You are on page 1of 10

Journal of Abnormal Psychology Copyright 2005 by the American Psychological Association

2005, Vol. 114, No. 2, 249 –258 0021-843X/05/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0021-843X.114.2.249

Sleep-Related Attentional Bias in Good, Moderate, and Poor


(Primary Insomnia) Sleepers

Barry T. Jones, Lauren M. Macphee, and Benedict C. Jones


Niall M. Broomfield University of Aberdeen
University of Glasgow

Colin A. Espie
University of Glasgow
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Evidence was sought of an attentional bias toward a highly representative object of the bedroom
environment in good, moderate, and poor (primary insomnia) sleepers. Using a flicker paradigm for
inducing change blindness, the authors briefly presented a single scene comprising a group of bedroom
environment and neutral objects to participants and then briefly replaced this scene with an identical
scene containing a change made to either a bedroom environment or a neutral object. In a 3 ⫻ 2 entirely
between-participants design, change-detection latencies revealed a sleep-related attentional bias in poor
sleepers but not in good sleepers. A possible bias in moderate sleepers was also revealed. It is suggested
that attentional bias has a role in the perpetuation and possibly precipitation of primary insomnia.

Keywords: attentional bias, insomnia, charge-detection, sleep disruption, circadian

Sleep disruption is not unusual. Although good sleep usually have developed when disproportionate processing resources ap-
returns, either through the disappearance of the precipitating agent pear to be automatically allocated to exemplars, as compared with
or through the operation of mechanisms that appear to combat other otherwise equivalent stimuli—producing a disproportionate
sleep disruption (the sleep homeostat or the circadian timer; Bor- impact on current cognitions. This article is based, first, on a
bely, 1994), sleep disruption sometimes persists. When it persists framework that has been helpful in addressing many other clinical
for longer than a month (and is not a side effect of other medical, disorders and, second, on an extension of this framework to
neurologic, psychiatric, or psychological disorders or the result of encompass subclinical features of sleep disorder. This extension
a circadian rhythm disorder), it is called primary insomnia derives from research on attentional bias in relation to understand-
(Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. ing alcohol consumption variability along the entire alcohol use–
[DSM–IV]; American Psychiatric Association, 1994). This report misuse–abuse– dependence continuum, not just at the clinical pole.
explores a possible role for attentional bias in the perpetuation and First, support has been found implicating attentional bias in the
possibly the precipitation of primary insomnia. An attentional bias perpetuation of a wide range of anxiety-related psychological
toward a particular class of stimulus (e.g., sleep-related) is said to disorders and concerns (see Mogg & Bradley, 1998, for a review).
Support has also been found implicating attentional bias in the
perpetuation of a family of substance abuse and dependence dis-
Editor’s Note. Colin MacLeod served as the action editor for this arti- orders: for example, with alcohol (Sharma, Albery, & Cook,
cle.—TBB 2001), heroin (Franken, Kroon, Wiers, & Jansen, 2000) and nic-
otine (Waters & Feyerabend, 2000). Principally, through the use of
the emotional Stroop paradigm (Williams, Mathews, & MacLeod,
Barry T. Jones and Lauren M. Macphee, Department of Psychology,
University of Glasgow, Glasgow, Scotland; Niall S. Broomfield and Colin
1996), the extent to which attention is captured by disorder-related
A. Espie, Division of Community-Based Sciences in Psychological Med- stimuli as compared with neutral stimuli has been revealed. Gen-
icine, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, Scotland; Benedict C. Jones, erally, but not exclusively, an attentional bias has been found in
School of Psychology, Aberdeen University, Scotland. those who have a clinical diagnosis but absent in those who have
This research and preparation of the article was supported in part by not—this would be more completely termed a differential atten-
Grant CZH/4/2 from the Chief Scientist Office, Scottish Executive Health tional bias. Finding such a differential attentional bias has helped
Department; the Carnegie Trust for the Universities of Scotland; and explain why disorders (e.g., Mogg, Mathews, Bird, & MacGregor-
Glasgow University and St Andrews University graduate scholarships. Morris, 1990) and abuses and dependences (e.g., Drummond,
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Barry T.
Tiffany, Glautier, & Remington, 1995) are frequently self-
Jones, Department of Psychology, University of Glasgow, Glasgow G12 8
QQ, Scotland, United Kingdom or Colin A. Espie, Division of
maintaining and why relapse so frequently occurs after initially
Community-Based Sciences in Psychological Medicine, University of successful treatment. In explanations such as these, attentional bias
Glasgow, Academic Centre, Gartnavel Royal Hospital, 1055 Great West- is conceived as an involuntary (i.e., unconscious, implicit) process
ern Road, Glasgow G12 0XH, Scotland, United Kingdom. E-mail: but (important to note) gives rise to voluntary (i.e., conscious,
barry@psy.gla.ac.uk or c.espie@clinmed.gla.ac.uk explicit) processes. These cognitions feature as part of the disorder

249
250 JONES, MACPHEE, BROOMFIELD, JONES, AND ESPIE

and can contribute to subsequent behavior decisions. Franken, Moreover, it has been found in social drinkers, using explicit (e.g.,
Kroon, and Hendriks (2000); Franken, Kroon, Wiers, and Jansen Lee, Greeley, & Oei, 1999; McMahon, Jones, & O’Donnell, 1994)
(2000); and Ingjaldsson, Thayer, and Laberg (2003), in particular, and implicit (Gadon, Bruce, McConnochie, & Jones, 2004; Gadon
related attentional bias and craving with risk of relapse in sub- & Jones, 2002; Leigh & Stacy, 1998) methodologies, that the
stance abuse and dependence. number, range, and severity of negative expectancies increase as
Williams et al.’s (1996) model of emotional Stroop performance consumption increases along the continuum from infrequent and
(extending Cohen, Dunbar, & McClelland’s, 1990, network model lighter to frequent and heavier drinker—predicting a systemati-
of the standard Stroop effect) accounts for attentional bias at the cally changing attentional bias along the continuum. It is important
clinical pole of disorders, such as those mentioned above, through to note that the progressive increase in negative expectancies
the increased responsivity of network input units representing would not necessarily restrain consumption (a macrobehavior)
disorder-related concepts—an increase caused by “a history of until a threshold was exceeded (B. T. Jones, 2004; B. T. Jones &
association with [anxiety-generating] threat or loss and thereby McMahon, 1998) but that while subthreshold, it would progres-
subject to neuromodulatory control affecting the responsivity of sively impact on microbehaviors such as emotional Stroop perfor-
those units” (Williams et al., 1996, p. 17). As noted earlier, mance. While subthreshold, consumption would be driven by
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

