You are on page 1of 16

tselief in a .!

ust Wor[d

Carolyn L. Hafer and Robbie Sutton

8.1 lntroduction vidual's character is adjusted, so the individual's


fate seems more deserved and, therefore, just
Lerner's (!g77)justice motive theory is one of (see Ellard, Harvey, & Callan, 2016, Chap.7 of
the most influential justice theories in social psy- this handbook, for a sustained discussion of the
chology. Testifying to its impact, the theory has motive to perceive the world as a just place).
spawned two very different lines of research: Rubin and Peplau (1973) proposed that only
One focused on experimental tests of motiva- individuals who have a strong dispositional
tional processes; and another, largely correla- belief in a just world (BJW) would react as pre-
tional, line of ' research emphasizing stable dicted by justice motive theory. In doing so,
individual differences. Ellard, Harvey, and Callan Rubin and Peplau (1973) assumed a closo cor-
(2016) provide a detailed review of the former, respondence between the strength of (a) the
experimental literature. In this chapter, we review BJW and (b) the desire to believe in a just world.
past and current dbvelopments in the individuat Informed by this assumption, Rubin and Peplau
difference literature. (1973, 1975) developed the Just World Scale,
Based on a series, of experiments, Lerner which measures people's chronic belief that the
(1980) proposed that people need to believe that world is a just place in which individuals get
the world is a just place wherein individuals get what they deserve. Importantly, the Just World
what they deserve. Instances of undeserved out- Scale, like other BfW scales, does not explicitly
comes (i.e., injustice) are therefore'threatening. assess the need to believe in a just world.
People respond to such threats by trying to restore Further, as we shall see later in this chapter, it is
justice, often tluough defensive means. For unlikely that anything more than a very weak
example, people might defensively derogate the relation exists between scores on BJW scales
character of victims of misfortune and lionize the and the need to believe in a just world. Thus,
character of undeserving beneflciaries of good Rubin and Peplau's starting assumption now
fortune. In either case, assessment of the indi- seems misguided. Nonetheless; their research
was the first to explore the properties of BJW as
C.L. Hafer (X) an individual difference variable, and in doing
Brock University, Saint Catharines, Canada so made possible all the discoveries that have
e-mait: chafer@brocku.ca
subsequently arisen from studies of BJW.
R. Sutron Since Rubin and Peplau's (197 3, 197 5) seminal
University of Kent, Canterbury UK papers, the research has matured such that BJW
e-mail: R.Sutton@kent.ac.uk

O Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016


C' Sabbagh, M. Schmitt (eds.), Iiandbook of Social Justice Theory and Research,
DoI 10.1007/978-1-4939-321 6-0_8
C.L. Hafer and R. Sutton

is often examined as an important individual dif- Scale. For example, regarding constuct validiry
ference consffuct in its own right, and not solely Rubin and Peplau reported further evidence that
as a potential moderator of experimental effects individuals with a strong BJW derogate victims
predicted by justice motive theory. This research relative to individuals with a weaker BJW. In addi-
can be characterized by three broad trends: fur- tion, Rubin and Peplau summarized correlates of
ther development of BJW scales, studies of cor- BJW (e.g., authoritarianism) that are expected on
the basis of theory about the development and func-
relates of BJW, and research addressing the
broader significance of BJW. We organize our tions of the belief. Finally, Rubin and Peplau pre-
chapter according to these three themes' sented correlational evidence of predicted
sociopolitical "consequences" of BJW, such as

positive attitudes toward existing political and social


8.2 Scale DeveloPment institr.rtions, negative attitudes toward disadvan-
taged $oups, and lower levels of social activism'

8.2.1 Rubin and PePlau's Just


World Scale
8.2.2 Criticisms of Rubin
The Just World Scale is an explicit self-report and PePlau's JustWorld Scale
of a series of statements to
measure consisting
which people indicate the extent of their Despite preliminary evidence of acceptable reli-
agreement-disagreement on a numeric scale' An ability and validity for the Just World Scale,
initial 16-item version of the scale was followed researchers soon began to discover weaknesses
by a 2O-item version (see Rubin & Peplau, 1975)'
(for reviews, see Furnham & Procter, 1989;
Several items in the Just Wbrld Scale address a Furnham, 2003). First, although most initial
specific domain, such as the legal system and internal consistency coefficients were adequate,
health, whereas other items are general (e'g', later studies yielded poorer results (e'g',
"Basically, the world is a just place")' Some items Ambrosio & Sheehan, 1990; Hellman,
are phrased in the unjust world direction and are, Muilenburg-Trevino, &
Worley, 2008; Loo'
thus, reverse-scored (e.g., "I've found that a per- 2002a). Second, the factor sffucture of the Just
son rarely deserves the reputation he has")' World Scale was also criticized. Although the
To validate the 16-item scale, Rubin and Peplau scale is presumably meant to be unidimensional,
(1973) administered the measure to young men many studies showed evidence for several inter-
who were subiect to the 1971 military draft lottery pretable factors. Furthermore, the factor structure
in the United States. Men scoring higher onthe Just is not consistent across studies (Furnham, 2003)'
World Scale, compared to those scoring lower, had A third problem for the Just World Scale is
a greater tendency to react more favorably to 'lwin- that the meaning of its reverse-coded items is
ners" of the lottery (those whose randornly drawn ambiguous. Several authors have found that
number meant they were less likely to be drafted items worded in the just-world vs. unjust-wor1d
into military service) than to "losers" (those whose direction load onto different factors. In addition,
number meant they were more likely to be drafted)' separate just-world and unjust-world seales cre-
The findings were consistent with Rubin and ated from these differentially keyed items are
Peplau's (1973) claim that individuals who hold a only weakly correlated (if at all) and typically
strong BJW are more likely to respond to unde- predict different criteria (e.g., Couch, 1998;
served outcomes as hypothesized in justice motive Dalbert, Lipkus, SallaY, & Goch, 2001;
theory; that is, by derogating victims of undeserved Furnham, 1995; Loo, 2002a). These flndings
negative outcomes and lionizing beneflciaries of imply that the opposite of BfW is not necessarily
undeserved positive outcomes. belief in an uniust world. Furnham and Procter
Rubin and Peplau (1975) summarized early evi- (1989) suggested that another alternative to a
dence of the reliability and validity of the Just World BJW is a belief that the world is random and,
6 Belief in a Just World 147

therefore, not predicable at all. In contrast, an ences in BIW, the Dalbert and Lipkus measures
unjust world is a nonrandom world in which are gaining popularity. Indeed, the Dalbert measure
people predictably receive undeserved out- has been translated into several different lan-
cornes. Thus, unjust world items are likely inad- guages (e.g., Bastounis, Leiser, & Roland-L6vy,
equate as indicators of (low) BJW. 2004; Bastounis & Minibas-Poussard, 2012;
Dalbert & Katona-Sallay, 7996; Dalbert &
Yamauchi, 1994).
9,2.3 Attempts to lmprove the Just
World Scale BJW in dffirent domains. Some researchers have
argued that BfW might vary within individuals
Since Rubin and Peplau's groundbreaking work, depending on the domain specifled. Furnham and
the development of BfW scales has taken differ- Procter (1992) created the Multidimensional
ent directions, First, researchers have created BJW Scale, which taps into just world and unjust
more psychometrically sound measures of gen- world beliefs in three spheres of life: sociopoliti-
eral BJW Second, researchers have gone beyond cal, interpersonal, and personal domains.
the idea of general BfW to create scales assessing Unfortunately, the scales suffer from poor psy-
BJW in different domains of life. Third, some chometric properties (e.g., Furnham & Procter,
researchers have attempted to assess different i992; Lipkus, 1991) and thus have not been
ways of believing in ajust world. widely used.
More promising, Lucas and colleagues have
General BJW scales. Rubin and Peplau's scale created measures of distributive and procedural
includes both domain-specific and general items. BJW (e.g., Lucas, Alexander, Firestone, &
Two subsequent measures of BJW-a
6-item LeBreton, 2007). Distributive justice is the jus-
German scale by Dalbert, Montada, and Schmitt tice of distributions of resources or outcomes (e.g.,
(1987), and a 7-item English scale by Lipkus Deutsch, 1985). hocedural justice is the justice of
(199l)-include only general items. These scales mechanisms for making decisions about distribu-
address many of the criticisms of the Just World tions or outcomes, as well as the treafirent of those
Scale. Likely due to the use of only general items, affected by decisions (e.g., Bies & Moag, 1986;
the newer scales show increased reliability com- Lind & Tyler, 1988). Lucas's research is still in its
pared to the Just World Scale, as well as unidimen- early stages, but initial reliability and validity for
sionality (see Dalbert, 1999;Lipkus, 1991, though his scales are good. Interestingly, Lucas, Zhdanova,
see O'Connor et al., 1996). Cronbach's alpha is and Alexander (201 1) recently crossed procedural
often between .60 and .70 for the Dalbert scale vs. distributive BJW with BJW for self vs. others
(e.g., Dalbert, 1999; Dalbert & Yamauchi, i994; (the latter distinction is described next) to yield
Loo, 2002b), and above .80 for the Lipkus scale four domains of BJW, each of which might have
(e.g., Hafer, 2000; Lipkus , l99l; O'Connor et al., di fferent consequences.
1996). Also, items for both scales are keyed in the
just-world direction, thus avoiding problems asso- BJW for self vs. people generally: A more long-
ciated with some items indicating an unjust world. standing approach to assessing BJW in different
Scores on both the Dalben and Lipkus scales cor- domains is to distinguish a belief that one's own
relate with criteria based on past research with the world is just from a belief that the world is just
JustWorld Scale and on theorizing about the func- for people generally. According to justice motive
tions of BJW (e.g., Lipkus, i991; Montada, 1998). theory, the ultimate reason to believe that the
Overall, the psychometric properties of both newer world is just for people generally is that, if the
scales exceed those of the Rubin and Peplau world is 'Just for others,f it is also likely 'Just for
measure. me." Thus, the two domains of BJW, though sep-
Although the Just World Scale remains the arable, should be related. In addition, given the
most widely used measure of individual differ- more important goal of perceiving one's own
C.L. Hafer and R. Sutton
148

