You are on page 1of 85

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/301696606

Writer-Reader Contagion of Inspiration and Related States: Conditional Process


Analyses Within a Cross-Classified Writer × Reader Framework

Article in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology · April 2016


DOI: 10.1037/pspp0000094

CITATIONS READS

47 1,153

5 authors, including:

Todd M Thrash Victoria C. Oleynick


William & Mary University of Minnesota Twin Cities
56 PUBLICATIONS 8,062 CITATIONS 4 PUBLICATIONS 363 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

William Belzak
Duolingo
16 PUBLICATIONS 229 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Todd M Thrash on 30 April 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Running head: WRITER-READER CONTAGION 1

Writer-reader contagion of inspiration and related states:

Conditional process analyses within a cross-classified writer×reader framework

Todd M. Thrash

College of William and Mary

Laura A. Maruskin

University of California, Berkeley

Emil G. Moldovan

Northeastern University

Victoria C. Oleynick

University of Minnesota

Will C. Belzak

College of William and Mary

This copy is not the copy of record. For the published version, see:

Thrash, T. M., Maruskin, L. A., Moldovan, E. G., Oleynick, V. C., & Belzak, W. C. (2016, April
28). Writer–reader contagion of inspiration and related states: Conditional process analyses
within a cross-classified writer × reader framework. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000094
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 2

Abstract

A longstanding tradition in the humanities holds that a writer’s inspiration is infectious, but this

thesis has not been tested. We hypothesized that (a) inspiration is infectious, such that inspired

writers are more inspiring to the average reader, (b) contagion is mediated by the insightfulness

of the text, and (c) contagion is moderated by readers’ openness to experience, such that open

readers are more prone to contagion. To test these hypotheses, a sample of 195 student writers,

each of whom wrote one poem, was crossed with a sample of 220 student readers, who read all

poems. Data were available for 36,020 cells of the resulting writer×reader matrix. Our analytic

approach integrated cross-classified multilevel modeling with conditional process analysis. As

hypothesized, writers who were more inspired elicited higher levels of inspiration in the average

reader. Inspiration contagion was mediated by the insightfulness and pleasantness of the text and

was partially suppressed by originality. Inspiration contagion was moderated by reader openness.

Moderated mediation analyses indicated that open readers were prone to contagion because they

were tolerant of the originality and sublimity of inspired writing. Additional analyses

differentiated contagion of inspiration from contagion of its covariates (awe, positive affect),

documented effects of writer inspiration on reader enthrallment (awe, chills), and showed that

writer effort is a poor predictor of reader states. The infectiousness of inspiration—through

poetry, if not also through scripture and academic writing—suggests that a given instance of

inspiration may have far-reaching cultural implications, including dissemination of innovations

and ideologies.

Keywords: inspiration, contagion, openness to experience, cultural transmission, authenticity


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 3

The idea that a culture’s greatest writings are products of inspiration, a state of

heightened creative activity, has been a perennial thesis throughout the history of Western

scholarship (Clark, 1997). An even more provocative version of this thesis holds that the writer’s

inspiration is infectious, such that inspired texts inspire their readers. Plato posited that

inspiration is transmitted from the Muse, to poets, to rhapsodes (performers), and finally, to

audiences (Plato, 2012). Longinus (1890), writing in approximately the first century AD, argued

that sublime writing arises during moments of inspiration and, in turn, enthralls and inspires its

readers. Judeo-Christian theology holds that God’s revelation inspires prophets and apostles to

write scripture, which, in turn, produces a state of illumination in readers (Geisler & Nix, 1986).

Romantic poet Edward Young (1759/1918) suggested that genius inspires and is itself inspired.

Fellow Romantic Shelley (1977) portrayed inspiration in the writer and inspiration in the reader

as so intertwined that his descriptions blur the distinction between them (Clark, 1997).

In spite of the far-reaching implications of this venerable intellectual tradition, inspiration

contagion has not yet been the subject of empirical investigation. In this study, we conduct the

first test of writer-reader contagion of inspiration and related states, including awe and positive

affect. We also investigate the qualities of a text that mediate contagion of each state and the

traits of readers that moderate contagion effects. In the following, we identify obstacles that have

precluded study of inspiration contagion to date, as well as our strategies for overcoming them.

Obstacles to the study of writer-reader inspiration contagion

The first obstacle, ironically, is the unusually long history of the inspiration concept,

which brings connotations of muses and Romantic hyperbole (e.g., effortless creation). The

typical reaction among 20th century scholars was to dismiss the topic on the basis of these

associations. Recently, however, inspiration has undergone a renaissance in literary theory


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 4

(Clark, 1997; Roe & Stanco, 2007) and psychological science (Jones, Dodd, & Gruber, 2014;

Milyavskaya, Ianakieva, Foxen-Craft, Colantuoni, & Koestner, 2012; Thrash & Elliot, 2003).

The catalyst in both fields was a recognition that descriptions of inspiration have been

sufficiently consistent across history (Clark, 1997) or disciplines (Thrash & Elliot, 2003) that a

scholarly treatment demands serious consideration of these consistencies. In fact, noting trends in

literary theory, Clark (1997) concluded that inspiration is “the oldest and the most contemporary

theory of the genesis of the poetic” (p. 282). We employ a contemporary, empirically validated

conceptualization of inspiration (Thrash & Elliot, 2003, 2004) with the aim of documenting the

continuity of inspiration, from the genesis of the poetic to its reception.

Second, the inspiration contagion narrative has been largely overshadowed by a narrative

of perspiration. In defiance of Romantic conceptions of the inspired poet, Edgar Allen Poe

(1846) famously portrayed writing as a matter of cold calculation. Poet Paul Valéry (1958, 2007)

retained inspiration in the reader as the objective of the poet, but he argued that this end is

achieved primarily through hard work rather than inspiration (see also Fehrman, 1980). Within

the sciences, Martindale (2001) and Sawyer (2006) suggested that no theorist disputes Edison’s

claim that genius is 1% inspiration and 99% perspiration, and Martindale (1989) stated that “the

1% versus 99% partitioning of the ‘variance’ in creativity is probably close to the mark” (p. 213).

Invariably, such claims are not accompanied by references to studies in which inspiration was

assessed. In the present research, we treat inspiration contagion and Valéry’s rival account as

hypotheses subject to empirical test.

Finally, the far-reaching topic of inspiration contagion has lacked an intellectual home,

because it does not fit within the boundaries of contemporary academic literatures. Scientists

have generally invoked ad hoc, domain-specific conceptualizations of inspiration as a creative


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 5

(e.g., Martindale & Hasenfus, 1978) or interpersonal (e.g., Lockwood & Kunda, 1997) process,

and therefore the commensurability and continuity of inspiration across writers and readers has

not been apparent. This problem of conceptual fragmentation has been compounded by pressures

toward methodological specialization. Wimsatt and Beardsley (1946, 1949), champions of the

influential New Criticism, argued that analysis of a text cannot support valid inferences about

inspiration or other states in the writer or reader. The solution advocated by these theorists was to

constrict the focus of literary analysis to objective features of the text itself, relegating the study

of writer or reader states to other specialized literatures (e.g., reader response theory).

Unfortunately, these literatures have remained largely isolated, apparently because the integrated

study of the writer, text, and reader poses unwieldy methodological challenges. Perhaps the

greatest impediment to the study of inspiration contagion has been the boundary between the

humanities and the sciences, which separates theories of inspiration contagion, dating back to

Plato, from the quantitative methods and models needed to test them. In the following, we

present integrative solutions to these problems of conceptual, methodological, and statistical

fragmentation.

Integrative conceptualization, study design, and statistical model

Integrative conceptualization of inspiration. Thrash and Elliot (2003, 2004) have

argued that diverse forms of inspiration (e.g., spiritual, creative, interpersonal) share common

features. These researchers conceptualized inspiration as an epistemic-motivational episode

involving two component processes: being inspired by and being inspired to. Being inspired by is

an epistemic process in which one is awoken to new or better possibilities. This process may

occur spontaneously during a “eureka” moment or may be stimulated by the environment. Being

inspired to is a motivational process in which one feels compelled to bring one’s new vision into
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 6

fruition. Although dissociable, these processes tend to co-occur (Thrash & Elliot, 2004).

Inspiration has been theorized to serve the function of motivating transmission (e.g., articulation,

actualization, or extension) of ideas appraised as intrinsically valuable (Thrash & Elliot, 2004;

Thrash, Moldovan, Oleynick, & Maruskin, 2014).

This general conceptualization is applicable to both the writing and reading processes.

Applied to the writing process, or the creative process more generally, inspiration involves an

impulse to actualize an idea in the form of a concrete product. Studies have shown that

inspiration predicts indicators of creative output, including receipt of patents (Thrash & Elliot,

2003) and ratings of the creativity of poetry, fiction, and scientific essays (Thrash, Maruskin,

Cassidy, Fryer, & Ryan, 2010). Consistent with the posited transmission function, creative

ideation precedes inspiration, which, in turn, predicts the creativity of the resulting product

(Thrash, Maruskin, et al., 2010). Writers who are more inspired report that their ideas came to

them more fully formed; they write more efficiently; and they use shorter words—all suggesting

a swift articulation of ideas while they are fresh in the mind’s eye (Thrash, Maruskin, et al.,

2010). Inspiration is also applicable to the reading process but has not been studied in this

context. In this case, the written word is illuminating and awakens a desire to express or embody

this new epistemic awareness.

The transmission function of inspiration should be distinguished from the inspiration

contagion process investigated herein. In the transmission process, inspiration is a mediating

(intervening) variable that explains the transmission of perceived intrinsic value by a single

individual, such as a writer (idea writer inspiration text) or reader (text reader

inspiration future goal). In the contagion process, writer and reader inspiration serve as

independent and dependent variables, respectively, mediated by qualities of the text (writer
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 7

inspiration text reader inspiration). Thus, the inspiration contagion process complements

and extends the transmission function of inspiration, such that a given experience of inspiration

has the potential to spark reverberations in others via the written word, much as Plato, Longinus,

and others argued.

Integrative study design. Empirical study of writer-reader contagion requires not only

an integrative conceptualization of inspiration, but also an integrative study design that allows

data about the writing process, text, and reading process to be linked. Our strategy was to use the

poems written in a previous study of inspiration and the writing process (Thrash, Maruskin et al.,

2010, Study 3) as stimuli in a new study of inspiration and reader response. Our paradigm

involves fully crossing participants from the two samples (“writers” and “readers”), such that

each reader is asked to respond to each writer’s poem. Both samples consist of undergraduate

students. Poem characteristics are measured using the consensual assessment technique

(Amabile, 1996), in which a panel of judges rates each poem on multiple dimensions. Merging

the data sets yields the writer×reader data structure illustrated in Figure 1.

We acknowledge that crossing a sample of student writers with a sample of student

readers cannot be expected to yield the kinds of historically important inspiration episodes

discussed by Plato, Longinus, and other theorists who have posited inspiration contagion.

Nevertheless, our strategy allows the unwieldy topic of writer-reader inspiration contagion to be

broached using rigorous scientific methods. Strengths of this design include (a) sampling of

writers, poems, and readers, rather than use of hand-picked exemplars; (b) multidimensional

assessment of poem characteristics; (c) experimental assignment of readers to poems and the

writers “behind” them; and (d) direct, valid, and commensurate assessment of writer and reader

inspiration and known covariates. Moreover, we note that our hypotheses do not hinge on the
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 8

literary sophistication of the writer and reader samples. Once romanticized as the province of the

genius, inspiration is now understood to be consequential in the daily lives of all individuals

(Laski, 1961; Thrash & Elliot, 2003).

Integrative statistical model. Crossing a sample of readers with a sample of writers (or,

equivalently, with the corresponding sample of poems1) poses statistical challenges. Responses

from the same reader to different poems are non-independent due to nesting within readers;

similarly, responses to the same poem from different readers are non-independent due to nesting

within poems. These sources of non-independence must be modeled in order to avoid violation

of the independence assumption. A related consideration is that the particular readers and poems

under investigation are arbitrary samples rather than exhaustive populations. Accordingly, both

readers and poems must be modeled as random effects in order to permit generalization to their

respective populations (Hoffman & Rovine, 2007). Crossed patterns of non-independence and

crossed random effects may be modeled appropriately using a modeling technique called cross-

classified (or “mixed effects”) multilevel modeling (Judd, Westfall, & Kenny, 2012; Raudenbush

& Bryk, 2002).

Although the cross-classified multilevel model offers a rigorous way to analyze

reader×poem (“person×situation”) data, it is a univariate technique—that is, it accommodates

only a single dependent variable. Accordingly, it does not provide a natural way to accommodate

distal effects of writer states on poem characteristics, nor indirect effects of writer states on

reader states via poem characteristics (i.e., mediation). Fortunately, recent developments allow

cross-classified modeling to be integrated with the multivariate technique of structural equation

modeling (Asparouhov & Muthén, in press) and hence with conditional process analysis—that is,

analysis of mediation, moderation, and moderated mediation (Hayes, 2013). Next we present our
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 9

hypotheses, which were guided by our methodological-statistical framework and by relevant

theory, thus illustrating methodological-substantive synergy (Marsh & Hau, 2007).

Hypotheses

Writer-reader contagion of inspiration. From the perspective offered by our

writer×reader framework, a statistical test of contagion minimally requires a single reader. In this

case, contagion is present if writers who are more inspired evoke higher levels of inspiration in

that single reader. However, because reader response consists of general and idiosyncratic

components (Fish, 1970), we frame our contagion hypothesis in terms of the average effect

across multiple readers. Thus our contagion hypothesis specifies that, on average across readers,

writer inspiration has a positive effect on reader inspiration.

Mediation. We further posited a mediating process that explains how contagion occurs.

Specifically, we hypothesized that contagion is mediated by the insightfulness of the text, such

that writers who are more inspired write texts that reveal greater insight (as evaluated

independently of the writers’ and readers’ perceptions); and texts that are more insightful, in

turn, evoke higher levels of inspiration in the average reader. Our rationale is that an insightful

text serves as both the output of the epistemic transmission process in the writer and as the input

for an epistemic transmission process in the reader.

Insightfulness is not, in theory, the only intrinsically valuable quality that may be

transmitted by inspiration. However, insightfulness is particularly relevant to writer-reader

contagion because of its symmetric significance to writer and reader, a quality that we label

transitivity. For sake of discrimination, we also examine a related quality—originality—that was

expected not to mediate contagion because of its asymmetric significance. Although writer

inspiration is known to facilitate writers’ actualization (transmission) of original ideas (Thrash,


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 10

Maruskin et al., 2010), originality is unlikely to facilitate inspiration in the average reader,

because original ideas are regarded as belonging to their author and not their audience (Bennett,

2005). The problem of ownership does not apply to an insightful text, which casts light upon

(perceived) truths that transcend authorship.

Our theorizing regarding the problem of ownership is informed by Plato’s model of

contagion. The reason inspired writing was itself inspiring in Plato’s model was that the writer

was not the origin of ideas, but rather the mediator of the eternal wisdom of the Muse. The

Muse—daughter of Mnemosyne (memory)—may be understood as a symbol for the collective

memory of many generations of orator-poets in a predominantly oral culture (Clark, 1997). Thus

the writer who catches a glimpse of eternal wisdom and serves as its transmitter invites the

reader into a comparable state conducive to further inspired transmission.

Moderation. In positing writer-reader continuity of inspiration, we do not mean to imply

that inspiration spreads indiscriminately. Some readers may be more receptive than others.

