You are on page 1of 11

International Psychogeriatrics (2010), 22:4, 650–660 

C International Psychogeriatric Association 2010


doi:10.1017/S1041610210000074

Development and validation of the Caregiver


Guilt Questionnaire
.........................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................

Andrés Losada,1 Marı́a Márquez-González,2 Cecilia Peñacoba1


and Rosa Romero-Moreno1
1
Psychology Department, Universidad Rey Juan Carlos, Madrid, Spain
2
Clinical and Health Psychology Department, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain

ABSTRACT

Background: Family care of frail elderly people has been linked to significant negative consequences for
caregivers’ mental health. Although outcome variables such as burden and depression have been widely
analyzed in this population, guilt, an emotion frequently observed in caregivers, has not received sufficient
attention in the research literature.
Methods: Face-to-face interviews were carried out with 288 dementia caregivers. Guilt was measured using
the Caregiver Guilt Questionnaire (CGQ).
Results: Using principal components analysis, 22 items were retained and five factors were obtained which
explained 59.25% of the variance. These factors were labeled: guilt about doing wrong by the care recipient,
guilt about not rising to the occasion as caregivers, guilt about self-care, guilt about neglecting other relatives,
and guilt about having negative feelings towards other people. Acceptable reliability indexes were found, and
significant associations between the CGQ and its factors and the Zarit Burden Interview guilt factor were
also found. Caregivers with higher scores on the CGQ also scored higher in depression, anxiety, frequency
and appraisal of behavioral problems. Negative associations between the CGQ and its factors and frequency
of/and satisfaction with leisure and social support were also found. Being female and caring for a parent were
associated with higher scores on the CGQ.
Conclusions: Feelings of guilt are significantly related to caregiver distress. The CGQ may be a useful measure
for acknowledging feelings of guilt in caregivers; moreover, it can be used as an outcome variable for
psychoeducational interventions aimed at reducing caregiver distress.

Key words: anxiety, assessment, behavioral problems, burden, dementia, depression, leisure, social support

Introduction Cooper et al., 2008). There remain, however, some


emotions, such as guilt, which have not received
Family care of frail elderly people has been linked
adequate attention in the research literature, even
to significant negative consequences for caregivers’
though they are frequently observed in caregivers
mental health. Outcome variables such as burden
(Yaffe, 1988).
and depression have been widely analyzed, with
Guilt has been described as “the dysphoric
many research studies demonstrating that caregivers
feeling associated with the recognition that one
as a group, especially if they are caring for relatives
has violated a personally relevant moral or
with Alzheimer’s disease and other dementias, show
social standard” (Kugler and Jones, 1992). Guilt
higher scores on depression and stress than non-
has also been suggested as a factor potentially
caregivers (e.g. Pinquart and Sörensen, 2003).
contributing to depression and distress in non-
Other negative emotional outcomes in caregiving,
dementia caregivers (Boye et al., 2002; Spillers
such as anger or anxiety, have received more
et al., 2008) and non-caregiving samples (Ghatavi
attention in recent years (Coon et al., 2003;
et al., 2002) and some research considers it as a
Correspondence should be addressed to: Andrés Losada-Baltar, Facultad de main emotion for caregivers, which may exacerbate
Ciencias de la Salud. Departamento de Psicologı́a, Edificio Departamental II. their burden (e.g. Brodaty, 2007). The association
Avda. de Atenas, s/n. 28922 Alcorcón, Madrid, Spain. Phone: + 34 914888941. between guilt and burden or depression may be
Email: andres.losada@urjc.es. Received 30 Sep 2009; revision requested 26 Oct
2009; revised version received 4 Jan 2010; accepted 5 Jan 2010. First published either direct or indirect, that is, mediated or
online 22 February 2010. moderated by other relevant variables in caregiving,
Caregiver Guilt Questionnaire 651

such as social support or frequency of and sample of 70 dementia caregivers, finding no