world to be just, a personal BJW should be sffonger and seek vengeance against those who have
than a more general BfW. transgressed against the self, whereas BJW-self is

The distinction between personal and general positively associated with forgiveness (Lucas
Strelan & Sutton, 20 11 Alternatively,
has been operationalized in two ways' Lipkus, et al., 20 11; ).

Dalbert, and Siegler (1996) distinguished the these findings could be accounted for by the
BJW for the self from the BfW for others' Each relation between BJW-others and antisocial
domain is assessed with eight items. Items for tendencies (e.g., Sutton & Winnard, 2007).
BJW-self and BJW-others differ only in the use
of believing in a iust world.Maes
of pronouns (e.g., "I feel that the world treats me Dffirent ways
(e.g., Maes, 1998b; Maes & Schmitt, 1999) has
fairly" vs. "I feel that the world treats others
gone beyond general BfW scales by proposing
fairly"). In parallel, Dalbert (1999) designed the
of believing in a just world; These
6-item Personal BJW Scale as a counterpart to different ways
justice-that justice will
Dalbert's general BfW scale, noted earlier' The are a belief in ultimate
belief in imma-
personal BfW items are not tightly matched to prevail in the long-run-and
a

the general BJW items, as in the Lipkus scales' nent justice-that justice occurs in the present
(see Piaget, t93211965). Similar to BJW-others'
Research using the Lipkus and Dalbert scales
has produced similm flndings (e.g., Bbgue & belief in immanent justice is more strongly
responses to victims that are
Bastounis, 2003; Dalbert, 1999 Lipkus et a1', related to negative
in the justice motive literature,
1996; Sutton & Douglas, 2005). Consistent with typically
studied
justice motive theory the personal versions of BJW such as character derogation and blame.
are positively correlated with their more general Conversely, belief in ultimate justice is more
counterparts. A1so, personal BJW and BJW-self are related to prosocial responses to victims, such as
typically endorsed more sffongly than general BfW positive character evaluations and a willingness to
and BJw-others, respectively; though the relative help. Although few researchers outside of Maes's
sftength of the personal versions of BJW may be group have employed immanent and ultimate
culturally bound (see Wu et a1., 2011). BJW scales (though see BBgue, 2002), the notion
Despite the correlation between personal and of different ways of believing in a just world has
general versions of BfW, the two versions have promise for further refining the concept of BIW
unique correlates. Overall, personal BfW and
BIW-self tend to be associated with the theoreti-
cal "benefits" of BfW-like subjective well- 8.3 Correlates of BJW
being-moreso than general BIW and
BJW-others (e.g., BBgue & Bastounis, 2003; Whereas one research stream has focussed on the
Lipkus et al., 1996; Sutton et al', 2008; Sutton & development of various BJW scales, an otten-
Douglas, 2005; Sutton & Winnard, 2007)' The related line ofresearch involves the correlates of
more general forms of BfW, especially BJW- BJW. The majority of the correlational studies
others, are associated with traditional "costs" of were conducted in the 1970s and 1980s, during
BJW, including harsh, punitive responses to per- what has been called the construct validation
petrators of injustice and harsh responses to the phase of research on individual differences in
disadvantaged (e.g., Bdgue & Bastounis, 2003; BfW (Maes, 1998a). Relevant literature has been
Sutton & Douglas, 2005)' These findings suggest reviewed by several authors (e.g., Furnham,
that BJW-others is more closely linked than 2003; Furnham & Procter, 1989; Rubin & Peplau'
BJW-self with defenses typically associated with 1975). In the current chapter, we integrate and
justice motive theory, such as derogation of vic- update these reviews, focusing on the most com-
tims of injustice. A heightened defensiveness monly studied correlates of BfW-reactions to
about injustice might explain why BJW-others is victims and beneficiaries, and ideological
positively associated with the desire to ostracise variables.
g Belief in a Just World

9,3,1 Reactions to Victims and ciations between BfW and either self-blame or
Beneficiaries negative evaluations of the self are less reliable
than in the literature on reactions to others (cf.
14ost authors conceptualize BfW not only as a Carels et a1.,2009;Dalbert, 1998; Hafer & Correy,
belief that people get what they deserve, but also 1999; Kielcolt-Glaser & Williams, 1987; Riisch,
as a belief that people deserve what they get. Todd, Bodenhausen, & Conigan, 2010). Ego-
BfW should react
Thus, individuals with a strong defensive motives might reduce the likelihood
to people's outcomes in ways that reflect a bias that victims will interpret their own outcomes in a
toward seeing those outcomes as deserved. Such way that reflects badly on the self (e.g., through
rsactions should occur whether the target is the self-blame; see Hafer & Gosse, 2010). BIW is
self or another person, and whether the target's more consistenfly related to perceived fairness of
outcomes are negative, as in the case of "victims," one's negative outcomes, though the association
or positive, as in the case of "beneficiaries," is sometimes moderated by a third variable, such
as ambiguity of the situation (cf. Ball, Trevino, &
The other as victim. A large number of studies Sims, 1993; Choma, Hafer, Crosby, & Foster,
have examined relations between BfW and reac- 2012; Hafer & Correy, 1999; Hafer & Olson,
dons to victimized or disadvantaged others. The 1989; Hagedoorn, Buunk, & Van de Vliert,2OO2).
most commonly assessed reactions are attribu- Another cofltmon dependent variable is affective
tions about the causes of the misfortune and eval- reactions to one's negative outcomes (e.g., Hafer
uadons of the victim. Research generally shows & Olson, 1989, 1998; Hagedoorn et a1.,2002;
that a stronger BJW is related to blaming victims Kielcolt-Glaser & Williams, 1987). Hafer and
for their negative outcomes, as well as to nega- Correy (1999), for example, found that students'
tively evaluating victims (Furnham, 2003; Rubin BfW predicted their emotional reactions to a poor
& Peplau, 1975; for examples of more recent exam grade, mediated by perceived unfairness
work, see Bizer, Hart, & Jekogian, 2012;Ebneter, and attributions.
Latner, & O'Brien, 20ll; Keller & Siegrist,
2010; Sakalli-U[ur1u, Yalgm, & Gltck, 2007; Beneficiaries. Still less research has examined
Smith, Mao, Perkins, & Ampuero, 2011). Given the relation between BJW and reactions to recipi-
that blame and negative evaluations help to jus- ents of positive outcomes. In general, this
tify victims' fate as deserved, it is not surprising research shows that a strong BJW is associated
that researchers have also found an association with reactions that presumably rationalize a ben-
between BfW and perceived deservingness or eflciary's positive fate as deserved. For example,
fairness of victims' lot (e.g., Appelbaum, Lennon, individuals with a strong BJW associate higher
& Aber, 2006; Dalbert, Fisch, & Montada, 1992; status with positive characteristics more than do
Nudelman & Shiloh, 201 I ), individuals with a weaker BfW (e.g., Dion &
Several authors note that relations between Dion, 1987:' Oldmeadow & Fiske, 2007).
BJW and attributional or evaluative reactions to Similarly, BJW is associated with more positive
victims are small (e.g., Montada, 1998; Rubin & evaluations of powerful others (e.g., Rubin &
Peplau, 1975). Research showing that different Peplau, 1975; Smith, 1985).
measures of BJW predict different responses to We are aware of only one study on BJW and
victims (e.g., BfW-self vs. BJW-others, ultimate reactions to the self as beneficiary. Ellard and
vs. immanent BIW) helps account for the modest Bates (1990, Study 2) assigned participants a
findings. high status role, and an alleged other participant
a low status role. Individuals with a strong, but
The self as yictim. Relatively little research has not those with a weak, BJW rated their own
been devoted to BJW and reactions to one's own character traits more positively compared to the
victimized or disadvantaged state. Overall, asso- other's ffaits.
mlwl