Accordingly, we test whether contagion is moderated by (conditional upon) reader personality.

Openness to experience from the Five Factor Model (McCrae & Costa, 1997) has been

identified as the key trait that predicts proneness to inspiration (Thrash & Elliot, 2003, 2004;

Thrash, Maruskin, et al., 2010). We hypothesized that contagion is moderated by reader

openness, such that the effect of writer inspiration on reader inspiration is greater for readers

higher in openness. This hypothesis is consistent with Plato’s portrayal of poets and rhapsodes—

paragons of openness—as more receptive than the general public to inspiration from the Muse.

Contagion of related states. Although not yet investigated, it is likely that writer-reader

contagion occurs for a variety of emotions and basic affective states beyond inspiration. To

broaden the scope of our research, and to test whether the theorized mediator and moderator are
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 11

distinctively applicable to inspiration per se, we also examine contagion of two covariates—awe

and activated positive affect (PA)—that overlap substantially with inspiration but that represent

alternative theoretical traditions. Whereas the inspiration construct emerged from the human

motivation literature, the awe and PA constructs represent the discrete emotion and dimensional

affect literatures, respectively (Thrash, Moldovan, Oleynick et al., 2014).

Awe is an emotion that arises from the challenge of accommodating vast stimuli (Keltner

& Haidt, 2003; Shiota, Keltner, & Mossman, 2007). Romantic expression theories provide

grounds for positing contagion of awe and emotions more generally (Collingwood, 1938;

Tolstoy, 1897/2014; for related theory and research, see Oatley, 2003; Robinson, 1995). We

hypothesized that contagion of awe is mediated not by insightfulness, which implies ease of

accommodation, but by sublimity—a grand, powerful, exalting quality of a stimulus that

overwhelms one’s capacity for rational comprehension (Longinus, 1890). The sublime has long

been identified as the natural object of awe and related emotions (e.g., astonishment, reverence;

Burke, 1759; Longinus, 1890; Kant, 1764/1960; Konečni, 2008). Moreover, the concept of the

sublime was originally applied to written language (Longinus, 1890), which is arguably its most

potent elicitor (Burke, 1759). On the basis of past findings (Shiota, Keltner, & John, 2006; see

also recent research by Silvia, Fayn, Nusbaum, & Beaty, in press), we hypothesized that awe

contagion, like inspiration contagion, is moderated by reader openness.

PA is a basic affective state involving positive valence and arousal (Watson, Wiese,

Vaidya, Tellegen, 1999). The possibility of writer-reader contagion of PA is suggested by

evidence that affect-laden language spreads across social networks (Kramer, Guillory, &

Hancock, 2014). We hypothesized that PA contagion is mediated by the pleasantness of the text

and moderated by reader approach temperament, a sensitivity to positive incentives (Elliot &
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 12

Thrash, 2010). For thoroughness, we also test parallel hypotheses regarding negative affect

(NA), low pleasantness (unpleasantness), and avoidance temperament (Elliot & Thrash, 2010).

Although most relevant to PA and NA, pleasantness was also expected to contribute to contagion

of inspiration and awe, given the positive valence of these states.

Other relations between writer and reader states. Moving beyond contagion, we also

investigate alternative consequences of writer inspiration and alternative antecedents of reader

inspiration. Regarding the former, we hypothesized that writer inspiration predicts two indicators

of reader enthrallment: awe and chills (Konečni, 2008; Maruskin, Thrash, & Elliot, 2012; Silvia

& Nusbaum, 2011). We conceptualize enthrallment states as involving deep emotional

resonance, a focus on the evocative object, and (in contrast to inspiration) little immediate

motivational impetus. Because inspiration in a writer is theorized to transmit some admirable

qualities that do not lend themselves to further transmission—such as originality (Thrash,

Maruskin, et al., 2010) and sublimity (Longinus, 1890)—it may enthrall as well as inspire.

Indeed, Bowra (1951) and Lembke (1973) have previously argued that inspired writing evokes

both inspiration and chills.

Regarding alternative antecedents of reader inspiration, we test Valéry’s hypothesis that

reader inspiration is primarily a result of writer effort rather than writer inspiration. Although

writer effort is likely to contribute to the production of inspiring texts, we are skeptical of

reactionary, post-Romantic theorizing in which effort rather than inspiration is held to be the

process through which inspiring texts are produced. Indeed, Thrash, Maruskin et al. (2010) found

that inspiration and effort during the writing of fictional stories were related to the total number

of words typed and deleted, respectively. This finding suggests that inspiration and effort play

complementary roles in the writing process. Inspiration may also provide motivational support
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 13

for effort exertion (Thrash, Moldovan, Oleynick et al., 2014), a possibility that Valéry

acknowledged in his more nuanced writings (Valéry, 2007).

Method

In the following, we present methods concerning both the writer and reader data

collections. Methods and data concerning writer states and evaluative coding of poems, as well

as data concerning the relations between writer states and evaluative coding, have been published

previously (Study 3 of Thrash, Maruskin et al., 2010). Methods and data concerning reader

states, reader traits, and appraisal coding of the poems, as well as their relations to writer states

and evaluative coding, have not been published previously.

Participants

Writer sample. The writer sample consisted of 195 undergraduates (96 male, 99 female)

who participated in return for credit toward a research participation requirement in an

introductory psychology course. Ethnicity was distributed as follows: African American, 9.2%;

Asian, 4.6%; Caucasian, 80.0%; Hispanic, 2.6%; Native American, .5%; Other, 3.1%. One

additional participant had begun the study but quit prior to beginning the writing process

questionnaire because his or her English was too poor to understand it.

Reader sample. The reader sample consisted of 220 undergraduates (66 male, 154

female) who participated in return for credit toward a research participation requirement in a

course on personality and poetry. As an additional incentive, participants were offered feedback

about their scores upon completion of the study. Ethnicity was distributed as follows: African

American, 7.7%; Asian, 10.0%; Caucasian, 69.1%; Hispanic, 6.4%; Native American, .5%;

Other, 6.4%. Seven additional participants had begun the study but failed to complete the

personality questionnaire or failed to complete any poem questionnaires, and therefore data from
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 14

these participants were dropped prior to analysis. The writer and reader samples attended the

same competitive university, with a 5-year separation between data collections.

Procedure

Writer sample. Participants in the writer sample attended individual lab sessions.

Participants first completed a background questionnaire. They were then given 30 minutes to

write a poem about the human condition using a word processor, with additional time granted

upon request. Finally, participants completed a questionnaire regarding inspiration and other

states during particular stages of the writing process.

Reader sample. Participants in the reader sample attended a preliminary orientation

session and completed personality questionnaires near the beginning of the semester. Throughout

the remainder of the semester, they were asked to complete a series of online poem

questionnaires at times of their choosing in a private location. At the beginning of each poem

questionnaire, a poem was presented, and participants then completed measures concerning their

responses to the poem. Poems were presented in a different random order for each reader.

Following preliminary cleaning in which participant errors in entry of identifiers were

reconciled, the poem questionnaire data file consisted of 41,397 cases. The data file was then

cleaned by removing cases that met any of the following criteria: (a) no identifying information

was provided; (b) the data came from an individual who had neither provided consent nor

completed the personality questionnaires; (c) the participant responded affirmatively to a

question asking if he or she would prefer to redo the questionnaire on a later occasion due to

distraction or other factors that may invalidate the data; (d) the submission duplicated another

submission; (e) time stamps revealed that the participant spent less than two minutes on the

poem questionnaire (this cutoff was the nadir of a bimodal distribution); or (f) the data concerned
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 15

a poem that had been included in error (this poem was written by the writing study participant

whose English was too poor for him or her to fill out the writing process questionnaire). The

final, cleaned data file consisted of 36,020 poem questionnaires. In this final data set, the number

of poem questionnaires per reader ranged from 1 to 195 with a median of 193.

Measures

Writer states. Consistent with Thrash, Maruskin et al. (2010), for a given writer state

(e.g., inspiration), writers’ reports regarding each of three conceptually distinct stages of the idea

actualization process—the moment of getting the idea for a poem, the process of expressing the

idea, and the process of revising—were aggregated to yield an overall indicator of the state.

Writer inspiration was assessed using 3 of 4 items from the state version of the

Inspiration Scale (Thrash & Elliot, 2003), adapted to each stage of the process. The full set of 9

items, in order by stage, was as follows: “I felt inspired at those moment(s),” “Something

inspired me,” and “I was inspired to write”; “I felt inspired while expressing my idea(s),”

“Something inspired me,” and “I was inspired to write”; and “I felt inspired while revising and

finalizing this poem,” “Something inspired me,” and “I was inspired to revise this poem.” The

fourth item from the Inspiration Scale was not administered regarding any stage. Response

options ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). An inspiration composite was

formed by summing across the 9 items (M = 37.57; SD = 11.60; α = .91).

Writer awe was assessed using two items, “full of awe” and “full of wonder,” that were

administered with respect to each stage of the writing process. Items were rated from 1 (very

slightly or not at all) to 7 (extremely). An awe composite was formed by summing across the 6

items (M = 13.44; SD = 8.67; α = .95).

Writer PA was assessed, separately for each stage, using 4 of 5 items (“excited,”
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 16

“enthusiastic,” “alert,” “determined”) from a short version of the Positive and Negative Affect

Schedule (PANAS) (Kercher, 1992). The fifth item, “inspired,” was excluded in order to avoid

redundancy between the PA and inspiration variables. Items were rated from 1 (very slightly or

not at all) to 7 (extremely). A PA composite was formed by summing across the 12 items (M =

45.72; SD = 16.36; α = .93).

Writer NA was assessed, separately for each stage, using 5 items (“upset,” “distressed,”

“scared,” “nervous,” “afraid”) from a short version of the PANAS (Kercher, 1992). Items were

rated from 1 (very slightly or not at all) to 7 (extremely). A NA composite was formed by

summing across the 15 items (M = 25.25; SD = 12.75; α = .93).

Writer effort was assessed with two items that were adapted to each stage of the writing

process. The full set of 6 items, in order by stage, was as follows: “I was working hard at those

moment(s)” and “I was putting forth a great deal of effort at those moment(s)”; “I worked hard in

writing this poem” and “I put forth a great deal of effort into expressing my idea(s)”; and “I

worked hard on revising this poem” and “I put forth a great deal of effort into revising and

finalizing this poem.” Response options ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree).

An effort composite was formed by summing across the 6 items (M = 21.74; SD = 8.14; α = .89).

Reader states. Reader state measures were similar to those used for writer states but, of

course, were administered once rather than once per stage of the writing process.

Reader inspiration was assessed using the full 4-item state version of the Inspiration

Scale. One item from the original state version of the scale (“Something I encountered or

experienced inspired me”) was adapted to the poem-reading context (“Something about the poem

inspired me.”). Because reading may inspire actions other than writing (Thrash, Moldovan,

Fuller, & Dombrowski, 2014), the scale was not adapted to focus specifically on inspiration to
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 17

write. Items were rated from 1 (not at all) to 7 (very deeply or strongly). An inspiration

composite was formed by summing across items (M = 7.65; SD = 5.69; α = .98).

Reader awe was assessed using the two-item measure used for writer awe. Items were

rated from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely). An awe composite was formed by summing across

items (M = 2.58; SD = 1.26; α = .79).

Reader PA was assessed using the 4-item measure used for writer PA. Items were rated

from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely). A PA composite was formed by summing across items (M =

6.26; SD = 2.52; α = .70).

Reader NA was assessed using the 5-item measure used for writer NA. Items were rated

from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely). A NA composite was formed by summing across items (M =

6.33; SD = 2.38; α = .83).

Reader chills was assessed using a preliminary version of the 12-item Chills

Questionnaire (Maruskin et al., 2012). Items were as follows: “got goosebumps,” “got a shiver

down my spine,” “felt tickling sensations somewhere in my body,” “felt a chill pass through

me,” “felt a tingling sensation spread over me,” “felt hairs stand-on-end somewhere on my

body,” “got a cold sensation deep inside me,” “got a cool sensation on my skin,” “felt my

muscles quiver or shiver,” “got pins-and-needles or prickling sensations,” “got a shudder or

tremor,” “felt a wave of goosebumps come over me.” Items were rated on a scale from 0 (not at

all) to 6 (extremely). A chills composite was formed by summing across items (M = 1.60; SD =

4.72; α = .91).2

Reader traits. Openness to experience was assessed using the 12-item scale from the

NEO Five-Factor Inventory (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Items were rated from 1 (strongly

disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). An openness composite was formed by summing across the 12
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 18

items (M = 43.57; SD = 6.82; α = .79).

Approach and avoidance temperament were assessed using the 6-item scales from the

Approach-Avoidance Temperament Questionnaire (Elliot & Thrash, 2010). Items were rated

from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Approach temperament and avoidance

temperament composites were formed by summing across the respective sets of six items

(approach temperament: M = 32.92; SD = 5.04; α = .80; avoidance temperament: M = 24.45; SD

= 7.49; α = .82).

Poem coding. In light of our integrative design, the poems function not only as products

of writer states, but also as elicitors of reader states. Because the considerations involved in

coding poems as products are different than those involved in coding poems as stimuli, we

collected two sets of ratings and integrated them using factor analytic methods. We refer to the

first coding task, which focused primarily on the quality of the poems as products, as evaluative

coding, and the second, which focused primarily on psychologically meaningful stimulus

qualities, as appraisal coding. In the following, we describe the two coding tasks and then

describe our method for integrating data obtained through the two methods.

Evaluative coding. Evaluative coding was performed by a panel of 9 qualified coders,

who were advanced English majors or graduate students of American literature. Coders rated the

poems relative to one another (1 = very low, 5 = medium, 9 = very high) on each of the following

dimensions (both the labels and descriptions were provided to coders): grammar/spelling (“the

degree to which the poem has proper grammar and spelling”), punctuation/capitalization (“the

degree to which the poem has proper punctuation and capitalization”), finish (“the degree to

which the poem seems finished and polished, rather than unfinished and rough”), craftsmanship

(“the degree to which the poem displays craftsmanship, using your own subjective definition of
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 19

craftsmanship”), clarity (“the degree to which the ideas are expressed clearly”), sound/rhythm

(“the degree to which word sound and rhythm are used effectively”), richness of imagery (“the

degree to which vivid imagery is present”), beauty/aesthetic appeal (“the degree to which the

poem is beautiful or aesthetically appealing”), pleasantness (“the degree to which the poem is

pleasant, as opposed to unpleasant”), creativity (“the degree to which the poem is creative, using

your own subjective definition of creativity”), novelty of style (“the degree to which the style of

expression (e.g., word choice, literary devices, structure) is novel”), originality of idea (“the

degree to which the thematic idea is original”), unusualness (“the degree to which the poem is

unusual or out-of-the-ordinary”), profundity (“the degree to which the poem is profound,

showing depth and significance”), insightfulness (“the degree to which the poem transcends the

obvious or superficial and discerns the true or hidden nature of things”), organic/authentic

quality (“the degree to which the ideas and their expression are organic, natural, and authentic, as

opposed to manufactured, contrived, and phony”), evocation of emotion (“the extent and depth of

emotion that the poem evokes”), stimulation of thought (“the degree to which the poem

stimulates thought or reflection”), and sublimity (“the degree to which the poem has a moving,

exalting, or elevating effect”). The poems and variables to be coded were presented in different

orders for each coder. Inter-rater reliability was established for all variables (see Table 1).

