand-the-power-ai-driven- humans: The impact of artificial intelligence chatbot disclosure
autonomous-reps-bring-to-your-human-sales-team/. on customer purchases. Marketing Sci. 38(6):937–947.
Fügener A, Grahl J, Gupta A, Ketter W (2022) Cognitive challenges in McLeay F, Osburg VS, Yoganathan V, Patterson A (2021) Replaced
human–artificial intelligence collaboration: Investigating the path by a robot: Service implications in the age of the machine. J.
toward productive delegation. Inform. Systems Res. 33(2):678–696. Service Res. 24(1):104–121.
Gartner (2017) Gartner top strategic predictions for 2018 and Mende M, Scott ML, van Doorn J, Grewal D, Shanks I (2019) Service
beyond. Retrieved May 10, 2021, https://www.gartner.com/ robots rising: How humanoid robots influence service experien-
smarterwithgartner/gartner-top-strategic-predictions-for-2018- ces and elicit compensatory consumer responses. J. Marketing
and-beyond/. Res. 56(4):535–556.
Gefen D, Straub D (2003) Managing user trust in B2C e-services. Mingers J (2003) The paucity of multi-method research: A review of the
e-Service J. 2(2):7–24. information systems literature. Inform. Systems J. 13(3):233–249.
Hassanein K, Head M (2007) Manipulating perceived social pres- Morgan SL, Winship C (2015) Counterfactuals and Causal Inference
ence through the web interface and its impact on attitude toward (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, United Kingdom).
online shopping. Internat. J. Human-Comput. Stud. 65(8):689–708. Newell A, Simon HA (1972) Human Problem Solving, vol. 104 (Pren-
Hayes AF (2022) Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional tice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ).
Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach, 3rd ed. (Guilford Paschen J, Wilson M, Ferreira JJ (2020) Collaborative intelligence:
Publications, New York). How human and artificial intelligence create value along the
Hong W, Chan FK, Thong JY, Chasalow LC, Dhillon G (2014) B2B sales funnel. Bus. Horizons 63(3):403–414.
A framework and guidelines for context-specific theorizing in Pavlou PA, Gefen D (2004) Building effective online marketplaces
information systems research. Inform. Systems Res. 25(1):111–136. with institution-based trust. Inform. Systems Res. 15(1):37–59.
Huang M-H, Rust RT (2018) Artificial intelligence in service. J. Serv- Pega (2022) Pega email bot. Retrieved February 10, 2022, https://
ice Res. 21(2):155–172. www.pega.com/products/pega-platform/email-bot.
Huang N, Sun T, Chen P, Golden JM (2019) Word-of-mouth system Peppers D, Rogers M (2004) Managing Customer Relationships: A Stra-
implementation and customer conversion: A randomized field tegic Framework (John Wiley & Sons, Hoboken, NJ).
experiment. Inform. Systems Res. 30(3):805–818. Puntoni S, Reczek RW, Giesler M, Botti S (2021) Consumers and
Jain H, Padmanabhan B, Pavlou PA, Raghu T (2021) Editorial for artificial intelligence: An experiential perspective. J. Marketing
the special section on humans, algorithms, and augmented 85(1):131–151.
intelligence: The future of work, organizations, and society. Qiu L, Benbasat I (2009) Evaluating anthropomorphic product recom-
Inform. Systems Res. 32(3):675–687. mendation agents: A social relationship perspective to designing
Khan U, Dhar R, Wertenbroch K (2005) A behavioral decision information systems. J. Management Inform. Systems 25(4):145–182.
theory perspective on hedonic and utilitarian choice. Ratnesh- R Core Team (2022) R: A language and environment for statistical
war S, Mick DG, eds. Inside Consumption: Consumer Motives, computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved
Goals, and Desires (Routledge, London), 166–187. February 10, 2022, https://www.r-project.org/.
Adam, Roethke, and Benlian: Customer Responses to Human vs. Automated Sales Agents
1168 Information Systems Research, 2023, vol. 34, no. 3, pp. 1148–1168, © 2022 The Author(s)

