You are on page 1of 14

Consumer Responses to Advertising:

The Effects of Ad Content, Emotions, and


Attitude toward the Ad on Viewing Time
THOMAS J. OLNEY
MORRIS B. HOLBROOK
RAJEEV BATRA*

This study develops and tests a hierarchical model of advertising effects on viewing
time. The ads studied represent a sample of commercials aired during prime-time
broadcasts, and the effects are analyzed across the ads rather than across people.
Primary emphasis is placed on the attempt to explain a simulated behavioral measure
of attention to television commercials-that of channel switching (zapping) and fast-
forwarding through ads on prerecorded programs (zipping). In addition, the study
demonstrates a chain of effects from the content of television ads, through emotional
reactions and attitude toward the ad, to actual viewing behaviors.

P revious research on consumers' responses to ad-


vertising has begun to establish a hierarchical
a video cassette. By contrast, zapping occurs during
the broadcast itself and refers to the switching of the
model of advertising effects (for reviews, see Holbrook channel when a commercial appears. In either situa-
[1986] and Preston [1982]). Measures of advertising tion, exposure and attention are radically different
effects have thus far spanned all levels in the hierarchy, from the comparatively passive nature of the old days
moving backward from behavior (sales) to affect (at- when, stereotypically, viewers sat in front of their tele-
titude toward the brand or ad) to cognition (beliefs, visions and watched whatever paraded before their
evaluative judgments) to attention or exposure (read- eyes. While nonviewing behaviors such as talking,
ership or viewership). All of these are important to reading, or leaving the room did exist even then (An-
the attainment of advertising objectives. However, derson 1985), zipping and zapping have greatly am-
emerging technological developments have lately given plified the extent to which advertising viewing can be,
consumer researchers reason to put renewed emphasis and is, avoided. Indeed, studies in both the United
on the nature and antecedents of the attention and States (Heeter and Greenberg 1985; IRI Information
exposure variables that form the very first step in the Resources 1983, 1985; Kaplan 1985) and Great Britain
hierarchy. (Yorke and Kitchen 1985) have shown the pervasive-
Specifically, the development of remote control de- ness of both phenomena.
vices and VCRs has led to two practices that have dra-
matically altered the relationship between television THE STUDY
viewing and advertising exposure or attention-
Given this recent development, the present study
namely, zipping and zapping. Some confusion has ex-
uses a representative sample of television commercials
isted in the popular press over these terms. In our
to examine the advertising-related antecedents of
usage, zipping refers to fast-forwarding through ads
viewing time. Specifically, the study proposes a be-
previously recorded along with program material on
havioral measure of consumers' viewing responses to
television commercials based on a simulation of zip-
*T. J. Olney is assistant professor, Western Washington Univer- ping and zapping as indicants of attention to the ad.
sity, Bellingham, WA 98225. Morris B. Holbrook is the Dillard The antecedents of the measure that it investigated are
Professor of Business, Graduate School of Business, Columbia Uni-
versity, New York, NY 10027. Rajeev Batra is associate professor,
a multicomponent representation of attitude toward
School of Business Administration, University of Michigan, Ann the ad, two dimensions of emotional responses, and
Arbor, MI 48109. The authors gratefully acknowledge the support various aspects of advertising content. In particular,
of the Columbia Business School's Faculty Research Fund and the the study focuses on the variance in viewing time ex-
Faculty Development Fund of Western Washington University'S plained by three attitudinal components (hedonism,
College of Business and Economics.
utilitarianism, and interestingness), by two emotional
440
© 1991 by JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH, Inc .• Vol. 17 • March 1991
All rights reserved. 0093-5301/91/1704-0007$02.00
EFFECTS ON VIEWING TIME 441

FIGURE 1

HIERARCHICAL MODEL OF ADVERTISING EFFECTS

? ?
f- Ad content

-
(Appeals) Emotional
dimensions Attitudinal Viewing
components Time
(Pleasure (Hedonism, f----+ (zipping and
Ad content and Ut il ita ri an ism, zapping)
'-- (Uniqueness) Arousal) Interestingness)
?
I
dimensions (pleasure and arousal), and by the unique- 2. follows a practice common in advertising research
ness of ad content (versus a more standard set of ad- by treating the ad itself as the unit of analysis and
vertising appeals). Additionally, the study examines examining relationships across ads at the aggregate
the mediating roles of intervening variables along a level,
hierarchical chain of effects in which advertising con- 3. guards against method artifacts by using separate
tent influences emotions and attitude toward the ad, samples of judges to provide independently obtained
which, in turn, influence viewing time. measures of the variables at each stage of the model,
4. views these separate samples of judges as providing
The Model content analyses and related response measures for
Our tentative model for the hierarchical chain that the television commercials of interest,
culminates in viewing time appears in Figure 1. As 5. tests for mediating effects via the rule that Y mediates
shown by the diagram, we assume a forward recursive the effect of X on Z if and only if (1) X is related to
flow of effects from ad content through emotions and Z, (2) Y is related to Z, (3) X is related to Y, and (4)
attitudinal responses to viewing behavior. Working when Z is regressed on X and Y is controlled for, the
backward, we assume that viewing time depends di- significance of X in explaining Z decreases (partial
rectly on three components of attitude toward the ad- mediation) or disappears entirely (complete media-
namely, hedonism, utilitarianism, and interestingness. tion; see Baron and Kenny 1986).
These three influences on viewing time depend, in
turn, on emotional responses to the ad (we focus pri- Key Variables
marily on the emotional dimensions of pleasure and
arousal). Finally, we assume that these emotional re- Viewing Time (Zipping and Zapping). As dis-
sponses reflect advertising content (with respect to both cussed earlier, zapping is the act of using a remote
the ad appeals used and the uniqueness of the ad). control device to change the channel when a com-
In general, then, we view emotional responses and mercial comes on. By contrast, zipping refers only to
the three attitudinal components as intervening vari- prerecorded programs on video and occurs when the
ables that mediate the relationship between ad content viewer uses the controls of a VCR to fast-forward
and viewing time. The question marks in the diagram through commercials. While both zipping and zapping
indicate the issue of whether a variable exerts a direct have attracted much attention over the past few years,
effect beyond its indirect effect via mediation. neither has been thoroughly studied, in part because
both remain somewhat intractable in the practical
General Analytic Approach problems they present to the researcher.
In the absence of such research, doubt lingers re-
Our model of hierarchical effects raises a number of garding whether zipping and zapping behaviors are
issues that require empirical investigation. To that end, simply different manifestations of the same phenom-
the present study follows a procedure developed by enon. Conceptually, since both represent different
(among others) Holbrook and Batra (1987) in a dif- ways of decreasing viewing time of commercials, one
ferent context. Specifically, as applied here, this ap- would expect zipping and zapping to be strongly in-
proach terrelated. Indeed, theory drawn from psychology in
1. traces a hierarchical chain of effects from ad content general (see, e.g., Kahneman 1973) and from consumer
through emotional responses and attitude toward the behavior in particular (see, e.g., Bettman 1979) sug-
ad to viewing time (the criterion variable), gests that both phenomena represent attention as
442 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