Appraisal coding. Appraisal coding was carried out by a panel of 11 undergraduate

psychology research assistants. Whereas the evaluative coders had been selected on the basis of

their qualifications for evaluating poetry, these coders were instead drawn from a population that

resembles the population from which our readers were drawn. The following items were coded

using an absolute scale (1 = strongly disagree, 4 = neutral, 7 = strongly agree): original or novel,

beautiful or aesthetically appealing, revealing of truth, ordinary or mundane, morally good or


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 20

right, emotionally impactful, profound or significant, creative, contrived or arbitrary, rational or

logical, interesting, valuable or useful, meaningful, surprising, insightful, amusing, pleasant,

superficial or cliché, complex, unifying or integrative, humorous, and unique. The items

“amusing” and “humorous” were excluded from the primary analyses because they had low

means and variances and converged poorly with other variables. The poems and variables to be

coded were presented in different orders for each coder. Inter-rater reliability was established for

all variables (see Table 1).

Factor analytic integration of evaluative and appraisal coding. We used exploratory

structural equation modeling (Asparouhov & Muthén, 2009) to examine the factor structure of

the two sets of ratings after method variance associated with coder group had been removed.

Specifically, we adapted the correlated-traits-correlated-methods (CTCM) multitrait-

multimethod (MTMM) confirmatory factor analysis model, such that method factors (evaluative

coding method, appraisal coding method) were represented with confirmatory factors as in a

standard CTCM analysis, but substantive poem factors (“traits”) were modeled using exploratory

factor analysis with geomin oblique rotation. A model with two confirmatory method factors and

six exploratory substantive factors was found to have good fit, χ2 (741) = 1104.33, CFI = .95,

TLI = .92, SRMR = .017, and readily interpretable factors. On the basis of factor loadings, we

labeled the substantive factors as follows: mechanics, insightfulness, originality, clarity,

emotionality, and pleasantness/sublimity. Factor loadings are shown in Table 1. Notably,

conceptually similar variables from different coder groups loaded together as indicators of the

same factors, indicating convergent validity. Correlations between factors ranged from r = -.44

(originality and clarity) to .60 (mechanics and originality).

Because the insightfulness and originality factors were of direct theoretical interest, we
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 21

saved factor scores for these factors for use in subsequent analysis (insightfulness: M = .00, SD =

1.00; originality: M = .00, SD = .99).3 Because the pleasantness/sublimity factor is also relevant

but subsumes theoretically distinct pleasantness and sublimity constructs, we used ratings of

pleasantness and sublimity from the evaluative coding as separate variables in subsequent

analyses (pleasantness: M = 4.65, SD = 1.25; sublimity: M = 4.26; SD = 1.23).

Variable transformation and rescaling

Variable distributions were examined using Q-Q plots. Because of strong positive skew,

reader inspiration, PA, and NA were transformed using a log transformation. Because of severe

positive skew, reader awe and chills were transformed using an inverse transformation. In

supplemental analyses, we repeated our core analyses using outcome variables that were

trichotomized and modeled as ordered categorical (ordinal). These analyses yielded findings

generally comparable to those based on transformation (for details, see footnote 10).

To facilitate model estimation, variables were rescaled as needed so that their variances

fell in the 1-10 range (Muthén & Muthén, 2012). All predictor variables (i.e., writer states, poem

characteristics, and reader traits) were centered in order to facilitate interpretation of moderation

and moderated mediation analyses. Predictors were centered at the level at which they were

measured.4

Results

Analytic strategy

The labels Level 2A, Level 2B, and Level 1 (Muthén & Muthén, 2012) may be used to

characterize the nesting structure of the data. Writers (and their associated poems) are the Level

2A units, which may be regarded as populating the left margin of the writer×reader matrix in

Figure 1. Writer states and poem characteristics were assessed at this level (N = 195). Readers
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 22

are the Level 2B units, which may be regarded as populating the top margin of the writer×reader

matrix. Reader traits were assessed at this level (N = 220). Particular writer-reader pairings are

the Level 1 units, which correspond to cells within the writer×reader matrix. Reader states were

assessed at this level (N = 36,020). A given Level 1 unit is nested within a particular Level 2A

unit and within a particular Level 2B unit and hence is cross-classified.

We adopt a second set of labels—writer level, reader level, and writer×reader level—to

refer to three orthogonal levels of analysis at which a given variable may or may not have

variance. If one applies the values of variables measured at Level 2A (e.g., writer inspiration) to

the cells in their corresponding rows, the total variance in these variables across all 36,020 cells

is due entirely to writer-level variance (i.e., differences between writers). Likewise, if one applies

the values of variables measured at Level 2B (e.g., reader openness) to the cells in their

corresponding columns, the total variance in these variables across all 36,020 cells is due entirely

to reader-level variance (i.e., differences between readers). In contrast, the total variance in

variables measured at Level 1 (e.g., reader inspiration) may span three levels of analysis. Reader

inspiration, for instance, may vary at the writer level (i.e., some writers’ poems may tend to be

more inspiring than others’), reader level (i.e., some readers may tend to be more inspired than

others), and a residual writer×reader level (i.e., reader inspiration may also depend on the

particular writer-reader pairing).5

All analyses were conducted using Mplus 7.3 with Bayesian estimation (Asparouhov &

Muthén, 2010; Muthén & Muthén, 2012). We used Bayesian estimation because cross-classified

models are too computationally demanding for traditional estimation methods, and because it

allows rigorous testing of the significance of indirect effects and other derived parameters

(Muthén & Asparouhov, 2012; Yuan & MacKinnon, 2009). We used uninformative priors, rather
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 23

than informative priors based on theory or past findings. The Markov Chain Monte Carlo

(MCMC) algorithm based on the Gibbs sampler was used to generate the posterior distribution

for each parameter. Two MCMC chains were used, and the second half of each chain was

retained. Potential scale reduction (PSR) was used as the MCMC convergence criterion. To

minimize the possibility of premature convergence, analyses were repeated with the minimum

number of iterations set at 4 times the number of iterations from the initial analysis (Muthén, &

Asparouhov, 2012). All reported point estimates are medians of posterior distributions. Bayesian

95% credible intervals (CIs) are used for all significance tests and are presented in brackets.

References to marginal significance indicate that zero fell within the 95% CI but not within the

90% CI. Where reported, p values are one-tailed and indicate the proportion of the posterior

distribution that is below zero in the case of positive estimates or above zero in the case of

negative estimates.

In our primary analyses, we examined each writer/reader state (e.g., inspiration, awe) in

separate models, without controlling other states. In ancillary analyses reported at the end of the

Results section, we also examined the incremental predictive utility of each writer state with

other writer states controlled. Reasons for not controlling other states in our primary analyses are

as follows: (a) Retaining the full variance in each construct allows us to draw conclusions about

contagion of each construct per se. (b) Detecting contagion of the unique portions of each of a

set of constructs chosen on the basis of maximal redundancy would be unrealistic without

extraordinarily large samples. Indeed, detecting contagion of the unpartialed states may itself be

difficult, because contagion effects are inherently indirect (i.e., the text intervenes between writer

and reader) and therefore are likely to be modest in magnitude. Finally, (c) controlling other

writer states may or may not be considered desirable, depending on one’s assumptions about the
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 24

pattern of causal relations among writer states. Controlling a third variable is desirable if the

third variable is a common cause but undesirable if it is a mediator. Notably, these difficulties are

less relevant to the issues of multiple mediators and multiple moderators than to the issue of

multiple writer and reader states. Accordingly, unique mediation and moderation effects were

examined by modeling all mediators or all moderators simultaneously.

Variance decomposition of reader states

For each reader state, cross-classified intraclass correlations (ICCs; Raudenbush & Bryk,

2002) are presented in Table 2. These ICCs indicate the proportions of total variance found at the

writer, reader, and writer×reader levels of analysis. For reader inspiration, the percentages of the

variance each level were as follows: writer level, 5.7%; reader level, 42.8%, and writer×reader

level, 51.5%. These values reveal, respectively, a tendency for some poems to be generally more

inspiring than others; a stronger tendency for some readers to be generally more inspired than

others; and a particularly strong tendency for reader inspiration to hinge on the pairing of a

particular reader with a particular poem. Although the presence of writer-level variance in reader

inspiration is consistent with the hypothesis that writers’ inspiration tends to be infectious (as

tested in the following analysis of contagion), the larger proportions of variance at the reader and

writer×reader levels suggest that reader personality may play an important role as predictor of

reader inspiration and moderator of contagion (as tested in the subsequent moderation analysis).

As shown in Table 2, variance decompositions for reader awe, PA, NA, and chills were similar

to that for reader inspiration.

Contagion effects

For descriptive purposes, correlations among writer states, poem characteristics, and the

writer-level components of reader states were estimated using a random-intercepts model and are
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 25

reported in Table 3.

Contagion hypotheses were formally tested by estimating unstandardized effects in

random-slope-and-intercept models. In each analysis, a given reader state (e.g., inspiration) was

regressed on the corresponding writer state, with the intercept free to vary at all levels and the

slope free to vary across readers. Each reader’s slope reflects the effect of the writer state (left

column of Figure 1) on his or her own state (a particular column of the writer×reader matrix).

The fixed effect of a given writer state refers to the mean slope across readers. Fixed

effects are shown in Table 4. Regarding inspiration contagion, the fixed effect was positive as

hypothesized, although the 95% CI narrowly included zero (p = .053). Although the 95% CI

included zero in this model, it excluded zero in all subsequent models (mediation, moderation,

and moderated mediation). Taken together, these findings call for rejection of the null hypothesis

for inspiration contagion.6 We conclude that the inspiration of the average reader is predictable

from the inspiration of the unseen writer “behind” the text. Similarly, significant positive fixed

effects were documented for awe, PA, and NA. Thus contagion generalizes across a set of

motivation, discrete emotion, and dimensional affect constructs.

Although the unstandardized coefficients in Table 4 cannot be directly compared to one

another, the correlations reported in Table 3 are useful for gauging the relative magnitudes of the

contagion effects (inspiration, r = .13; awe, r = .23; PA, r = .15; NA, r = .20). Contagion of

inspiration was weaker (but not significantly so) than contagion of the other states (inspiration

versus awe, rdif = .091 [-.089, .272]; inspiration versus PA, rdif = .020 [-.133, .172]; inspiration

versus NA, rdif = .062 [-.151, .273]).

The random effect of a given writer state refers to variability in the writer-reader slope

across readers, capturing individual differences in readers’ receptivity to contagion. Random


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 26

effects are shown in Table 4. Next we report mediation and moderation analyses, which account

for the fixed and random aspects of contagion, respectively.

Mediation by poem characteristics

Four poem characteristics—insightfulness, pleasantness, originality, and sublimity—were

selected as candidate mediators on the basis of theory (see Introduction) and factor analysis (see

Method). We examined contagion of each writer/reader state in separate models, with all four

mediators modeled simultaneously. To test mediation, we used a Bayesian cross-classified

extension of multilevel structural equation modeling (Preacher, Zyphur, & Zhang, 2010; Yuan &

MacKinnon, 2009). Because the writer state and poem characteristics cannot vary across readers,

the paths (slopes) from the writer state to poem characteristics were modeled as fixed. Because

the effects of poem characteristics and the direct effect of the writer state on the reader state may

vary across readers, these paths were modeled as having both fixed and random components. For

a particular writer/reader state, the indirect effect via a given poem characteristic was computed

as the product of (a) the effect of the writer state on the poem characteristic and (b) the average

effect of the poem characteristic on the reader state.7 Fixed effects from the mediation models are

shown in Table 5. All effects discussed in this section concern average effects across readers.

As shown in Table 5 and Figure 2A, writer inspiration positively predicted insightfulness,

pleasantness, originality, and sublimity. Insightfulness and pleasantness, in turn, positively

predicted reader inspiration, whereas originality negatively predicted reader inspiration.

Accordingly, writer inspiration had positive indirect effects on reader inspiration via

insightfulness, B = .021 [.015, .026], and pleasantness, B = .034 [.024, .046], and a negative

indirect effect via originality, B = -.029 [-.038, -.020].8 These findings demonstrate that

inspiration is infectious due to the insightfulness and not the originality of inspired writing;
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 27

indeed, the originality of inspired writing suppressed contagion. The indirect effect via

pleasantness is consistent with the positive valence of inspiration.

Documentation of three highly significant indirect effects is noteworthy given the modest

(r = .13) and narrowly significant overall fixed contagion effect documented above (see Table 4

and footnote 6). This apparent contradiction may be explained as an instance of inconsistent

mediation (MacKinnon, Krull, & Lockwood, 2000). The overall contagion effect was only

modestly greater than zero not because writer inspiration had little effect, but rather because it

had multiple effects that varied in sign and therefore suppressed one another. The modest net

effect of writer inspiration belies its explanatory power, as becomes apparent from the moderated

mediation analysis below, in which the negative indirect effect via originality is essentially

switched on and off by a moderating variable.

Writer awe had positive indirect effects on reader awe via sublimity, B = .013 [.006,

.020], and pleasantness, B = .018 [.010, .026]. Writer awe also had a negative indirect effect via

originality, B = -.002 [-.003, -.000], such that writer awe led to higher levels of originality,

which, in turn, led to lower levels of reader awe. These awe findings parallel the inspiration

findings except that a double dissociation was documented: insightfulness mediated contagion of

inspiration but not awe, whereas sublimity mediated contagion of awe but not inspiration.

Writer PA had positive indirect effects on reader PA via pleasantness, B = .022 [.010,

.034], and insightfulness, B = .003 [.001, .005]. Given the similarity of the inspiration and PA

findings, we conducted an additional pair of analyses in which the effect of the other writer state

was controlled. In the analysis of inspiration contagion, all three indirect effects remained

significant when writer PA was controlled. In the analysis of PA contagion, the indirect effect

via pleasantness remained significant. The indirect effect via insightfulness was again significant
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 28

but reversed in sign, due to a negative unique effect of writer PA on insightfulness. Thus

insightfulness was distinctively implicated in contagion of inspiration.

Writer NA had positive indirect effects on reader NA via insightfulness, B = .002 [.001,

.004] and low pleasantness, B = .054 [.043, .066]. Writer NA also had a negative indirect effect

via sublimity, B = -.009 [-.014, -.005], such that writer NA led to lower sublimity, and lower

sublimity, in turn, led to lower reader NA.

These findings support our hypotheses that inspiration and awe contagion are mediated

by insightfulness and sublimity, respectively. Pleasantness, a fundamental stimulus dimension,

mediated contagion of PA and contributed to contagion of all other states we examined.

Moderation by reader personality

Having documented contagion and mediation for the average reader, we next tested

whether contagion effects vary as a function of reader personality. We tested each writer/reader

state in a separate model, with all candidate moderators (openness, approach temperament, and

avoidance temperament) modeled simultaneously. Moderation was tested by modeling reader

traits as predictors of random writer-reader slopes (as in hierarchical multilevel modeling) rather

than by modeling product terms (as in multiple regression).9 Fixed effects are reported in Table

6.

First we consider direct effects of reader traits on reader states. As shown in Table 6,

approach temperament positively predicted reader inspiration, awe, and PA, whereas avoidance

temperament positively predicted NA.