C.L. Hafer and R. Sutton


150

internal locus of control is consistent with justice


motive theory. Lerner (197'l) argues that BJW
Researchers have also frequently examined the helps people invest in long-term goals, presum-
relation between BfW and socio-political ideol- ably by providing confidence that their current
ogy, especially conservative ideology and religi- efforts will eventually pay off as deserved' Thus, '

osity (Furnham,2O}3;Furnham & Procter, 1989; BJW should increase the sense that one can influ-
Rubin & Peplau, 1975). BIW should correlate ence one's outcomes, as is chmacteristic of indi-
positively with these variables given that they viduals with a strong internal locus of control'
share the following content: adulation of high As noted by Dittmar and Dickinson (1993),
status and denigration of low status others; a the ideological correlates of BIW are themselves
focus on order, conEol, and support for the status intercorreiated, and high scores on these vari-
quo; and a belief in ultimate justice' ables are characteristic of right-wing ideology'
Most reviews of BfW mention associations Yet, BJW is conceptually and empirically dis-
between BfW and conservative ideology' More tinct from its socio-political correlates.
recent studies report similar relations, whether Concephrally, many theories of conservative
conservative ideology is assessed via authoritari- ideology claim that such belief-systems origi-
anism (e.g., Christopher, Zabel, Jones, & Marek, nate in personalities or social climates that pro-
2008; Henderson-King' Henderson-King, Bolea, mote feelings of threat and uncertainty (e'g',
Koches, & Kauffman, 2004; Reser & Muncer, Duckitt, 2001; Jost, 2009). In contrast, proposed
2004), selt'-identifi cation as liberal-conservative sources of BIW include an intrapsychic need
(e.g., Christopher et al,, 2008; Parikh, Post, & related to investment in long-term goals, experi-
Flowers, 2011; but see Choma et al',20t2), ot ence with iniustice, and social learning (see sec-

endorsement of traditionally conservative policy tion on bases of BfW). Empirically, a distinction


opinions and social attitudes (e'g', Bastounis is by factor analyses (e'g', Lerner,
suggested
et al., 2004; Bbgue & Bastounis, 2003; 1978) and by the many studies in which BJW
Lodewijkx, Kersten, & v an Zomeren, 2008)' accounts for unique variance in relevant criteria,
Religiosity is also a relatively consistent cor- over and above ideological correlates (e'g',
relate of BJW, though there is evidence that the Appelbaum, 2002; Bbgue &
Bastounis,2003,
expected positive correlation does not occur for Study 5; Henderson-King et a1., 2004; Martin &
all religious groups (Furnham, 2003; Furnham & Cohn, 2004; Ng & Allen, 2005)' In addition,
Procter, 1989; Rubin & Peplau, 1975)' A more BJW might act as a mediator between ideologi
of BJW is
consistent quasi-religious correlate cal variables and criterion variables. For exam-
endorsement of the Protestant Work Ethic ple, Schlenker, Chambers, and Le (2012, Study
(Furnham & Procter, 1989; Rubin & Peplau, 1) found that the relation between political con-
1975). Recent authors (Christopher et al', 2008; servatism and psychological adjustment was
Ghorpade, Lackritz, & Singh, 2006) argue that mediated by BJW and related perceptions of
BJW is more strongly associated with speciflc fairness,
facets of Protestant Work Ethic beliefs, such as
the (nonreligious) belief that hard work leads to
good outcomes. 8.3.3 Other Correlates of BJW
Many researchers have measured locus of
control along with socio-political ideology and A few other noteworthy correlates add to the
BJW. Greater internal locus of control and simi- discriminant validity of BJW. Small correla-
lar constructs (e.g., mastery) are related to a tions between BJW and sensitivity to injustice'
stronger BJW (Bastounis et a1., 2004; Choma as measured by Schmitt and colleagues' justice

eta1.,2012; Christopher et a1., 2008; Furnham & sensitivity scales (Schmitt, Gollwitzer, Maes, &
Procter, 1989; Ghorpade et a1., 2006; Rubin & Arbach, 2005), indicate that these constructs are
Peplau, 1975). A correlation between BfW and not equivalent. Similarly, BJIV does not seem to
g Belief in a Just World 151

6e equivalent to a preference for the equity or Dalbert, 1999) conceptualized BJW as a "positive
perit principle of distributive justice (Davey, illusion"-in other words, a belief that is psycho-
Bobocel, Son Hing, & Zanta, 1999; Montada, logically beneflcial, yet unwarranted by facts and
1998), although BJW is construed as a belief logic (Taylor & Brown, 1988). tnspired by this
that the world works according to rules for conceptualization, researchers have gathered a
deservingness (which is often equated with large body of evidence suggesting that BJW is
equitY or merit). associated with good psychological adjustment
Finally, BfW shows small correlations with (Dalbert, 2009; Furnham, 2003). As noted earlier,
some personality traits (Nudelman, 2013), suph personal BJW appears to be more shongly assoc!
as an inverse correlation with the Big Five factor ated with psychological well-being than general
of Neuroticism. Yet, multiple regression analyses forms of BJW (Dalbert,2009). Furthermore, the
show that BJW predicts criteria over and above relation between personal forms of BfW and well-
personality (e.9., Dette, St<iber, & Dalbert, 2004; being appears both in Western (e.g., Correia &
Keller & Siegrist, 2010; Lipkus et al., 1996). Dalbert, 2007; Sutton & Winnard, 2007) and col-
Thus, BJW explains human psychological varia- lectivistic cultures (e.g,, Fatima & Suhail, 2010;
don that is not accounted for by broad dimen- Kamble & Dalbert, Zr0t2;Xie,Liu, & Gan,2011).
sions of personality. However, for individuals living under very adverse
circumstances, general forms of BIW may provide
more effective solace than personal forms (e.g.,
8.4 The Significance of BJW Dalbert, 1998; McParland & Knussen, 2010; Wu
in People's Lives et al., 201 1; but see Xie et al., 201 1).

The literature reviewed so far has advanced the Physical health. A few studies suggest that at
measurement, validation, and refinement of the least certain forms of BJW are associated with
construct of BfW Other studies address (directly physical health (e.g., Agrawal & Dalal, 1993;
or indirectly) the significance of
BfW in people's Lucas, Alexander, Firestone, & LeBreton, 2008).
iives. Much of this work has been conducted There are several potential mechanisms underly-
post-1990 and continues to be an important ing a BJW-health relation. For example, perhaps
theme in BfW research. We organize the relevant a strong BfW conhibutes to physical health
research around three key questions: Is BJW because it is associated with adaptive appraisals
adaptive for the self?, is BJW adaptive for soci- of stressors, which lower damaging physiotogi-
ety?, and what are the bases of BfW? cal responses to stress (see Tomaka & Blascovich,
1994). BIW might also benefit physical health
because it equips people to pursue long-term
8.4.1 ls BJW Adaptive for the Self? rewards: Thus, BfW could help people to adopt
healthy behaviors and to refrain from behaviors
Psychological well-being. According to justice that damage their physical health in the long run
motive theory, BJW affords psychological bene- (see Lucas et al., 2008). Alternatively, people
fits by providing people with a sense that their with a strong BfW might accept that certain dis-
lives are meaningful, predictable, and controlla- eases are preventable (see Lucas, Alexander,
ble (see Dalbert, 2001; Lerner, 1980). BJW Firestone, & LeBreton, 2009), which would con-
should thus protect people from negative affect
ceivably lead to appropriate action to prevent
associated with worries about the future
and also such diseases.
buffer them from the adverse psychological
con- Studies on BJW and risk suggest that BJW
sequences of negative and
undeserved outcomes could, for certain people, lead to more unhealthy
tn lheir past
and present. behaviors. Lambert, Burroughs, and Nguyen
The systematic examination of BJW and well- (1999) found that, among individuals who are
being starred in the
1990s, when Dalbert (e.g., particularly vulnerable to perceived threat, a
152 C,L. Hafer and R. Sutton

strong BJW predicted lower perceived risk of projects, and societies surely function better
becoming a victim of negative events, including when they are populated by happier, more pro-
disease (e.g., AIDS). By protecting against fear ductive people. Yet, BJW is associated with harsh
of future illness, BfW could in turn lead to more attitudes to victims, which can be expected to
risky health behaviors (see Hafer, Bogaert, & lead to adverse social outcomes such as height-
McMullen,2001). ened disadvantage and inequality'