measured by viewing time. In general, the use oflook- Mitchell and Olson 1981) raises an issue that suggests
ing time to measure attention has a venerable history another potential research opportunity. Specifically,
in the psychological literature on exploratory behavior research by Berlyne (1960) and his colleagues has con-
(see, e.g., Berlyne 1960). In this tradition, looking time sistently found a nonmonotonic (inverted-U-shaped)
has been related to the novelty of stimuli (Leckart relation between interestingness and overall liking,
1966; Leckart and Bakan 1965, 1969) and to such other suggesting that interestingness should be kept separate
collative properties as uncertainty and complexity rather than combined into one global measure of
(Berlyne 1958, 1963). Looking time has also prompted overall attitude toward the ad.
some applications in advertising research in which, for Previous empirical research on attitude toward the
example, greater visual complexity has been shown to ad has tended to focus primarily on establishing ad
increase the length of time that subjects spend looking attitude as of theoretical and practical importance in
at print ads presented with a slide projector (Morrison mediating advertising effectiveness, without develop-
and Dainoff 1972). Hence, we investigate whether zip- ing the nature ofthis construct more precisely. As two
ping and zapping appear to constitute cognate phe- exceptions, Burke and Edell (1986) investigated a di-
nomena. In sum, as indicated by the diagram shown mensional representation of attitude toward the ad
in Figure 1, the present study focuses on looking time based on a combination of 38 adjectives, while Mad-
as the key outcome of a hierarchical model that, in the den, Allen, and Twible (1988)-following Shimp
spirit of Berlyne (1960), considers viewing behavior as (1981)-pursued an attempt to decompose attitude
dependent on such antecedent variables as the effects toward the ad into its cognitive and affective compo-
of advertising uniqueness, as mediated by emotional nents. Such empirical research is in keeping with the
arousal and attitudinal interest. theoretical framework developed by MacKenzie and
Attitude toward the Ad. A review of recent litera- Lutz (1989), who suggest various antecedents of overall
ture on attitude toward the ad indicates that there is attitude toward the ad. The present study, which looks
probably more to this variable than overall evaluation at specific components, builds on such efforts by ex-
as captured by a unidimensional global affect. For ex- amining a multidimensional structure of attitude to-
ample, several recent studies have used a four-item ward the ad based on three attitudinal components-
index (good-bad, like-dislike, irritating-not irritating, hedonism, utilitarianism, and interestingness-the
and uninteresting-interesting; Gardner 1985; Mitchell first two taken from Batra and Ahtola (1991), and the
1986b; Mitchell and Olson 1981), yet these four items third from Berlyne (1960).
accounted for only 68 percent of the evaluative vari- Thus, in this conception, instead of evaluating ad
ance in the study in which they were first used (Mitchell attitudes through overall items (such as good-bad, like-
and Olson 1981). This suggests that attitude toward dislike), we use items for each specific component: he-
the ad involves additional phenomena that might be donism (an evaluation along the entertainment di-
captured by other measures. Indeed, related work by mension), utilitarianism (an evaluation of usefulness),
Batra and Ahtola (1991) supports the notion that at- and interestingness (an evaluation of curiosity). Such
titude involves more than just an overall, unidimen- disaggregation will permit the study of the possibly
sional evaluative measure of global affect. That study differing behavioral (viewing) effects of each of these
found two dimensions of attitude-one labeled "he- attitudinal components. Thus, in the hierarchical
donic" and the other "utilitarian." Of these, the he- model of advertising responses (Fig. 1), viewing time
donic dimension (corresponding to an evaluation of depends on these three attitudinal components, which
pleasure) appears to resemble the "entertaining" di- depend, in turn, on emotions and advertising content.
mension discussed in earlier copy-testing research by
Schlinger (1979), which rated the ad on being "plea- Emotional Dimensions. Thanks to calls for ex-
surable," and the "humor" dimension of Wells, Leav- amining the role of emotions and affect in advertising
itt, and McConville (1971), which rated the ad on its (e.g., Batra and Ray 1986; Holbrook and O'Shaugh-
"playfulness." The utilitarian attitude component, nessy 1984; Mitchell 1986a), abundant current re-
conversely, evaluates the ad on how useful it is and search indicates that we can view the place of emotion
appears to correspond to Schlinger's "relevant news" in advertising from diverse perspectives. These include
dimension ("useful") and to Wells et al.'s "personal the warmth focus of Aaker, Stayman, and Hagerty
relevance" factor ("important to me, valuable"). (1986), the irritation concerns of Aaker and Bruzzone
Clearly, it is of interest to see if these two attitude (1985), the use of a large battery of subjective feelings
components have differential relationships to ad- by Edell and Burke (1987), the Izard (1977) DES-II
viewing time. emotional measurement instrument adopted by West-
Further, the frequent use of interestingness as part brook (1987), and investigations into the interactions
of a multi-item measure of global attitude toward the between ads and their program context (Goldberg and
ad (Gardner 1985; Lutz, MacKenzie, and Belch 1983; Gorn 1987). Aaker, Stayman, and Vezina (1988) pro-
MacKenzie, Lutz, and Belch 1986; Mitchell 1986b; vide an impressive inventory of feelings elicited by ad-
EFFECTS ON VIEWING TIME 443

vertising. Batra and Holbrook (1990) pursue similar monotonic effect on the emotional dimension of plea-
ends in creating an affective typology. sure and the hedonism component of attitude toward
Thus, one finds a variety of competing perspectives the ad.
on the measurement of emotions in advertising from Previous advertising research has examined the in-
which to choose. However, the emotional measure- formativeness of ads with respect to the usefulness of
ment system that has probably received the greatest that information (Marquez 1977; Pollay, Zaich-
attention from consumer researchers thus far is the kowsky, and Fryer 1980; Resnik and Stern 1977). De-
PAD scheme for assessing the emotional dimensions spite this literature, some doubt remains concerning
of pleasure, arousal, and dominance (Mehrabian and how one should assess new information (novelty) in
Russell 1974). The Mehrabian-Russell PAD dimen- the present context. From one perspective, which we
sions have been employed by such researchers as Christ shall call "uniqueness," novelty represents the degree
(1985), Christ and Biggers (1984), Donovan and Ros- to which a particular ad differs from other exemplars;
siter (1982), and Holbrook et al. (1984). Further, using this perspective is similar to that of Mandler (1982),
principal components analysis on a battery of emo- whose work suggests that we might view the novelty
tional items derived from many different sources, of a television commercial as an incongruity between
Holbrook and Batra (1987) recovered these three a viewer's existing schema for television commercials
emotional dimensions of pleasure, arousal, and dom- and the nature of the commercial in question. From
ination. Recently, Russell (1980; Russell, Weiss, and another perspective, which we might call "familiarity,"
Mendelsohn 1989) has suggested that the third dimen- novelty depends on an individual's (lack of) prior ex-
sion (dominance) can be dispensed with. Although this perience with a given ad or brand (cf. Baker et al. 1986).
remains an empirical question, in the present study, From the Berlynian viewpoint, both seem relevant.
we found no important effects of dominance. Hence, However, the nature of our design (which involved
we shall discuss only the roles of pleasure and arousal. assessing advertising content after the ads had already
Specifically, as indicated by the hierarchical model appeared on television) impaired our ability to rely
in Figure 1, we propose that pleasure and arousal confidently on the use of judges in content analysis to
should intervene between ad content and attitude to- measure the familiarity of the commercials. Hence,
ward the ad in explaining viewing time. Here, in par- we shall focus here on those results that bear on the
ticular, it makes sense to argue that interestingness judges' assessments of advertising uniqueness (i.e., the
should reflect arousal, which should, in turn, depend extent to which a commercial differs from other ad-
on the uniqueness of ad content (as discussed below). vertising).
Meanwhile, hedonism should depend on pleasure,
which should reach its peak at an intermediate level Research Questions and Hypotheses
of uniqueness (also discussed below). As its main thrust, this study attempts to develop a
Ad Content. The present study employs measures model for the explanation of viewing time. Each set
of advertising appeals drawn from a variety of sources of variables (ad content, emotional responses, attitude
(e.g., Holbrook and Batra 1987; Schlinger 1979; Stew- toward the ad) was expected to influence viewing time
art and Furse 1984; Wells 1964; Wells et al. 1971). to some extent. In particular, we expected that viewing
However, because of our focus on attention as mea- time would respond to uniqueness according to the
sured by viewing time and on the relevance of the work aforementioned nonmonotonic function. Further, we
by Berlyne, we thought it appropriate to give special expected that this inverted-U-shaped relationship
emphasis to the aspects of advertising content related would be fully or partially mediated by the positive
to uniqueness. intervening effects of both the emotional dimensions
Berlyne (1960) developed a set of variables that rep- (pleasure and arousal) and two of the ad-attitude com-
resent the amount of new information in a stimulus, ponents (hedonism and interestingness). We left the
especially as it relates to previously encountered stim- directions of the effects of the specific ad-content fac-
uli. These include such properties as novelty, surprise, tors and the possible role of utilitarianism as research
complexity, and uncertainty. Berlyne found a consis- questions for exploratory investigation.
tent relationship between these properties and the
amount of attention paid to a stimulus. This relation- METHOD
ship has generally assumed the form of an inverted U,
with maximum attention paid to stimuli at interme- The Unit of Analysis
diate levels of novelty, surprise, complexity, or uncer- The present study continues the tradition established
tainty. Hence, we would expect the same nonmono- by advertising researchers such as Wells (1964), Wells
tonic relationship between viewing time for ads and et al. (1971), Schlinger (1979), Holbrook and Lehmann
their levels of these properties in general. Additionally, (1980), Stewart and Furse (19!55, 1986), Pechmann and
Berlyne's (1971) later work on aesthetics suggests that Stewart (1985), Holbrook and Batra (1987), and Thor-
these stimulus properties should also have a non- son, Heide, and Page (1987), in which the ad itself is
444 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