Next we consider moderating effects of reader traits. In the inspiration analysis,

contagion was moderated by openness, B = .005 [.001, .010], such that contagion was stronger

for readers higher in openness. We also examined conditional effects—that is, contagion effects
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 29

at particular levels of the moderator. In this and subsequent analyses, low, moderate, and high

levels of the moderator refer to the observed minimum, mean, and maximum, respectively.10 The

conditional effect of writer inspiration on reader inspiration was significantly greater than zero

for readers moderate, B = .036 [.001, .073], or high, B = .062 [.022, .105], but not low, B = .002

[-.042, .046], in openness. Figure 3 illustrates the conditional effect and 95% CI band at all levels

of reader openness. In this plot, the moderation effect corresponds to the positive slope of the

conditional effect line, and conditional contagion effects are significant at levels of reader

openness where the CI band excludes zero. This moderation analysis reveals an additional reason

(in addition to suppression by originality) that the average contagion effect was modest in

magnitude: contagion effects were present for readers at or above the mean in openness, but not

for those below the mean.

The hypothesis that awe contagion is moderated by reader openness was not supported, B

= .002 [-.004, .008]. Contagion of awe was instead moderated by reader approach temperament,

β = .007 [.002, .013], such that contagion was stronger for readers higher in approach

temperament. The conditional effect of writer awe on reader awe was significantly greater than

zero for readers moderate, B = .053 [.020, .087], or high, B = .087 [.045, .127], but not low, B =

-.021 [-.085, .045], in approach temperament (see Figure 4).

Contagion of PA was likewise moderated by approach temperament, β = .006 [.003,

.010], such that contagion was stronger for readers higher in approach temperament. The

conditional effect of writer PA on reader PA was significantly greater than zero for readers

moderate, B = .026 [.001, .051], or high, B = .054 [.024, .083], but not low, B = -.036 [-.079,

.008], in approach temperament (see Figure 5).

Contagion of NA was moderated (marginally) by avoidance temperament, β = .004 [-


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 30

.000, .009] (p = .036), such that contagion was stronger for readers higher in avoidance

temperament. The conditional effect of writer NA on reader NA was significantly greater than

zero for readers low, B = .059 [.018, .108], moderate, B = .082 [.054, .127], and high, B = .105

[.065, .159], in avoidance temperament (see Figure 6A). Contagion of NA was also moderated

by approach temperament, β = .007 [.003, .012], such that contagion was stronger for readers

higher in approach temperament. The conditional effect of writer NA on reader NA was

significantly greater than zero for readers moderate, B = .082 [.054, .127], or high, B = .115

[.080, .171], but not low, B = .010 [-.046, .064], in approach temperament (see Figure 6B).

These findings reveal a double dissociation in the moderation of contagion of inspiration

and its covariates. Openness to experience moderated contagion of inspiration but not of awe or

PA; conversely, approach temperament moderated contagion of awe and PA but not of

inspiration. Contagion of NA was moderated by approach and avoidance temperaments.

Moderated mediation

The above mediation analyses documented the characteristics of inspired writing through

which, for the average reader, inspiration contagion was facilitated (insightfulness, pleasantness)

or suppressed (originality). The moderation analyses documented for whom inspiration

contagion effects were stronger versus weaker than average (readers higher versus lower in

openness). Considered together, these answers to questions of “how?” and “for whom?” raise a

further question: is the greater proneness of open readers to inspiration contagion a result of

drawing more inspiration from particular characteristics of inspired writing? To answer this

question, we specified a model that integrated the above mediation and moderation models.

Reader traits were specified to moderate the random effects of the mediators on reader

inspiration, as well as the random direct effect of writer inspiration on reader inspiration.
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 31

Much as moderation may be indexed as the effect (e.g., the slope in Figures 3-6) of a

moderator on the conditional effect of a writer state on a reader state, moderated mediation may

be indexed as the effect (slope) of a moderator on the conditional indirect effect via a particular

mediator. In the present context, in which the second but not the first path of an indirect effect is

potentially moderated, this index is computed as the product of (a) the effect of the writer state

on a given mediator and (b) the effect of the moderator on the slope relating the mediator to the

reader state (Hayes, 2015; for a generalization, see Wang & Preacher, 2015). Fixed effects,

including indexes of moderated mediation, are shown in Table 7.

As indicated in Table 7, reader openness and approach temperament were both

documented as moderators of indirect effects. In the following, we first present openness

findings, followed by approach temperament findings.

Openness as a moderator of indirect effects. Reader openness was found to moderate

the indirect effect of writer inspiration on reader inspiration via originality, B = .003 [.001, .005],

such that the indirect effect was less negative for readers higher in openness. The conditional

indirect effect via originality was significantly less than zero for readers low, B = -.046 [-.062, -

.032], moderate, B = -.028 [-.036, -.020], and high, B = -.013 [-.027, -.000], in openness,

although it approached nonsignificance at the highest levels of openness. The moderated

mediation effect corresponds to the positive slope of the conditional indirect effect line plotted in

Figure 7C. Because reader traits cannot impact the effect of writer inspiration on poem

characteristics, moderation of the indirect effect via originality is attributable to moderation of

the effect of originality on reader inspiration (see Table 7).

Reader openness was also found to moderate (marginally) the indirect effect via

sublimity, B = .003 [-.000, .006] (p = .030), such that the indirect effect was initially less
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 32

negative and then more positive with increasing levels of openness. The conditional indirect

effect via sublimity was negative and marginally significant for readers low in openness, B = -

.020 [-.044, .003] (p = .048), close to zero and nonsignificant for readers moderate in openness,

B = -.002 [-.018, .013], and positive but nonsignificant for readers high in openness B = .012 [-

.010, .034]. The moderated mediation effect corresponds to the positive slope of the conditional

indirect effect line plotted in Figure 7D. (Marginal) moderation of the indirect effect via

sublimity is attributable to (marginal) moderation of the effect of sublimity on reader inspiration

(see Table 7).

Openness was found not to moderate indirect effects via insightfulness or pleasantness.

As illustrated in Figures 7A and 7B, conditional indirect effects via insightfulness and

pleasantness were uniformly positive and significant across levels of openness. The lack of

moderation of the indirect effects via insightfulness and pleasantness is manifest as horizontal

slopes in these plots.

These moderated mediation findings explain why openness moderated contagion of

inspiration. At all levels of openness, individuals showed a proneness to contagion via the

insightfulness and pleasantness of inspired writing. These effects were offset (suppressed) by a

negative indirect effect via originality to a greater degree among individuals lower in openness.

In addition, the sublimity of inspired writing showed a tendency to suppress contagion at low

levels of openness and a tendency to facilitate contagion at high levels of openness. We conclude

that open readers were not more responsive to the positively inspiring aspects of inspired writing

(insightfulness, pleasantness); rather, they were more tolerant of the incidental characteristics of

inspired writing (originality, sublimity) that undermined contagion in readers low in openness.

Approach temperament as a moderator of indirect effects. Reader approach


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 33

temperament was found to moderate the indirect contagion effect via pleasantness, B = .004

[.002, .007], such that the indirect effect was more positive for readers higher in approach

temperament. The conditional indirect effect via pleasantness was significantly greater than zero

for readers moderate, B = .034 [.023, .047], or high, B = .054 [.038, .070], but not low, B = -.008

[-.034, .018], in approach temperament. This moderated mediation effect is manifest as a positive

slope in Figure 8B. Moderation of the indirect effect via pleasantness is attributable to

moderation of the effect of pleasantness on reader inspiration (see Table 7).

Reader approach temperament was also found to moderate the indirect effect via

originality, B = -.003 [-.005, -.001], such that the indirect effect was more negative for readers

higher in approach temperament. The conditional indirect effect via originality was significantly

less than zero for readers moderate, B = -.028 [-.036, -.020], or high, B = -.043 [-.055, -.031], but

not low, B = .005 [-.016, .024], in approach temperament. This moderated mediation effect is

manifest as a negative slope in Figure 8C. Moderation of the indirect effect via originality is

attributable to moderation of the effect of originality on reader inspiration (see Table 7).

Approach temperament did not moderate indirect effects via insightfulness or sublimity.

As illustrated in Figure 8A and 8D, the conditional indirect effect via insightfulness was

uniformly positive and significant across levels of reader openness, and the conditional indirect

effect via sublimity was uniformly nonsignificant. The lack of moderation of the indirect effects

via insightfulness and sublimity is manifest as roughly horizontal slopes in these plots.

Moderation of mediation by approach temperament may seem surprising given that

approach temperament had not emerged as a moderator of inspiration contagion (see Table 6).

These findings may be reconciled as a case of a novel statistical phenomenon that we label (by

analogy to inconsistent mediation) inconsistent moderation of mediation, in which a null or weak


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 34

moderation effect belies the existence of multiple moderated mediation effects that vary in sign

and therefore offset one another. In this case, the failure of approach temperament to

significantly moderate inspiration contagion belies the fact that individuals higher in approach

temperament were both more prone to contagion via the pleasantness of inspired writing and less

prone to contagion via the originality of inspired writing.

Alternative consequences of writer inspiration and antecedents of reader inspiration

Next we consider alternative consequences of writer inspiration and alternative

antecedents of reader inspiration. We conducted a series of analyses in which one of five writer

states (including effort) was modeled as a predictor of one of five reader states (including chills).

Mediators were modeled in all analyses in order to facilitate interpretation. Total effects of each

writer state on each reader state are reported in the top of Table 8. In the following, we focus on

results relevant to the issues of reader enthrallment and the perspiration narrative.

Writer inspiration as an antecedent of reader enthrallment. As shown in the top of

Table 8, writer inspiration positively predicted both indicators of reader enthrallment, awe and

chills. In fact, as shown in Table 3, writer inspiration predicted reader awe (r = .30) and chills (r

= .24) more strongly than reader inspiration (r = .13), although only the contrast with reader awe

was significant (prediction of reader inspiration versus awe, rdif = .168 [.052, .290]; prediction of

reader inspiration versus chills, rdif = .103 [-.063, .272].

Indirect effects of writer inspiration on reader awe and chills are illustrated in Figure 2B-

C. In the prediction of reader awe, writer inspiration had positive indirect effects via

insightfulness, B = .006 [.001, .010], pleasantness, B = .023 [.014, .031], and sublimity, B = .022

[.008, .033], and a negative indirect effect via originality, B = -.009 [-.015, -.002]. In the

prediction of reader chills, writer inspiration had positive indirect effects via insightfulness, B =
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 35

.015 [.011, .020], and sublimity, B = .039 [.027, .054], and negative indirect effects via

pleasantness B = -.016 [-.025, -.008], and originality, B = -.009 [-.017, -.003]. As revealed in

Tables 3 and 8, writer inspiration had relatively strong and highly significant total effects on

reader enthrallment in spite of suppression from negative indirect effects.

Notably, sublimity was a pivotal mediator that distinguished the enthralling effects of

writer inspiration from its inspiring effects. Writer inspiration predicted sublimity, which, in turn,

predicted reader awe and chills but not reader inspiration. Sublime stimuli are natural elicitors of

awe and chills, but they elude the grasp of understanding and hence do not instigate an inspired

epistemic transmission process in the average reader.

Writer effort as an antecedent of reader inspiration. As shown in the top of Table 8,

writer effort positively predicted reader inspiration, consistent with the perspiration narrative.

Indirect effects are shown in Figure 2D. Writer effort had positive indirect effects via

insightfulness, B = .007 [.005, .010], pleasantness, B = .015 [.010, .020], and low originality, B =

.003 [.002, .005].

Although writer effort predicted reader inspiration, so too did all other writer states that

we examined. This finding raises the question of whether effort contributes uniquely to the

prediction of reader inspiration. We address this question and other questions of incremental

predictive utility in the following section. We also note that writer effort was the only writer

variable that failed to predict any reader states beyond reader inspiration.

Ancillary analyses: Incremental predictive utility of writer states

Finally, we conducted a set of ancillary analyses in which all five writer states were

modeled as simultaneous predictors of a given reader state, again with mediators modeled in

order to facilitate interpretation. Total effects from these analyses are shown in the bottom of
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 36

Table 8. We emphasize that these analyses are stringent and are not necessarily more “correct”

than the analyses reported above. As discussed above, the desirability of controlling other writer

states depends on assumptions about the pattern of causal relations among writer states,

assumptions that cannot be tested with the present design.

Robustness of writer-reader contagion effects. As shown in the bottom of Table 8, the

inspiration contagion total effect became nonsignificant when other writer states were controlled.

However, all three indirect effects of writer inspiration on reader inspiration remained

significant: insightfulness, B = .027 [.020, .035], pleasantness, B = .005 [.003, .008], and

originality, B = -.033 [-.043, -.022]. Controlling other writer states weakened the positive

indirect effect via pleasantness, thus explaining why the total (net) effect was reduced to zero.

The awe contagion total effect also became nonsignificant when other writer states were

controlled. The indirect effect via pleasantness remained significant, B = .004 [.002, .007]. The

negative indirect effect via originality became positive, B = .004 [.001, .006], due to a negative

rather than positive unique effect of writer awe on originality. In addition, there was now a

negative indirect effect via insightfulness, B = -.003 [-.006, -.001], such that writer awe uniquely

predicted lower insightfulness, and lower insightfulness, in turn, predicted lower reader awe.

Most noteworthy, the positive indirect effect via sublimity became nonsignificant, B = .000 [-

.001, .000].

The PA contagion total effect also became nonsignificant. The indirect effect via

pleasantness remained significant, B = .017 [.006, .027], whereas the indirect effect via

insightfulness became non-significant, B = .000 [-.001, .000].

The NA contagion total effect remained significant when other writer states were

controlled. All three indirect effects documented above remained significant: pleasantness, B =
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 37

.045 [.035, .056], insightfulness, B = .003 [.001, .006], and sublimity, B = -.007 [-.011, -.004]. In

addition, there was now a positive indirect effect via originality, B = .001 [.000, .002].

In sum, in these stringent ancillary analyses in which other writer states were controlled,

contagion effects became nonsignificant for inspiration, awe, and PA, whereas the NA contagion

effect remained significant. This pattern of findings is understandable in light of the considerable

overlap among writer inspiration, awe, and PA (rs = .44 to .70; see Table 3) and the negligible

overlap between these variables and writer NA (rs = -.09 to .10). The hypothesized indirect

effects were robust for contagion of inspiration, PA, and NA, but not awe (i.e., the indirect effect

via sublimity became nonsignificant).

Robustness of the enthralling effects of inspiration. Next we examined the robustness

of the enthralling effects of inspiration when other writer states are controlled. The total effect of

writer inspiration on reader awe remained significant (see the bottom of Table 8), as did all four

indirect effects: insightfulness, B = .008 [.002, .014], pleasantness, B = .004 [.002, .006],

originality, B = -.011 [-.019, -.003], and sublimity, B = .016 [.007, .025]. Likewise, the total

effect of writer inspiration on reader chills remained significant, as did all four indirect effects:

insightfulness, B = .019 [.013, .025], pleasantness, B = -.002 [-.004, -.001], originality, B = -.012

[-.020, -.003], and sublimity, B = .027 [.017, .037]. Thus, the enthralling effects of writer

inspiration were robust when other writer states are controlled.

Robustness of the effect of writer effort on reader inspiration. Finally, we examined

the robustness of the effect of writer effort on reader inspiration. The total effect of writer effort

was no longer significant when other writer states were controlled (see the bottom of Table 8).

Of the three indirect effects documented above, only the positive indirect effect via low

originality remained significant, B = .014 [.009, .018]. These findings raise further doubts about
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 38

the centrality of effortful processes in the generation of deeply affecting texts.11

Discussion

Contagion of inspiration via the written word is, by most measures, one of the “big” ideas

of Western scholarship—it has a long and rich history in the humanities, it concerns peaks of

human experience, it has far-reaching implications for the transmission of culture, and it is

broadly relevant across the aesthetic, spiritual, and intellectual domains. Nevertheless, the

concept of inspiration contagion has been conspicuously absent from the contemporary research

literature. In the following, we summarize the results of our investigation and discuss their

implications and limitations.