satisfaction with leisure or pleasant activities. significant association between the two variables. It
Despite the fact that researchers have called for is important to note, however, that the six items
the analysis of guilt in both community and long- making up the Caring Guilt Scale are written in
term care (e.g. Hamel, 1995; Ankri et al., 2005), conditional terms (e.g., “I would worry about my
the number of studies analyzing guilt in dementia relative if I did not care for them as I do”), so that the
caregiving remains small. answers may be reflecting not the current situation,
Gonyea et al. (2008) analyzed guilt by rating but rather an imagined one. Also, the psychometric
answers to an open-ended question posed to 66 data the authors provided for the scale were scarce.
caregivers of elderly relatives (“Some women tell Considering that (a) guilt is an important feeling
us that they feel guilty about their caregiving: how for caregivers, (b) improving our knowledge about
guilty would you say you feel about it?”). Drawing this variable will increase our ability to understand
upon the stress and coping model, these authors the factors contributing to caregiver distress, and
found that guilt contributed to the explanation of a (c) there are few instruments available for
significant proportion of burden, even when other measuring this construct, the aim of this study is to
significant variables were controlled for (e.g. basic develop a measure for assessing guilt in dementia
and instrumental activities of daily living). caregivers and to assess its psychometric properties.
Guilt has also been analyzed in dementia
caregiving as one of the components (factors) of
burden. Through principal component analysis of Methods
the Zarit Burden Interview (ZBI; Zarit et al.,
1980), Ankri et al. (2005) obtained a factor made Population
up of four items, which they called “guilt”, as Participants were caregivers of relatives with
most of these items assessed a general sense of dementia living in the community. All caregivers
inadequacy as a caregiver (e.g. “Do you feel that were recruited through social and health care
you should be doing more for your relative?” centers in Madrid (Spain) between September 2005
or “Do you feel you could do a better job and May 2009. Once located, an initial interview
in caring for your relative”). They found this was carried out by telephone in order to confirm
subscale to be significantly related to the patient’s that volunteers were the primary caregivers of family
behavioral problems (e.g. verbal aggressiveness and members with dementia. In order to participate
sadness). In a study with caregivers of children in the study, caregivers were required to identify
with intellectual disabilities, Gallagher et al. (2008) themselves as the primary source of help for their
found that guilt, as assessed with the ZBI, was the relatives, and to report devoting more than one
strongest predictor of caregiver anxiety. However, hour per day to caregiving duties for more than
even though the ZBI’s guilt factor may function three months during the assessment period. Two-
as a good indicator of a general sense of guilt hundred-and-ninety caregivers were called for the
in caregivers, we consider this feeling to be a assessment interview. Two of them quitted the
rather complex one, since as well as this general interview referring to lack of time due to caregiving
feeling of inadequacy as a caregiver there may duties. The sample characteristics of the final
be different types of guilt specifically associated sample (288 caregivers) are shown in Table 1.
with different sources or situations potentially Caregivers gave their consent to participate in the
affecting a caregiver’s emotional state; for example, study, and approval for the research was obtained
interactions with the care recipient, role conflict, from both the Spanish Ministry of Education and
devoting time to themselves, etc. The analysis of the Ethics Committee at the Rey Juan Carlos
the specific type of guilt or the diverse sources University (Madrid).
contributing to the generation of this emotion may
be very helpful for clinicians working with the
population in question. Measures
To our knowledge, there is only one instrument Face-to-face interviews were carried out by trained
that has been specifically developed for measuring psychologists with each of the caregivers at
guilt in dementia caregivers, namely, the Caring the participating social and health centers. The
Guilt Scale developed by Martin et al. (2006). interviews lasted approximately 90 minutes, and
This scale focuses on feelings of regret and sense included the following variables (means, standard
of responsibility (e.g. “If I did not spend my deviations and range for the assessed variables are
time caring for my relative I know I would feel shown in Table 2):
deep regret”). Martin et al. (2006) analyzed the Sociodemographic information. This comprised the
relationship between guilt and depression in a caregiver’s and care recipient’s age, gender and
652 A. Losada et al.

Table 1. Sample characteristics


N % M SD RANGE
...........................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................

Caregiver age (years) 288 59.63 12.60 29–87


Caregiver sex
Female 228 79.2
Male 60 20.8
Daily hours caring 10.91 7.84 1–24
Time caring (in months) 53.15 46.10 3–312
Co-residence
Yes 225 78.1
No 63 21.9
Care recipient
Spouse 107 37.2
Parent 166 57.6
Other relative (father-in-law, 15 5.2
mother-in-law,
aunt, etc.)
Care recipient age 78.97 8.35 48–97
Relative’s illness
Alzheimer’s disease 167 58.4
Other dementia 121 41.6

Table 2. Means, standard deviations and range for Burden. The Zarit Burden Interview (Zarit et al.,
the assessed variables 1980) was used, and the ZBI guilt factor obtained by
Ankri et al. (2005) was used to analyze convergent
M SD RANGE
........................................................................................................................................................
validity. Because the burden scale was included in
Burden 30.50 15.90 1–69 the latter stages of the study, data for this instrument
Frequency of behavioral 34.90 14.68 0–83 are only available for 166 caregivers. The internal
problems consistency found in this study for this scale was
Appraisal of problem 17.81 14.62 0–64 0.90 (Cronbach’s α).
behaviors Frequency and appraisal of behavioral problems.
Care recipient functional 66.34 31.01 0–100 The Revised Memory and Behavior Problems
status Checklist (Teri et al., 1992) was used to assess these
Frequency of leisure 8.21 4.41 0–23 variables. It includes 24 items which, through two
activities subscales, assess both the frequency of behavioral
Satisfaction with leisure 6.57 3.56 0–19
problems and the appraisal of these behaviors as
activities
being irritating or upsetting for the caregivers. In
Social support 10.63 3.84 1–18
Anxiety 16.14 8.91 0–36 this study, the internal consistency found for the
Depression 18.00 11.73 0–55 frequency scale was 0.82, and for the appraisal scale
(45.49∗ ) was 0.88 (Cronbach’s α).
∗ Percentage
Functional status. The care recipient’s functional
of caregivers above clinical cut-off for depression. status was assessed with the Barthel Index
(Mahoney and Barthel, 1965). Higher scores on this
kinship, time spent caring (in months), and number measure are indicative of better physical functioning
of hours per day devoted to care. of the relative. Internal consistency of this scale was
Guilt. Following a review of the literature, and 0.92. (Cronbach’s α).
based on the clinical experience of the authors and Frequency of and satisfaction with leisure.
three other experts consulted for this purpose, we Satisfaction with leisure time was rated using the
developed an initial pool of 34 items measuring Stevens et al. (2004) Leisure Time satisfaction
guilt. All the items were originally created for the measure. This instrument consists of six items
development of the Caregiver Guilt Questionnaire measuring the level of satisfaction felt by caregivers
and no initial domains or factors were hypothesized, with regard to the amount of time spent on leisure
given the limited literature in this area. Response activities over the past month (quiet time for
options range from 0 (“never”) to 4 (“always or yourself, engaging in hobbies, etc.). It was measured
almost always”) (see Appendix). on a three-point Likert-type scale (0 = “not at
Caregiver Guilt Questionnaire 653