Investment in long-term goals and


trust. Pro and antisocial behavior. Another way that
According to Lerner (1917), BJW is adaptive in BJW may affect societal-level outcomes is via its
part because it encourages investment in long- association with pro and antisocial behavior.
term goals. From an individual difference per- Because BfW entails that prosocial and antiso-
spective, individuals who develop a strong BJW cial acts will receive the rewards or punishments
believe they will be treated fairly and should feel they deserve, BJW should be negatively associ-
confident that, as long as they invest the resources ated with antisocial behavior and positively asso-
deemed necessary to reach long-term goals, they ciated with prosocial behavior.
will be rewarded accordingly (Dalbert, 2001; With regard to antisocial behavior, Hafer
Hafer, 2000). Indeed, researchers have found a (2000) found negative correlations between BfW
positive relation between BfW and perceived and the tendency to employ antisocial means to
liketihood of achieving one's goals (e.g., Dette obtain goals (but see Cohn & Modecki, z}0fl).
et al., 2004; Otto & Dalbert, 2005; Sutton & Studies also show that BJW is inversely related
Winnard, 2007). The benefits of BIW for long- to positive attitudes toward bullying, as well as
term goal pursuit might not apply universally, bullying behavior (e.g., Correia & Dalbert, 2008;
however. Findings from Laurin, Fitzsimons, and Donat, Umlauft, Dalbert, & Kamble, Zol2;Fox,
Kay (201 1) suggest that BJW encourages invest- Elder, Gater, & Johnson, 2010; but see Almeida,
ment in long-term goals for members of disad- Correia, & Marinho, 2010).
vantaged groups, but not advantaged groups. On the downside, BJW may support "antiso-
If BJW imbues confidence that one will be cial" behaviors if those behaviors are construed
treated fairly, it is perhaps unsurprising that BJW as just punishment for moral violations perpe-
is associated with indices of trust, including trated by others. General BfW appears to be
interpersonal and institutional trust (e.g., Bdgue, especially important in this regard, predicting
2002; Coneia & Vala, 2004, Study 3; Lipkus, support for vengeance against the perpetrators of
199 1 ). The high trust among just-world believers terrorist attacks (Kaiser, Vick, & Major, 2004).ln
can have additional benefits. For example, Lipkus addition, studies have shown general BfW to be
and Bissonnette (1996) found that BJW predicted positively associated with self-reported venge-
trust in intimate partners, which in turn was asso- fulness against people who have wronged the
ciated with partners' willingness to accommodate self, whereas personal BIW is positively associ-
to each others' needs, and so to relationship ated with forgiveness (Lucas et al., 2011; Strelan
satistaction. & Sutton, 2011). The associations with general
BJW are consistent with the previously noted
relation between general forms of BJW and
8.4.2 ls BJW Adaptive for SocietY? harsh, punitive responses to offenders.
As well as antisocial behavior, BJW appears
The implications of BJW for societal-level out- to be relevant to prosocial behavior. For example,
comes are more ambivalent than they are for self- studies show a positive association between BJW
relevant outcomes. As noted in the previous and donation to charity (Furnham, 1995; Kogut
section, BJW is associated with trust, which is a & futov, 2011), helping strangers (Bierhoff,
boon to social cooperation. BJW is also linked to Klein, & Kramp, l99l;Zuckerman, 1975), buy-
psychological health and pursuit of long-term ing ethical products (White, MacDonnell,
Belief in a Just World
1s3

&, Ellard, 2012), and voluntarily sharing with action is viewed as efficacious (see also Miller,
others in economic games (Dalbert & Umlauft, 1977). These flndings are consistent with the
ZN9). As noted in the section on scale develop- justice motive theory claim that people will
s1ent, measures of belief in ultimate justice, as attempt to maintain a threatened BJW through
opposed to immanent justice, are more strongly prosocial action (rather than rationalization)
rclaled to prosocial responses to victims, even primarily when action is a viable option.
after controlling for immanent justice.
'Interestingly, Montada and Schneider (19g9)
found evidence that BIW might be positively 8.4.3 What are the Bases of BJW?
(rather than negatively) related to prosocial
behavior once cognitive rationalizations related An examination of the significance of BJW in
to BJW are statistically accounted for. people's lives would be incomplete without
discussion of the developmental foundations of
Maintenance of the status quo. On balance,.the BJW. Researchers have suggested several bases
literature on BJW andpro and antisocial behavior of BIW, First, BfW is often assumed to reflect
suggests that BJW is adaptive for sociery. A dif- individual differences in the need to believe in a
ferent perspective (e.g., Jost & Hunyady, 2005; just world that is described in justice motive the_
Olson & Hafer, 2001; Sidanius & pratto, 1999) ory (cf. Dalbert,2009). According to the theory a
views BfW as maladaptive because it legitimizes motive or need to believe in ajust world develops
existing societal systems, even systems that work as children learn to forgo immediate gratification
against the best interests of thd perceiver (e:g., and instead pursue long-terrn goals. Interestingly,
Jost & Banaji, 1994). There is empirical evidence placing the roots of BJW in this need to believe in
that BJW is related to perceived fairness or legiti- a just world is at odds with a central precept of
macy of societal systems (e.g., Bastounis et al., justice motive theory-that the need to believe
in
2004;Martin & Cohn, 2004; Ng & Allen, 2005; a just world is an intrinsic part of normal human
Rubin & Peplau, 1975; Smith, 19g5). BfW hkely development and is, therefore, near-universal
leads to perceived legitimacy because it biases (Lerner, 1980). Thus, the theory posits that virru-
one to view systems as fair or deserved, often ally all people should be morivared to believe thar
through the kinds of evaluations and attributions the world is just, and variability in the intensity of
described under "Reactions to Victims and this motive results from situational (rather than
Beneficiaries" (see Hafer & Choma, 2OOg).
dispositional) pressures, such as exposure to
One implication of this relation between BfW
events that threaten the notion of a just world.
and the perceived fairness and legitimacy of
Evidence that BfW arises from the need to
broad systems is that people witl
a strong nfW believe in a just world is sparse. For example,
perceive less discrimination (e.g.,
Birt & Dion, according to this perspective, BJW is ,,irraiio-
]?87;
cto*u et al.,2012; fipms & Siegler, nal," resulting less from a rational assessment of
1993). Anorher implication
is that BfW pr.ji.r, reality and more from an intrapsychic need.
tesswillingness to take action aimed at changing
However, research does not tend to support this
the status quo, because
vtewed as unnecessary
change i,
p."ru-Iuty view. First, researchers have found nonsignifl-
(e.g., Beierlein, Werner, cant relations between measures of irrationaU
& Wermuth, 2011; Hafer & Olson, 1993;
I*l-t..
Parikh er al., 20ll;
rational thinking and BJW (Shorkey, 19g0;
Rubin & peplau, 1973; bu,t Stowers & Durm, 1998; Thalbourne, 1995).
see LodewUkx
et al., 200g). The BJW_acrion Second, there is evidence ofreality constraints on
relation is
moderated by the perceived efficacy of BJW Those who can be expected to have experi_
oehaviors
aimed at change (e.g., Beierlein et al., ence with injustice
in the world have a lower
101t,
tutotiyeddini &
Montada, 1998). For BfW, though results are admittedly mixed
White et at)s (Z}tZ) research suggesrs (Schmitt; 1998). Also, there is evidence that dif_
:J,.ip].,
mat BJW predicts
greater social action when the ferences between individuals, endorsement of

L*
7-'
:l r!