attentional biases, which reflect discrete changes in the direction of positive expectancies (e.g., Goldman, Del Boca, & Darkes, 1999)
attentional focus in response to “threat,” have been revealed in the and because an attentional bias would prime positive expectancies,
anxiety disorders (e.g., Keogh, Dillon, Georgiou, & Hunt, 2001). the apparent paradox is resolved that at levels of social drinking,
We would argue that attentional bias may also be a strong candi- increases in anxiety caused by increases in negative expectancies
date model for the perpetuation of primary insomnia. Sleep is a would increase consumption.
fundamental life process much like eating or drinking. Thus, Little comparable research (either at the clinical pole or along
inability to sleep can be conceptualized as a significant threat as the sleep-problems continuum) has been carried out on sleep-
well as an anxiety generator (Espie, 2002). We believe that atten- related attentional bias, which is surprising because interest in the
tional biases favoring sleep-related stimuli will, during an acute control that sleep-related objects might have over sleep behavior is
insomnia phase, lead to persistent insomnia in two ways: by long established (Bootzin, 1972). For example, within a condition-
promoting sleep preoccupation and by driving heightened sleep ing framework, bedroom environment objects might become dis-
effort. This hypothesis is consistent with the reported association criminative stimuli for sleep (e.g., Bootzin, Epstein, & Wood,
between insomnia and both excessive presleep cognitive activity 1991), but when the bedroom–sleep contingencies are broken, they
(Wicklow & Espie, 2000) and effortful attempts to control sleep might become discriminative stimuli for wakefulness. Meta-
onset (Broomfield & Espie, 2003). Together, these two processes analyses have suggested that treatments for insomnia that purport
are likely to prevent successful cognitive and somatic de-arousal, to modify the stimulus control the bedroom environment has over
which will inhibit the recovery of normal sleep (Espie, 2002). sleep are indeed effective (Morin et al., 1999; Murtagh & Green-
Harvey (2002) makes parallel predictions. In her cognitive model, wood, 1995; Smith et al., 2002)—to the extent that they are
worries regarding inability to sleep, and the consequent daytime recommended by the American Academy of Sleep Medicine as the
impact of sleep loss, are thought to promote autonomic arousal and standard nonpharmacological intervention (Chesson et al., 1999).
anxiety that in turn drive selective attention toward internal– Yet, evaluations of the principles of stimulus control within the
external sleep-related threat cues. Again, it is proposed that sleep sleep environment have produced inconsistent data (see Espie,
disturbance is maintained by an attentional bias favoring sleep- 2002, for a review), which raises the possibility that considerations
relevant cues. of stimulus control extend beyond the conditioning framework and
Second, although differential attentional biases have generally that treatments might have other active ingredients.
been sought between those who have a diagnosed disorder and In an effort to more fully understand the impact that the bed-
those who do not, efforts to understand alcohol consumption room environment might have on sleep, this article goes beyond
variability show that a systematically changing differential atten- the conditioning framework described above (which addresses one
tional bias toward alcohol-related stimuli might be present along form of stimulus control) to a framework of attentional bias (which
the consumption continuum (of use, misuse, abuse, dependence), addresses another). Two previous studies have explored sleep-
not just at its clinical pole. For example, a differential attentional related attentional bias using a textual Stroop paradigm. In one,
bias has been found with (a) “alcoholic” drinkers (using “nonal- Lundh, Froding, Gyllenhammer, Broman, and Hetta (1997) found
coholic” controls; Johnsen, Laberg, Cox, Vaksdal, & Hugdahl, that individuals with primary insomnia showed no effect. In the
1994), (b) “problem” drinkers (“nonproblem” controls; Sharma et other, Taylor et al. (2003) found an attentional bias in cancer
al., 2001), (c) “heavy” social drinkers (“light” social drinking patients with persistent insomnia (12–18 months) as compared
controls; Bruce & Jones, 2005), and (d) “frequent social” drinkers with cancer patients with acute insomnia (0 –3 months). In the
(“occasional social” drinking controls; Townshend & Duka, 2001). former study, however, only one fourth sleep-related words rep-
Moreover, even at the clinical pole itself, Ryan (2002) and Jones, resented bedroom objects and, in the latter, only 3/20; conse-
Bruce, Livingstone, and Reed (in press) have shown that the extent quently, although informative, neither study addressed the possible
of attentional bias exhibited by problem drinkers in treatment is influence of the bedroom environment on sleep.
positively related to their problem severity. Applying Williams et Rather than use the textual Stroop paradigm, we explore sleep-
al.’s (1996) model to the anxiety generated within the context of related attentional bias with digitized objects using a flicker par-
the negative expectations (or expectancies) of future consumption adigm (Rensink, O’Regan, & Clark, 1997; Simons & Levin, 1997)
held by those at the clinical pole (B. T. Jones, 2004; B. T. Jones, featuring a perceptual phenomenon called induced change blind-
Corbin, & Fromme, 2001) helps explain this differential bias. ness (ICB; Rensink, 2002; Simons, 2000; Simons & Rensink,
ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN INSOMNIA 251

2005). This research reveals that when a change is made to a visual .84; Backhaus, Junghanns, Broocks, Riemann, & Hohagen, 2002). Finally,
scene (and the process of change is hidden from view), it is more a local questionnaire was given that collected information on age, gender,
difficult to detect than might be expected. Normally in this para- and whether individuals had suffered from any psychopathology or other
digm, a single feature of a visual scene is changed between illness known to affect sleep or had any sleep complaint.
The global PSQI score has been shown to be effective in discriminating
successively repeated brief presentations until the change is de-
between nonclinical (0 –5) and clinical (6 –21) sleepers (e.g., Backhaus et
tected. Change-detection latency is explained by a change’s “grab-
al., 2002), and we use it for this purpose. In addition, however, we seek to
biness” (O’Regan & Noe, 2000), and this depends not just on the explore the possible continuity of attentional bias along the sleep-problems
object’s physical features that carry the change but also on the continuum by using the PSQI’s continuous measure of sleep problems to
viewer’s history in relation to that object. Using this technique, further classify nonclinical sleepers using the end regions (0 –2 and 4 –5) of
researchers (B. C. Jones, Jones, Blundell, & Bruce, 2002; B. T. the nonclinical PSQI range (0 –5). We identify participants as “good” and
Jones, Jones, Smith, & Copely, 2003) have found differential “moderate” sleepers, respectively, discriminating them from “poor” sleep-
attentional biases between two levels of social use of alcohol and ers and thereby creating three points on the sleep-problems continuum that
cannabis. Here, we extend their approach to explore differential can be compared. We acknowledge that our concept of moderate sleeper
attentional biases along the sleep-problems continuum. has, perhaps, more limitations than good sleeper, but we justify inclusion
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

of this group for two reasons. First, epidemiological data clearly demon-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