all”; 2 = “a lot”). Frequency of leisure activities (eigenvalues from 4.08 for the first factor to 1.77 for
was measured using an adaptation of the Leisure the fifth factor) explained 59.25% of the variance.
Time satisfaction measure. Caregivers were asked Factor loadings are shown in Table 3.
to report the extent to which they had participated The first factor includes seven items referring to
in the assessed activities over the previous month. caregivers’ feelings of guilt associated with negative
This was measured on a five-point Likert-type scale feelings, emotions or acts towards their relative
(0 = “not at all”; 4 = “a lot”). Internal consistency (e.g. “I have felt bad about getting angry with the
indices in the present study were 0.83 for the person I’m caring for”). This factor, of which all
satisfaction scale and 0.73 for the frequency scale items are positively scored, was labeled guilt about
(Cronbach’s α). doing wrong by the care recipient. The six items of
Social support. The Psychosocial Support the second factor mainly reflect caregivers’ feeling
Questionnaire (PSQ; Reig et al., 1991) was used. that, in general, they could be doing a better job
This scale has six items (e.g. “My friends and/or as a caregiver (e.g. “I have thought that perhaps
relatives pay me visits at home”) that were used to I’m not caring well for my relative”). This factor
assess caregivers’ perceptions of the frequency of was labeled guilt about failing to meet the challenges
support they receive. Response options range from of caregiving. All items are positively scored except
0 (“never”) to 3 (“very often”). In the present study item number 6, which is inversely scored. The third
we found an internal consistency index for this scale factor contains four items regarding caregivers’
of 0.78 (Cronbach’s α). negative feelings about looking after themselves
Anxiety. The Tension-Anxiety subscale from the and taking part in activities other than caring for
Profile of Mood States (POMS; McNair et al., their relative (e.g. “I have felt bad for leaving my
1971) was used. It consists of nine items (e.g. relative in the care of someone else while I had
“tense”), and caregivers are asked to rate on a fun”). This factor was labeled guilt about self-care,
Likert-type scale (0 = “not at all”; 4 = “extremely”) and all its items are positively scored. The fourth
how they felt during the previous week. Internal factor is made up of two items reflecting caregivers’
consistency for this scale in this study was 0.85 negative feelings associated with the fact of not
(Cronbach’s α). being able to devote as much time as they would
Depression. Depression was assessed by means of wish to their other relatives (e.g. “I have felt bad
the Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression for not looking after my other relatives [husband,
Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977), a 20-item measure wife, children, etc.] as I should”). This factor was
that assesses depressive symptomatology (e.g. “I felt labeled guilt about neglecting other relatives, and all its
sad”), with response options ranging from 0 (“rarely items are positively scored. The fifth and final factor
or none of the time [less than one day per week]”) includes three items about caregivers’ negative
to 3 (“most or all of the time [5–7 days perweek]”). feelings related to having had negative emotions or
Scores of 16 or higher may be indicative of clinical feelings towards other people who do not devote
depression (Radloff and Teri, 1986). Cronbach’s α time and effort to caregiving duties as they should,
coefficient for this scale was 0.90. or who do not have such responsibilities. This
factor was labeled guilt about having negative feelings
towards other people, and all its items are positively
scored.
Results Scores for the resulting guilt subscales are
determined by computing the sum of the scores
Factorial validity of the Caregiver Guilt of the items belonging to the corresponding scale,
Questionnaire with item number 6 being first reversed. The total
Following the usual procedures for the development score on the Caregiver Guilt Questionnaire (CGQ)
of assessment instruments, a principal components consists of the sum of the scores of all the subscales.
analysis of the 34 initial items of the scale was A higher score reflects greater guilt.
carried out using varimax rotation, in order to
maximize the orthogonality of the factors. Five
factors were obtained with eigenvalues higher than Reliability
1.0. The ten items showing the lowest factor Acceptable to good reliability indices were obtained
loadings (<0.45) on the corresponding factor for the CGQ factors and the full scale. Specifically,
were removed. In subsequent analysis, five factors a Cronbach’s α of 0.88 was obtained for the total
explained 56.58% of the variance. Next, another scale, 0.89 for the guilt about doing wrong by the
two items with the lowest factor loadings on care recipient factor; 0.76 for the guilt about failing
the corresponding factor (<0.45) were deleted. to meet the challenges of caregiving factor; 0.69 for
The resulting 22 items loaded on five factors the guilt about self-care factor; 0.86 for the guilt
654 A. Losada et al.

Table 3. Factor loadings of the CGQ items


FAC TO R S

CGQ ITEMS 1 2 3 4 5
...........................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................

11. I have felt bad about telling off the person I’m caring for, for some 0.849
reason
10. I have felt bad about getting angry with the person I’m caring for 0.832
14. I have felt bad about not having more patience with the person I’m 0.754 0.375
caring for
2. I have felt guilty about the way I’ve sometimes behaved with my 0.753
relative
12. I’ve got angry with myself for having negative feelings toward the 0.693
person I’m caring for
8. I have felt bad about things I may have done wrong with the person 0.602 0.340
I’m caring for
20. I have felt guilty about having so many negative emotions in relation to 0.513 0.341
caring
22. I have felt guilty thinking that my lack of information and 0.771
preparedness might mean that I’m not handling the care of my relative
in the best way possible
9. I have thought that perhaps I’m not caring well for my relative 0.317 0.711
21. I have thought that the way I care for my relative may not be 0.636
appropriate and may make his/her problem get worse
13. I’ve found myself thinking that I’m not up to the job 0.588
5. I have thought that I’m not doing things right with the person I’m 0.545
caring for
6. I have thought that, given the circumstances, I’m doing a good job as a −0.510 −0.330
caregiver
16. I have felt bad for leaving my relative in the care of someone else while 0.774
I had fun
15. I have felt bad about leaving my relative in the care of someone else 0.706
while I do my own things (e.g. work, shopping, going to the doctor)
7. When I’ve gone out to do some pleasant activity (e.g. eating out in a 0.684
restaurant), I’ve felt guilty and unable to stop thinking that I should be
caring for my relative
1. I have felt bad about having made some plan or done some activity 0.661
without taking my relative into account
3. I have felt bad for not looking after my other relatives (husband, wife, 0.873
children . . .) as I should, due to my caregiving
4. I have felt bad about not being able to devote more time to my family 0.844
(husband, wife, children . . .), due to my caregiving
18. I have felt like a bad person for hating and/or envying other relatives 0.833
who could have taken responsibility for some caring and do not do so
19. I have felt bad for having negative feelings (e.g. hate, anger or 0.766
resentment) toward some relatives
17. I have felt guilty about having wished that others “could have this 0.590
burden” or suffer as I do
Note: Numbers in bold in each column represent items clustered to the corresponding factor. Items <0.30 are not shown.