the unit of analysis, and the measurements of ad-re- ad. However, we would amplify Edell and Burke's ar-
lated factors involve assessments made by samples of gument to suggest that feelings result from the inter-
judges who perform a role closely akin to that more action of people with ads. Though both sides of the
generally associated with content analysis (Holsti 1969; interaction are important, one can legitimately focus
Krippendorff 1980; Weber 1985). This use of content on either side. Edell and Burke have chosen to examine
analysis in a consumer-behavior context follows sug- the psychological process at the individual level. For
gestions made by Kassarjian (1977) and Holbrook some purposes, however, one may wish to focus on
(1977) regarding the rich potential of the method for the hierarchical effects of the ads themselves. For ex-
researching the nature of consumer-directed com- ample, getting favorable results in the marketplace may
munications and their effects. depend on finding and manipulating emotional aspects
Notice that our approach departs somewhat from of advertising that are relatively homogeneous across
the emphasis on objective assessment of message ele- members of the audience (thereby obtaining high in-
ments that is normally associated with content anal- tersubjective agreement). If the consumers in a target
ysis, and this departure increases as we move from segment all respond differently to the emotional con-
analysis of ad content to measures of emotion and at- tent of a firm's ad, its effects on market behavior may
titude (see Fig. 1). Specifically, our measures of ad disappear. Hence, a focus on the ad as the unit of anal-
content (discussed later) are intended to provide rel- ysis remains relevant to those concerned with out-
atively objective assessments of the appeals and comes in terms of advertising effectiveness and other
uniqueness associated with the message. By contrast, marketing-related variables.
the emotional measures include a subjective aspect re-
lated to how the judge feels when watching the ad The Sample
(happy or unhappy, etc.). Finally, the attitudinal mea-
sures impose a layer of evaluative judgments on the We created the sample of ads for the present study
viewing experience (entertaining or not entertaining, by taping prime-time television in a northeastern city
etc.). In these more subjective and evaluative types of on random nights for a week, numbering all the distinct
ratings, one hopes for intersubjective validity based 30-second commercials that appeared on the tapes, and
on consensus rather than any stricter type of objec- then randomly choosing a sample of 150 ads. Given
tivity. this procedure, our sample included commercials for
Notice also that this aspect of our method causes both national and regional goods and services. These
certain closely related aspects of advertising content ads were then edited by a video production company
and subjective responses to reappear under different onto three different tapes (described later). However,
interpretations at succeeding stages of the hierarchical because of technical problems, four of the 150 were
chain. For example, as one aspect of ad content, an lost, leaving 146 ads in the final sample.
"enjoyment" appeal was rated for each ad by the This sample tends to represent what viewers actually
judges. In reporting emotions, a separate set of judges watch and, as such, departs from other research in
assessed how strongly the ad contributed to their plea- which the ads have reflected a particular range of emo-
sure on such items as "happy-sad." Still another set tions or executions of special interest to the investi-
of judges evaluated the ad on its degree of hedonism gators. Specifically, instead of choosing ads to cover
with respect to such terms as "entertaining." Clearly, as wide a range of emotional responses as possible, our
one would expect those particular facets of the overall study uses a sample representative of ads actually
hierarchical model to be related: enjoyment appeals broadcast to estimate the hierarchical model of ad-
should enhance pleasure, which, in turn, should con- vertising effects. This allows us to draw conclusions
tribute to hedonism. Indeed, the need to avoid shared- about what ads actually do, whereas conclusions drawn
methods variance attributable to lexical redundancy from carefully chosen samples of ads tell us more about
among these and other subsets of items across types what ads can do (Douglas M. Stayman, conversation
of measures is the primary reason for collecting our with authors, 1986). Hence, in this sense, any findings
ratings of ads at the different stages of the model from that emerge from the present study should represent
independent samples of judges (cf. MacKenzie et al. typical commercials, at least for prime-time television
1986). during the late 1980s in the Northeast.
Notice finally that in treating the ad (rather than the
respondent) as the unit of analysis, we run counter to The Judges
Edell and Burke's emphasis on the importance of an-
alyzing feelings across respondents (rather than across For each level of the hierarchical model shown in
ads). Specifically, Edell and Burke (1987, p. 430) argue Figure 1, different judges responded to the ads on the
that "feelings appear to be properties of the individual" variables of interest. That is, separate samples of judges
and conclude that "feelings . . . may not be appro- evaluated the ads for the types of salient appeals,
priate descriptors of ads." We agree completely that uniqueness, the emotional response dimensions (plea-
people may differ in their emotional responses to an sure and arousal), the components of attitude toward
EFFECTS ON VIEWING TIME 445