C.L. Hafer and R. Sutton

personal vs. general BJW are largely reality-based attempting to convey a positive image to others
(Sutton eta1.,2008; Sutton & Winnard, 2007;Wu (e.g., Alves & Correia, 2008) and judge others
et a1.,2013). Note that, if the primary source of more favorably when they affirm, vs. deny, n
BJW is not the motivation to believe in a just BJW (Alves & Correia, 2008, 2010a: see also
world described in justice motive theory it is not Testd, Maisonneuve, Assilam6hou, & Perrin,
surprising that individual differences in BfW 2012). Thus, BJW may be uansmirted and main-
rarely moderate experimental effects taken as tained by communication processes because of
demonstrations of a need to believe in a just the social purposes that it serves.
world (see Hafer & Bbgue, 2005). More generally, societal-level factors may
If BJW is not entirely irrational, it might be important determinants of BJW. Allen, Ng,
partly be based on an individual's or group's and Leiser (2005) found that BJW was stron-
experience with injustice. As noted above, the ger among modernized and growing econo-
evidence for a relation between experience and mies, and somewhat lower in countries that
BJW is mixed. Many studies investigate BJW were high in social capi[al (e.g., social ties and
scores as a function of membership in relatively cohesion). Furnham (1993) observed differ-
disadvantaged vs. advantaged groups (e.g., ences in BJW across l2 countries. These dif-
Calhoun & Cann, 1994; Hunt, 2000; O'Connoq ferences were associated with variations in
Morrison, Mcleod, & Anderson, 1996): Yet, the power-distance norms, which are shared
theoretical meaning of these demographic differ- beliefs that reify social hierarchy. This finding
ences is unclear, given that demographic groups provides evidence of a social determinant and
differ on many other characteristics (e.9., educa- a social function of BfW-the need to justify
tion, religiosity, culture) aside from experience social inequality.
with injustice. Research using direct measures of
people's experience as a victim of injustice bet-
ter supports an experience basis for BfW (e.g., 8.5 Summaryand Future
Adoric & Kvartuc, 2007; Fasel & Spini, 2010; Directions
Fischer & Holz,2010; Steensma & van Dijke,
2005-06). Most of these direct-measure studies, The study of individual differences in BfW has
howeveq are cross-sectional surveys; thus, the continued to thrive since Rubin and Peplau,
causal direction of associations is speculative. inspired by Lerner's (1977) justice motive theory,
Notably, people who experience injustice by vir- developed the Just World Scale in 1973. Early
tue of belonging to a group that perpetuates critiques of the scale have spawned a number of
injustice can also show heightened BfW, per- alternative instruments that are increasingly pop-
haps reflecting an effort to justify their unfair ular. The recent trend is toward multiple scales
advantage (see Furnham, 1985). assessing different forms of BJW. Personal vs.
Several authors have proposed social learning general torms of BJW is clearly one fundamental
origins of BJW. From this perspective, BJW is distinction. Other, less-researched distinctions
encouraged through such processes as ideologi- also show promise. Given the popularity of dual
cal teachings (e.g., Dittmar & Dickinson, 1993), process theories in psychology (Cawronski &
popular culture (e.g., Gunter & Wober, 1983), Creighton, 2013), we expect that a division
and parental influence (e.g., Schdnpflug & Bilz, between implicit vs. explicit BIW (cf. Dalberr,
2004). Again, evidence is based on cross- 2001) will add to the roster in the future. Though
sectional survey studies, making causal infer- newer measures have helped refine the concept
ences impossible. Alves and Correia (2008, of BJW, attempts to assess different forms of
2010a,2010b) suggested that BJW is valued for BJW have tended to occur in isolation of one
its social function in enhancing productivity and another. Researchers should now begin to inte-
maintaining order and cohesion. Indeed, people grate the forms of BJW into a coherent theoreti-
tend to affirm a BJW more skongly when cal framework.
6 Belief in a Just World
155

The overall concept of BIW has been well and injustice,


. social learning, and so on. Relevant
validated by studies of correlates. First, BfW future research should
include longitudinal
shows logical associations with presumed conse- designs that address developmental proc-esses,
as
quences of BJW, though research has focussed on well as tests of multiple pathways to individual
reactions to victims, neglecting implications of differences in BJW.
BfW for reactions to advantaged members of In summary, there have been several major
society. Second, BJW is conceptually and empiri- advances in research on BJW over the past 40
cally distinct from related individual differences. years. Yet, more work is needed. Given the
current
More research, however, should explore reasons momentum in the field of BfW, we expect that
for differential prediction by various forms of the next decade will be an exciting time in which
BfW. Such research will encourage theoretical researchers will considerably deepen our under_
integration of different forms of BfW. standing of the nature, foundations, and conse_
Research on the significance of BJW in peo_ quences ofBJW
ple's lives suggests that, overall, BJW beneflts
the self. Investigation of physical health benefits
of BJW is a promising avenue for further References
researcho given recent attention to the link
between poor health and perceived unfairness Adoric, V. C., & Kvartuc, T. (2007).Effects of mobbing
(e.g., Jackson, Kubzansky, & Wright,2006). The
' on justice beliefs and adjustment.
Europ"an
benefits vs. costs of BIW to society are more Psychologist, I 2, 261-21 l.
Agrawal, M., &
Dalal, A. K. (1993). Beliefs abour the
ambiguous. Certain forms of BJW seem to align world and recovery from myocardial infarction.
with benefits, and other forms with costs: mus, it JournalofSocial psychology, I33,395-394.
..
is theoretically possible to reap some of the ben_ Allen, M. W., Ng, S., & Leiser, D. (2005). Adulteconomic
efits of BfW while avoiding its pitfalls (Suton & model and values survey: Cross_national differences
in economic beliefs. Journal of Economic psychology,
Douglas, 2005), a prospect that could be exam- 26,159_185.
ined in further research. Overall, distinguishing Almeida, A., Correia, I., & Marinho, S. (2010). Moral
dis_
between various forms of BJW or ways oi b"U"r_ engagement, normative beliefs ofpeer group, and atti_
ing in a just world is crucial in studying the rela_ tudes regarding roles in bullying. Journal of School
Volence, 9,23-36.
tion between BJW and various criteria, In doino Alves, H., & Correia, I. (200g). On the normativity of
so, it is important to control or" fo.r., Sffi expressing the belief in a just world: Empirical evi_
while examining the other. For example, since "f dence. Social Justice Research, 2 l, 106_llg.
personal and general BJW are inherently Alves, H., & Correia, I.
(2010a). personal and general
corre_ belief in a just world as judgeme ntnorms.Intemitional
lated, both should be measured, and their unique
Journal of P sycho lo gy, 45, Z2L-23 l.
effects interpreted. Unless the closely relaied Alves, H., & Correia, I. (2010b). The sfategic expression
form of BfW is accounted for, researchlrs might of personal belief in a just world. Eiropean
draw unwarranted conclusion, frorn tireu aat-a] P sycholo gist, I 5, 202-210.
Ambrosio, A. L., & Sheehan, E. p. (1990). Factor analysis
Regarding, the bases of BJW, there is little of the just world scale. Journal of Social psychollgy,
empirical or theoretical support for the notion 130,413415.
that individual differences in BJW reflecr Appelbaum, L.D. (2OOZ). Who deserves help? Students,
varia_
tion in the motive to believe in a just world opinions about the deservingness of different
$oups
described by Lerner (1977). yet,
living in Germany to receive aid. Social justie
many research_ Research, I5,201-225.
ers continue to equate individual differences in Appelbaum, L. D., Lennon, M. C., & Aber, J. L.
BIW with individual differences in the need (2006). When effort is rhreatening: The influence
to
believe in a just world. A review of the belief in a just world on Americans, attitudes
incorporating an-tipoverry pohcy. political psychology,
unpublished data that is nonsupportive !"1vild_
ofequiva_ 27,387-402.
lence might help end this Ball, G. A., Trevino, L. K., & Sims. H. p., Jr. (1993).
ongoing confusi,on in
the librature. More plausibt" Justice and organizational punishment: Anitudinal
Uuse, of individual oui-
differences comes of disciplinary events. Social Justice Research,
in BfW are exnerience with justice
6,39-67 .
C.L. Hafer and R. Sutton
156

(2007)' Belief in a just world'


Bastounis, M., Leiser, D., & Roland-L6vy,
c. (2004). *
correia, I., & Dalbert, c'
*liil);",flr"#'io":f
Psychosocial variables involved in-the
constructions
*t:::"
schoors European rournat fi#,ti":;
or Psvchotosv or ,i
!ia

ffflffii?lJ['3:l?i:::XlJli::#1TJ:::"T
of Economic Psychotogy' 25i' iiu'otion' 22' 42143.. '
*
,i
national suley.Journal
C'i"A'{- *oalbert' C' (2008)' School bullying belief in
263-278. -"'"-p.r*"rl just world of bullies' victims' and defend-
ri
*
M,, &Minibas-poussard, J. (2012). Causal l
Bastounis,
t'orto Zi' f*opio'n'Psychologist' 13'248-254'
attributions of workplace gender equality' iust i., u
j' (2oo+l' netiet in a just world' sub- I
'1

belief, and the self/otner dlstinction. social


c*.i.,
Behavioi y.t]t,.
i
andPersonality,40,433452,itr.tivewell.beingandEustofyoungadults'In {
faith in.peopre: 'ii:i:y;',:*it:::l,Y:;;!"7";::!;;:"To':::"t:
,1

B.gue, L. (2002). Beriefs ln:*ti" and 'ti


"';;:"::,!i;,,)T#ilf,,,ffk;*"#T;:l?i'-iT,1 i
'.i

*"
^w: ,"g[s#*:,",*Y;:'*x;xr*;
f*" spheres of belief Co,lJr.'J-. V. ?iSgti Anotttet psvchometric evaluation of
82'
I
l