about neglecting other relatives factor; and 0.61 for were analyzed through t tests. The results of these
the guilt about having negative feelings towards other analyses are shown in Table 4. Female caregivers
people factor. scored significantly higher than male caregivers on
the factors guilt about neglecting other relatives (t =
−2.94; p < 0.01) and guilt about having negative
Associations with demographic variables thoughts towards other people (t = −2.15; p < 0.05).
Mean differences in the CGQ factors and total Female caregivers also obtained higher CGQ total
score across sex and kinship (spouses and children) scores (t = −2.16; p < 0.05). Significant differences
Caregiver Guilt Questionnaire 655

Table 4 Guilt mean scores (and standard deviations) across sex and kinship
G U I LT
G U I LT ABOUT
G U I LT ABOUT H AV I N G
ABOUT FA I L I N G TO N E G AT I V E
DOING MEET THE G U I LT A B O U T FEELINGS
WRONG BY CHALLENGES G U I LT NEGLECTING T OWA R D S
THE CARE OF ABOUT OTHER OTHER C G Q T O TA L
RECIPIENT CAREGIVING SELF-CARE R E L AT I V E S PEOPLE SCORE
.............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................

Sex
Female 8.83 (6.45) 5.14 (4.66) 1.98 (2.74) 1.06 (1.87) 1.64 (2.23) 18.69 (12.83)
Male 7. 51 (6.11) 4.55 (4.62) 1.24 (2.39) 0.50 (1.13) 1.10 (1.57) 14.86 (11.81)
Care recipient
Spouse 7.33 (6.27) 4.03 (4.07) 1.34 (2.50) 0.61 (1.27) 0.89 (1.46) 13.71 (10.95)
Parent 9.70 (6.38) 5.65 (5.01) 2.13 (2.79) 1.17 (2.02) 1.99 (2.42) 20.85 (10.08)
Other relative 7.56 (6.20) 4.96 (4.35) 1.81 (2.63) 0.89 (1.62) 1.35 (1.92) 14.40 (11.38)

Table 5. Correlations and intercorrelations between guilt and other variables


G U I LT G U I LT
G U I LT ABOUT ABOUT
ABOUT FA I L I N G G U I LT H AV I N G
DOING TO MEET ABOUT N E G AT I V E
WRONG THE CHAL- N E G L E C T- FEELINGS
BY THE L E N G E S O F G U I LT ING T OWA R D S CGQ G U I LT
CARE RE- C A R E G I V- ABOUT OTHER OTHER T O TA L (ZBI
CIPIENT ING SELF-CARE R E L AT I V E S PEOPLE SCORE FAC TO R )
.........................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................

Guilt (ZBI factor) 0.401∗∗ 0.352∗∗ 0.182∗ 0.136 0.177∗ 0.455∗∗ –


Frequency of 0.334∗∗ 0.331∗∗ 0.324∗∗ 0.102 0.230∗∗ 0.418∗∗ 0.338∗∗
behavioral problems
Appraisal of behavioral 0.456∗∗ 0.457∗∗ 0.228∗∗ 0.232∗∗ 0.158∗ 0.509∗∗ 0.337∗∗
problems
Functional status 0.091 0.149∗ −0.055 −0.090 −0.031 0.057 −0.070
Frequency of leisure −0.145∗ −0.113 −0.171∗∗ −0.075 −0.096 −0.192∗∗ −0.195∗
Satisfaction with −0.248∗∗ −0.165∗∗ −0.151∗ −0.097 −0.140∗ −0.286∗∗ −0.271∗∗
leisure
Social support −0.168∗∗ −0.157∗ −0.034 −0.073 −0.061 −0.188∗∗ −0.305∗∗
Depression 0.388∗∗ 0.364∗∗ 0.248∗∗ 0.212∗∗ 0.120∗ 0.462∗∗ 0.308∗∗
Anxiety 0.440∗∗ 0.368∗∗ 0.173∗∗ 0.177∗∗ 0.198∗∗ 0.462∗∗ 0.346∗∗
Guilt about doing – 0.559∗∗ 0.352∗∗ 0.218∗∗ 0.339∗∗ 0.854∗∗ 0.401∗∗
wrong by the care
recipient
Guilt about not rising – 0.364∗∗ 0.159∗∗ 0.255∗∗ 0.765∗∗ 0.352∗∗
to the occasion as
caregivers
Guilt about self-care – 0.210∗∗ 0.283∗∗ 0.585∗∗ 0.182∗
Guilt about neglecting – 0.142∗ 0.383∗∗ 0.136
other relatives
Guilt about having – 0.507∗∗ 0.177∗
negative feelings
towards other people
p < 0.05; ∗∗ p < 0.01

in CGQ score and all the factors were also found on about failing to meet the challenges of caregiving (F =
considering kinship. Spouses obtained lower CGQ 5.51; p < 0.01), guilt about self-care (F = 5.84; p <
total scores than those caring for a parent, and 0.01), guilt about neglecting other relatives (F = 3.02;
scored lower in all CGQ factors: guilt about doing p < 0.05) and guilt about having negative thoughts
wrong by the care recipient (F = 5.44; p < 0.01), guilt towards other people (F = 12.43; p < 0.01).
656 A. Losada et al.