the ad (hedonism, utilitarianism, and interestingness), starting points on both tapes in the zapping simulation.
and viewing behavior (zipping and zapping). The two tapes were played on separate videocassette
This approach assumes homogeneity among sepa- recorders running simultaneously. Viewers controlled
rate samples of judges. To this end, all judges and be- which tape they watched by means of an AlB cable
havioral viewers came from a pool of business students push-button switch of the kind used for switching be-
with no more exposure to academic marketing than a tween two signals in multi-source installations.
first course at the MBA level. Wherever possible, the Mounted on a stand, this switch permitted the viewer
various judging tasks were assigned randomly. For to execute a clean zap from one channel to the other
some of the measures, especially those involving and back. The initial starting position of the switch
lengthy sets of ratings, judges received a token payment was also randomized for each viewer. Viewers were
for their participation. For other measures, especially instructed to watch the ads and to use the switch as
those based on zipping and zapping behavior, respon- they chose. Before the tapes began rolling, viewers were
dents fulfilled a course requirement for the aforemen- asked to push the buttons once or twice to get a feel
tioned first marketing course. Details on each mea- for it. This demonstrated to them that they had a choice
surement and the relevant respondents follow. of two channels to watch. As in the zipping condition,
another VCR recorded everything that actually ap-
The Measures peared on the screen as controlled by the viewer. Be-
cause of the randomization, it remained entirely pos-
Viewing Time. Combining two behavioral assess- sible for an individual never to see any given ad, clearly
ments from two separate conditions into one variable a realistic aspect of the simulation. For both zipping
(Viewing Time) provides our operationalization of the and zapping, data for Viewing Time was obtained by
theoretical construct of attention (our main criterion using an ordinary stopwatch to record the amount of
variable as measured by looking behavior). Specifi- time each ad appeared on each viewer's monitor tape
cally, in this study, viewers recruited from the general at normal speed.
pool of MBA students and tested in private screenings
Commercial Appeals. To understand the kinds of
watched 75 minutes of ads in either a simulated zipping
appeals likely to demonstrate the greatest effect, we
or a simulated zapping condition. On each day of test-
culled 34 items from 66 items used in an earlier study
ing, subjects all performed either the zipping or the
by Holbrook and Batra (1987). Specifically, 25 of these
zapping simulation. Sets of subjects were randomly
items came directly from one of the three measurement
assigned to the two conditions, so that 50 viewers saw
instruments in that study, and the remaining nine
the ads in the zipping condition, and 52 in the zapping
items, which needed rewording, were taken from the
condition.
other two instruments. The resulting set of 34 appeals,
In the zipping condition, the 146 ads appeared on
listed in Exhibit 1, was presented in random order,
a tape in random order with the full sequence repeated
and each item was rated within a seven-position range
again after the last ad. The production company had
from weak to strong.
inserted a red and white sign saying "stop" between
Using this type of rating task as the basis for content
each of the ads. Viewers were equipped with a remote
analysis precludes the need to provide judges with the
control device that allowed them to speed up or stop
sorts of detailed coding instructions and category def-
the VCR. As the session began, the experimenter dem-
initions normally associated with this procedure (as-
onstrated the operation of the VCR, which started at
random positions on the tape, and subjects watched a suming, of course, that satisfactory interjudge reli-
ability can be attained on the basis of the item names
sample series of ads. Then they were instructed to
themselves). Thus, 12 judges for content analysis were
watch the tape and to use the fast-forward or pause
paid simply to rate each ad from "weakly" to
button, however they chose, with the proviso that
"strongly" on its use of each appeal. Judges identified
whenever they saw a stop sign, they should momen-
and rated each ad on a sheet of paper while viewing
tarily switch back to regular play if they were fast-
the zipping tape, which provided the stimuli for all the
forwarding. Further, they were instructed to watch
judgment tasks. For the measures of ad appeals, judges
the tape for a total of 75 minutes (the normal playing
started at staggered points on the tape to minimize the
time of the tape) and, if they reached the end of the
sequence effects inherent in taped stimuli. Completing
tape before this time, they were to rewind it and con-
the judging task for ad appeals required about four
tinue watching until the experimenter returned to
hours per judge. Content judges performed the rating
indicate the end of the session. Viewer behavior was
task individually in a room by themselves and re-
recorded by another VCR that taped everything tran-
mained in the room the entire time except for short
spiring on the monitor as seen and controlled by the
breaks.
viewer.
The zapping condition used two tapes with different Uniqueness. A battery of 18 items represented the
random orders of the ads, this time with no pauses novelty-related factors studied by Berlyne (1960).
between commercials. Again viewers began at random However, because of the possible effects of elapsed time
446 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

EXHIBIT 1
ASSESSMENT ITEMS

Ad content Emotional dimensions of response


Ad appeals (rated from weak to strong over a seven-position Pleasure
range) Happy-unhappy
Convenience Pleased-annoyed
Artistic merit Satisfied-unsatisfied
Health and well-being Contented-melancholic
Beauty Hopeful-despairing
Fear Relaxed-bored
Superior design Arousal
Duty Stimulated-relaxed
Comfort Excited-calm
Rational appeal Frenzied-sluggish
Product features Jittery-dull
Guarantees Wide awake-sleepy
Price or value Aroused-unaroused
Spirituality Attitudinal components
Sex appeal Hedonism
Status Unpleasant-pleasant
Enjoyment Fun to watch-not fun to watch
Status Not entertaining-entertaining
Emotional appeal Enjoyable-not enjoyable
Slice of life Utilitarianism
Loyalty of existing customers Important-not important
Evaluation appeal Informative-uninformative
Self-esteem Helpful-not helpful
Efficiency and performance Useful-not useful
Competence enhancement Interestingness
Virtue Makes me curious-does not make me curious
Solution to a problem Not boring-boring
Company image, reputation Interesting-not interesting
Craftsmanship Keeps my attention-does not keep my attention
Safety
Sensory character (taste, smell)
Appetite appeal (hunger, desire)
Attributes, ingredients, components
Aesthetics
Quality
Factual information
Uniqueness (bipolar adjectives rated over a seven-position range)
Peculiar-ordinary
Just like any other ad-different from any other ad
Average-special
Weird-normal
Nothing special-outstanding

in our data collection methodology, as previously developed by Mehrabian and Russell (1974) to assess
mentioned, we felt that items intended to assess fa- emotional response. The six items used to assess the
miliarity should not be used for the present study. We emotional dimension of pleasure and the six assessing
therefore confined our attention to five pairs of bipolar arousal are listed in Exhibit 1. The remaining six items
adjectives intended to represent uniqueness. As listed represented dominance, but, as previously noted, these
in Exhibit 1, these were presented in random order showed no important effects and will not be pursued
and with their directions randomized within a seven- further in the present discussion. All items were pre-
position check-mark format. A separate group of 12 sented in random order with a seven-position rating
paid judges rated the ads on the novelty-related items. format.
With fewer items for this set of measures, the time Unpaid graduate students (from the same overall
required to complete the rating task was somewhat pool as for the other measures) watched 30 ofthe com-
less than that for the ad appeals-about three and a mercials and rated their feelings toward each. As with
half hours, including breaks. other measures, to reduce the sequence effects inherent
Emotional Measures. Another independent group in taped stimuli, judges began at random starting
of separate judges rated the ads on a set of 18 items points on the randomized zipping tape. Each ad re-
EFFECTS ON VIEWING TIME 447