Writer-reader inspiration contagion

Our core finding is that student writers’ privately reported inspiration predicts the

privately reported inspiration of the average student reader, despite lack of contact between

writers and readers beyond the intervening text. This finding attests to the power of the written

word as a vehicle for sharing the peaks of human experience among individuals separated in time

or place. As Bowra (1951) suggested, the inspired poem “creates in us the kind of exaltation

which the poet himself has known in his times of vision and enraptured creation…the central,

final, inescapable fact is that inspired words create life in us because they are themselves alive”

(p. 36).

Mediation of inspiration contagion

Such descriptions of a text as alive or inspired vividly convey the transitivity of inspired

writing; they imply a vital inspiration process through which the words were produced and a

homologous process through which they are received. However, we have not formally

conceptualized texts as inspired, because states of inspiration properly belong to individuals.12


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 39

Conceptualizing a text as inspired is, at best, redundant with conceptualizing writers or readers as

inspired and, at worst, prone to the kinds of inferential ambiguities and circularity of reasoning

that have led some theorists to be needlessly suspicious of the inspiration concept (e.g., Fehrman,

1980; Valéry, 1958; Wimsatt & Beardsley, 1946). Instead, we have conceptualized texts as

displaying specific observable characteristics (e.g., insightfulness), as evaluated by independent

judges. From this perspective, the transitivity of inspired writing may be operationalized as

positive indirect effects via qualities of the text.

Our mediation findings may be summarized as follows: Writer inspiration had positive

effects on poem insightfulness, pleasantness, originality, and sublimity. In turn, insightfulness

and pleasantness had positive effects on the inspiration of the average reader, whereas originality

had a negative effect and sublimity had a null effect. Accordingly, writer inspiration had positive

indirect effects on the inspiration of the average reader via insightfulness and pleasantness and a

negative indirect effect via originality.

The positive indirect effects via insightfulness and pleasantness support our theorizing. In

particular, the indirect effect via insightfulness is consistent with the theorized epistemic

transmission function of inspiration operating in the writer and reader. That is, an insightful text

functions both as the concretization of the writer’s inspiration and as the elicitor of a comparable

state in the reader. In addition, the indirect effect via pleasantness is consistent with the pleasant

and appetitive nature of inspiration. We presume that this indirect effect is extraneous to

epistemic transmission and instead reflects an inherent compatibility of pleasant ideas with the

appetitive inspired state (but see also Thrash, Moldovan, Oleynick et al., 2014, regarding ways in

which inspiration may be negative or unpleasant).

Also important is the discrimination provided by the finding that contagion was not
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 40

mediated by sublimity or originality, qualities theorized to lack the property of transitivity.

Unlike insightfulness, sublimity does not lend itself to further transmission, because a sublime

stimulus eludes comprehension. Although it may be deeply moving, it remains inherently other,

without being fully internalized. Originality, similarly, “belongs” to its author and therefore is

not available for further transmission. Further research is needed to determine why originality

had a negative rather than null effect on reader inspiration. One possibility is that the author

salience evoked by an original text yields an unflattering social comparison (Lockwood &

Kunda, 1997) (e.g., originator vs. spectator or imitator). Another possibility is that originality per

se, above and beyond insightfulness, pleasantness, and sublimity, is not regarded as intrinsically

valuable and merely produces discomfort.

The fact that writer inspiration had both positive and negative indirect effects indicates

inconsistent mediation. “Inconsistent” here does not mean unreliable; it means that the indirect

effects vary in sign and therefore offset rather than augment one another. Thus the total

contagion effect (r = .13) indicates the net result of the inspiring and uninspiring effects of

inspired writing. We note that the complexity of this model is not captured by the term

contagion, which falsely implies that writer inspiration promotes reader inspiration through a

single transitive process. Nevertheless, we have embraced this term because it connects our

findings to established literatures on contagion of emotion (Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson,

1993), affect (Kramer et al., 2014), and motivation (Radel, Sarrazin, Legrain, & Wild, 2010).

Moderation of inspiration contagion

Reader response is widely recognized as having both generalized and idiosyncratic

aspects (e.g., Fish, 1970). Whereas the contagion and mediation effects discussed above concern

generalized (average) responses, our moderation analyses are attempts to explain idiosyncratic
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 41

responses using basic personality traits as moderators of contagion.

As hypothesized, readers’ openness to experience was found to moderate inspiration

contagion, such that writer inspiration predicted reader inspiration more strongly among readers

higher in openness. Conditional effect analyses indicated that contagion was absent for readers

low in openness but present for readers moderate or high in openness. These findings are

consistent with past findings regarding the evocation of inspiration (Thrash & Elliot, 2004;

Thrash, Maruskin et al., 2010) and with Plato’s model of contagion, which suggests that only

certain kinds of individuals—poets and rhapsodes—are sufficiently receptive that they hold

privileged positions in the dissemination of eternal wisdom.

Moderated mediation of inspiration contagion

Although the mediation and moderation analyses address important “second-generation”

research questions, they are limited in important respects. The mediation analysis focuses on

indirect effects only for the average reader. The moderation analysis concerns moderation only

of the overall (total) contagion effect, which may be attributable to moderation of one or more

indirect effects. Accordingly, we also conducted moderated mediation analyses, which more

fully exploit the power of conditional process analysis (Hayes, 2013). Two reader traits,

openness and approach temperament, were found to moderate indirect effects. Given the

complexity of these analyses, we focus on the openness findings, which are particularly relevant

to our theorizing. However, we encourage readers to also consider the provocative implications

of the approach temperament findings and of the novel statistical phenomenon that we described

above as inconsistent moderation of mediation.

Reader openness was found to positively moderate the indirect effect via originality, such

that the conditional indirect effect was less negative for readers higher in openness. Reader
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 42

openness was also found to positively moderate the indirect effect via sublimity, such that the

conditional indirect effect initially became less negative, and then became more positive, with

increasing levels of reader openness. In contrast, reader openness did not moderate indirect

effects via insightfulness or pleasantness, which yielded uniformly positive conditional indirect

effects. These findings explain why openness moderated contagion of inspiration. Readers higher

in openness were more prone to contagion because they were less susceptible to the suppressing

effect of the originality of inspired writing, and because the sublimity of inspired writing tended

to promote rather than suppress contagion for these individuals.

A novel aspect of these findings is that the proneness of open readers to inspiration

contagion appears not to be due to an openness to inspiring qualities per se. Open readers were

not more responsive to the positively inspiring aspects of inspired writing (i.e., insightfulness,

pleasantness)—these qualities were generally inspiring for readers regardless of their levels of

openness. Rather, open readers were more open to incidental qualities of inspired writing

(originality, sublimity) that undermined the inspiration of readers low in openness. We conclude

that the potential for inspiration contagion via the written word lies in most individuals but tends

to be manifest only in those who are sufficiently open to the more exotic (i.e., novel and sublime)

writings in which inspired and inspiring (i.e., insightful and pleasant) ideas tend to be found.

Discrimination of inspiration contagion from contagion of related states

In addition to documenting writer-reader contagion of inspiration, we also documented

contagion of an emotion, awe, and of two basic affective states, PA and NA. These findings

indicate that writer-reader contagion generalizes across a variety of motivational, emotional, and

affective states.

Core findings regarding the mediation and moderation of these other states may be
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 43

summarized as follows. As hypothesized, contagion of awe was mediated by sublimity rather

than insightfulness, but contrary to our hypothesis, awe contagion was moderated by reader

approach temperament and not openness. PA and NA contagion were mediated primarily by

pleasantness (high or low levels, respectively) and were moderated by approach temperament

and avoidance temperament, respectively. Approach temperament also moderated NA contagion.

These findings indicate that inspiration contagion cannot be reduced to contagion of

emotion or affect. Insightfulness was distinctively implicated as a mediator of inspiration

contagion, and openness was distinctively implicated as a moderator of inspiration contagion.

Consistent with the epistemic function of inspiration, this mediator and moderator are cognitive

in focus, whereas their counterparts for awe, PA, and NA are affective in focus. Evidence of

distinct contagion processes is noteworthy given that awe and PA are closely related to

inspiration, both theoretically and empirically (Thrash, Moldovan, Oleynick et al., 2014).

Enthralling effects of inspiration

Moving beyond contagion, we found that writer inspiration predicted not only reader

inspiration, but also two indicators of enthrallment: awe and chills. Enthrallment effects were

positively mediated by insightfulness and sublimity and were negatively mediated by originality.

(In addition, the pleasantness of inspired writing facilitated awe but undermined chills.) Thus

sublimity played a pivotal role in distinguishing the enthralling effects of writer inspiration from

its inspiring effects—specifically, writer inspiration predicted sublimity, which, in turn, predicted

awe and chills but not inspiration (see Figure 2A-C). Due to the additional positive indirect effect

via sublimity, the effects of writer inspiration on reader awe (r = .30) and chills (r = .24) were

stronger than the inspiration contagion effect (r = .13), significantly so in the case of reader awe.

These findings highlight the fact that reader inspiration is not the only consequence of
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 44

writer inspiration and may not be its most natural consequence. We emphasize that the function

of inspiration is transmission (of ideas), not contagion (of inspiration). Contagion is merely one

consequence of transmission by a writer, and enthrallment is another. Structural asymmetries in

the writer-reader relation, particularly the reader’s relative passivity and less clearly specified

avenues for action, may render enthrallment the more natural consequence. However,

enthrallment states may themselves be viewed as incipient forms of inspiration that have the

potential to become fully manifest following further contemplation of the inspired text.

Our inclusion of chills as an outcome is noteworthy given debates about the value of

chills in pointing the way to truth and beauty in literature (Fish, 1970; Wimsatt & Beardsley,

1949) and to inspiration in the writer (Bowra, 1951; Lembke, 1973). Thus it is striking that

readers’ chills responses were predicted by both insightfulness and sublimity—which correspond

roughly to truth and beauty (but see Burke, 1759, and Ishizu & Zeki, 2014, regarding the

distinction between beauty and sublimity)—and were distinctively diagnostic of writer

inspiration (see Table 8). These findings link deeply felt bodily responses of readers to a deeply

felt motivation in writers, suggesting a resonance driven by a shared perception of what is true

and sublime about the human experience.

Inspiration versus perspiration

Consistent with Valéry’s theorizing, poems written by writers who exerted more effort

were more inspiring to the average reader. However, we note three caveats regarding this

finding. First, the effect of effort on reader inspiration was (partially) mediated by low rather

than high originality. Second, the effect of effort was not unique in the sense that all other writer

states (inspiration, awe, PA, NA) also predicted reader inspiration. Third, effort was the only

writer state that failed to predict any other reader states (awe, PA, NA, chills). These findings
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 45

suggest that effortful processes do not play a central role in the generation of deeply affecting

language.

In future theory and research, it may be useful to distinguish two roles of effort. In the

case of motivated effort, one exerts oneself as an expression of an underlying motivational

impetus such as inspiration. In the case of volitional effort, one exerts oneself through an act of

willpower because motivational resources are lacking. Although both kinds of effort are

important, neither is consistent with the suggestion that genius is 99% perspiration and 1%

inspiration. In the case of motivated effort, the purported benefits of perspiration may be

attributable to inspiration or other motivations that underlie it. In the case of volitional effort, it

would be peculiar to suggest that a chronic preponderance of willpower (which is ego-depleting;

Muraven, Tice, & Baumeister, 1998) over spontaneous inspiration (which is vitalizing; Thrash,

Elliot, Maruskin, & Cassidy, 2010) is desirable, much less the mark of genius.

Broader implications

Implications for the writer. Sadly, many students find writing to be a source of dread.

Our diagnosis is as follows. Writing, like talking, is naturally a process of expression or

articulation. However, many students understand this process in truncated form, as an act of text

generation, a creation of something from nothing. Exacerbating the problem, students are

expected to be original and may be unaware that, as Collingwood (1938) and Barthes (1967)

have argued, great writers draw heavily upon others’ work. Lacking inspiration, the student

contrives a text through volitional effort. The result is a “brain-spun, invented work,” as Tolstoy

(1897/2014) described it. “People are taught how to write a many-paged composition, without

having anything they wish to say, on a theme about which they have never thought…This is

taught in schools” (p. 135). Our finding that inspiration is infectious points to a solution: students
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 46

could be encouraged to look for stimulation in the most inspired and insightful works in their

field and beyond. The act of writing then presents itself as a natural vehicle for capturing ensuing

insights and elaborating upon them. Such insights are valuable whether they are revelatory (i.e.,

they involve the discovery and unveiling of existing ideas that have been overlooked or

underappreciated) or creative (i.e., they involve a novel and useful integration of existing ideas)

(see also Derrida, 2002). For students higher in openness, who are more tolerant of the novel and

sublime, inspiration is likely to be maximized, we propose, by a literature search or reading list

that is more integrative and cross-disciplinary.

A second implication for the student writer is that one’s level of inspiration while writing

provides veridical feedback about the likely inspiration, awe, and chills of future readers. This

finding contradicts the presumption of some theorists that inspiration is merely subjective. For

instance, Valéry (1958) stated, “The treasures [inspiration] illuminates in our own mind’s

eye…are very far from having the same value in the eyes of others...What is of value to us alone

has no value” (pp. 213-214). Valéry’s admirable concern about subjectivity is misplaced,

because he implicates inspiration only by neglecting two kinds of base-rate information.

Specifically, although it is true that inspired writing sometimes fails to inspire, (a) the fate of

uninspired writing is worse, and (b) writing generated through other means (e.g., volitional

effort) is also subject to falling flat, if not more so. The aspiring writer may also benefit from the

knowledge that only readers high in openness are prone to contagion.

Implications for the self. In applying a general conceptualization of inspiration to

writers and readers, we have implicitly identified the literary concept of authorship with the

psychological concept of selfhood. We now develop a self-as-author metaphor more explicitly.

First, both author and the self are implicated in writing—a text in one case and a life narrative in
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 47

the other. Second, both encounter epistemic obstacles of existential significance. The author

confronts a blank page, unsure what to say, and the self struggles with an unwritten future. Third,

both have been celebrated as seats of an originating agency by some theorists (Young,

1759/1918; Deci & Ryan, 1985) and ridiculed as illusory by others, who instead emphasize

deterministic effects of the stimulus environment (Barthes, 1967; Skinner, 1972). Finally, both

find their voice when inspired (Bowra, 1951; Thrash & Elliot, 2004). During these elusive

moments, determinism and agency are experienced as harmonized—inspiration is drawn

(directly or indirectly) from the stimulus milieu, with the full endorsement of and active

collaboration by a vitalized author-self. The author-self, at last, speaks with authority and

authenticity.13

We call for self and well-being researchers to attend to this emergent, mediating self,

which is not purely I or me, nor purely agentic or communal—such bifurcations are serviceable

in the realm of mundane experience but not in the realm of self-transcendence. Like Plato’s poet

and rhapsode, the individual who participates in the transcendent is in a mode of transmission—

expressing, actualizing, or embodying (Thrash, Moldovan, Fuller et al., 2014). As described by

Nozick (1989), who used light as a metaphor for truth, beauty, goodness, and holiness, “The

ethic of light calls for a being to be its vessel. To be a being of light is to be its transmitter” (p.