We postulate that a systematically changing attentional bias will


strate that prevalence of insomnia varies depending on the question asked.
be found (at different points of the sleep-problems continuum) to
Whereas 25–30% of the adult population report dissatisfaction with their
exist toward changes made to bedroom environment objects as sleep, the true prevalence of the clinical disorder is around half that rate,
compared with changes made to other objects. We predict it will be 10 –15% (Ford & Kamerow, 1989; Gallup Organization, 1991; Oyahon,
greatest in poorest sleepers (having primary insomnia diagnosed), Caulet, & Guilleminault, 1997). Second, because (a) there were clear
reduced in moderate sleepers (not having a clinical diagnosis but criteria for establishing a poor sleeper category, (b) the distance between
having more sleep problems than good sleepers), and absent in the poor sleeper and good sleeper category was substantial, and (c) we
good sleepers (having no or few sleep problems). We propose that wished to test the hypothesis that there would be systematically modified
(a) anxiety is a mediator of sleep-related attentional bias at the attentional bias across the sleep-problems continuum, we felt that the
clinical pole (Espie, 2002; Harvey, 2002), just as with alcohol- conceptual delineation of a middle category would be appropriate.
related attentional bias at the clinical pole of the consumption
continuum, and (b) that different subclinical points of the sleep- Design
problems continuum will generate different levels of anxiety, just
as with subclinical levels of alcohol problems. Through Williams The hypothesis tested was that participants with PSQI-indicated sleep
et al.’s (1996) model, we predict an increasing sleep-related atten- problems will detect a change made to one of a collection of sleep-related
tional bias toward bedroom environment objects from good objects quicker than a change made to one of a collection of neutral objects,
through moderate to poor sleepers. We use a highly representative as compared with participants with fewer PSQI-indicated sleep problems.
set of bedroom objects and test this hypothesis with a change An entirely between-participants design was adopted with the following
carried by the one most representative. three factors: nature of change to be detected (two levels: sleep-related and
neutral), gender (two levels: male and female), and PSQI-indicated sleep
quality (three levels: good, moderate, and poor). The dependent variable
Method was change-detection latency.
Sleep problems approximately double from adulthood to later life, and
Participants women present about twice as often as men (e.g., Espie, 2002; Ford &
Kamerow, 1989; Li, Wing, Ho, & Fong, 2002). Accordingly, groups were
Students, staff, and visitors on the campus of a large city university were matched for age (on an individual basis rather than a group-mean basis),
asked to take part in a 15-min experiment in a nearby quiet room. Each of and gender was included as a factor in the design.
the opportunistically recruited 302 volunteers were tested singly. Using the A target of approximately 300 participants was estimated to be necessary
procedures described below, we selected 192 for analysis (M age ⫽ 32.1 to allow for the application of the exclusion and matching procedures
years, SD ⫽ 12.6). described below. Each participant was given the flicker ICB change-
detection task followed by the BDI, the PSQI, and a local questionnaire.
Measures Once recruited into the experiment, participants were randomly assigned to
one of four groups generated by crossing the factors of nature of change
Each participant completed a computer task that measured his or her and gender. Group sizes of 75 or 76 were planned. For each of the four
visual change-detection latency (the dependent variable) to changes in groups, participants who (a) self-reported an illness known to affect sleep
photographic scenes described below. They then completed three question- (i.e., 2), (b) scored above 9 on the BDI (10 –18 indicates moderate-mild
naires (used for participant exclusion and retrospective group assignment depression [Beck & Steer, 1987]; no participants were found to be in this
with age-matching). First, the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck & category), or (c) failed to satisfy the task requirements (i.e., to correctly
Steer. 1987) was given, followed by the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index identify the change to which they were exposed—9 participants) were
(PSQI; Buysse, Reynolds, Monk, Berman, & Kupfer, 1989). The PSQI excluded from the analyses. For analyses purposes, the remaining partic-
provides a global measure of sleep quality by aggregating the scores on a ipants of each of the four groups were retrospectively assigned to one of the
0 –3 subscale (3 is the negative pole) of seven different areas of sleep: three levels of the factor of sleep quality, on the basis of their PSQI scores:
subjective sleep quality, sleep latency, sleep duration, habitual sleep effi- good sleepers (PSQI score 0 –2 inclusive), moderate sleepers (4 –5 inclu-
ciency, sleep disturbances, use of sleep medication, and daytime dysfunc- sive), and poor sleepers (⬎ 5). Assignment to the newly formed 12 groups
tion. The PSQI is a widely used, internally consistent (Cronbach’s alpha ⫽ (2 ⫻ 2 ⫻ 3) was constrained by matching for age (to within 4 years) and
.83) screening instrument for the detection of significant sleep disturbance the goal of equal group sizes (to obtain the best estimate of between-groups
(using a threshold score of 6). Recent, independent study has validated this variation). Retrospective group assignment was blind to the dependent
cut-off and confirmed reliability (Cronbach’s alpha ⫽ .85, test–retest r ⫽ variable of the analyses: change-detection latency. Unmatched participants
252 JONES, MACPHEE, BROOMFIELD, JONES, AND ESPIE

were excluded from the analyses. Analyses were carried out on the 192
remaining participants, with a group size of 16.

Apparatus and Stimuli


An Apple iBook (MacOS 9.1) and the experiment-generation package
SuperLab 1.75 (Cedrus Corporation, San Pedro, CA) were used to imple-
ment the flicker paradigm. Photographic stimuli were presented centrally
and almost filled the iBook’s 28-cm (diagonal) screen. The viewing dis-
tance was approximately 65 cm normal to the screen.
A different flicker pair of stimuli were used for each of the two levels of
the nature of change factor (sleep-related and neutral). Each of the two
pairs contained the same original stimulus (OS) shown in Figure 1. The OS
was a full-color photograph (1280 ⫻ 960 pixels) taken in natural daylight
of seven sleep-related objects and an equal number of neutral objects
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

arranged in two collections on either side of the scene midline. The second
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