cei ved 12-15 ratings for each of the two emotion in- for each ad. This resulted in a final data set consisting
struments. Judges for these two sets of measures came of 146 observations on each variable, one observation
from an introductory MBA marketing class in which per ad.
the research participation satisfied a course require-
ment. They required 48-80 minutes to complete the Assessment of Reliabilities
rating of 30 ads.
The nature of the measurement task required that
Attitudinal Components. Measures of the three se- reliabilities be assessed on each of the separate items
lected components of attitude toward the ad again em- and indices-specifically, for those involvinr the ap-
ployed 12 paid judges drawn from the same general peals, uniqueness, emotional, and attitudinal items.
pool of students after it was verified that they had not Coefficient alpha provided a suitable method for as-
participated in any of the other judgment tasks. Judges sessing these reliabilities in all possible configurations:
rated their attitudes toward the ad on separate four- for individual items across judges, for indices within
item indices of Hedonism, Utilitarianism, and Inter- judges, for within-judge indices across all judges, and
estingness on a check-mark bipolar format with seven for the aggregate index composed of the mean of all
positions. The items, appearing in Exhibit 1, were ran- judges on each item. (For brevity, not all of these re-
domized in both order and direction. As before, judges liabilities are reported in what follows; those not men-
began at starting points systematically varied along the tioned can be obtained by writing to the first author.)
randomized tape containing the ads to reduce sequence
effects. Judges watched each ad and then responded
to all attitudinal items for that ad before viewing the Principal Component Scores
next one on the tape. The total time required for each To remove multicollinearity from the subsequent
judge to complete this measurement task was 2-3 regressions, principal components analyses were per-
hours. formed on the appropriate sets of content, emotional,
and attitudinal items. These created uncorrelated
Timing standardized component scores for use as independent
Because of limitations in our available financial re- variables.
sources, we could not collect all of the measures just To retain the a priori Uniqueness measures of in-
described simultaneously. We did manage to obtain terest in the ad-content judgments while removing any
the zipping and zapping measures within a few weeks remaining correlations between the Uniqueness and
of the time the commercials were taped from prime- the ad-appeals measures, the effects of Uniqueness and
time television. However, there was a six-month delay Uniqueness Squared were partialed out of the ad-ap-
in obtaining the emotion measures, followed by a sim- peals scores before performing an analysis of principal
ilar delay in collecting the ad-attitude and ad-content components on the latter variables. In other words,
measures. As previously mentioned, possible distor- each ad appeal was regressed on Uniqueness and
tions due to this latter delay caused us to drop the Uniqueness Squared, with analysis of principal com-
familiarity measures from the items intended to rep- ponents performed on the residuals. This step removed
resent novelty and to focus instead on the measures all multicollinearity from the ad-content measures and
of uniqueness. It should be noted that, if anything, the yielded ad-content factors totally orthogonal to judg-
timing factors pertinent to our study should exert a ments of the ad's uniqueness.
downward bias on our ability to find the hypothesized
relationships under investigation, since they would Model Testing
weaken observed relationships. Hence, our approach
appears to be conservative in this respect. As the model proposed is fully recursive, OLS
regressions provided standardized beta weights and
appropriate tests of significance to answer the questions
Data Transformations raised earlier. In general, our procedure followed the
As previously described, judges provided the mea- logic described regarding the nature of a mediating
sures for nearly all the variables in the model, with the effect (Baron and Kenny 1986). In particular, with
major exception of the behavioral measures of the ul- respect to the question of whether emotions and atti-
timate criterion variables, zipping and zapping. In the tudinal components play an intervening role in the
case of the emotion measures, for which each ad was relationship between ad content and viewing time, full
judged by 12-15 judges, the ratings of the twelfth judge or partial mediation can be inferred if and only if the
and any beyond the twelfth were combined and av- following conditions are met: (1) ad content explains
eraged to form a twelfth pseudojudge so as to attain viewing time; (2) emotions and attitudinal components
comparability for purposes of assessing reliabilities. together explain viewing time; (3) ad content explains
To enable an across-ads analysis, we first computed emotions; (4) ad content and emotions together ex-
mean values for each measured variable across judges plain attitudinal components; (5) the effect of ad con-
448 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

tent on viewing time declines (partial mediation) or these two factors, hereafter called Facts and Feelings,
disappears (full mediation) when the effects of emo- along with the aforementioned factor of Uniqueness
tions and attitudinal components are statistically con- and Uniqueness Squared, as our measures of adver-
trolled for in explaining viewing time (via multiple tising content.
regression): ad content, emotions, and attitudinal
components together explain viewing time. Emotional Indices: Pleasure and Arousal. The
single-item interjudge reliabilities for the emotional
items ranged from .79 for "wide awake" to .53 for
RESULTS "contented," with a median of .70. As one would ex-
pect, the multi-item reliabilities ofthe six-item indices
Reliabilities and Validities of the Measures based on scores averaged across judges were higher,
Viewing Time: The Zipping and Zapping Mea- .95 for Pleasure and .97 for Arousal. Our averaged
sures. As we expected, across the 146 ads, the view- multi-item emotional indices, therefore, benefit from
ing-time measures showed a high degree of intercor- the variance-reducing tendencies inherent in taking
relation (r = .75). For this reason, we combined the averages of averages (a helpful feature of our design).
standardized values for these measures into a two-item As a check on the dimensional structure of the emo-
index of Viewing Time. tions, we performed a principal components analysis
on the across-judge means for the full set of items.
Advertising Content: Appeals and Uniqueness. After a varimax rotation, the structure of the factor
Alphas for single-item interjudge reliabilities on each loadings corroborated the anticipated pattern. Hence,
of the 34 ad-appeal items ranged from a high of .92 to take advantage of their freedom from intercorre-
for the appetite appeal to a low of .31 for the evaluative lations, we computed the rotated component scores
appeal, with a median of .76. The five items for for Pleasure and Arousal and used these uncorrelated
Uniqueness attained interjudge reliabilities ranging emotional dimensions for all subsequent analyses.
from a low of .84 for "weird" to a high of .87 for "av-
erage," with a median of .86. A more important mea- Attitude toward the Ad. For attitude toward the
sure of reliability concerns that for the index formed ad, each item belonged to one of three a priori attitude
by summing the Uniqueness measures across all items indices. The reliability for individual items, as mea-
within each judge and then calculating the interjudge sured by the interjudge coefficient alpha, ranged from
reliability on that sum; the resulting coefficient alpha .87 for "the ad is entertaining" to .65 for "makes me
for interjudge reliability was .89. Finally, the multi- curious," with a median of .80. The multi-item reli-
item reliability for across-judge averages is .97 for the abilities for the four-item indices averaged across
five Uniqueness items. judges were .95, .90, and .94 for Hedonism, Utilitar-
ianism, and Interestingness, respectively.
Advertising Content: Principal Components. For As a check on the structure of our attitude toward
the cleanest possible measure of Uniqueness, we per- the ad indices, the 12 relevant mean across-judge items
formed a principal components analysis on the five were subjected to principal components analysis. The
Uniqueness measures. This process resulted in a first first three components accounted for 91 percent of the
factor explaining 88 percent of the variance. Stan- variance in these data and, after varimax rotation,
dardized scores on this first principal component were faithfully recreated the a priori indices of Hedonism,
calculated and used as the measure of Uniqueness in Utilitarianism, and Interestingness. Accordingly, to
all further analyses. avoid multicollinearity among the attitudinal com-
As previously stated, the measures of ad appeals were ponents, we used scores on each factor in all subse-
regressed on Uniqueness and Uniqueness Squared to quent analyses.
remove any correlations between the Uniqueness and
ad-appeals variables, with principal components anal- Research Questions and Hypotheses
ysis performed on the residuals. The pattern of eigen- Tables 1 and 2 indicate the beta coefficients and R 2 s
values suggested retaining either a four-factor solution obtained when various combinations of the indepen-
(which explained 62 percent of the variance) or a two- dent variables are used to predict the dependent mea-
factor solution (which explained 42 percent of the sures of interest.
variance). After varimax rotation, we found the latter,
more parsimonious solution to be more interpretable, Viewing Time. Table 1 presents the key regression
as the two factors clearly suggested the traditional dis- results for Viewing Time as the dependent variable.
tinction between "facts" (e.g., efficiency, convenience, Column 1 shows that Viewing Time depends strongly
rational, and factual appeals) and "feelings" (e.g., aes- on the ad-content variables. The expected nonmono-
thetic, artistic, emotional, and beauty appeals). And, tonic effects of Uniqueness are reflected in the signif-
in subsequent regressions, the four factors explained icance of both terms, and there is an exploratory but
only marginally greater variance in viewing time than plausible positive effect of Feelings and a negative effect
did the two first factors alone. We therefore retained of Facts. When considered in isolation (col. 2), the
EFFECTS ON VIEWING TIME 449

TABLE 1

HIERARCHICAL REGRESSION ANALYSIS OF CONTRIBUTIONS TO VIEWING TIME


(STANDARDIZED BETA WEIGHTS, R 2 s, AND HIERARCHICAL F TESTS)

Viewing time (dependent variable)

Independent variables 2 3 4 5

Ad content:
Facts -.11' -.14'
Feelings .34**** .22***
Uniqueness .67**** .24"
Uniqueness Squared -.24 **** -.15"
Emotions:
Pleasure .35**** .12+ .06 (NS)
Arousal .55 **** .19" .19"
Attitudinal components:
Hedonism .41···· .32**** .16"
Utilitarianism -.14" -.15" -.00 (NS)
Interestingness .64 **** .50**** .37****
R2 .54 **** .43**** .60**** .63**** .67****
Hierarchical F tests:"
Ad content 41.59···· 4.97'"
Emotions 53.01···· 4.67" 4.29'
Attitude components 71.02 •••• 24.81···· 6.79'"
Emotions and attitude components 10.95 ••••

a F is the test statistic indicating the significance of the incremental change in R2 that results from adding the designated set of variables to the equation.
+p<.10 .
• p < .05 .
•• p < .01 .
••• P < .001 .
•••• P < .0001.