214). As long as psychologists neglect the mediating self and its embeddedness in transmission

and contagion processes, they will misdiagnose the struggle to find one’s voice as a case of, say,

not working hard enough, not relating well enough to others, or not holding oneself in

sufficiently high regard.

Implications for culture. Consistent with the self-as-author metaphor, inspiration

contagion is posited to be broadly relevant across domains, rather than specific to the literary
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 48

domain investigated herein. In the spiritual domain, for instance, the impulse to capture spiritual

insight (revelation) in written form, coupled with a homologous impulse in readers to give voice

to scriptural insights in their own lives, are arguably the cornerstones of the major world

religions. Inspiration contagion is also applicable within the intellectual sphere, where the written

text is the primary medium for transmitting insight in diverse academic fields. Even direct

interpersonal contagion, which superficially has nothing to do with writing, may be viewed with

greater fidelity through the self-as-author lens. Being inspired by another is not a result of

encountering an individual of superlative virtue or standing, nor of social comparison with such

individuals—this is the heteronomous realm of the inspirational, not the authentic realm of the

inspiring. Rather, inspiration results from encountering another whose actions speak the things

one had wanted to say (or would have wanted to say) but had not been prepared to say unaided.14

In addition to being broadly relevant, inspiration contagion is far-reaching in its

implications, playing a role in both the origins and evolution of culture. Regarding origins, the

literary processes examined herein have a primordial, oral-aural counterpart dating back at least

to archaic Greece, in which wisdom was disseminated through the live performances of poets

and rhapsodes (Clark, 1997; Nagy, 1989; Rubin, 1995). We propose further that cultural

evolution (Henrich, 2001) is shaped by inspiration contagion processes, in which individuals

high in openness—a trait that has also been called culture—are inspired to build upon one

another’s most insightful ideas, innovations, and ideologies. Thus our model brings attention to

the motivational and personality dynamics of cultural evolution.

Limitations and future directions

Effect size. A critic might argue that the inspiration contagion effect (r = .13) is weak by

traditional standards. However, traditional standards are not applicable to our paradigm because
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 49

they do not take causal distance into consideration. Inspiration contagion is inherently indirect in

a truly distal sense. We did not examine, say, mediators situated “between” the text and the

reader, but rather examined the distal effects of the writer “behind” the text. In addition, our

paradigm is minimalistic relative to other contagion paradigms in that it concerns contagion of

privately reported experiences (rather than of overt behavioral expressions) in entirely separate

samples who never come into contact.

Moreover, for the following reasons, the impact of writer inspiration is greater than is

suggested by the overall contagion effect. First, given inconsistent mediation, the overall

contagion effect is necessarily less than the total absolute magnitude of the positive and negative

effects of writer inspiration. Second, the contagion effect gauges impact for a single, average

reader and therefore does not speak to total impact as multiplied across readers. Third, given

moderation by reader openness, the contagion effect is stronger among open individuals—in

whom contagion actually occurs—than it is on average. Finally, as noted, writer inspiration may

lead more naturally to reader enthrallment than inspiration, and therefore the stronger

enthrallment effects may provide a better indication of the impact of writer inspiration.

Generalizability. We expect our core findings to generalize beyond the particular writing

domain, populations, study design, and cultural context investigated in this study, although

additional research would needed to test this prediction empirically. We have already discussed

generalizability to intellectual and spiritual domains. We now consider generalizability to other

populations, study designs, and cultural contexts.

A strength of this study is that writers (and their poems) and readers were modeled as

crossed random factors, establishing generalizability from our student samples to the populations

from which they were drawn. Although we expect our core findings to generalize from student
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 50

populations to populations of experienced writers and their readership, some differences in

findings may be anticipated. For instance, it is possible that writer effort would be more

predictive of reader impact in a study of experienced writers, who have a fuller repertoire of

skills that may be summoned at will. Because the inspiration-perspiration distinction is a false

dichotomy, this possibility would not imply that inspiration is less valuable to experienced

writers. On the contrary, this same repertoire of skills may allow experienced writers to translate

inspired ideas into completed products with greater efficiency and fidelity. We caution that

investigation of contagion with elite writers may call for a commensurate population of elite

readers. Keltner (2009) reported that exposing college students to top-tier poetry left them

confused rather than awestruck, apparently because they did not understand the poetry or found

it irrelevant to their concerns.

Our experimental study design involved active assignment of readers to poems and their

authors. A benefit of this design is that the resulting writer×reader matrix had little missing data.

A naturalistic design, in contrast, would likely yield a sparse matrix, because particular writer-

reader pairings may not occur without experimenter intervention. This complication aside, we

predict that contagion processes may be documented using a naturalistic design (see also Clark,

1997). Some important differences may be anticipated, however. In a naturalistic setting, readers

have the freedom to selectively seek out preferred texts or genres. Our moderated mediation

analyses suggest that if readers low in openness (see the left sides of Figures 7A-D) gravitate

toward texts that are pleasant and insightful but not particularly novel or sublime—preventing

the suppression of contagion that would otherwise occur—they too may participate in inspiration

contagion processes. Thus our model may explain the evolution not only of revelatory and

creative aspects of culture, but also its more traditional and mainstream aspects.
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 51

We acknowledge cultural constraints on contagion processes. Although inspiration

contagion is posited to occur in all cultures, cultural influences on perceptions of insightfulness

are likely to constrain trans-cultural contagion. As an extreme example, it is possible that the

propaganda of Al-Qaeda and ISIS terrorists (Sivek, 2013) is both inspired and inspiring, but

contagion is likely to be limited to the population of readers sympathetic to Islamist extremism.

The possibility that some ideas are inspiring regardless of culture awaits future research.

Causal inference. Our integrative writer-reader study design may be regarded as an

experimental-correlational hybrid. As noted, readers were assigned to poem stimuli (as in a

standard experiment); however, writers and poems were sampled and their attributes (writer

states, poem characteristics) were measured (as in a standard correlational study) rather than

varied through the direct control of an experimenter. Lack of direct experimental control

compromises the isolation of causal factors, and therefore a strong causal interpretation of our

documented contagion and enthrallment effects is not warranted. However, experimental control

of writer states is not possible in principal, because experimental control applies to manipulated

stimuli, not to resulting states, which are dependent variables (for details, see Thrash, Moldovan,

Oleynick et al., 2014).

Although experimental control of writer states is not possible, we controlled covariates

statistically in ancillary analyses. We caution that controlling other writer states is a conservative

strategy, as discussed in the Results section. In the following, we note two key findings from

these analyses. First, regarding overall effects of writer states on reader states, the enthralling

effects of writer inspiration remained significant, whereas contagion of most states (inspiration,

awe, PA) and the effect of writer effort on reader inspiration became nonsignificant. These

findings bolster our conclusion that, for the average reader, enthrallment may be a particularly
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 52

important consequence of writer inspiration.

Second, regarding mediation, the hypothesized indirect effects remained significant in the

cases of inspiration, PA, and NA contagion; however, in the case of awe contagion, the

hypothesized indirect effect via sublimity became nonsignificant. Although this null finding

raises questions about the role of sublimity as a mediator of awe contagion, it may be an artifact

of controlling an additional mediator unwittingly. Specifically, we propose that awe contagion is

mediated not only by sublimity but also by writer inspiration, as in the following sequence:

writer awe writer inspiration poem sublimity reader awe. This model introduces writer

inspiration as a motivational intermediary (see also Adler, 2015), without which writer awe

might not yield a text, much less an awe-inspiring one. We also note that this model is a

straightforward extension of our enthrallment model. If this model is correct, then an indirect

effect via sublimity would indeed be expected to become nonsignificant when writer inspiration

is controlled, as in our ancillary analysis. However, firm conclusions await future studies

designed to examine the temporal precedence of awe and inspiration in the writer.

Conclusion

Previous research has demonstrated that inspiration is existentially exhilarating because

the individual participates, as mediator, in the transmission of intrinsic value (Thrash, Elliot et

al., 2010; Thrash, Maruskin et al., 2010). The present contagion and enthrallment findings situate

the inspired individual in an even broader context of meaning. Our findings suggest that the

inspired writer participates in the sweep of history, producing a text that is not just valuable but

that enlightens, inspires, and raises the hairs on the arms of future generation of thinkers.
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 53

References

Adler, N. J. (2015). Finding beauty in a fractured world: Art inspires leaders—leaders change the

world. Academy of Management Review, 40, 480-494.

http://dx.doi.org/10.5465/amr.2015.0044

Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

Asparouhov, T, & Muthén, B. (2009). Exploratory structural equation modeling. Structural

Equation Modeling, 16, 397-438. doi:10.1080/10705510903008204

Asparouhov, T. & Muthén, B. (2010). Bayesian analysis using Mplus: Technical implementation

(version 3). Unpublished manuscript. https://www.statmodel.com/download/Bayes3.pdf

Asparouhov, T. & Muthén, B. (in press). General random effect latent variable modeling:

Random subjects, items, contexts, and parameters. Chapter to appear in Advances in

multilevel modeling for educational research: Addressing practical issues found in real-

world applications.

Barthes, R. (1967). Death of the author. Aspen, no. 5-6. Retrieved from

http://www.ubu.com/aspen/aspen5and6/threeEssays.html

Bennett, A. (2005). The author: The new critical idiom. Routledge: New York.

Bowra, C. M. (1951). Inspiration and poetry. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Burke, E. (1759). A philosophical enquiry into the origin of our ideas of the sublime and

beautiful. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Clark, T. (1997). The theory of inspiration: Composition as a crisis of subjectivity in romantic

and post-romantic writing. Manchester: Manchester University Press.

Collingwood, R. G. (1938). The Principles of Art. London: Oxford University Press.

Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). The Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO–PI–R) and
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 54

NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO–FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological

Assessment Resources.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human

behavior. New York: Plenum.

Derrida, J. (2002). Without alibi. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2010). Approach and avoidance temperament as basic dimensions

of personality. Journal of Personality, 78, 865-906. doi:10.1111/j.1467-

6494.2010.00636.x

Fehrman, C. (1980). Poetic creation: Inspiration or craft. Minneapolis, MN: University of

Minnesota Press.

Fish, S. E. (1970). Literature in the reader: Affective stylistics. New Literary History, 2, 123-162.

doi:10.2307/468593

Geisler, N. L., & Nix, W. E. (1986). A general introduction to the Bible (2nd ed.). Chicago, IL:

Moody Press.

Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1993). Emotional contagion. Current Directions

in Psychological Science, 2, 96-99. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.ep10770953

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A

regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press.

Hayes, A. F. (2015). An index and test of linear moderated mediation. Multivariate Behavioral

Research, 50, 1-22. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2014.962683

Henrich, J. (2001). Cultural transmission and the diffusion of innovations: Adoption dynamics

indicate that biased cultural transmission is the predominate force in behavioral change.

American Anthropologist, 103, 992-1013. doi:10.1525/aa.2001.103.4.992


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 55

Hoffman, & Rovine (2007). Multilevel models for the experimental psychologist: Foundations

and illustrative examples. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 101-117.

doi:10.3758/BF03192848

Ishizu, T., & Zeki, S. (2014). A neurobiological enquiry into the origins of our experience of the

sublime and beautiful. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1-10.

doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00891

Jones, S., Dodd, A., & Gruber, J. (2014). Development and validation of a new multidimensional

measure of inspiration: Associations with risk for bipolar disorder. Plos One, 9, 1-11.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0091669

Judd, C. M., Westfall, J., & Kenny, D. A. (2012). Treating stimuli as a random factor in social

psychology: A new and comprehensive solution to a pervasive but largely ignored

problem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 54-69.

doi:10.1037/a0028347

Kant, I. (1764/1960). Observations on the feeling of the beautiful and the sublime. Berkeley, CA:

University of California Press.

Keltner, D. (2009). Born to be good: The science of a meaningful life. New York, NY: Norton.

Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (2003). Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion.

Cognition and Emotion, 17, 297-314. doi:10.1080/02699930302297

Kercher, K. (1992). Assessing subjective well-being in the old-old: The PANAS as a measure of

orthogonal dimensions of positive and negative affect. Research on Aging, 14, 141-168.

doi:10.1177/0164027592142001

Konečni, V. J. (2008). Does music induce emotion? A theoretical and methodological analysis.

Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2, 115–129. doi:10.1037/1931-


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 56

3896.2.2.115

Kramer, A., Guillory, J., & Hancock, J. (2014). Experimental evidence of massive-scale

emotional contagion through social networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of

Sciences, 111, 8788-8790. doi:10.1073/pnas.1320040111

Laski, M. (1961). Ecstasy in secular and religious experiences. Los Angeles: Jeremy P. Tarcher,

Inc.

Lembke, J. (1973). Bronze and iron: Old Latin poetry from its beginnings to 100 B.C. Berkeley:

University of California Press.

Lockwood, P., & Kunda, Z. (2007). Superstars and me: Predicting the impact of role models on

the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 91-103. doi:10.1037/0022-

3514.73.1.91

Longinus (1890). On the sublime. New York: Macmillan.

MacKinnon, D. P., Krull, J. L., & Lockwood, C. M. (2000). Equivalence of the mediation,

confounding and suppression effect. Prevention Science, 1, 173-181.

doi:10.1023/A:1026595011371

Marsh, H. W., & Hau, K.-T. (2007). Applications of latent-variable models in educational

psychology: The need for methodological-substantive synergies. Contemporary

Educational Psychology, 32, 151–171. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2006.10.008

Martindale, C. (1989). Personality, situation, and creativity. In J. A. Glover, R. R. Ronning, & C.

R. Reynolds (Eds.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 211-232). New York: Plenum.

Martindale, C. (2001). Oscillations and analogies. American Psychologist, 56, 342-345.

doi:10.1037/0003-066X.56.4.342

Martindale, C., & Hasenfus, N. (1978). EEG differences as a function of creativity, stage of the
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 57

creative process, and effort to be original. Biological Psychology, 6, 157-167.

doi:10.1016/0301-0511(78)90018-2

Maruskin, L. A., Thrash, T. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2012). The chills as a psychological construct:

Content universe, factor structure, affective composition, elicitors, trait antecedents, and

consequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 135-157.

doi:10.1037/a0028117

McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1997). Conceptions and correlates of openness to experience. In

R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of Personality Psychology (pp. 825-

847). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Milyavskaya, M., Ianakieva, I., Foxen-Craft, E., Colantuoni, A., & Koestner, R. (2012). Inspired

to get there: the effects of trait and goal inspiration on goal progress. Personality and

Individual Differences, 52, 56-60. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.08.031

Muraven, M., Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Self-control as a limited resource:

Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 774-

789. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.74.3.774

Muthén, B., & Asparouhov, T. (2012). Bayesian structural equation modeling: A more flexible

representation of substantive theory. Psychological Methods, 17, 313-335.

doi:10.1037/a0026802

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2012). Mplus User’s Guide (version 7). Los Angeles, CA:

Muthén & Muthén.

Nagy, G. (1989). Early Greek views of poets and poetry. In G. A. Kennedy (Ed.), The

Cambridge history of literary criticism, Vol. 1, Classical criticism (pp. 1-77). Cambridge:

Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CHOL9780521300063.002


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 58

Nozick, R. (1989). The examined life: Philosophical meditations. New York, NY: Simon &

Schuster.

Oatley, K. (2003). Creative expression and communication of emotion in the visual and narrative

arts. In R. J. Davidson, K. R. Scherer, & H. H. Goldsmith (Eds.), Handbook of Affective

Sciences (pp. 481-502). New York: Oxford University Press.

Plato (2012). Ion in Halcyon Classics. Houston, TX: Halcyon Press.