stimulus of each pair was identical to the OS but for one small change: a
sleep-related change was introduced for the “sleep” level of the nature of
change factor (stimulus conditioned stimulus [CS-S]) and a neutral change
for the “neutral” level of the same factor (stimulus CS-N). The two
changed stimuli are shown in Figure 1 along with their common originating
stimulus. Within a flicker paradigm, the two stimuli of a pair (OS/CS-S for Figure 2. A flicker paradigm for inducing change blindness illustrating a
the six sleep change groups and OS/CS-N for the six neutral change change cycle or single “flicker.” os ⫽ original stimulus; cs ⫽ changed
groups) are presented in continuous succession (each replacing the other, in stimulus.
register, on the iBook screen) until the change from the original to the
changed stimulus (or from the changed stimulus to the original stimulus) is was done in three stages. First, by asking 60 students (50% female) to list
detected (see Figure 2). In the current experiment, following usual practice at least 5 objects that they would associate with “going to bed to sleep.”
to suppress visual transients produced by the change process, a mask Their lists were compiled to identify the 12 most listed objects. Second,
comprising a rectilinear matrix of Xs was presented on the screen between examples of these objects were photographed singly, and the set of 12
the OS and the changed versions (CS-S or CS-N). The OS and either the objects were embedded in another set of 12 photographs of neutral objects
CS-S or the CS-N were presented for 250 ms, and the mask was presented (thought not to be bedroom and sleep-related by the experimenters and
for 80 ms with no other interstimulus interval and no intertrial interval none of which had appeared in the 60 lists of sleep-related objects collected
(B. C. Jones et al., 2002; B. T. Jones et al., 2003). earlier). This created a 4 ⫻ 6 matrix of photographs of 12 sleep-related and
Precautions were taken to ensure that the seven sleep-related objects 12 neutral objects. Finally, another 30 students (50% female) were asked to
included in the OS were highly representative of the sleep environment and rate the 24 photographs of the objects on a 1–10 “sleep-relatedness” scale
that the object carrying the sole sleep-related change to be detected (1 ⫽ highly sleep-related, 10 ⫽ not sleep-related at all). The 7 objects with
throughout the experiment was the most representative of the seven. This the highest mean sleep-relatedness score (mean range ⫽ 1.2–5.8) were
selected for the sleep-related collection of objects in the OS, and the 7 with
the lowest mean score (all had means of 10) were selected for the neutral
collection. The sleep-related objects were teddy bear, pajamas, pillow,
alarm clock, hot water bottle, hand cream, and pair of slippers. The pair of
slippers had the highest sleep-related score. The neutral objects were
rucksack, journal issue, files, bottle of ink, office tray, telescoped umbrella,
and pair of gloves. The CS-S was made by removing one of the pair of
slippers (the most representative sleep-related object) from the OS and
photographing the remaining scene, and the CS-N was made by removing
one of the pair of gloves from the OS. (In the absence of differential
sleep-relatedness scores in this group, this object was chosen because it
was physically most similar to the object carrying the sleep-related
change.)
In earlier applications of the flicker paradigm to explore attentional
biases in the social use of alcohol and cannabis, B. C. Jones et al. (2002)
and B. T. Jones et al. (2003) used both normal and mirror-reversal versions
of the original and changed stimuli as a between factor. This was done to
control for possible carry-over effects from, for example, lifelong reading
practices generating side preferences in visual processing. In the four
studies they reported, however, B. C. Jones et al. (2002, B. T. Jones et al.,
2003) found no significant main effects for or interactive effects implicat-
ing the factor of normal/mirror-reversal. For this reason, mirror-reversals
of stimuli OS and CS-S/CS-N were not included in the current experiment.

Figure 1. Gray-scale versions of the full color stimuli used: original Procedure
stimulus (OS) and the two changed stimuli for each of the two levels of the
nature of change factor—sleep-related change (CS-S) and neutral change Potential participants were approached throughout the campus and asked
(CS-N). to take part in an experiment. Care was taken during the recruitment period
ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN INSOMNIA 253

to ensure that the sleep-related nature of the experiment was not apparent. erate, and good sleepers for the sleep-related change (Ms ⫽ 14.5,
Those who agreed to take part were taken to a nearby quiet room and asked 15.5, and 23.1, respectively; SDs ⫽ 9.4, 10.7, and 7.6, respec-
to read the instructions on an iBook screen on what they should expect. tively), F(2, 180) ⫽ 6.59, p ⬍ .01. These differences were located
Participants were then asked to press the space bar when they were ready, using t tests. It was found that poor sleepers detected the sleep-
at which time they would see a visual scene presented on the screen for less
related change significantly more quickly than good sleepers (M ⫽
than a second. It would keep appearing and disappearing for as long as it
14.5 vs. M ⫽ 23.1; SDs ⫽ 8.5 vs. 7.6), t(191) ⫽ 3.33, p ⬍ .01, and
took them to spot a small change made to the scene between successive
presentations. When they had spotted the change, participants were asked moderate sleepers detected it significantly more quickly than good
to immediately press the space bar, which would be recorded by the sleepers (M ⫽ 15.5 vs. M ⫽ 23.1, SDs ⫽ 9.1 vs. 10.1), t(191) ⫽
computer, and then to say what the change was. Participants were then 2.90, p ⬍ .01. There was no difference in the change-detection
asked to complete the BDI, the PSQI, and the local questionnaire. It would latency for the sleep-related change between poor and moderate
only have been through reading the contents of the PSQI and the local sleepers, however (M ⫽ 14.5 and M ⫽ 15.5, respectively; SDs ⫽
questionnaire (given after the flicker paradigm) that participants would 9.4 and 10.7, respectively), t(191) ⫽ 0.34, p ⬎ .05.
have been explicitly exposed to the sleep-related nature of the experiment. Second, poor sleepers detected the sleep-related change signif-
Once the questionnaires had been completed and passed to the experi- icantly more quickly than the neutral change (Ms ⫽ 14.5 and 21.9,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

menter, the true purpose of the experiment was explained. Participants


respectively; SDs ⫽ 9.4 and 12.1, respectively), F(1, 180) ⫽ 7.11,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