Convergent and criterion validity structure. The factors were labeled guilt about doing
Correlations between CGQ factors and total score wrong by the care recipient, guilt about failing to meet
are shown in Table 5. Convergent validity was the challenges of caregiving, guilt about self-care, guilt
assessed through the correlation between CGQ about neglecting other relatives and guilt about having
factors and total score, and guilt as measured by negative feelings towards other people. A significant
the ZBI guilt factor (Ankri et al., 2005). As it can be proportion of variance of the guilt construct is
seen, significant correlations were found between explained by these factors, and reasonable internal
the ZBI guilt factor and CGQ factors and total consistency indices were found for all the factors
score, except in the case of the factor Guilt about and for the total questionnaire.
neglecting other relatives. No significant differences The convergent validity of the CGQ was
were found in guilt scores between those caregivers supported by the substantial correlations found
who completed the ZBI and those who did not between the CGQ (total score and some of its
except for the factor Guilt about neglecting other factors) and the ZBI guilt factor (Ankri et al., 2005),
relatives. Caregivers who completed the ZBI had especially with the first two factors, guilt about doing
higher scores (M = 1.15; SD = 1.01) in this factor wrong by the care recipient and guilt about failing to
than those who did not (M = 0.71; SD = 1.38) meet the challenges of caregiving. This result is not
(t = −2.21; p < 0.05). surprising if we consider that the content of the
Significant associations between CGQ factors items included in these two factors is similar to that
and total score and appraisal of problem behaviors, of the items included in the ZBI guilt factor: guilt
depression and anxiety were also found. Caregivers associated with a general sense of not being a good
with higher CGQ scores also scored higher on caregiver. The low, though significant, correlations
appraisals, depression and anxiety. Similar findings between the ZBI guilt factor and the CGQ factors
were obtained for frequency of behavioral problems, guilt about self-care and guilt about having negative
except that the association between this variable and feelings towards other people, on the one hand, and
the CGQ factor guilt about neglecting other relatives the non-significant association with the factor guilt
was not significant. Functional status was found about neglecting other relatives, on the other, suggest
to be significantly associated only with guilt about that the CGQ measures dimensions of guilt which
failing to meet the challenges of caregiving factor: are not tapped by the ZBI guilt factor. Furthermore,
caregivers who report higher functional status of the low to moderate intercorrelations between the
their care recipients also score higher scores on CGQ factors, together with the different patterns
this CGQ factor. Caregivers who are less satisfied of associations found between the CGQ factors
with their leisure time score higher on the CGQ and the other variables analyzed in the study,
and all its factors, except for the factor guilt about reveal that guilt may be better conceived as a
neglecting other relatives. Similar findings were found multidimensional and domain-specific experience.
on analyzing frequency of leisure activities: those However, this should not necessarily be taken
caregivers who report taking part in more leisure as an argument against the usefulness of a total
activities score lower on guilt about doing wrong CGQ score, which is supported by the interesting
by the care recipient, guilt about self-care and CGQ correlations found between this global score and the
total score. Caregivers with higher scores on social variables included in the study.
support present lower scores on guilt about doing In this regard, similar results to those found by
wrong by the care recipient, guilt about failing to meet Ankri et al. (2005) with the ZBI guilt factor were
the challenges of caregiving and CGQ total score. obtained. Caregivers looking after their parents had
Similar associations to those found with the CGQ higher guilt scores than spouses in CGQ total score
were found between the ZBI guilt factor and all and all its factors. This result could be explained by
the other variables assessed: all associations were the fact that adult children giving care, especially
significant, except that with functional status. daughters, may have more sources of guilt, as they
are more likely to experience conflict between their
multiple roles (caregiver, mother or father, worker,
Discussion etc.) and to perceive interference of their caregiving
role with work and family (Yee and Schulz, 2000).
The main aim of this study was to examine In this regard, adult children caregivers have been
the psychometric properties of a new scale found in some studies to admit potentially harmful
for measuring guilt among dementia caregivers, behaviors (e.g. verbal aggression, ignoring) towards
namely, the Caregiver Guilt Questionnaire (CGQ). their care recipients to a greater extent than spouse
Through principal component analysis, 22 items caregivers (Sasaki et al., 2007). Such behaviors are
were found to be represented by a five-factor likely to generate guilt in caregivers.
Caregiver Guilt Questionnaire 657