Rai A, Constantinides P, Sarker S (2019) Editor’s comments: Next- Tan X, Wang Y, Tan Y (2019) Impact of live chat on purchase in
generation digital platforms: Toward human–AI hybrids. MIS electronic markets: The moderating role of information cues.
Quart. 43(1):iii–ix. Inform. Systems Res. 30(4):1248–1271.
Real E, Arce C, Sabucedo JM (2000) Classification of landscapes using The Relevancy Group (2017) The Return of Email Personalization.
quantitative and categorical data, and prediction of their scenic Retrieved May 10, 2020, https://cdn2.hubspot.net/hubfs/
beauty in north-western Spain. J. Environ. Psych. 20(4):355–373. 4835225/_OneSpot/Resources/White-Papers-and-Strategy-Guides/
Reynolds SJ (2006) Moral awareness and ethical predispositions: OneSpot_White_Paper_The-Return-On-Email-Personalization.pdf.
Investigating the role of individual differences in the recogni- Venkatesh V, Brown SA, Bala H (2013) Bridging the qualitative-
Downloaded from informs.org by [2a09:8044:418b::6] on 27 March 2024, at 12:39 . For personal use only, all rights reserved.

tion of moral issues. J. Appl. Psych. 91(1):233–243. quantitative divide: Guidelines for conducting mixed methods
Salesforce (2022) Salesforce Einstein. Retrieved February 10, 2022, research in information systems. MIS Quart. 37(1):21–54.
https://www.salesforce.com/eu/products/einstein/faq/#:~:text= Venkatesh V, Morris MG, Davis GB, Davis FD (2003) User accept-
Salesforce%20Einstein%20is%20the%20first,brings%20AI%20to% ance of information technology: Toward a unified view. MIS
20trailblazers%20everywhere. Quart. 27(3):425–478.
Schanke S, Burtch G, Ray G (2021) Estimating the impact of human- Wang W, Benbasat I (2007) Recommendation agents for electronic
izing customer service chatbots. Inform. Systems Res. 32(3):736–751. commerce: Effects of explanation facilities on trusting beliefs. J.
Senecal S, Nantel J (2004) The influence of online product recommenda- Management Inform. Systems 23(4):217–246.
tions on consumers’ online choices. J. Retailing 80(2):159–169. Woodpecker (2022) Send emails, deliver results. Retrieved February 22,
Short J, Williams E, Christie B (1976) The Social Psychology of Telecom- 2022, https://woodpecker.co/blog/category/cold-email-basics/.
munications (Wiley, New York). Xiao B, Benbasat I (2007) E-commerce product recommendation agents:
Simon HA (1978) Information-processing theory of human problem Use, characteristics, and impact. MIS Quart. 31(1):137–209.
solving. Estes WK, ed. Human Information Processing: Handbook Xu J, Benbasat I, Cenfetelli RT (2014) Research note—the influ-
of Learning and Cognitive Processes (Psychology Press, London), ences of online service technologies and task complexity on
271–295. efficiency and personalization. Inform. Systems Res. 25(2):
Simon P (2015) Message Not Received: Why Business Communication Is 420–436.
Broken and How to Fix It (John Wiley & Sons, New York). Xu J, Benbasat I, Cenfetelli RT (2017) A two-stage model of
Snyder CR, Fromkin HL (1980) Uniqueness: The Human Pursuit of generating product advice: Proposing and testing the com-
Difference (Springer Science & Business Media, New York). plementarity principle. J. Management Inform. Systems 34(3):
Srinivasan R, Sarial-Abi G (2021) When algorithms fail: Consumers’ 826–862.
responses to brand harm crises caused by algorithm errors. J. Xu J, Benbasat I, Cenfetelli RT (2020) The relative effect of the con-
Marketing 85(5):74–91. vergence of product recommendations from various online
Statista (2021) In what ways do you think that artificial intelligence sources. J. Management Inform. Systems 37(3):788–819.
could improve email marketing performance? Retrieved May 10, Zhang X, Kumar V, Cosguner K (2017) Dynamically managing
2021, https://www.statista.com/statistics/710338/email-marketing- a profitable email marketing program. J. Marketing Res. 54(6):
artificial-intelligence-impact/. 851–866.
Tam KY, Ho SY (2005) Web personalization as a persuasion strat- Zhao X, Lynch JG Jr, Chen Q (2010) Reconsidering Baron and
egy: An elaboration likelihood model perspective. Inform. Sys- Kenny: Myths and truths about mediation analysis. J. Consumer
tems Res. 16(3):271–291. Res. 37(2):197–206.

You might also like