were also provided with contact information in case they wanted to be


p ⬍ .01, displaying a sleep-related attentional bias. Although
informed of the outcome of the project when it was completed.
moderate sleepers showed the same difference direction as poor
sleepers, the difference between sleep-related and neutral change
Results detections was not significant (Ms ⫽ 15.5 and 18.4, respectively;
As indicated earlier, age and BDI-indicated depression associ- SDs ⫽ 10.7 and 10.9, respectively), F(1, 180) ⫽ 1.29, p ⬎ .05. By
ates with sleep problems. Consequently, two analyses of variance contrast, good sleepers detected the change within the neutral
(ANOVAs) were carried out prior to the main analyses. A objects significantly more quickly than within the sleep-related
matching-check for age across the 12 groups was carried out using objects (Ms ⫽ 16.5 and 23.1, respectively; SDs ⫽ 6.8 and 7.6,
an ANOVA that was identically structured to the main analysis, respectively), F(1, 180) ⫽ 6.21, p ⬍ .05, showing a bias toward
that is, a three-factor between-participants ANOVA (N ⫽ 192, n ⫽ neutral rather than sleep-related objects. The effect size for poor
16) but with age as the dependent variable and with the following sleepers (see Figure 4) was significant and “moderate” using
factors and levels: nature of change (sleep-related, neutral), gender Cohen’s (1992) classificatory scheme for effect size comparisons
(male, female), and sleep quality (poor, moderate, good). With age (Cohen’s d ⫽ 0.60; 95% confidence limits of 0.07 and 1.06) as
as the dependent variable, there were no main, two-way, or three- compared with a “small” and nonsignificant effect size for mod-
way interactive effects. This indicates a good degree of equiva- erate sleepers (d ⫽ 0.21; 95% confidence limits of ⫺0.17 and
lence in age between the 12 different groups. A second identical 0.51). The effect size of the opposite bias shown by the good
ANOVA was carried out with BDI scores as the dependent vari- sleepers was significant and “large” (Cohen’s d ⫽ ⫺0.91; 95%
able; this analysis also showed no main or interactive effects confidence limits of ⫺0.41 and ⫺1.46).
(overall BDI score: M ⫽ 3.1, SD ⫽ 2.1), indicating a correspond- The foregoing is consistent with poor, moderate, and good
ing equivalence. sleepers, together responding equivalently to the neutral change
but responding differently to the sleep-related change. Further, no
difference between the neutral and sleep-related change is found
Analysis 1: ANOVA
for the moderate sleepers but for the poor sleepers, there is a
The number of flickers to correct change detection for each decrease in change-detection latency whereas for good sleepers,
participant was entered into a three-factor between-participants there is an increase.
ANOVA (structured as above; N ⫽ 192, n ⫽ 16). There were no
significant main effects. Sleep-related change-detection latencies Analysis 2: Regression
were no different from neutral change-detection latencies (Ms ⫽
17.7 and 19.0, respectively; SDs ⫽ 12.3 and 11.1, respectively), Two hierarchical regression analyses were carried out for the
F(1, 180) ⫽ 0.51, p ⬎ .05; womens’ change-detection latencies two different levels of the nature of change factor (sleep-related
were no different from mens’ (Ms ⫽ 18.2 and 18.5; SDs ⫽ 7.9 and and neutral) in order to test the relationship between change-
9.4, respectively), F(1, 180) ⫽ 0.07, p ⬎ .05; and there were no detection latency and a continuous representation of the global
differences between the change-detection latencies of poor, mod- PSQI score (rather than a categorical representation, as was used in
erate, and good sleepers (Ms ⫽ 18.4, 16.9, and 19.8, respectively; the ANOVA). With change-detection latency as the dependent
SDs ⫽ 7.8, 7.1, and 9.7, respectively), F(2, 180) ⫽ 1.35, p ⬎ .05. variable, age, gender (for reasons outlined earlier), and the BDI
Of the one 3-way and three 2-way interactions, only the Change ⫻ score were first added to the regression model and then the PSQI
Sleep Quality interaction was significant, F(2, 180) ⫽ 7.12, p ⬍ score. Although BDI scores above 9 represent the severity of a
.01 (see Figure 3). The interaction was interpreted using tests for clinical diagnosis associated with increasingly poor sleep, this does
simple main effects. This was done in two ways. not necessarily mean that the BDI score range 0 –9 can be used to
First, for the neutral change, no differences in change-detection represent states increasingly proximate to a clinical diagnosis (and
latency were found between the poor, moderate, and good sleepers with commensurate associations with sleep quality). Nevertheless,
(Ms ⫽ 21.9, 18.4, and 16.5, respectively; SDs ⫽ 12.1, 10.9, and entering the “subclinical” BDI score into the regression model
6.8, respectively), F(2, 180) ⫽ 1.69, p ⬎ .05. By contrast, differ- prior to the PSQI score appears to be a sensible precaution. The
ences in change-detection latency were found between poor, mod- incremental variance explained when the PSQI score is added to
254 JONES, MACPHEE, BROOMFIELD, JONES, AND ESPIE
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 3. Mean sleep-related and neutral change-detection latencies (number of flickers to detection) in poor,
moderate, and good sleepers. PSQI ⫽ Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index; SEM ⫽ standard error of the mean.

the model already containing age, gender, and the BDI score is the raised resting activity level is the basis of the attentional bias we
critical test of the relationship. predict will be found for bedroom objects in primary insomnia.
In both models (sleep-related change and the neutral change), This means that representations of the bedroom environment will
there was no significant variance explained in change-detection be more likely to enter current cognitions, generate anxiety, and
latency when age, gender, and the BDI scores were added. When disrupt sleep.
the PSQI score was added to the model for the sleep-related Our results support this prediction, showing an attentional bias
change, however, the model deviated from the null, F(4, 92) ⫽ toward a representative bedroom object in sleepers who satisfy the
2.51, p ⬍ .05. The incremental variance explained on the basis of minimal criteria for primary insomnia (our poor sleepers); change-
adjusted R2 was 10.6% ( p ⬍ .05), and the beta for PSQI was detection latencies in poor sleepers were quicker when the change
⫺0.310 ( p ⬍ .05). For the neutral change, the model did not was made to the sleep-related object than to the neutral object.
deviate from the null when the PSQI score was added, F(4, 92) ⫽ There was also found a marked differential attentional bias be-
2.29, p ⬎ .05. tween poor sleepers and the good-sleeping controls. However,
Thus, there is no relationship between PSQI-indicated sleep whereas the poor sleepers showed a bias toward the sleep-related
problems and change-detection latency for the neutral change, but object as predicted, the good sleepers showed a bias toward the
for the sleep-related change, change-detection latency decreases as
neutral object. This part of our prediction, therefore, does not
sleep problems increase.
appear to be supported—namely, that because good sleepers would
be reporting few if any sleep problems, there would be little
Discussion anxiety generated within the sleep-problems context, negligible
Espie’s (2002) and Harvey’s (2002) models of primary insom- changes in the resting activity levels of the sleep-related network
nia identify attentional bias toward sleep-related concepts as con- input units, and therefore a negligible sleep-related attentional
tributing to the perpetuation of primary insomnia. In both models, bias. There are two explanations why the good-sleeping compo-
the heightened arousal accompanying insomnia and the belief that nent of the differential attentional bias might occur.
the night’s sleep problems will negatively impact on next day’s First, it is possible that the good sleepers’ choices are driven by
activities generate anxiety. Williams et al. (1996) postulated that the physical properties (physical saliencies) of the sleep-related
anxiety raises the resting activity level of network inputs repre- and neutral objects themselves, rather than semantic saliencies.
senting concepts with which the anxiety temporally occurs. The Particularly in complex stimuli (e.g., real-world objects collected
ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN INSOMNIA 255
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 4. Effect size (Cohen’s d) and 95% confidence limits of net attentional bias in poor, moderate, and good
sleepers (net attentional bias calculated as sleep-related minus neutral change-detection latencies). PSQI ⫽
Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index.