In this study, guilt has also been found to be of guilt in caregiving outcomes. Strong associations
significantly associated with behavioral problems in were found between guilt and important outcome
the care recipient. As Pagel et al. (1985) argue, variables such as depression or anxiety, suggesting
caregivers facing more behavioral problems may a significant relationship between guilt feelings and
make stronger attempts to control their relative’s caregiver distress. Similarly, Gallagher et al. (2008)
behaviors. Given the difficulty of controlling such found guilt, as measured with the ZBI guilt factor,
behaviors, their attempts may be frustrating and/or to be associated with greater anxiety in caregivers
unsuccessful, thus generating feelings of guilt. of children with intellectual disabilities. It is our
In a similar way to what has been found for other hypothesis that caregiver guilt contributes to the
distress-related variables in caregiving research (e.g. generation and exacerbation of depression and
Pinquart and Sörensen, 2003), female caregivers burden. However, the cross-sectional nature of this
scored significantly higher than male caregivers, study precludes us from drawing any conclusions
specifically in the factors guilt about neglecting other about causal links between guilt and other negative
relatives and guilt about having negative feelings emotional outcomes in caregiving.
towards other people. Caring for others is a more Another relevant finding of this study that
central element in their identity than it is for men, endorses the need to take guilt feelings into account
and female caregivers are usually more involved on analyzing caregiver distress are the significant
than male caregivers in the roles of assisting and associations between guilt and other variables
nurturing all family members, besides the care pertaining to key dimensions in theoretical models
recipient. Given that women usually perceive more such as the stress and coping model (e.g. Knight
responsibility for caring for all family members, they et al., 2000). For example, caregivers who report
are likely to be more vulnerable to guilt associated having guilt feelings about doing wrong by their care
with the perception that they are neglecting other recipient or failing to meet the challenges of care-
relatives due to caregiving. In fact, female caregivers giving report low scores on leisure (frequency and,
have been found to report more role conflict, more to a greater extent, satisfaction with leisure) and so-
caregiving costs and more interference with family cial support. Once again, the cross-sectional design
and leisure time than do men (Ingersoll-Dayton of the study does not permit us to make causal
and Raschick, 2004). The finding that female inferences in this regard, but it would be really
caregivers report more guilt than men about having interesting for future studies to analyze whether
negative feelings towards other people could be guilt feelings have any influence on the reduction of
explained by the fact that female caregivers report time devoted to leisure or of social support levels.
greater role strain than male caregivers (Ingersoll- Some other limitations of this study should
Dayton et al., 1996) and receive less formal and be acknowledged. With regard to random error,
informal support (Yee and Schulz, 2000). These although internal consistency of the scale (and
differences between male and female caregivers its factors) has been found to be reasonable, no
may be related to different coping styles, but test-retest assessment of guilt has been made, so
also to different internalized norms about caring data on the stability over time of CGQ scores are
and nurturing responsibilities, with women more not available. With regard to potential sources of
liable to assume caregiving as a moral (filial) systematic error, some of these can be addressed.
responsibility they have to bear, that is, as “the For example, regarding content validity, other
normal way” they should behave according to domains or experiences that may be sources of guilt
social and family expectations (Ingersoll-Dayton for caregivers (e.g. thoughts about placing the care
and Raschick, 2004). As suggested by Gonyea recipient in a nursing home) are not tapped by the
et al., (2008), role theory can provide interesting CGQ, even though they may influence caregiver
insights for understanding the emergence of guilt distress. Furthermore, as several empirical studies
feelings among adult children caregivers, especially have shown in recent years, there are substantial
women. These gender differences may explain why differences in caregivers’ experience of distress
female caregivers have been systematically found across cultures (e.g. Knight et al., 2000), and guilt
in the caregiving literature to report more burden, is considered to be a culturally sensitive emotion,
anxiety, depression and health problems than male strongly influenced by cultural perspectives and
caregivers (Pinquart and Sörensen, 2003; Yee and social norms (Bierbrauer, 1992). In order to test the
Schulz, 2000). These negative emotional outcomes cross-cultural content validity of the CGQ, it seems
may facilitate the generation of negative thoughts or necessary to apply this instrument in other cultures
feelings towards other people, such as irritability or so as to confirm the findings of the present work.
resentment. Finally, the convenience-based nature of the sample
The results of this study are consistent with limits its representativeness and, in consequence,
those of previous studies suggesting the relevance the generalizability of the results.
658 A. Losada et al.

As has been already noted, the construct of other chronic conditions (e.g. terminal illnesses
guilt has not yet been adequately integrated into such as cancer).
empirically based models of personality and/or In conclusion, the CGQ presents acceptable
emotion (Kugler and Jones, 1992). Currently, solid psychometric properties, and may contribute to
theoretical accounts of this emotional experience the development of sensitive and conceptually valid
are still lacking, especially in caregiving research, outcome measures for psychosocial intervention
where this variable has started to be analyzed only research (Moniz-Cook et al., 2008). It is our hope
very recently. Hence, there is a clear need for that future studies using this instrument in our
empirical studies, such as that presented here, which context and in other countries will permit further
may contribute to improve our understanding of the analysis of its utility for getting closer to our shared
psychological and behavioral implications of guilt, objective of improving our ability to help caregivers.
and the dynamic of its relationships with caregivers
distress and coping behaviors. These studies may
eventually help us to develop more comprehensive Conflict of interest
caregiving models which include this variable and
None.
thus describe more accurately caregivers’ subjective
experiences. The main contributions of the present
study to the literature on guilt may be summarized Description of authors’ roles
as follows: it provides evidence on (a) the multi-
dimensional and domain-specific nature of guilt; Andrés Losada and Marı́a Márquez-González both
(b) the association between guilt and caregivers’ worked on the design of the study and its imple-
distress; and (c) potential behavioral implications mentation, the development of the CGQ items, the
of guilt (frequency of and satisfaction with leisure), data analysis and the writing of the paper. Cecilia
which may mediate its relationship with distress. Peñacoba collaborated on the development of the
It is clear that there are many questions regarding CGQ items. Rosa Romero-Moreno worked on the
guilt which deserve further attention, such as the study implementation and the writing of the paper.
relationship between guilt and caregivers’ values
(e.g. familism), moral or religious beliefs, or
dysfunctional thoughts about caregiving. These Acknowledgments
questions could be explored in future studies which
The authors would like to thank Nancy Pachana,
would provide important insights into the nature of
Ph.D., from the University of Queensland, for her
guilt and its role in the caregiving stress process.
help with the CGQ back-translation. This study
The practical implications of our findings are
was partially funded by grants from the Spanish
evident: they indicate the importance of addressing
Ministry of Education (SEJ2006–02489/PSIC)
guilt feelings in interventions and helping caregivers
and from the Spanish Ministry of Science and
to acknowledge and manage them, as a way of
Innovation (PSI2009-08132/PSIC).
reducing their distress and of promoting more
adaptive ways of coping with caregiving. In this
regard, helping caregivers to review and adjust
their expectations and standards in relation to their
References
obligations and capacities, to accept their limitations Ankri, J., Andrieu, S., Beaufils, B., Grand, A. and
and weaknesses, and to acknowledge their needs, Henrad, J. C. (2005). Beyond the global score of the Zarit
might facilitate a reduction in their guilt feelings Burden Interview: useful dimensions for clinicians.
which, in turn, could lead to the development of International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 20, 254–260.
more adaptive behaviours, such as increasing their Bierbrauer, G. (1992). Reactions to violation of normative
leisure activities or seeking social and instrumental standards: a cross-cultural analysis of shame and guilt.
support, which constitute important targets in International Journal of Psychology, 27, 181–193.
cognitive-behavioral interventions (e.g. Coon et al., Boye, B., Bentsen, H. and Malt, U. F. (2002). Does guilt
2003). Even when this study has focused on proneness predict acute and long-term distress in relatives
the measurement of guilt in dementia caregivers, of patients with schizophrenia? Acta Psychiatrica
Scandinavica, 106, 51–57.
this emotional phenomenon is considered to be
Brodaty, H. (2007). Meaning and measurement or caregiver
a key experience in many different caregiving outcomes. International Psychogeriatrics, 19, 363–381.
experiences, and it has been found to be associated Coon, D. W., Thompson, L., Steffen, A., Sorocco, K. and
with psychosocial maladjustment in caregivers (e.g. Gallagher-Thompson, D. (2003). Anger and depression
Spillers et al., 2008). In this sense, it might be management: psychoeducational skill training interventions
interesting to explore the usefulness of the CGQ to for women caregivers of a relative with dementia. The
assess guilt in caregivers of people suffering from Gerontologist, 43, 678–689.
Caregiver Guilt Questionnaire 659