BEgue, L., & Bastounis, frn. iiOOli Reports'


in justice: Extensive support for the. bidimensio.nai
'iuiot tit lry yorld scale' Psychological
;*#I;il. or Personatitv,
,rl3X';r1]i3*rl;1,."1., a just worrd, weu-being. and
--toping
with'a1un'iust"fate' In L'*Montada
H+i"lrr.j",* & M' J'
Beierlein, C., Werner, C. S., Preiser, S',
& Wermuth' S' and belief
just-world beliefs compatible withjustitj- L"Jt".; Got.), 'R esponses to vicrtmizations
(2011). Are al-rosi' New York' NYI Plenum
u. u -ol- l''"'i"''
ing inequality? Collective political .tn.u.y Press'
'"'tatpp'
entot. Social Justice neseirch' 24'278-296'- . . pai""' c. (1999). The world is more iust for me than
p. (1991). Evidence "-^gt"ttaivt
Bierhoff, H. w., Klein, R., & Kramp, etout the oersonal belief in a just world
for the altruistic personality from data on-accid#
personality, 59,263-280. i"ur.'t raioitv' social Justice Research' 12'79-98'
research. Joumal of . . Dd;, c. tzotirl' The iustice motive as a personal
Moag, J. F."iifi;i. inieractional iustice: -
J., & life
Bies, R.
In R. J. I ewickj,, ,rroun": Dealing wiih chnll'ng's and critical
communication criteria of fairness. Nt* vo't' NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum'
(Eds')' n"'iii
B. H. Sheppard' A Nt' tr' s-"t*an ""'i''c-tzoo9)' Belief in a iust world' In M' R' Leary
on negotiation in orgo;irl;;n iv"i
r, pp. +:-sii.
J )' ourl.ti,
" - differ'
press. tn' H' Hoyle (EdsJ' Haidbook of individual
Greenwich, cr: JAI (pp' New York'
Birt, c. M., & Dion, r. i.irsszl. Relative
deprivation )n i, i, social behavio, 288-297)'

theory and responses io discrimination


in a gay rnale NY: Guilford Press'

and lesbian sampte. Briri;; Journal ,7 Srlirt Dalbert,C.,Fisch,U',&Montada'L'(1992)'Isinequality


Psychology,26,1,39_145,-unjustrEvaluatingwomen'scareerchances.Revue
E'i'opA"""' de Psichologie Appliquie' 42' ll-17 '
jef,ogian' A' M' (2012)' Belief in
Brzer,G.Y., Hart, J., & oulb"'t,'c', & Katona-sallai' H' ilssol' The "belief
in a
a just world -u ,".i.i'1"*""". "ri***", just in Hungary' Journal of cross'
Evidence for a mediational pathway
pr"oi.ting r,"!;- world" construct
individuTis Cultural Psvcholoet' 27'293-314'
tive attitudes *d d;;#;;;n ugui,.,*
patUert, C., Lipkus, t], SuUuy' H" & Goch' I' (2001)' A
with mental i*ness. persorz ality and. maiuii)i just.and an unjust world; Structure and validity of
dif-
Differences, 52,42g432, beliefs' Personality and Individaal
Differences in ferent world--
Calhoun, L' G', & Cann' A' (1994)'
u:"i*otriiiltt'nitlty and point of Dffirences' 30' 561-577 '
assumptions uUor,
134'765.jnl;' Daltert'
- C" Montada' L" & Schmitt' M' (1987)' Glaube
view.Journal of Social Psychology' Validierungskorrelate
Carels, R. A., Young, f' M', Wott' C'
B" Uarper' f" un"in"gt"chteweltalsMotiv:
just world: Validation cor-
fr^. zweier 5kalen [Belief in a
Gumble, ,q.., Wugn.rtffoU;r: il.' & Ctayton,^,L.' Beitrase' 2e'
two scaresl' Psvchotogische
(z,oos). inte,.nuri,io *.r#,iie;^.1*"lil*:t:f# :.j;:.;Ji justice
";:i;'n,wi:;ilr:::;::,5?:Hffij:nsao** Daiberr, c,
& umrauft, S (200e): rheroleof-the
of
motive in-"tonon''it decision making' Journal
B., Hafer, C. L', Crosby, F J" & Foster' M' D'
Choma, Economic Psvchologv' 30' 172-180'
(2012). Percepti"", ;f;;;;;ar *e* oisctiminaiion' c., & YamauJhi, L' A' (1994)' Belief in a
just
The role of betief in i iust world and .itrutioiai Dalbert,

ambiguitv. tou,noi" i:'io'ioi r'v'notosv"7'i)" ;::*':ll:'Xffif jilfit]Htffi:::lil"'ttrlii


24'
568-5 85.
R., & Marek,
p. of lpplied social Psvchologv'
g:yu:y:!**al
christopher, A. N., Zabel, K. L., Jones, J. 1612-1626'
(2008)' Protestant ethic ideology: tts multifaceted Davev' L' M" Bobocel' D' R'' Son Hing' L' s''
&za'ld0a'
o"til;;;;;
relationships wittriuiiw-o'rJttri;i" *otiur p. qSS)'-neference for the merit principle scale:
uuttoritu,iuJr*. f,i. (f
orientarion, and right-wing An individual difference measure of distributive
jus-
personality and Individial Differences, 45,4n;;. Research' 12'
Cohn, E. S., & Modecki, K' L' (200?)' Cenaet
OfferJnces tice -preferences' Social Justice

in predicting Otfinq'ltnt utttu'ioi: Do individual


dif- 223-240'
*tj,t;*:tn[:*'1,$tli;f;,t:i:l]"":""Y;ffii:,11
rerences *u,t",t soii'ii'i'"ii"*ap*'onotiit'"ii'
359-374.
g Belief in a Just World 157

C. Dalbert & H. Sallay (Eds.), I&e justice motive in Furnham, A., & Procter, E. (1992). Sphere-speciflc just
ado I e s c e nc e and y oun g adultho o d : O ri g ins and c ons e - world beliefs and attitudes to AIDS. Human Relations,
quences (pp. 231-247). New York, NY: Routledge. 45,265-280.
Deutsch, M. (1985). Distributive jtntice: A social psychologi- Gawronski, B., & Creighton, L. A. (2013). Dual-process
cal perspective. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. theories. In D. E. Carlston(Ed.),The Oxford. handbook
Dion, K. L., & Dion, K. K. (1987). Belief in a just world of social cognition (pp. 282-312). New York, NY:
and physical attractiveness stereotyping. Journal of Oxford University Press.
Pe r s onality and S o c ial P sy cholo gy, 5 2,'7 7 5 -7 80. Ghorpade, J., Lackritz, J., & Singh, G. (2006). Correlates
Diftmar, H,, & Dickinson, J. (1993). The perceived rela- of the protestant ethic of hard work: Results from a
tionship between the belief in a just world and sociopo- diverse ethno-religious sanple. Jounml of Applied
litical ideology. Social Justice Research, 6,257-272. S oc ial P sy cholo gy, 3 6, 2449-247 3.

Donat, M., Umlauft, S., Dalbert, C., & Kamble, S. V. Gunter, B., & Wober, M. (1983). Television viewing and
(2012). Belief in a just world, teacher justice, and bul- public trust. British Journal of Social Psychology, 22,
lying behavior. A ggressive Behavior; J8, 85-193. 174-176.
Duckjn, J. (2001). A dual process cognitive-motivational Hafer, C. L. (2000). Investment in long-term goals and
theory of ideology and prejudice. In M. Zanna (Ed.), commitment to just means drive the need to believe in
Advances in expeimental social psychology CVol. 33, a just world. Personality and Social Psychology
pp. 4l-1 I 3). San Diego, CA: Academic. Bulletin, 26, 1059-1073.
Ebneter, D. S., Latner, L D., & O'Brien, K. S; (2011). Just Hafer, C. L., & BEgue, L. (2005). Experimental research
world beliefls, causal beliefs, and acquaintance: on just-world theory: Problems, developments, and
Associations with stigma toward eating disorders and future challenges. Psychological Bulletin, 13l,
obeslty. Personality and Individual Dffirences, 51, 128-167.
618-622. Hafer, C. L., Bogaert, A. F., & McMullen, S. (2001).
Ellard, J. H., & Bates, D. D. (1990). Evidence for the role Belief in a just world and condom use in a sample of
of the justice motive in status generalization pro- gay and bisexual mer. Jountal of Applied Social
cesses. Social Justice Research, 4, L15-134, Psychology, 3 I , 1892-1910.
Ellard, J. H., Harvey, A., & Callan, M. i. (2016). The jus- Hafer, C. L., & Choma, B. L. (2009). Belief in a iust
tice motive: History theory, and research. In world, perceived fairness, and justiflcation of the sta-
C. Sabbagh & M. Schmitt (Eds.), Handbook of social tus quo. In J. Jost, A. C. Kay, & H. Thorisdottir (Eds.),
justice theory and research (pp.127-143). New York, Social and psychological bases ofideology and system
NY: Springer. justification (pp. 107-125). New York, NY: Oxford
Fasel, R., & Spini, D. (2010). Effects of victimization on Universiry Press.
the belief in a just world in four ex-Yugoslavian coun- Hafer, C. L., & Coney, B. L. (1999). Mediators of the
ties. Soc ial Justic e Re s e arch, 2 3, 17 -j 6. relation between beliefs in a just world and emotional
Fatima, I., & Suhail, K. (2010). Belief in a just world and responses to negative outcomes. social Justice
subjective well-being: Mothers of normal and down Research, t2,189-204.
syndrome children. International Joumal of Hafer, C. L., & Gosse, L. (2010). Preserving the belief in
P sycho lo gy, 4 5, 461468. a just world: Wlen and for whom are different strate-
Fischer, A. R., & Holz, K. B. (2010). Testing a model of gies preferred? In D. R. Bobocel, A. C. Kay, M. P.
women's personal sense of justice, control, well- Zmna, &J. M. Olson @ds.),The psychology of justice
being, and dishess in the context of sexist discrimina- and legitimacy: The Ontario symposium (Vo1. 11,
tion. Psychology of Women QuanerLy, i4,297_310. pp.79-102). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
Fox, C. L.. Elder, T., Gater, J.. & Johnson, E. (2010). The Hafer, C. L., & Olson, J. M. (1989). Beliefs in a just world
association between adolescents' beliefs in a just and reactions to personal deprivation. Journal of
world and their attitudes to victims of bullying. Bririsft Pe rs on ality, 57, 7 99-823.
- l ou mal of Educational P sychology, 80, I 83- 198. Hafer, C. L., & Olson, J. M. (1993); Beliefs in a just
Furnham, A. (1985). Just world Uel.iefs in an unjust soci- world, discontent, and assertive actions by working
ety: A cross-cultural comparison. European Journal of women. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Sr4inl Psychology, l'5,363-366. 19, 30-38.
-
t'urnham, A. ( I 993). Just world beliefs in twelve
societies. Hafer, C. L., & Olson, J. M. (1998). Individual differences
The Journal of SociaL Psychology, 1jj,317-329. in the belief in a just world and responses to personal
^
nurnham, A. (1995). The just world, charirable giving misfortune. In L. Montada & M. J. Lerner (Eds.),
and
attitudes to disability. Personality and Individual Responses to victimizatiois and belief in a just world
Difle rences. I 9. 577-583. (pp. 65-86). New,York, NY: Plenum Press.
Furnham, A. (2003). Belief in a just world: Research Hagedoorn, M., Buunk, B. P.,& Van de Vliert, E. (2002).
progess over the past decade. Personality and Do just world believers process unfair authoritative
Individual Dffirences, 34, jgs-glj. decisions differently? Applied Psychology: An
^l'urnham. A., & Procter, E. (1989). Belief in a just world: httemational Review, 51 , 126-145.
Reviep and critique of the individual diflerence lirerature. Hellman, C. M., Muilenburg-Trevino, E. M., & Worley, J. A.
Eritish Joumal of Sociat Psychology, 28,365-384. (2008). The belief in a just world: An examination of