into a scene), values of the objects’ physical properties will com- the design), there will be little associated anxiety and, conse-
pete for attention resources and contribute to, or even drive, what quently, negligible sleep-related attentional bias. In addition, in
is attended to and what reaches consciousness. Physical properties such a case, any attentional bias would be driven by the physical
such as object size, color, brightness, complexity, and overlap as saliencies of the grouped objects. We would conclude, therefore,
well as relative positional and configurational aspects will be that either the group of neutral objects in which the neutral change
important in this respect. This means that any single attentional was embedded, or the neutral objects that carry the neutral change
bias measure (e.g., the comparison between the change-detection had more “grabby” physical properties than did the corresponding
latencies of the group of good sleepers given the sleep-related sleep-related objects; this would explain the neutral attentional
change with the other group of good sleepers given the neutral bias shown by the good sleepers. When measuring differential
changes) is ambiguous because it confounds the sleep salience attentional bias, however (i.e., the difference in attentional bias
manipulation (i.e., the sleep-related change vs. the neutral change) between poor sleepers and good sleepers), we are able to control
with a range of other nonsleep saliencies. If the good sleepers have the physical saliencies (and any other nonsleep semantic salien-
comparatively problem-free sleep (a group assignment feature of cies) through giving the poor and good sleeping groups the same
256 JONES, MACPHEE, BROOMFIELD, JONES, AND ESPIE

stimulus presentations. The interaction representing the differential If sleep problems are maintained beyond an acute phase by atten-
attentional bias between poor and good sleepers is, therefore, the tional bias, however, and if the length of time satisfies the criterion
properly controlled measure of the effect of the PSQI-derived for primary insomnia, it suggests that attentional bias might not
sleep-problem manipulation on sleep-related attentional bias. In- only be implicated in perpetuating primary insomnia but also in
deed, it follows from this explanation that the apparent sleep- precipitating it.
related attentional bias shown by the poor sleepers will be an Our study is the first to report a sleep-related differential atten-
underestimate of the real sleep-related attentional bias and will tional bias between good and poor sleepers and a possible corre-
only be provided through the measure of differential attentional sponding differential bias between good and moderate sleepers. It
bias. also identifies a likely source perpetuating primary insomnia and a
Second, there is an alternative explanation for the neutral bias possible precipitating factor. We find only partial support for a
found in good sleepers that makes no reference to physical salien- continuity of sleep-related attentional bias corresponding to the
cies. It is possible that our good sleepers were low in anxiety- sleep-problems’ continuum, however, because of the failure to find
sensitivity (Reiss, 1997), given that, using a dot probe paradigm, a differential attentional bias between the moderate and poor
Keogh et al. (2001) have shown that participants with low anxiety- sleepers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

sensitivity under some circumstances orient away from threatening Our findings and conclusions are limited by a number of con-
stimuli rather than toward them—in which case, our good sleepers siderations. First, the clinical pole (primary insomnia) was repre-
should orient away from the set of bedroom objects if they were sented by participants with PSQI scores greater than 5. The incor-
low in anxiety-sensitivity. If this occurred, it would assist neutral poration in future research of an additional “primary insomnia”
change detection but hinder sleep-related change detection and group with a higher inclusion threshold would provide an addi-
explain the bias toward the neutral set of stimuli that good sleepers tional opportunity to compare more distant clinical and subclinical
show. Although there might be grounds for suggesting that good points on the sleep-problems continuum as well as points within
sleepers might indeed be low in trait anxiety (because this dispo- the diagnostic category—providing a more extensive test of the
sition might either contribute to or derive from their good-sleeping hypothesis of continuity of attentional bias. Second, only a single
status), there is less to suggest that they might be low in anxiety- sleep-related change and neutral change were used, and this might
sensitivity and, as Keogh et al. noted, it is the latter rather than the compromise generalizing the findings and conclusions to other
former that would be an important feature of this type of expla- bedroom and neutral objects. Care was taken, however, to ensure
nation. Nevertheless, it remains a possibility. that all the objects used were either highly representative of the
However the differential attentional bias between two poles of bedroom environment or not representative at all. Further, the most
the sleep-problems continuum is explained, it remains that the poor representative bedroom object was chosen to carry the sleep
sleepers appear to attend to a sleep-related object more than a change. Finally, Bruce and Jones (2005) have recently shown with
neutral object. If our finding with one sleep object generalizes to social drinkers that change detection performance in a flicker
others, the attentional bias that it represents will feed the processes paradigm is almost entirely driven by the set of different objects in
that maintain or perpetuate primary insomnia described by Espie which the single object carrying the change (also a set member) is
(2002) and Harvey (2002). There is some indication that atten- embedded rather than by the single object itself. If this finding
tional bias might be an agent for not just the perpetuation of extends to our sleep study (which uses similar stimulus configu-
primary insomnia but also its precipitation—this possibility is rations), this limitation is reduced because seven sleep objects
raised through a consideration of the moderate sleepers’ perfor- make up the sleep set and another seven make up the neutral set.
mance. On the surface, the moderate sleepers do not appear to Nevertheless, our finding requires replication, and future research
show an attentional bias toward the sleep-related object as com- should extend the range of objects used. Furthermore, we do not
pared with the neutral object. If, however, the explanation of the have anxiety and anxiety-sensitivity data, and the possibility that
performance of the good sleepers is indeed based on physical good sleepers low in anxiety sensitivity might be orienting away
saliencies (as described above), then the good-sleepers perfor- from the sleep-related objects cannot be evaluated. Future research
mance represents a baseline condition against which the perfor- needs to pay attention to this potential explanatory source by
mance of the moderate sleepers can be judged, with all nonsleep collecting anxiety data.
saliencies controlled. When this comparison is made, good and Although it is some 30 years since stimulus control was pro-
moderate sleepers do not treat the neutral change differently (and posed as offering an explanatory, and potentially modifiable, par-
neither do poor sleepers); by critical contrast, however, the mod- adigm in insomnia (Bootzin, 1972), contemporary sources agree
erate sleepers detect the sleep-related change significantly more that its role in the development and amelioration of persistent
quickly than do the good sleepers. The apparent lack of attentional insomnia remains unclear (Edinger & Wohlgemuth, 1999; Espie,
bias in moderate sleepers, derived from comparing only their 2002; Morin et al., 1999). In large part, this is because research
detection latencies to the sleep-related and neutral change, should effort has focused on treatment efficacy derived from stimulus
not obscure the fact that a differential attentional bias is revealed control intervention rather than on experimental studies. We are
from good to moderate sleepers when more appropriately con- now in the position where a psychological rather than pharmaco-
trolled comparisons are made. If moderate sleepers do indeed logical approach is seen as the treatment of choice for persistent
exhibit a sleep-related attentional bias— generated by the anxiety primary insomnia (Espie, 1999; Smith et al., 2002) and stimulus
accompanying acute sleep problems, not diagnosed as the more control instructions meet American Academy of Sleep Medicine
chronic primary insomnia—it could help maintain these sleep criteria for “standard treatment” (Chesson et al., 1999). The
problems in much the same way as Espie (2002) and Harvey present study, which uses a novel experimental approach well
(2002) suggested that attentional bias maintains primary insomnia. suited to the systematic investigation of the stimulus control par-
ATTENTIONAL BIAS IN INSOMNIA 257