Poe, E. A. (1846). The philosophy of composition. Graham’s Magazine, 28, 163-167. Retrieved

from http://www.eapoe.org/works/essays/philcomp.htm

Preacher, K. J., Zyphur, M. J., & Zhang, Z. (2010). A general multilevel SEM framework for

assessing multilevel mediation. Psychological Methods, 15, 209-233.

doi:10.1037/a0020141

Radel, R., Sarrazin, P., Legrain, P., & Wild, T. C. (2010). Social contagion of motivation

between teacher and student: Analyzing underlying processes. Journal of Educational

Psychology, 102, 577-587. doi: 10.1037/a0019051

Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data

analysis methods (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Robinson, J. (2005). Deeper than Reason: Emotion and its Role in Literature, Music, and Art.

New York: Oxford University Press.

Roe, J., & Stanco, M. (2007). Inspiration and technique: Ancient to modern views on beauty and

art. Oxford: Peter Lang.

Rubin, D. C. (1995). Memory in oral traditions: The cognitive psychology of epic, ballads, and

counting-out rhymes. New York: Oxford University Press.

Sawyer, R. (2006). Explaining creativity the science of human innovation. New York, NY:
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 59

Oxford.

Shelley, P. B. (1977). A defense of poetry. In D. H. Reiman and S. B. Powers (Eds.), Shelley’s

poetry and prose. New York: Norton.

Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., & John, O. P. (2006). Positive emotion dispositions differentially

associated with Big Five personality and attachment style. The Journal of Positive

Psychology, 1, 61-71. doi:10.1080/17439760500510833

Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., & Mossman, A. (2007). The nature of awe: Elicitors, appraisals, and

effects on self-concept. Cognition and Emotion, 21, 944-963.

doi:10.1080/02699930600923668

Silvia, P. J., Fayn, K., Nusbaum, E. C., & Beaty, R. E. (in press). Openness to Experience and

awe in response to nature and music: Personality and profound aesthetic experiences.

Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. Doi:10.1037/aca0000028

Silvia, P. J. , & Nusbaum, E. C. (2011). On personality and piloerection: Individual differences

in aesthetic chills and other unusual aesthetic experiences. Psychology of Aesthetics,

Creativity, and the Arts, 5, 208-214. doi:10.1037/a0021914

Sivek, S. C. (2013). Packaging inspiration: Al Qaeda’s digital magazine Inspire in the self-

radicalization process. International Journal of Communication. 7, 584-606. Retrieved

from http://ijoc.org/ojs/index.php/ijoc/article/view/1670

Skinner, B. F. (1972). A lecture on “having” a poem. In B. F. Skinner (Ed.), Cumulative

record (3rd ed., pp. 345–355). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Thrash, T. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2003). Inspiration as a psychological construct. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 871-889. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.871

Thrash, T. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2004). Inspiration: Core characteristics, component processes,
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 60

antecedents, and function. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 957-973.

doi:0.1037/0022-3514.87.6.957

Thrash, T. M., Elliot, A. J., Maruskin, L. A., & Cassidy, S. E. (2010). Inspiration and the

promotion of well-being: Tests of causality and mediation. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 98, 488-506. doi:10.1037/a0017906

Thrash, T. M., Maruskin, L. A., Cassidy, S. E., Fryer, J. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2010). Mediating

between the muse and the masses: Inspiration and the actualization of creative ideas.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 469-487. doi:10.1037/a0017907

Thrash, T. M., Moldovan, E., Fuller, A. F., & Dombrowski, J. T. (2014). Inspiration and the

creative process. In J. C. Kaufman (Ed.), Creativity and mental illness (pp. 343-362).

Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Thrash, T. M., Moldovan, E. G., Oleynick, V., & Maruskin, L. A. (2014). The psychology of

inspiration. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8/9, 495-510.

doi:10.1111/spc3.12127

Tolstoy, L. (1897/2014). What is art? New York: Bloomsbury Academic.

Valéry, P. (1958). Remarks on poetry. In Collected works of Paul Valéry, Vol. 7, The art of

poetry (D. Folliot, trans., pp. 196-215). New York: Pantheon Books.

Valéry, P. (2007). Poetry and abstract thought. The American Poetry Review, 36, 61-66.

Retrieved from http://m.friendfeed-

media.com/526799b134a9db2402a3ef922c11a65c29aa72c1

Wang, L., & Preacher, K. J. (2015). Moderated mediation analysis using Bayesian methods.

Structural Equation Modeling, 22, 249-263. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2014.935256

Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J., & Tellegen, A. (1999). The Two General Activation Systems
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 61

of Affect: Structural Findings, Evolutionary Considerations, and Psychobiological

Evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 820-838.

doi:10.1037//0022-3514.76.5.820

Wimsatt, W. K., & Beardsley, M. C. (1946). The intentional fallacy. The Sewanee Review, 54,

468-488. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/27537676

Wimsatt, W. K., & Beardsley, M. C. (1949). The affective fallacy. The Sewanee Review, 57, 31-

55. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/27537883

Young, E. (1759/1918). Conjectures on original composition. Manchester: Manchester

University Press.

Yuan, Y., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2009). Bayesian mediation analysis. Psychological Methods, 14,

301-322. doi:10.1037/a0016972
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 62

Footnotes

1. Because of the 1-1 correspondence between writers and poems in our framework, we make no

distinction between the sample of writers and the sample of poems. Our framework could be

expanded to involve multiple poems per writer, in which case poems would be nested within

writers, and both would be crossed with readers.

2. A subsequent version of this questionnaire was optimized for the separate assessment of

distinct types of chills—in particular, goosetingles and coldshivers (Maruskin et al., 2012). We

focus on overall chills in this study given that goosetingles and coldshivers are not clearly

distinguished with the version of the Chills Questionnaire employed here.

3. It was not possible to use ESEM factors in our subsequent cross-classified modeling analyses,

because ESEM in Mplus currently requires maximum likelihood estimation, whereas cross-

classified modeling requires Bayesian estimation.

4. This study was conducted as part of a larger project. We have also conducted preliminary

analyses for a separate publication that will address a distinct topic, with partial overlap of

variables. Although we collected additional variables not reported here, our analyses were guided

systematically by our comprehensive statistical framework, theory, and a priori decisions.

5. With one observation per cell by design, between-cell writer×reader variance cannot be

distinguished from within-cell variance (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). We use the label

“writer×reader level” because our focus is on predicting reliable between-cell variance. In a

design with multiple observations per cell, such that each reader responds to each writer’s poem

more than once, the writer×reader and within-cell levels could be decomposed, resulting in four

distinct levels of analysis.

6. Whereas the proportion of the posterior distribution below zero for the contagion effect was p
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 63

= .053 in the present model, it was p = .003 in the mediation model, p = .020 in the moderation

model, and p = .006 in the moderated mediation model. On average across models, the

proportion of the posterior distribution below zero was p = .021, warranting rejection of the null

hypothesis. Slight differences in estimates and CIs across models are not surprising given that

the models varied with respect to whether mediators and/or moderators were included.

7. Extending Preacher et al.’s (2010) notation to the case of cross-classified models, our

mediational model could be described as a 2A 2A 1 mediation model. Whereas Preacher et

al.’s two-level 2 2 1 model necessarily involves fixed slopes for both the a and b paths, the

cross-classified context allows for the possibility that the b path varies at level 2B (i.e., across

readers). Accordingly, we estimated the indirect effect as a×b as in Preacher et al.’s 2 2 1

model, except that b was estimated as the average of readers’ randomly varying slopes, rather

than as a nonvarying fixed slope. Given that our b path may vary randomly, our model also

resembles Preacher et al.’s 2 1 1 model, which allows for the possibility of varying b paths.

However, because our mediator exists at level 2A rather than level 1, the complication of

between- and within-level confounding of the b slope does not arise in our case.

8. Unless otherwise indicated, positive indirect effects discussed in the text are products of

positive a and b paths. That is, higher levels of the writer and reader state are related to higher

levels of the mediator. In instances where a positive indirect effect is the product of negative a

and b paths, we indicate that the effect is mediated by low levels of the mediator.

9. In all moderation and moderated mediation analyses, main effects of reader traits were

modeled with random intercepts but fixed slopes, because preliminary models that included

random slopes for reader traits (in addition to random intercepts and slopes for all writer-level

predictors) were excessively slow to converge or could not be estimated successfully.


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 64

10. The minimum, mean, and maximum of the moderator correspond to the leftmost portion of

the X axis, the 0 point, and the rightmost portion of the X axis, in all moderation and moderated

mediation figures. We caution that our conditional effects analyses describe conditional effects

across the full range of the moderator, and therefore “low” and “high” levels of the moderator

are more extreme in the present context than they are when standardized scores of -1 and 1 are

used.

11. As noted, given that the dependent variables were highly skewed, we repeated our core

analyses using outcomes that were trichotomized and modeled as ordered categorical (rather than

transformed and modeled as continuous, as in our primary analyses). These supplemental

analyses have the advantage of not violating distributional assumptions, but they have the

disadvantage of loss of variance. These supplemental analyses yielded findings generally

comparable to our primary findings. A summary of the findings based on ordered categorical

outcomes is as follows: (a) In the inspiration contagion analysis (see Table 4), the nonsignificant

fixed effect of writer inspiration became marginally significant (B = .026 [-.005, .055], p = .050).

(b) Regarding mediation of inspiration contagion (see Table 5), all indirect effect findings

remained the same. (c) Regarding moderation of inspiration contagion (see Table 6), the

significant moderating effect of openness became marginally significant (B = .006 [-.001, .013],

p = .038). (d) Regarding moderated mediation (see Table 7), findings for indexes of moderated

mediation remained the same, except that avoidance temperament emerged as a significant

moderator of the indirect effect via sublimity (B = .003 [.000, .005]). (e) Regarding alternative

antecedents and consequences (see top of Table 8), findings related to total and indirect effects of

writer inspiration on reader awe and chills remained the same, except that the significant indirect

effect of writer inspiration on reader awe via originality became marginally significant, (B = -
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 65

.006 [-.012, .000], p = .035). Findings related to the total and indirect effects of writer effort on

reader inspiration remained the same. (f) Regarding tests of incremental predictive utility (see

bottom of Table 8), total and indirect effects of writer inspiration on reader inspiration remained

the same. Total and indirect effects of writer inspiration on reader awe and chills remained the

same, except that the significant indirect effect of writer inspiration on reader awe via originality

became marginally significant (B = -.007 [-.015, .000], p = .030). Total and indirect effects of

writer effort on reader inspiration remained the same. In sum, the two methods of addressing the

skewness issue yielded comparable findings in most cases. In most instances where findings

differed between the two methods, differences were modest, but the effect happened to cross the

threshold of significance (or marginal significance) in one direction or the other.

12. As long as its meaning is clear, we see no problem in using the phrase “inspired text” as

shorthand for “a text written by an inspired writer.” We do so in the present article.

13. The terms author, authority, and authenticity have common etymological roots.

14. We could not have articulated this point without the guiding voice of Collingwood (1938).
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 66

Table 1

Poem coding: Inter-rater reliability and standardized factor loadings


Inter- Standardized factor loading
rater Pleasant-
Mech- Insight- Orig- Emotion-
reliability Clarity ness/
anics fulness Inality ality
(α) Sublimity
Grammar/spellinge .76 .67 -.00 .06 .31 -.05 -.07
Finishe .73 .60 .26 -.03 .25 .04 .09
Sound/rhythme .70 .55 -.09 .14 -.02 .04 .09
Craftsmanshipe .82 .54 .14 .23 .06 .10 .07
Punctuation/capitalizatione .72 .45 .04 .12 .09 -.03 -.00
Beauty/aesthetic appeale .83 .40 .02 .03 -.09 .26 .38
Revealing of trutha .73 -.02 .97 -.04 .11 -.08 -.08
Insightfula .78 .01 .93 .14 -.04 -.16 -.03
Meaningfula .80 .01 .93 -.02 -.03 .10 -.02
Valuable or usefula .72 -.02 .92 .14 .08 -.17 .04
Profound or significanta .81 .05 .91 .03 -.16 .00 -.01
Unifying or integrativea .71 .00 .85 -.12 .06 -.25 .29
Morally good or righta .78 -.16 .61 -.09 .05 -.14 .42
Profunditye .76 .07 .61 .09 -.21 .13 .07
Insightfulnesse .72 -.02 .53 .23 -.16 .14 .14
Contrived or arbitrarya .77 -.10 -.56 -.20 -.05 -.40 .03
Stimulation of thoughte .71 -.07 .44 .36 -.22 .15 .07
Ordinary or mundanea .82 -.28 -.44 -.35 .15 -.08 .04
Unusualnesse .83 .15 -.07 .88 .02 -.06 -.03
Uniquea .84 .09 .12 .86 .06 .00 -.03
Originality of ideae .80 .01 .00 .84 -.01 -.01 .08
Original or novela .84 .11 .25 .75 -.00 .04 -.02
Surprisinga .76 -.03 .14 .72 -.13 -.02 -.17
Novelty of stylee .85 .43 -.03 .59 -.02 -.12 .02
Organic/authentic qualitye .65 -.24 -.02 .54 .19 .52 .04
Creativitye .85 .38 .02 .49 -.07 .03 .15
Superficial or clichéa .80 -.04 -.40 -.49 .11 -.25 .09
Creativea .87 .34 .28 .48 -.07 -.02 .10
Interestinga .81 .23 .42 .43 .01 .13 -.02
Claritye .79 .01 .02 -.06 .87 .19 .01
Rational or logicala .64 -.11 .45 .00 .66 .01 .00
Complexa .87 .18 .45 .20 -.53 -.01 -.03
Evocation of emotione .79 .06 .16 .13 .01 .65 .09
Emotionally impactfula .84 .10 .51 -.03 .03 .63 -.12
Pleasanta .86 .01 .01 .10 .26 -.18 .90
Pleasantnesse .76 .04 -.07 .07 .15 .04 .82
Sublimitye .77 .13 .18 -.01 -.03 .29 .58
Beautiful or aesthet. appealinga .88 .39 .20 -.07 -.02 .22 .52
Richness of imagerye .92 .32 -.06 .19 -.09 .32 .36
e
Evaluative coding; a Appraisal coding

Note. Loadings ≥ .40 are shown in bold.


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 67

Table 2
Cross-classified ICCs

Level of Reader state


analysis Inspiration Awe PA NA Chills
Writer .057 .040 .027 .133 .033
[.046, .070] [.033, .050] [.022, .034] [.111, .160] [.027, .041]
Reader .428 .353 .479 .286 .415
[.382, .473] [.311, .396] [.432, .526] [.248, .327] [.369, .460]
Writer×reader .515 .606 .494 .580 .551
[.472, .557] [.564, .646] [.450, .538] [.543, .615] [.509, .594]
Note. The ICCs for a given reader state indicate the proportions of total variance at particular

levels of analysis. For effects in bold, 95% CIs exclude zero.