Cooper, C., Katona, C., Orrell, M. and Livingston, G. Reig, A., Ribera, D. and Miguel, J. (1991). Psychological
(2008). Coping strategies, anxiety and depression in support and daily stress in non-institutionalized elderly.
caregivers of people with Alzheimer’s disease. International European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 7, 191–200.
Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 23, 929–936. Sasaki, M., Arai, Y., Kumamoto, K., Abe, K., Arai, A.
Gallagher, S., Phillips, A. C., Oliver, C. and Carroll, D. and Mizuno, Y. (2007). Factors related to potentially
(2008). Predictors of psychological morbidity in parents of harmful behaviors towards disabled older people by family
children with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Pediatric caregivers in Japan. International Journal of Geriatric
Psychology, 33, 1129–1136; Psychiatry, 22, 250–257.
Ghatavi, K., Nicolson, R., MacDonald, C., Osher, S. and Spillers, R. L., Wellisch, D. K., Kim, Y., Matthews, B. A.
Levitt, A. (2002). Defining guilt in depression: a and Baker, F. (2008). Family caregivers and guilt in the
comparison of subjects with major depression, chronic context of cancer care. Psychosomatics, 49, 511–519.
medical illness and healthy controls. Journal of Affective Stevens, A. B. et al. (2004). Measurement of leisure time
Disorders, 68, 307–315. satisfaction in family caregivers. Aging and Mental Health,
Gonyea, J. G., Paris, R. and Zerden, L. (2008). Adult 8, 450–459.
daughters and aging mothers: the role of guilt in the Teri, L., Truax, P., Logsdon, R., Uomoto, J., Zarit, S.
experience of caregiver burden. Aging and Mental Health, and Vitaliano, P. P. (1992). Assessment of behavioral
12, 559–567. problems in dementia: the revised memory and behavior
Hamel, G. (1995). Guilt: a hidden problem in caregiving. problems checklist. Psychology and Aging, 7, 622–631.
Provider, 21, 59–60. Yaffe, M. J. (1988). Implications of caring for an aging parent.
Ingersoll-Dayton, B. and Raschick, M. (2004). The Canadian Medical Association Journal, 138, 231–235.
relationship between care-recipient behaviors and spousal Yee, J. L. and Schulz, R. (2000). Gender differences in
caregiving stress. The Gerontologist, 44, 318–327. psychiatric morbidity among family caregivers: a review
Ingersoll-Dayton, B., Starrels, M. E. and Dowler, D. and analysis. The Gerontologist, 40, 147–164.
(1996). Caregiving for parents and parents-in-law: is Zarit, S. H., Reever, K. E. and Bach-Peterson, J. (1980).
gender important?. The Gerontologist, 36, 483–491. Relatives of the impaired elderly: correlates of feelings of
Knight, B. G., Silverstein, M., McCallum, T. J. and Fox, burden. The Gerontologist, 20, 649–655.
L. S. (2000). A sociocultural stress and coping model for
mental health outcomes among African American
caregivers in southern California. Journal of Gerontology:
Psychological Sciences, 55, 142–150. Appendix: Caregiver Guilt Questionnaire (CGQ)
Kugler, K. and Jones, W. H. (1992). On conceptualizing and
Below is a list of some feelings and thoughts.
assessing guilt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
62, 318–327.
Please tell us how often you have had these feelings
Mahoney, F. L. and Barthel, D. W. (1965). Functional and thoughts during the past few weeks using the
evaluation: the Barthel Index. State Journal, 4, 61–65. following options:
Martin, Y., Gilbert, P., McEwan, K. and Irons, C. (2006). 0 = never; 1 = rarely; 2 = sometimes; 3 = several
The relation of entrapment, shame and guilt to depression, times; 4 = always or almost always
in carers of people with dementia. Aging and Mental Health,
10, 101–106. 1. I have felt bad about having made some plan or
McNair, D., Lorr, M. and Droppleman, L. (1971). Profile done some activity without taking my relative into
of Mood States. Manual. San Diego, CA: Educational and account [Me he sentido mal por haber hecho algún
Industrial Testing Service. plan o actividad sin contar con mi familiar].
Moniz-Cook, E. et al. and the INTERDEM Group 2. I have felt guilty about the way I’ve sometimes
(2008). A European consensus on outcome measures for behaved with my relative [Me he sentido culpable
psychosocial intervention research in dementia care. Aging por la forma en que me he comportado en ocasiones
and Mental Health, 12, 14–29. con mi familiar].
Pagel, M. D., Becker, J. and Coppel, D. B. (1985). Loss of 3. I have felt bad for not looking after my other relatives
control, self-blame, and depression: an investigation of (husband, wife, children . . .) as I should, due to my
spouse caregivers of Alzheimer’s disease patients. Journal of caregiving [Me he sentido mal por no atender a
Abnormal Psychology, 94, 169–182. mis otros familiares (marido, mujer, hijos. . .) como
Pinquart, M. and Sörensen, S. (2003). Differences between deberı́a, debido al cuidado].
caregivers and noncarevigers in psychological health and 4. I have felt bad about not being able to devote more
physical health: a meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 18, time to my family (husband, wife, children. . .), due
250–267. to my caregiving [Me he sentido mal por no poder
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D scale: a self-report dedicar más tiempo a mi familia (marido, mujer,
depression scale for research in the general population. hijos . . .) como deberı́a, debido al cuidado].
Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401. 5. I have thought that I’m not doing things right with
Radloff, L. S. and Teri, L. (1986). Use of the Center for the person I’m caring for [He pensado que no estoy
Epidemiological Studies-Depression Scale with older haciendo las cosas bien con la persona a la que estoy
adults. In T. L. Brink (ed.), Clinical Gerontology: A Guide to cuidando].
Assessment and Intervention (pp. 119–136). New York: 6. I have thought that, given the circumstances, I’m
Haworth Press. doing a good job as a caregiver [He pensado
660 A. Losada et al.