adigm, illustrates one way forward for research in this area. Evi- disturbances and psychiatric disorders. Journal of the American Medical
dence presented of attentional bias toward the bedroom environ- Association, 262, 1479 –1484.
ment, and our previously reported evidence of attentional bias Franken, I. H. A., Kroon, L. Y., & Hendriks, V. M. (2000). Influence of
toward textual representations of sleep and sleeplessness (Taylor et individual differences in craving and obsessive cocaine thoughts on
al., 2003), raises the possibility that dysfunctional stimulus control attentional processes in cocaine abuse patients. Addictive Behaviors, 25,
99 –102.
in insomnia develops from the conditioning of nonverbal and
Franken, I. H. A., Kroon, L. Y., Wiers, R. W., & Jansen, A. (2000).
verbal signals as threat cues that impact on selective attention Selective cognitive processing of drug cues in heroin dependence. Jour-
through the principles of Williams et al.’s (1996) model—starting nal of Psychopharmacology, 14, 395– 400.
a vicious cycle. The feasibility of objectively and accurately mea- Gadon, L., Bruce, G., McConnochie, F., & Jones, B. T. (2004). Negative
suring this bias, therefore, yields new opportunity for both exper- alcohol consumption outcome associations in young and mature adult
imental and clinical investigations of the development of primary social drinkers: The route to drinking restraint? Addictive Behaviors, 29,
insomnia during remission and treatment response. 1373–1387.
Gadon, L., & Jones, B. T. (2002). Implicit alcohol associations increase in
number as time goes by—Though somewhat unevenly. Alcoholism:
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

References Clinical and Experimental Research, 26, 34A.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Gallup Organization. (1991). Sleep in America. Princeton, NJ: Author.


American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical man- Goldman, M. S., Del Boca, F. K., & Darkes, J. (1999). Alcohol expectancy
ual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. theory: The application of cognitive neuroscience. In K. E. Leonard &
Backhaus, J., Junghanns, K., Broocks, A., Riemann, D., & Hohagen, F.
Howard T. Blane (Eds.), Psychological theories of drinking and alco-
(2002). Test-retest reliability of the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index in
holism (2nd ed., pp. 203–246). New York: Guilford Press.
primary insomnia. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 53, 737–740.
Harvey, A. G. (2002). A cognitive model of insomnia. Behaviour Research
Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1987). Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)
and Therapy, 40, 869 – 893.
manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
Ingjaldsson, J. T., Thayer, J. F., & Laberg, J. C. (2003). Craving for alcohol
Bootzin, R. R. (1972). A stimulus control treatment for insomnia. Pro-
and pre-attentive processing of alcohol stimuli. International Journal of
ceedings of the American Psychological Association, 395–396.
Psychophysiology, 49, 29 –39.
Bootzin, R. R., Epstein, D., & Wood, J. M. (1991). Stimulus control
Johnsen, B. H., Laberg, J. C., Cox, W. M., Vaksdal, A., & Hugdahl, K.
instructions. In P. Hauri (Ed.), Case studies in insomnia (pp. 19 –28).
(1994). Alcoholic subjects’ attentional bias in the processing of alcohol-
New York: Plenum Press.
related words. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 8, 111–115.
Borbely, A. A. (1994). Sleep homeostasis and models of sleep regulation.
Jones, B. C., Jones, B. T., Blundell, L., & Bruce, G. (2002). Social users
In M. H. Kryger, T. Roth, & W. C. Dement (Eds.), Principles and
of alcohol and cannabis who detect substance-related changes in a
practice of sleep medicine (3rd ed., pp. 309 –320). Philadelphia: Saun-
change blindness paradigm report higher levels of use than those detect-
ders.
ing substance-neutral changes. Psychopharmacology, 165, 93–96.
Broomfield, N. M., & Espie, C. A. (2003). Initial insomnia and paradoxical
Jones, B. T. (2004). Changing alcohol expectancies: Techniques for alter-
intention: An experimental investigation of putative mechanisms using
subjective and actigraphic measurement of sleep. Behavioural & Cog- ing motivations for drinking. In W. M. Cox & E. Klinger (Eds.),
nitive Psychotherapy, 31, 313–324. Handbook of motivational counseling: Motivating people for change
Bruce, G., & Jones, B. T. (2004). A pictorial Stroop paradigm reveals an (pp. 373–387). Chichester, England: Wiley.
alcohol attentional bias in heavier as compared with lighter social Jones, B. T., Bruce, G., Livingstone, S., & Reed, E. (in press). Alcohol-
drinkers. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 18, 527–533. related attentional bias in problem drinkers with the flicker change
Bruce, G., & Jones, B. T. (2005, June). Alcohol-related attentional bias in blindness paradigm. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors.
social drinkers. Paper presented at the 28th Annual Scientific Meeting of Jones, B. T., Corbin, W., & Fromme, K. (2001). A review of expectancy
the Research Society on Alcoholism, Santa Barbara, CA. theory and alcohol consumption. Addiction, 96, 55–70.
Buysse, D. J., Reynolds, C. F., Monk, T. H., Berman, S. R., & Kupfer, D. J. Jones, B. T., Jones, B. C., Smith, H., & Copely, N. (2003). A flicker
(1989). The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument for paradigm for inducing change blindness reveals alcohol and cannabis
psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatric Research, 28, 193–213. information processing biases in social users. Addiction, 98, 235–244.
Chesson, A. L., Anderson, W. M., Littner, M., Davilia, D., Hartse, K., Jones, B. T., & McMahon, J. (1998). Alcohol motivations as outcome
Johnson, S., et al. (1999). Practice parameters for non-pharmacologic expectancies. In W. M. Miller & N. Heather (Eds.), Treating addictive
treatment of chronic insomnia. Sleep, 22, 1128 –1133. behaviors (2nd ed., pp. 75–91). New York: Plenum Press.
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159. Keogh, E., Dillon, C., Georgiou, G., & Hunt, C. (2001). Selective atten-
Cohen, J. D., Dunbar, K., & McClelland, J. L. (1990). On the control of tional biases for physical threat in physical anxiety sensitivity. Anxiety
automatic processes: A parallel distributed processing account of the Disorders, 15, 299 –315.
Stroop effect. Psychological Review, 97, 181–200. Lee, N. K., Greely, J., & Oei, T. P. (1999). Relationship of positive and
Drummond, D. C., Tiffany, S. T., Glautier, S., & Remington, B. (1995). negative alcohol expectancies to patterns of consumption of alcohol in
Addictive behaviour: Cue exposure theory and practice. New York: social drinkers. Addictive Behaviors, 24, 359 –369.
Wiley. Leigh, B. C., & Stacy, A. W. (1998). Individual differences in memory
Edinger, J. D., & Wohlgemuth, W. (1999). The significance and manage- associations involving the positive and negative outcomes of alcohol
ment of persistent primary insomnia: The past, present and future of use. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 12, 9 – 46.
behavioral insomnia therapies. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 3, 101–118. Li, R. H. Y., Wing, Y. K., Ho, S. C., & Fong, S. Y. Y. (2002). Gender
Espie, C. A. (1999). Cognitive behavioral therapy as the treatment of differences in insomnia—A study in the Hong Kong Chinese population.
choice for primary insomnia. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 3, 97–99. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 53, 601– 609.
Espie, C. A. (2002). Insomnia: Conceptual issues in the development, Lundh, L.-G. Froding, A., Gyllenhammer, L., Broman, J.-K., & Hetta, J.
persistence, and treatment of sleep disorders in adults. Annual Review of (1997). Cognitive bias and memory performance in patients with per-
Psychology, 53, 215–243. sistent insomnia. Scandanavian Journal of Behaviour Therapy, 26, 27–
Ford, D. E., & Kamerow, D. B. (1989). Epidemiologic study of sleep 35.
258 JONES, MACPHEE, BROOMFIELD, JONES, AND ESPIE