emotional dimensions also predict Viewing Time, via ad content and Viewing Time. However, this media-
the expected positive contribution of both Pleasure and tion of the effects of ad content on Viewing Time is
Arousal. Viewing Time depends strongly on the atti- only partial. Table 1 shows that Facts, Feelings,
tudinal components as well, reflected by highly sig- Uniqueness, and Uniqueness Squared remain signifi-
nificant positive contributions from Hedonism and cant (p < .05 or better) in predicting Viewing Time
Interestingness and by a significant negative contri- when the emotions and attitudinal components are
bution from Utilitarianism (see col. 3). controlled for, indicating that these aspects of ad con-
Mediation. We next discuss how emotions and ad tent continue to exert direct effects beyond the me-
attitudes intervene in the relationship between ad diating role of emotions and attitudinal components.
content and Viewing Time. Here, we use the five con-
ditions needed to establish mediation (as previously Relationships among Antecedent Variables. We
summarized). We have shown that Viewing Time de- have noted that ad content influences Pleasure and
pends strongly on the ad-content variables (condition Arousal. Columns 6-8 of Table 2 further show that
1), and Table 1 shows that Viewing Time also depends the emotions, in turn, explain Hedonism (with the ex-
on the emotions and attitudinal components jointly pected positive effect of Pleasure) and Interestingness
(condition 2). Columns 1 and 2 of Table 2 show that (with the expected positive effect of Arousal). By
ad content significantly influences Pleasure and themselves, the ad-content variables influence each of
Arousal, thus satisfying condition 3; further, columns these attitudinal components (see Table 2). These re-
9-11 of Table 2 show that ad content and the emotions lationships do not appear to be strongly mediated by
significantly explain Hedonism, Utilitarianism, and the emotions in that the contributions of ad content
Interestingness, so that condition 4 is also met. With decline only slightly or not at all when emotions are
respect to condition 5, column 5 of Table 1 shows that added to the equation in columns 9-11 of Table 2.
the effects of ad content on Viewing Time are reduced Hence, significant direct effects-above and beyond
in strength and significance (from F(4,141) = 41.59, the mediating role of the emotions-continue to ap-
p < .0001, to F(4,136) = 4.97,p < .001) when emotions pear in the anticipated directions for the ad-content
and attitudinal components are added to the equation variables in explaining the attitudinal components. In
predicting Viewing Time. Thus, emotions and atti- particular, Feelings versus Facts work in opposite di-
tudinal components do appear to intervene between rections to influence Hedonism and Utilitarianism,
450 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

TABLE 2

EMOTIONS AND ATTITUDINAL COMPONENTS AS FUNCTIONS OF AD CONTENT AND EMOTIONS

Dependent variables

Attitudinal components as a function Attitudinal components as a function Attitudinal components as a function


Emotions of ad content of emotions of ad content and emotions
Independent
variables Pleasure Arousal Hedonism Utilitariansim Interestingness Hedonism Utilitarianism Interestingness Hedonism Utilitarianism Interestingness

Ad content:
Facts .08 (NS) -.01 (NS) -.21** .42**** .15* -.25"'*** .40**** .15**
Feelings .22** .05 (NS) .28*"'*'" -.27**** .13* .17** -.34**** .12*
Uniqueness .39**** .49**** .43**** -.19** .66**** .26*** -.37**** .48****
Uniqueness-
squared -.43**** -.03 (NS) -.36**** -.31**** -.01 (NS) -.14* -.17** -.02 (NS)
Emotion:
Pleasure .63**** .22** .12+ .50**"'* .31***'* -.05 (NS)
Arousal .07 (NS) -.10 (NS) .63**** -.05 (NS) .12+ .40****
R2 .29**** .23**** .34**** .42**** .47**** .40**"'* .06** .41 **** .53**** .49**** .60****
Hierarchical
F tests:·
Ad
Content 14.37**** 10.69**** 18.52**** 25.83**** 31.06**** 9.44**** 29.56**** 15.78****
Emotions 48.36**** 4.55** 50.54**** 27.64**** 9.45**** 22.20****

• F is the test statistic indicating the significance of the incremental change in R2 that results from adding the designated set of variables to the equation.
+ p < .10.
* P < .05.
** P < .01.
*** P < .001.
**** P < .0001.

while Uniqueness plays a key direct role in influencing to assess these and other questions concerning internal
Interestingness. validity must await further research.
As another potential topic for further investigation,
DISCUSSION one might wonder how our measures of zipping and
zapping would compare to those based on viewing
Limitations commercials naturalistically in the context of actual
televised program content. Only future research can
Like other research on consumer behavior, the fully test the extent to which these potential threats to
present study illuminates the subject only within the external validity might have created or not created
bounds of its own particular limitations. As with vir- problems for the generalizability of our results to other
tually any investigation, the nature of the stimuli, re- situations relevant to the effectiveness of commercials
spondents, and instruments might have affected the in broadcast television.
magnitudes or even the directions of the results re-
ported here. Hence, as always, the possibility exists
that different commercials, different judges, or differ- Conclusions
ent measurement techniques could have resulted in
findings either more or less strong than those repre- Subject to these limitations, we find the results of
sented by the present data. For example, our measures this study encouraging. Because the tasks that respon-
of ad content (appeals and uniqueness) offer just one dents completed to collect the measures of zipping and
version of a general type of copy testing that has ap- zapping were fairly lengthy, we feared that any signal
peared widely under diverse guises in the advertising contained therein might be swamped by fatigue effects
research literature cited earlier. Different schemes- and other types of noise or error. However, inasmuch
such as those presented by Wells, Leavitt, and Schlin- as two measures collected separately in very different
ger-might have produced different results. Also, along circumstances yielded a fairly high correlation (r
similar iines, we must emphasize that, at best, our = .75), our fears appear to have been exaggerated. This
sample of 146 ads represents prime-time commercials establishes the convergent validity of the zipping and
aired in the northeastern United States during the late zapping measures and the reliability of the viewing-
1980s. Further, we cannot be sure that ads scoring time index based on their combination. Support for
higher or lower in Facts versus Feelings 'Hid not differ the nomological validity of this index appears in the
in some other way (such as local vs. national origin or strong ability of the separately judged measures of ad
high vs. low frequency of prior exposure). Replications content, emotions, and attitude toward the ad to ex-
EFFECTS ON VIEWING TIME 451