que, teniendo en cuenta las circunstancias, estoy mientras yo hacı́a mis tareas (p.ej., trabajo, compra,
haciendo bien mi tarea como cuidador/a]. ir al médico, etc.)].
7. When I’ve gone out to do some pleasant activity (e.g. 16. I have felt bad for leaving my relative in the care of
eating out in a restaurant), I’ve felt guilty and unable someone else while I had fun [Me he sentido mal
to stop thinking that I should be caring for my por dejar a mi familiar al cuidado de otra persona
relative [Cuando he salido a hacer alguna actividad mientras yo me divertı́a].
agradable (p.ej., ir a cenar a un restaurante), me 17. I have felt guilty about having wished that others
he sentido culpable y no he dejado de pensar “could have this burden” or suffer as I do [Me he
que deberı́a estar cuidando o atendiendo a mi sentido culpable por desear que a otros “les toque
familiar]. esta cruz” o sufran lo mismo que yo].
8. I have felt bad about things I may have done wrong 18. I have felt like a bad person for hating and/or
with the person I’m caring for [Me he sentido mal envying other relatives who could have taken
por cosas que quizá habı́a hecho mal con la persona responsibility for some caring and do not do so [Me
a la que estoy cuidando]. he sentido mala persona por odiar y/o envidiar a
9. I have thought that perhaps I’m not caring well otros familiares que podrı́an hacerse responsables
for my relative [He pensado que quizá no estoy parcialmente del cuidado y no lo hacen].
cuidando bien]. 19. I have felt bad for having negative feelings (e.g., hate,
10. I have felt bad about getting angry with the person anger or resentment) toward some relatives [Me he
I’m caring for [Me he sentido mal por haberme sentido mal por tener sentimientos negativos (p.ej.,
enfadado con la persona a la que estoy cuidando]. odio, enfado o rencor) hacia algunos familiares].
11. I have felt bad about telling off the person I’m caring 20. I have felt guilty about having so many negative
for, for some reason [Me he sentido mal por haber emotions in relation to caring [Me he sentido
regañado por alguna razón a la persona a la que culpable por el hecho de tener tantas emociones
estoy cuidando]. negativas en relación al cuidado].
12. I’ve got angry with myself for having negative 21. I have thought that the way I care for my relative may
feelings toward the person I’m caring for [Me he not be appropriate and may make his/her problem
enfadado conmigo mismo/a por tener sentimientos get worse [He pensado que la forma en la que cuido
negativos hacia la persona a la que cuido]. de mi familiar podrı́a no ser adecuada y contribuir
13. I’ve found myself thinking that I’m not up to the a que su problema vaya a peor].
job [Me he encontrado pensando que no estoy a la 22. I have felt guilty thinking that my lack of information
altura de las circunstancias]. and preparedness might mean that I’m not handling
14. I have felt bad about not having more patience with the care of my relative in the best way possible
the person I’m caring for [Me he sentido mal por [Me he sentido culpable al pensar que mi falta
no tener más paciencia con la persona a la que estoy de información y preparación podrı́a hacer que no
cuidando]. estuviera manejando el cuidado de mi familiar de la
15. I have felt bad about leaving my relative in the care mejor forma possible].
of someone else while I do my own things (e.g. work,
shopping, going to the doctor) [Me he sentido mal Note: The Spanish items are shown in square
por dejar a mi familiar al cuidado de otra persona brackets.

You might also like