l&
'158 C.L. Hafer and R. Sutton

reliability estimates affoss three measures. Journal oJ Lind, E. A., & Tyler, T. R. (1988). The socialpsychology
Personality As ses sment, 90, 39940 l. of procedural jusrice. New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Henderson-King, D., Henderson-King' E., Bolea, B" Lipkus, I. (1991). The construction and preliminary vali-
Koches, K., & Kauffman, A. (2004). Seeking under- dation of a global belief in ajust world scale and the
standing or sending bombs: Beliefs as predictors of exploratory analysis of the multidimensional belief in
responses to terrorism. Peace andConflict: Journal of a just world scale. Personality and Individual
1 0, 67 -84.
Peace P sychologY, Dffi renc e s, I 2, ll7 l-ll7 8'
Hunt, M. O. (2000). Status, religion, and the "belief in a Lipkus, L, & Bissonnette, V. L' (1996). Relationships
just world": Comparing African Americans, Latinos, in a just world, willingness to accom-
among belief
and Whites. Social Science Quarterly, 81,325-343' modate, and marital well-being. Personality and
Jackson, B., Kubzansky, L. D., & Wright' R. J' (2006)' Social P sychology Bulletin, 22, 1043-1056.
Linking perceived unfairness to physical health: The Lipkus, I. M., Dalbert, C., & Siegler, I. C. (1996). The
perceived unfairness model. Review of General importance of distinguishing the belief in ajust world
Psychology, 10,2140. for self versus for others: Implications for psychologi-
Jost, J. T. (2009). "Elective affinides": On the psychologi- cal well-being. Personality and Social Psychology
ca1 bases of left-right differences. Psychological Bulletin, 22,666417.
Inquiry, 20,129-141. Lipkus, L M., & Siegler, I. C. (1993). The belief in a just
Jost, J. T., & Banaji, M. R. (1994). The role of stereotyp- world and perceptions of disoriminatiort- Joumal of
ing in system-justification and the production of false Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 127,
consciousness. British Jounml of Social Psychology, 465474.
33, L-21 . Lodewijkx, H. F. M., Kersten' G' L. E.' & van Zomeren,
Jost, J. T., & Hunyady, O. (2005)' Antecedents and conse- M. (2008). Dual pathways to engage in 'silent
quences of
system-justifying ideologies' Cttrrent marches' against violence: Moral outrage, moral
i) i re c t io ns in al S c ie nc e, I 4, 260-265'
P sy cho I o g ic cleansing and modes of identification. Joumal of
Kaiser, C. R., Vick, S. 8., & Major, B. (2004). A prospec- Community & Applied Social Psychology, 18,

tive investigation of the relationship between just- I53- I 67.


a just world: Support for inde-
Loo, R. (2002a). Belief in
world beliefs and the desire for revenge after
September 11, 2001. Psychological Science, 15, pendent just world and unjust world dimensions.
Pe r s o nal ity and Indiv idaal D ffi renc e s, 3 3, 7 03-:7 | 1.
503-506.
Kamble, S. V., & Dalbert, C. (2012). Belief in a just world Loo, R. (2002b). A
psychometric and cross-national
and wellbeing in Indian schools. International Jountal examination of a belief in a just world scale. Journal
of Psychology, 47, 269-2'18. of Apptied Social Psychology, 32, 1396-1406.
Keller, C., & Siegrist, M. (2010). Psychological resources Lucas, T., Alexander, S., Firestone, I., & LeBreton, J. M.
and attitudes toward people with physical disabilities'
(2007). Development and initial validation of a proce-
J o um al of Ap pti e d S o c ial P sy chol o gy, 4 0, 389 401' dural and distributivejust world measure' Personaliry
Kielcolt-Glaser, I. K., & Williams, D. A. (1987). Self- and Indiv idual Differenc e s, 4 3,'l 7-82.
blame, compliance, and distress among burn patients' Lucas, T., Aiexander, S., Firestone, I., & LeBreton, J' M'
JoumaL of Personality and Social Psychology, 5j, (2008). Just world beliefs, perceived sffess, and health
r8'7-193. behavior: The impact of a procedurally just world.
Kogut, T., & Ritov, I. (2011). 'Protective donation' When P syclulogy & Health, 23, 849-865.

iefusing a request for a donation increases the sense of Lucas, T., Alexander, S., Firestone, I., & LeBreton, J. M.
rf (2009). Beliefin ajust world, social influence, and ill-
vulnerability. Joumal Experimental Social
P sychology, 47, 1059'1069. ness attributions: Evidence of a just world boomerang

Lambert, A. J., Burroughs, T., & Nguyen, T. (1999)' effect. Joumal of Health Psychology, 14,258-266.
Perceptions of risk and the buffering hypothesis: The Lucas, T., Zhdanova, L., &
Alexander, S. (2011)'
role ofjust world beliefs and right-wing authoritarian- Procedural and distributive justice beliefs for self and
ism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, othels. Joumal of hdividual Differences, 32, l+25'
643-656. Maes, J. (1998a). Eight stages in the development of
Laurin, K., Fitzsimons, G. M., & Kay, A. C. (2011). Social research on the construct of the belief in ajust *orld'
disadvantage and the seif-regulatory function of jus- In L. Montada & M. J. Lerner (Eds.), Responses to
tice beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social victimizations andbetief in a justworld(pp. 163-185)'
P sy cholo gy, I 00, 1 49-17 1. New York, NY: Plenum hess.
Lemer, M. J. (1977). The justice motive: Some hypotheses as Maes, J. (1998b). Immanent justice and ultimate justice:
to its origins atdforms. Joumal of Personality, 45,1-52' Two ways of believing in justice. In L. Montada &
Lerner, M. J. (1978). "Belief in a just world" versus the M. J. Lemer (Eds.), Responses to victimizations atxd
"authoritarianism" syndrome... but nobody liked the belief in a just world (pp. 9-40). New York, NY:
Indians. Ethnicity, 5, 229-237. Plenum Press.
Lerner, M. i. ( 1980). The belief in a just world: A funda- Maes, J., & Schmitt, M. (1999). More on ultimarc and
mental delusion New York, NY: Plenum Press' immanent justice: Results from the research project
8 Belief in a Just World
159