plain convincingly the variance in Viewing Time in a we believe the accumulated history of experience with
sound theoretical framework (overall R2 = .67). this approach suggests that it might prove useful in
It appears that advertising content does explain other consumer-research contexts, such as those in-
Viewing Time (R 2 = .54) and that these effects are volving brand choice, product usage, or the meanings
partially mediated by the emotional dimensions and of possessions, especially if its results can be satisfac-
the components of attitude toward the ad via two torily replicated in more conventional across-subjects
primary "routes" to viewing time: (1) Feelings applications.
and Uniqueness/Uniqueness Squared - Pleasure - Finally, our analyses have added to the growing body
Hedonism - Viewing Time; (2) Uniqueness - of knowledge about the hierarchical chain of effects
Arousal - Interestingness - Viewing Time. However, from advertising content through emotional responses
beyond these mediating effects, there remains a sig- and ad attitude to actual viewing behavior. We found
nificant direct contribution of ad content to the ex- that, for a reasonably representative set of commer-
planation of Viewing Time. This remaining direct ef- cials, the emotional dimensions, attitudinal compo-
fect comes, in part, from the hypothesized nonmon- nents, and viewing behavior could be moderately well
otonic influence of Uniqueness and Uniqueness explained by various aspects of advertising content.
Squared, but it also depends on an exploratory finding In particular, in findings reminiscent of the aforemen-
concerning the negative contribution of Facts and the tioned copy-testing literature, we have seen that the
positive contribution of Feelings. Though not explic- uniqueness of ads can be meaningfully assessed by
itly hypothesized and subject to the aforementioned content judges and that these ratings do, in fact, pro-
limitations concerning potential competing explana- vide useful information for predicting emotional re-
tions, it appears throughout our findings that ads ap- sponses, attitude toward the ad, and viewing behavior.
pealing to feelings exert positive direct influences on That is, judged measures of Uniqueness significantly
Pleasure, Hedonism, and Viewing Time, even when explain additional variance in Mehrabian and Russell's
other mediating variables are controlled for. This result Pleasure and Arousal dimensions, in the attitudinal
suggests that people attend selectively to positively va- components of Hedonism, Utilitarianism, and Inter-
lenced messages that make them feel good. Meanwhile, estingness, and in Viewing Time. Further, where ex-
factual appeals contribute negatively to Hedonism, but pected, Uniqueness demonstrated an inverted-U-
exert a direct positive effect on Utilitarianism and a shaped relationship with the dependent variables of
direct negative effect on Viewing Time-again, with interest, peaking well within the range of our data and
the appropriate controls for intervening variables. thereby replicating in a consumer-behavior context the
These effects of Facts and Feelings-the apparent ten- work of Berlyne and his colleagues, who have generally
dency of Facts to discourage while Feelings entice focused on manipulating experimental stimuli in
viewing behavior-clearly merit further investigation. carefully controlled laboratory settings rather than
In further specific findings, our investigation of the measuring the novelty aspects of messages drawn from
attitudinal components supports the notion of a the real world.
multidimensional construct in which Viewing Time
depends on such key influences as Hedonism, Utili- Summary
tarianism, and Interestingness. As mentioned, prior
research on attitude toward the ad has tended to use In sum, this study has developed and tested a hier-
indices that aggregate over, and thus fail to detect, the archical model of advertising effects on viewing time.
individually varying effects of these different compo- It has placed primary emphasis on the attempt to ex-
nents. Only through dis aggregate analyses can we find plain a simulated behavioral measure of zipping and
(as here) that Hedonism and Interestingness, but not zapping responses to television commercials. In ad-
Utilitarianism, are the major influences on Viewing dition, the study has demonstrated a chain of effects
Time. from the content of television ads, through emotional
Our study once again demonstrates the feasibility reactions and attitude toward the ad, to these actual
of content analysis in studying communications phe- viewing behaviors.
nomena across objects (i.e., messages) to supplement
findings from analyses conducted across subjects (i.e., [Received May 1989. Revised July 1990.]
receivers). Based on common copy-testing applications
(Schlinger 1979; Wells 1964; Wells et al. 1971), this REFERENCES
approach has now achieved a relatively strong track
Aaker, David A. and Donald E. Bruzzone (1985), "Causes
record in regard to print ads (Holbrook and Lehmann
ofIrritation in Advertising," Journal 0/ Marketing, 49
1980), television commercials (Holbrook and Batra (2),47-57.
1987; Pechmann and Stewart 1985; Stewart and Furse - - - , Douglas M. Stayman, and Michael R. Hagerty
1985, 1986), and consumption experiences (Havlena (1986), "Warmth in Advertising: Measurement, Impact
1985; Havlena and Holbrook 1986). Keeping in mind and Sequence Effects," Journal o/Consumer Research,
the aforementioned caveats by Edell and Burke (1987), 12 (March), 365-381.
452 JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

- - - , Douglas M. Stayman, and Richard Vezina (1988), Goldberg, Marvin E. and Gerald J. Gorn (1987), "Happy
"Identifying Feelings Elicited by Advertising," Psy- and Sad TV Programs: How They Affect Reactions to
chology and Marketing, 5 (Spring), 1-16. Commercials," Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (De-
Anderson, Daniel R. (1985), "Online Cognitive Processing cember), 387-403.
of Television," in Psychological Processes and Adver- Havlena, William J. (1985), "The Varieties of Consumption
tising Effects, ed. Linda F. Alwitt and Andrew A. Experience," unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Graduate
Mitchell, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 177-199. School of Business, Columbia University, New York,
Baker, William, J. Wesley Hutchison, Danny Moore, and NY 10027.
Prakash Nedungadi (1986), "Brand Familiarity and - - - and Morris B. Holbrook (1986), "The Varieties of
Advertising: Effects on the Evoked Set and Preference," Consumption Experience: Comparing Two Typologies
in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 13, ed. Richard of Emotion in Consumer Behavior," Journal of Con-
J. Lutz, Ann Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer Re- sumer Research, 13 (December), 394-404.
search, 637-642. Heeter, Carrie and Bradley S. Greenberg (1985), "Profiling
Baron, Reuben M. and David A. Kenny (1986), "The Mod- the Zappers," Journal of Advertising Research, 25
erator-Mediator Variable Distinction in Social Psycho- (April/May), 15-19.
logical Research: Conceptual, Strategic, and Statistical Holbrook, Morris B. (1977), "More on Content Analysis in
Considerations," Journal of Personality and Social Consumer Research," Journal of Consumer Research,
Psychology, 51 (December), 1173-1182. 4 (December), 176-177.
Batra, Rajeev and Olli T. Ahtola (1991), "The Measurement - - - (1986), "Emotion in the Consumption Experience:
and Role of Utilitarian and Hedonic Attitudes," Mar- Toward a New Model of the Human Consumer," in
keting Letters, in press. The Role of Affect in Consumer Behavior: Emerging
- - - and Morris B. Holbrook (1990), "Developing a Ty- Theories and Applications, ed. R. A. Peterson et aI.,
pology of Affective Responses to Advertising: A Test of Lexington, MA: Heath, 17-52.
Reliability and Validity," Psychology and Marketing, - - - and Rajeev Batra (1987), "Assessing the Role of
7 (1), 11-25. Emotions as Mediators of Consumer Responses to Ad-
- - - and Michael L. Ray (1986), "Affective Responses vertising," Journal of Consumer Research, 14 (Decem-
Mediating Acceptance of Advertising," Journal of Con- ber),404-420.
sumer Research, 13 (September), 234-249. - - - , Robert W. Chestnut, Terence A. Oliva, and Eric A.
Berlyne, Daniel E. (1958), "The Influence of Complexity Greenleaf (1984), "Playas a Consumption Experience:
and Novelty in Visual Figures on Orienting Responses," The Roles of Emotions, Performance, and Personality
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55 (3), 289-296. in the Enjoyment of Games," Journal of Consumer Re-
- - - (1960), Conflict, Arousal and Curiosity, New York: search, 11 (September), 728-739.
McGraw-Hill. - - - and Donald R. Lehmann (1980), "Form versus Con-
- - - (1963), "Complexity and Incongruity Variables as tent in Predicting Starch Scores," Journal ofAdvertising
Determinants of Exploratory Choice," Canadian Jour- Research, 20 (4),53-62.
nal of Psychology, 17 (3), 274-290. - - - and John O'Shaughnessy (1984), "The Role ofEmo-
- - - (1971), Aesthetics and Psychobiology, New York: tion in Advertising," Psychology and Marketing, 1
Appleton-Century-Crofts. (Summer), 45-64.
Bettman, James R. (1979), An Information Processing The- Holsti, Ole R. (1969), Content Analysis for the Social Sci-
ory of Consumer Choice, Reading, MA: Addison-Wes- ences and Humanities, Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
ley. IRI Information Resources (1983), "Viewer Zapping of TV
Burke, Marian C. and Julie A. Edell (1986), "Ad Reactions Commercials; Pay TV Audience Accumulation over
over Time: Capturing Changes in the Real World," Time; Efficiently Reaching Heavy Grocery Buyers,"
Journal of Consumer Research, 13 (June), 114-118. proprietary study, IRI, 520 North Dearborn, Chicago,
Christ, William G. (1985), "Voter Preference and Emotion: IL60610.
Using Emotional Response to Classify Decided and - - - (1985), "Network Audience Loss during Commercial
Undecided Voters," Journal ofApplied Social Psychol- Breaks," proprietary study, IRI, 520 North Dearborn,
ogy, 15 (3), 237-254. Chicago, IL 60610.
- - - and Thompson Biggers (1984), "An Exploratory In- Izard, Carroll E. (1977), Human Emotions, New York:
vestigation into the Relationship between Television Plenum.
Program Preference and Emotion-eliciting Qualities- Kahneman, Daniel (1973), Attention and Effort, Englewood
a New Theoretical Perspective," Western Journal of Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Speech Communication, 48 (Summer), 293-307. Kaplan, Barry M. (1985), "Zapping-the Real Issue Is
Donovan, Robert J. and John R. Rossiter (1982), "Store Communication," Journal of Advertising Research, 25
Atmosphere: An Environmental Psychology Ap- (April/May), 9-13.
proach," Journal of Retailing, 58 (1), 34-57. Kassarjian, Harold H. (1977), "Content Analysis in Con-
Edell, Julie A. and Marian C. Burke (1987), "The Power of sumer Research," Journal of Consumer Research, 4
Feelings in Understanding Advertising Effects," Journal (June), 8-18.
of Consumer Research, 14 (December), 421-433. Krippendorf, Klaus (1980), Content Analysis: An Introduc-
Gardner, Meryl Paula (1985), "Does Attitude toward the tion to Its Methodology, Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Ad Affect Brand Attitude under a Brand Evaluation Leckart, Bruce T. (1966), "Looking Time: The Effects of
Set?" Journal of Marketing Research, 22 (May), 192- Stimulus Complexity and Familiarity," Perception and
198. Psychophysics, 1 (5), 142-144.
EFFECTS ON VIEWING TIME 453