McMahon, J., Jones, B. T., & O’Donnell, P. (1994). Comparing positive Sharma, D., Albery, I. P., & Cook, C. (2001). Selective attentional bias to
and negative alcohol expectancies in male and female social drinkers. alcohol-related stimuli in problem drinkers and non-problem drinkers.
Addiction Research, 1, 349 –365. Addiction, 96, 285–295.
Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1998). A cognitive-motivational analysis of Simons, D. J. (2000). Current approaches to change blindness. Visual
anxiety. Behavioral Research Therapy, 36, 809 – 848. Cognition, 7, 1–15.
Mogg, K., Mathews, A., Bird, C., & MacGregor-Morris, R. (1990). Effects Simons, D. J., & Levin, D. T. (1997). Change blindness. Trends in
of stress and anxiety on the processing of threat stimuli. Journal of Cognitive Sciences, 1, 261–267.
Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1230 –1237. Simons, D. J., & Rensink, R. A. (2005). Change blindness: Past, present,
Morin, C. M., Hauri, P. J., Espie, C. A., Spielman, A., Buysse, D., Bootzin, and future. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 16 –20.
R. R., et al. (1999). Nonpharmacologic treatment of insomnia: An Smith, M. T., Perlis, M. L., Park, A., Smith, M. S., Pennington, J., Giles,
American Academy of Sleep Medicine review. Sleep, 22, 1134 –1156. D. E., & Buysse, D. J. (2002). Comparative meta-analysis of pharma-
Murtagh, D. R., & Greenwood, K. M. (1995). Identifying effective psy- cotherapy and behavior therapy for persistent insomnia. American Jour-
chological treatments for insomnia: A meta-analysis. Journal of Con- nal of Psychiatry, 159, 5–11.
sulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 79 – 89. Taylor, L. M., Espie, C. A., & White, C. A. (2003). Attentional bias in
O’Regan, J. K., & Noe, A. (2000, June) Experience is not something we people with acute versus persistent insomnia secondary to cancer. Be-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

feel but something we do: A principled way of explaining sensory havioral Sleep Medicine, 1, 200 –212.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

phenomenology, with change blindness and other empirical conse- Townshend, J. M., & Duka, T. (2001). Attentional bias associated with
quences. Paper presented at the Fourth Conference of the Association for alcohol cues: Differences between heavy and occasional social drinkers.
the Scientific Study of Consciousness, Brussels, Belgium. Psychopharmacology, 15, 67–74.
Oyahon, M. M., Caulet, M., & Guilleminault, C. (1997). How a general Waters, A. J., & Feyerabend, C. (2000). Determinants and effects of
population perceives its sleep and how this relates to the complaint of attentional bias in smokers. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 14,
insomnia. Sleep, 20, 715–723. 111–120.
Reiss, S. (1997). Trait anxiety: It’s not what you think it is. Journal of Wicklow, A., & Espie, C. A. (2000). Intrusive thoughts and their relation-
Anxiety Disorders, 11, 201–214. ship to an actigraphic measurement of sleep: Towards a cognitive model
Rensink, R. A. (2002). Change detection. Annual Review of Psychology, of insomnia. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 38, 679 – 693.
53, 245–277. Williams, J. M. G., Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (1996). The emotional
Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. K., & Clark, J. J. (1997). To see or not to see: Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 3–24.
The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological
Science, 8, 368 –373.
Ryan, F. (2002). Attentional bias and alcohol dependence: A controlled Received April 8, 2003
study using the modified Stroop paradigm. Addictive Behaviors, 27, Revision received July 16, 2004
471– 482. Accepted August 4, 2004 䡲

You might also like