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 68

Table 3

Correlations at the writer level of analysis

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13
1. Writer inspiration
2. Writer awe .44

3. Writer PA .70 .52


-.03 .10 -.09
4. Writer NA
.28 .15 .35 .08
5. Writer effort
.17 .00 .09 .04 .05
6. Poem insightfulness
7. Poem pleasantness .24 .18 .31 -.15 .09 .15
.26 .04 .16 -.01 -.03 .40 .22
8. Poem originality
9. Poem sublimity .32 .16 .29 -.06 .13 .44 .77 .41
.13 .12 .21 -.12 .13 .42 .55 -.11 .47
10. Reader inspiration
11. Reader awe .30 .23 .30 -.18 .03 .32 .73 .16 .72 .71
.09 .08 .15 -.16 .09 .18 .41 .00 .32 .77 .49
12. Reader PA
13. Reader NA -.07 -.21 -.19 .20 -.05 .26 -.56 .17 -.18 -.38 -.44 -.31

14. Reader chills .24 .01 .08 .07 .03 .61 .18 .20 .50 .41 .41 .21 .51

Note. These correlations are based on covariances at the writer level of analysis and are standardized with respect to model estimates
of writer-level variances. For effects in bold, 95% CIs exclude zero.
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 69

Table 4

Writer-reader contagion effects

Contagion process Fixed effect Random effect


(coefficient) (variance)
.031 [-.008, .067] .002 [.001, .004]
Writer inspiration reader inspiration
Writer awe reader awe .052 [.011, .084] .006 [.004, .008]

Writer PA reader PA .024 [.001, .046] .002 [.001, .003]

Writer NA reader NA .084 [.031, .136] .003 [.002, .004]

Note. For effects in bold, 95% CIs exclude zero.


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 70

Table 5

Fixed effects from mediation models

Writer state (X) and reader state (Y)


Effect
Inspiration Awe PA NA
Direct effects
.007 .023 .004 .025
X Y
[-.016, .033] [-.001, .047] [-.012, .021] [-.005, .065]
.107 .004 .056 .026
X insightfulness
[.101, .114] [-.003, .011] [.049, .062] [.019, .033]
.129 .105 .169 -.090
X pleasantness
[.124, .135] [.099, .111] [.164, .174] [-.096, -.084]
.166 .030 .101 -.008
X originality
[.160, .173] [.023, .038] [.095, .108] [-.016, -.001]
.170 .090 .154 -.034
X sublimity
[.165, .176] [.084, .096] [.148, .159] [-.040, -.028]
.195 .057 .050 .094
Insightfulness Y
[.145, .245] [.018, .097] [.011, .090] [.034, .157]
.266 .172 .130 -.599
Pleasantness Y
[.183, .352] [.098, .242] [.061, .202] [-.728, -.486]
-.172 -.054 -.039 .077
Originality Y
[-.227, -.119] [-.099, -.015] [-.085, .001] [.002, .136]
-.012 .140 -.017 .267
Sublimity Y
[-.103, .071] [.066, .218] [-.093, .063] [.162, .406]
Indirect effects
.021 .000 .003 .002
X insightfulness Y
[.015, .026] [-.000, .001] [.001, .005] [.001, .004]
.034 .018 . 022 .054
X pleasantness Y
[.024, .046] [.010, .026] [.010, .034] [.043, .066]
-.029 -.002 -.004 -.001
X originality Y
[-.038, -.020] [-.003, -.000] [-.009, .000] [-.002, .000]
-.002 .013 -.003 -.009
X sublimity Y
[-.018, .012] [.006, .020] [-.014, .010] [-.014, -.005]
.031 .052 .022 .072
Total effects
[.009, .056] [.028, .076] [.005, .041] [.039, .115]

Note. For effects in bold, 95% CIs exclude zero.


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 71

Table 6

Fixed effects from moderation models

Writer state (X) and reader state (Y)


Effect
Inspiration Awe PA NA

Direct effects

X Y .036 [.001, .073] .053 [.020, .087] .026 [.001, .051] .082 [.054, .127]

Openness Y -.031 [-.125, .061] -.060 [-.146, .027] -.074 [-.164, .017] -.038 [-.106, .036]

Approach temperament Y .111 [.027, .194] .124 [.046, .202] .120 [.036, .200] .062 [-.003, .128]

Avoidance temperament Y .081 [-.004, .166] .008 [-.064, .082] -.006 [-.088, .078] .113 [.051, .174]

Moderation effects

Openness (X Y) .005 [.001, .010] .002 [-.004, .008] .000 [-.004, .004] -.001 [-.006, .004]

Approach temperament (X Y) .002 [-.003, .006] .007 [.002, .013] .006 [.003, .010] .007 [.003, .012]

Avoidance temperament (X Y) -.001 [-.005, .003] .000 [-.005, .006] -.002 [-.006, .001] .004 [-.000, .009]

Note. For effects in bold, 95% CIs exclude zero. Arrows pointing to effects within parentheses indicate moderation of those effects.
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 72

Table 7
Fixed effects from moderated mediation inspiration contagion model

Effect Estimate [CI]


Direct effects

Writer inspiration insightfulness .107 [.101, .114]

Writer inspiration pleasantness .129 [.124, .135]

Writer inspiration originality .166 [.160, .173]

Writer inspiration sublimity .171 [.165, .176]

Writer inspiration reader inspiration .005 [-.019, .030]

Insightfulness reader inspiration .195 [.142, .244]

Pleasantness reader inspiration .265 [.182, .358]

Originality reader inspiration -.167 [-.216, -.119]

Sublimity reader inspiration -.012 [-.104, .078]

Openness reader inspiration -.032 [-.128, .062]

Approach temperament reader inspiration .113 [.026, .197]

Avoidance temperament reader inspiration .083 [-.001, .167]

Indirect effects

Writer inspiration insightfulness reader inspiration .021 [.015, .027]

Writer inspiration pleasantness reader inspiration .034 [.023, .047]

Writer inspiration originality reader inspiration -.028 [-.036, -.020]

Writer inspiration sublimity reader inspiration -.002 [-.018, .013]

Total effect .030 [.007, .053]

Moderation effects

Openness (writer inspiration reader inspiration) .000 [-.004, .004]

Openness (insightfulness reader inspiration) -.001 [-.014, .011]

Openness (pleasantness reader inspiration) -.002 [-.022, .019]


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 73

Openness (originality reader inspiration) .018 [.006, .031]

Openness (sublimity reader inspiration) .017 [-.001, .035]

Approach temperament (writer inspiration reader inspiration) .002 [-.002, .006]

Approach temperament (insightfulness reader inspiration) .004 [-.007, .015]

Approach temperament (pleasantness reader inspiration) .033 [.015, .051]

Approach temperament (originality reader inspiration) -.020 [-.031, -.008]

Approach temperament (sublimity reader inspiration) -.008 [-.024, .008]

Avoidance temperament (writer inspiration reader inspiration) -.002 [-.006, .001]

Avoidance temperament (insightfulness reader inspiration) .000 [-.011, .011]

Avoidance temperament (pleasantness reader inspiration) -.001 [-.020, .017]

Avoidance temperament (originality reader inspiration) -.003 [-.014, .008]

Avoidance temperament (sublimity reader inspiration) .013 [-.003, .028]

Moderated mediation effects

Openness (writer inspiration insightfulness reader inspiration) .000 [-.001, .001]

Openness (writer inspiration pleasantness reader inspiration) .000 [-.003, .002]

Openness (writer inspiration originality reader inspiration) .003 [.001, .005]

Openness (writer inspiration sublimity reader inspiration) .003 [-.000, .006]

Approach (writer inspiration insightfulness reader inspiration) .000 [-.001, .002]

Approach (writer inspiration pleasantness reader inspiration) .004 [.002, .007]

Approach (writer inspiration originality reader inspiration) -.003 [-.005, -.001]

Approach (writer inspiration sublimity reader inspiration) -.001 [-.004, .001]

Avoidance (writer inspiration insightfulness reader inspiration) .000 [-.001, .001]

Avoidance (writer inspiration pleasantness reader inspiration) .000 [-.003, .002]

Avoidance (writer inspiration originality reader inspiration) -.001 [-.002, .001]

Avoidance (writer inspiration sublimity reader inspiration) .002 [-.000, .005]


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 74

Note. For effects in bold, 95% CIs exclude zero. Arrows pointing to effects within parentheses indicate

moderation of those effects.


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 75

Table 8

Total effects of writer states on reader states

Reader state
Writer state
Inspiration Awe PA NA Chills
Not controlling other writer states:
.031 .059 .013 -.035 .042
Inspiration
[.009, .056] [.037, .082] [-.003, .031] [-.060, .006] [.018, .062]
.033 .052 .015 -.083 .004
Awe
[.008, .059] [.028, .076] [-.004, .034] [-.117, -.039] [-.020, .026]
.050 .061 .022 -.072 .017
PA
[.029, .074] [.038, .083] [.005, .041] [-.104, -.044] [-.007, .038]
-.032 -.036 -.036 .072 .017
NA
[-.057, -.006] [-.062, -.011] [-.053, -.013] [.039, .115] [-.009, .039]
.031 .011 .010 -.018 .004
Effort
[.005, .056] [-.015, .033] [-.010, .032] [-.054, .020] [-.019, .030]
Controlling other writer states:
-.007 .037 -.007 .050 .069
Inspiration
[-.045, .026] [.014, .066] [-.041, .026] [-.001, .099] [.038, .098]
.014 .023 .007 -.073 -.022
Awe
[-.017, .046] [-.004, .051] [-.021, .033] [-.120, -.027] [-.047, .006]
.038 .026 .020 -.055 -.018
PA
[.004, .076] [-.005, .057] [-.015, .054] [-.107, -.005] [-.050, .015]
-.028 -.035 -.026 .084 .019
NA
[-.060, -.000] [-.061, -.012] [-.054, -.002] [.044, .118] [-.007, .045]
.023 -.014 .011 -.010 -.005
Effort
[-.009, .056] [-.040, .011] [-.017, .037] [-.062, .036] [-.031, .020]
Note. For effects in bold, 95% CIs exclude zero.
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 76

Figure 1

Readers

Personality of Personality of Personality of



Reader 1 Reader 2 Reader b

↓ ↓ ↓ ↓
Writers Poems Writer/Poem × Reader Pairings

State elicited State elicited State elicited


State of Characteristics
Writer 1 → of Poem 1 → by Poem 1 in
Reader 1
by Poem 1 in
Reader 2
… by Poem 1 in
Reader b

State elicited State elicited State elicited


State of Characteristics
Writer 2 → of Poem 2 → by Poem 2 in
Reader 1
by Poem 2 in
Reader 2
… by Poem 2 in
Reader b

… → … → … … … …

State elicited State elicited State elicited


State of Characteristics
Writer a → of Poem a → by Poem a in
Reader 1
by Poem a in
Reader 2
… by Poem a in
Reader b

Figure 1. Illustration of the cross-classified writer×reader data structure.


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 77

Figure 2

A. Insightfulness

.107 .195

Pleasantness .266
.129
Writer .007 Reader
inspiration .166 inspiration
-.172
Originality
.170 -.012

Sublimity

B. Insightfulness

.054
.107

.129 Pleasantness .175

Writer .019 Reader


inspiration .166 awe
-.056
Originality
.171
.129

Sublimity

C. Insightfulness

.107 .141

.129 Pleasantness -.125

Writer .014 Reader


inspiration .166 chills
-.056
Originality
.170 .230

Sublimity

Figure 2 (cont.)
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 78

D. Insightfulness

.036 .195

.055 Pleasantness .270

Writer .007 Reader


effort -.021 inspiration
-.165
Originality
.078 -.021

Sublimity

Figure 2. Mediation via poem characteristics of the effect of (A) writer inspiration on reader inspiration, (B)

writer inspiration on reader awe, (C) writer inspiration on reader chills, and (D) writer effort on reader

inspiration. Solid/dashed arrows indicate that 95% CI excludes/includes zero, respectively.


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 79

Figure 3

0.12

0.1
Effect of writer inspiration on reader inspiration

0.08

0.06

0.04

0.02

-0.02

-0.04

-0.06
-7 -6 -5 -4 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6

Reader openness to experience

Figure 3. Moderation of inspiration contagion by reader openness. The straight line indicates the conditional

effect of writer inspiration on reader inspiration at particular levels of reader openness. The arcs demarcate the

95% CI band.
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 80

Figure 4

0.14

0.12

0.1
Effect of writer awe on reader awe

0.08

0.06

0.04

0.02

-0.02

-0.04

-0.06

-0.08

-0.1
-12 -10 -8 -6 -4 -2 0 2 4 6

Reader approach temperament

Figure 4. Moderation of awe contagion by reader approach temperament. The straight line indicates the

conditional effect of writer awe on reader awe at particular levels of reader approach temperament. The arcs

demarcate the 95% CI band.


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 81

Figure 5

0.1

0.08

0.06
Effect of writer PA on reader PA

0.04

0.02

-0.02

-0.04

-0.06

-0.08

-0.1
-12 -10 -8 -6 -4 -2 0 2 4 6

Reader approach temperament

Figure 5. Moderation of PA contagion by reader approach temperament. The straight line indicates the

conditional effect of writer PA on reader PA at particular levels of reader approach temperament. The arcs

demarcate the 95% CI band.


WRITER-READER CONTAGION 82

Figure 6

A.
0.18

0.16

0.14
Effect of writer NA on reader NA

0.12

0.1

0.08

0.06

0.04

0.02

-0.02

-0.04

-0.06
-7 -6 -5 -4 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Reader avoidance temperament

B.
0.18

0.16

0.14
Effect of writer NA on reader NA

0.12

0.1

0.08

0.06

0.04

0.02

-0.02

-0.04

-0.06
-12 -10 -8 -6 -4 -2 0 2 4 6

Reader approach temperament

Figure 6. Moderation of NA contagion by reader avoidance temperament (A) and approach temperament (B).
The straight lines indicates the conditional effects of writer NA on reader NA at particular levels of reader
avoidance or approach temperament. The arcs demarcate the 95% CI bands.
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 83

Figure 7

A. B.
0.08 0.08
Indirect effect via insightfulness

Indirect effect via pleasantness


0.06 0.06

0.04 0.04

0.02 0.02

0 0

-0.02 -0.02

-0.04 -0.04

-0.06 -0.06

-0.08 -0.08
-7 -6 -5 -4 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 -7 -6 -5 -4 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6

Reader openness to experience Reader openness to experience

C. D.
0.08 0.08

0.06 0.06
Indirect effect via originality

Indirect effect via sublimity

0.04 0.04

0.02 0.02

0 0

-0.02 -0.02

-0.04 -0.04

-0.06 -0.06

-0.08 -0.08
-7 -6 -5 -4 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 -7 -6 -5 -4 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4 5 6

Reader openness to experience Reader openness to experience

Figure 7. Moderation by reader openness to experience of the indirect effects of writer inspiration on reader

inspiration via poem (A) insightfulness, (B) pleasantness, (C) originality, and (D) sublimity.
WRITER-READER CONTAGION 84

Figure 8

A. B.
0.08 0.08

Indirect effect via pleasantness


Indirect effect via insightfulness

0.06 0.06

0.04 0.04

0.02 0.02

0 0

-0.02 -0.02

-0.04 -0.04

-0.06 -0.06
-12 -10 -8 -6 -4 -2 0 2 4 6 -12 -10 -8 -6 -4 -2 0 2 4 6

Reader approach temperament Reader approach temperament

C. D.
0.08 0.08
Indirect effect via originality

Indirect effect via sublimity

0.06 0.06

0.04 0.04

0.02 0.02

0 0

-0.02 -0.02

-0.04 -0.04

-0.06 -0.06
-12 -10 -8 -6 -4 -2 0 2 4 6 -12 -10 -8 -6 -4 -2 0 2 4 6

Reader approach temperament Reader approach temperament

Figure 8. Moderation by reader approach temperament of the indirect effects of writer inspiration on reader

inspiration via poem (A) insightfulness, (B) pleasantness, (C) originality, and (D) sublimity.

View publication stats

You might also like