- - - and Paul Bakan (1965), "Complexity Judgments of per No. 85-112, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN
Photographs and Looking Time," Perceptual and Motor 37203.
Skills, 21 (1), 16-18. Pollay, Richard W., Judy Zaichkowsky, and Christina Fryer
- - - and Paul Bakan (1969), "Reliability of Looking (1980), "Regulation Hasn't Changed TV Ads Much!"
Time," Perception and Psychophysics, 5 (5), 313-314. Journalism Quarterly, 57 (3), 438-446.
Lutz, Richard J., Scott B. MacKenzie, and George E. Belch Preston, Ivan L. (1982), "The Association Model of the Ad-
(1983), "Attitude toward the Ad as a Mediator of Ad- vertising Communication Process," Journal of Adver-
vertising Effectiveness: Determinants and Conse- tising, 11 (2), 3-15.
quences," in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 10, Resnik, Alan and Bruce L. Stern (1977), "An Analysis of
ed. Richard P. Bagozzi and Alice M. Tybout, Ann Arbor, Information Content in Television Advertising," Jour-
MI: Association for Consumer Research, 532-539. nal of Marketing, 41 (January), 50-53.
MacKenzie, Scott B. and Richard J. Lutz (1989), "An Em- Russell, James A. (1980), "A Circumplex Model of Affect,"
pirical Examination of the Structural Antecedents of Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39 (6),
Attitude toward the Ad in an Advertising Pretesting 1161-1178.
Context," Journal of Marketing, 53 (April), 48-65. - - - , Anna Weiss, and Gerald A. Mendelsohn (1989),
- - - , Richard J. Lutz, and George E. Belch (1986), "Role "Affect Grid: A Single-Item Scale of Pleasure and
of Attitude toward the Ad as a Mediator of Advertising Arousal," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Effectiveness: A Test of Competing Explanations," 57 (3), 493-502.
Journal of Marketing Research, 23 (May), 130-143. Schlinger, Mary Jayne (1979), "A Profile of Responses to
Madden, Thomas J., Chris T. Allen, and Jacquelyn L. Twible Commercials," Journal ofAdvertising Research, 19 (2),
(1988), "Attitude toward the Ad: An Assessment ofDi- 37-46.
verse Measurement Indices under Different Processing Shimp, Terence A. (1981), "Attitude toward the Ad as a
'Sets,' " Journal of Marketing Research, 25 (August), Mediator of Consumer Choice," Journal ofAdvertising,
242-252. 10 (2),9-15.
Mandler, George (1982), "The Structure of Value: Ac- Stewart, David W. and David H. Furse (1984), "Analysis of
counting for Taste," in Affect and Cognition, ed. Mar- the Impact of Executional Factors on Advertising Per-
garet Sydnor Clark and Susan T. Fiske, Hillsdale, NJ: formance," Journal ofAdvertising Research, 24 (6), 23-
Erlbaum. 26.
Marquez, F. T. (1977), "Advertising Content: Persuasion, - - - and David H. Furse (1985), "Effects of Television
Information or Intimidation?" Journalism Quarterly, Advertising Execution on Recall, Comprehension, and
54 (3), 482-491. Persuasion," Psychology and Marketing, 2 (Fall), 135-
Mehrabian, Albert and James A. Russell (1974), An Ap- 160.
proach to Environmental Psychology, Cambridge, MA: - - - and David H. Furse (1986), Effective Television Ad-
MIT Press. vertising, Lexington, MA: Heath.
Mitchell, Andrew A. (1986a), "Some Issues Surrounding Thorson, Esther, Maria Papas Heide, and Thomas Page
Research on the Effects of 'Feeling Advertisements,' " (1987), "Prediction of Viewer Response Profiles from
in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 13, ed. Richard Executional Aspects of Commercials," unpublished
J. Lutz, Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, working paper, School of Journalism and Mass Com-
623-628. munication, University of Wisconsin-Madison, 53706.
- - - (1986b), "Effect of Verbal and Visual Components
of Advertisements on Brand Attitudes and Attitude to- Weber, Robert Philip (1985), Basic Content Analysis, Bev-
ward the Advertisement," Journal of Consumer Re- erly Hills, CA: Sage.
search, 13 (June), 12-24. Wells, William D. (1964), "EQ, Son ofEQ, and the Reaction
- - - and Jerry C. Olson (1981), "Are Product Attribute Profile," Journal of Marketing, 28 (4), 45-52.
Beliefs the Only Mediator of Advertising Effects on - - - , Clark Leavitt, and Maureen McConville (1971), "A
Brand Attitude?" Journal of Marketing Research, 18 Reaction Profile for TV Commercials," Journal of Ad-
(August), 318-332. vertising Research, II (December), 11-17.
Morrison, Bruce J. and M. J. Dainoff(1972), "Advertisement Westbrook, Robert A. (1987), "Product/Consumption-
Complexity and Looking Time," Journal of Marketing based Affective Responses and Postpurchase Process,"
Research, 9 (November), 396-400. Journal of Marketing Research, 24 (August), 258-270.
Pechmann, Connie and David W. Stewart (1985), "The Yorke, David A. and Philip J. Kitchen (1985), "Channel
Multidimensionality of Persuasive Communications: Flickers and Video Speeders," Journal of Advertising
Theoretical and Empirical Foundations," Working Pa- Research, 25 (April/May), 21-25.

You might also like