"Justice as a problem within reunified Germany,'. Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgment of the child
Social Justice Research, 12,6518. (M. Gabain, Trans.). New York, Ny: The Free press.
Martin, T. A., & Cohn, E. S. (2004). Anitudes toward rhe (Original work published 1932)
criminal legal system: Scale development and predic- Reser, J. P., & Muncer, S. (2004). Sense-making in the
tors. Psychology, Crime & lnW 10,367-391. wake of September ltth: A network analysis of lay
McParland, J. L., & Knussen, C. (2010). Just world beliefs understandings. British Journal of psychology, 95,
moderate the relationship ofpain intensity and disabil- 283_296.
ity with psychological distress in chronic pain support Rubin,2., & Peplau, A. (1973). Belief in a just world and
group member s. Euro p e an J o untal of pain, I 4, 7 1-:7 6, reactions to another,s lot: A study ofparticipants in the
Mil1er, D. T. (1977). Altruism and threat ro a belief in a national draft lottery. Jountal of Social Issues, 29,
jlstwotld. Joumal of Experimental Social psychology, 73-93.
13, tt3-124. Rubin,2., & Peplau, L. A. (1975). Who believes in a jusr
Mohiyeddini, C., & Montada, L. (1998). BJW and self- world? Joumal of Social Issues, 3 I, 65_g9,
efficacy in coping with observed victimization: Rtisch, N., Todd, A. R., Bodenhausen, G. V., & Corrigan,
Results from a study about unemployment. In P. W. (2010). Do people with mental illness deserve
L. Montada & M. J. Lerner (Eds.), Responses to vic- what they get? Links between meritocratic world_
timizations and belief in a just world (pp. 4l-54). views and implicit versus explicit stigma. European
New York, NY: Plenum Press. Archiv e s of P sy chi atry & Clin ic al N e uro s c ie nc e,
26 0,
Montada, L., & Schneider, A. (1989). Jusrice and emo- 617-625.
tional reactions to the disadvantaged,. Social Justice Sakalli-Ugurlu, N,, Yalgrn, Z. 5., & Glick, p. (2007).
Research, 3,313-344. Ambivalent sexism, belief in a just world, and empa_
Montada, L. (1998). Justice: Just a rational choice? Social thy as predictors of Tukish shrdents, attitudes toward
Justice Research, I 1, 81-l}l. rape victims. Sex Roles,5Z 889-895.
Ng, S. H., & Allen, M. W. (2005). Perception of economic Schlenker, B. R., Chambers, J. R., & Le, B. M. (2OlZ).
distributive justice: Exploring leading theories,,locial Conservatives are happier than liberals, but why?
e havior and Personality, 3 3, 435454.
B Political ideology, personaiity, and life satisfaction.
Nudelman, G. (2013). The belief in a just world and per- Journal of Research in PersonaLity, 46, l}1,-146.
sonality: A meta-analysis. Social Jusice Research,26, Schmitt, M. J. (1998). Methodological strategies in
105-1 I9. research to validate measures of belief in a jusi world.
Nudelman, G., & Shiloh, S. (2011). Who deserves to be In L. Montada & M. J. Lerner (Eds.), Responses to
sick? An exploration of the relationships between victimizations and belief in a just world(pp.187_215).
belief in a just world, illness causal attributions and New York, NY: Plenum press.
their fairness judgements. Psychology, Health & Schmitt, M., Gollwitzer, M., Maes, J., & Arbach, D.
Medicine, 16,675485. (2005). Justice sensitivity: Assessment and location in
O'Connor, W. E.. Morrison, T. G., Mcleod, L. D., & the personality space. European Journal ol
Anderson, D. (1996). A meta-anal1tic review of therela- Psychological Assessment, 2 t , ZOZ-211.
tionship between gender and belief in a just world. Schrinpflug, U., & Bilz, L. (2004). Transmission of the
Journal of Social Behavior & personality, I 1,141_14g. belief in a just world in the family. In C. Dalbert &
O'Connor, W E., Monison, T. G., & Mbrrison, M. A. H. Sallay (Eds,), The justice motive ilt adolescence
(1996). The reliability and factor structure of the and young aduhhood: Origins and consequences
global belief in a just world scale. Journal of Social (pp.43-63). New York, Ny Routledge.
P sy cholo gy, I 3 6, 667 Shorkey, C. T. (1980). Relationship between rational
-668.
Oldmeadow, J., & Fiske, S. T. (2007). System-justifying thinking and belief in a just world. psychological
ideologies moderate status competence stereotypei: Reports,46, 16l-162.
=
Roles for beliefin a just world and social dominance Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social dominance: An
orientation. European Joumal of Social psychalogy, intergroup theory of social hierarchy and oppression.
J7. I t35_l t48. New York, NY: Cambridge University press.
Olson, J. M., & Hafer, C. L. (2001). Tolerance ofpersonal
Smith, K. B. (1985). Seeing justice in poverty: The belief
deprivation. In J. T. Jost & B. Major (Eds.), The psy- in a just world and ideas about inequalities.
chology oflegitimacy: Emerging perspectives on iie- Sociological Spectum, 5, 17*Zg.
ology, justice, and intergroup relations (pp. 157-175). Smith, L., Mao, S., Perkins, S., & Ampuero, M. (2011).
New York, NY: Cambridge University press. The relationship of clients' social class to early thera-
^
Otto. K.. & Dalbert, C. (200i). Belief in just
a world and peutic impressions. Counselling psychology
its functions for young prisoners. Journal of Researclt
Quarterly, 24, 15-27 .
i n Pe rs onality,
39, 559-57 3. Steensma, H., & van Dijke, R. (2005-06). Attributional
Puikh. S. B., Poit, p., & Flo*".s,
C. (201 I ). Relationship styles, self-esteem, and just world belief of victims of
between a belief in a just world and social justicl bullying in Dutch organizations. Interuational
advocacy attitudes of school counselors. Couiseling
Quarterly of Community Health Education, 25,
and Values, 56, 5i-72. 381-392.
160 clL Haf:: and ,: t:llon

Stowers. D. A,, & Durm, M. W. ( 1998). Is belief in a just Thalboume, M. A. ( 1995). Further studies of rhe measuremenr
worid rational ? P s yt'h o ct gic a I Rep on s. 83. 423426.
I and conelates of belief in the paranormal. Journal of
Strelan. P.. & Suuon. R. M. (201 l). When jusr-world the Atnerican Soc'ieq, .for Ps1t6!1irn1 Research, 89.
beliefs promote and when they inhibit tbrgiveness. 233-247.
Pe rs ona litt, an d I ndi v idu al D iffe rc nc e s, 50, I 63-1 68. Tomalia, J.. & Blascovich. J. (1994). Effecrs of.iustice treliets
Sutton. R. lvl., & Douglas. K. N{. (2005). Justice fbr all. or on cognitive appraisal of and subjective, physiological.
just for me? More evidence of the importance of the and behavioral responses to potential stress..loumal 14
sell'-other disdnction in jusr-wor1d beiiefs. Pe rs o na li t t Pe rs onal i 4, an d S od a I P w c hokt g y, 67, 7 32-'7 40.
artd I nd it, idu a L D iffe re n c e,s, 3 9. 6 -17 -645. White. K.. MacDonnell. R.. & Ellard, J. H. (2012). Belief
Sutton. R. M.. Douglas. K. M., Wilkin, K.. Elder. T. J.. in a just world: Consumer intentions and behaviors
Cole. J. M., & Stathi. S. (2008). Justice for whom. toward ethical products. Jounnl of Marketing, 76.
exactly? Beliefs in justice for the self and various orh- 103-t 18.
ers. Personali* and Stxial P.sv.hology Bulletin, 34, Wu, M. S.. Sutton, R. M., Yan, X., Zhou, C,. Chen, y..
528-541. Zhu.2., & Han. B. (2013). Time frame and justice
Sutton, R. M.. & Winnard . E. J. (2007). Seeing the future motive: Future perspective moderates the adaptive
through lenses ofjustice: The relevance ofjust-world function of general belief in a just world. PLOS ONE.
belief's to intentions and confidence in the future. 8(11), e80668.
Briti.slt Jcturnal of Social Pstc.luilog1,, 46.649-666. Wu. M. S., Yan, X.. Zhou, C.. Chen, Y., Li,l..Zhu.Z., ..
Taylor, S. E.. & Brown. G. (1988). lllusion and well- Han, B. (2011). General belief in a jusr world and
being: A social psychological perspective on mental resilience: Evidence from a collectivistic culture.
health. Ps.vc*o/ o g i c a I B ul I e r h, I 0 3. 193 -21 0, Eu ro p e an J ou rn a I r t f Pe rx nalit1,, 2 5, 13 -442. 1

Testd, 8.. Maisonneuve, C,, Assilamdhou, Y.. & Perrin. S. Xie. X., Liu. H., & Gan, Y. (201 I ). Betief in a just wortu
(2012). What is an "appropriate" migrant? Impact of when encountering the 5/12 Wenchuan earthquak!..
the adoption of meritocratic worldviews hy potential Enyi ronnrcnt and Behavior: 4 3, 566-586.
nelvcomers on their perceived ability to integrate into Zuckerman, M. (1975). Belief in a just world and altruistj,
a Western society. Eurutpean Jr,,urual of Social behavior. Jounr al of Personality and Social Psttcholog...
P s.tcho I t gv, 4 2. 261- )68.
r
-11.972-976.

You might also like