You are on page 1of 7

DESC_013.

fm Page 55 Tuesday, January 28, 2003 11:46 AM

Developmental Science 6:1 (2003), pp 55–61

REPORT
Blackwell Science, Ltd

From hand to mouth in the evolution of language: the influence


Laterality in gestural and vocal communication

of vocal behavior on lateralized hand use in manual gestures by


chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes)
William D. Hopkins1,2 and Monica Cantero3
1. Division of Psychobiology, Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Atlanta, Georgia, USA
2. Department of Psychology, Berry College, Mount Berry, Georgia, USA
3. Department of Foreign Languages, Berry College, Mount Berry, Georgia, USA

Abstract
Studies in human subjects indicate that manual gestures accompanied by speech are produced more often by the right compared
to the left hand. Additional studies indicate that the production of sign language is controlled by the same brain areas as speech,
suggesting similar neurobiological substrates for language that are not modality specific. We report evidence that chimpanzees
exhibit preferential use of the right hand in gestural communication. Moreover, use of the right hand in gestural communication
is significantly enhanced when accompanied by a vocalization, particularly among human-reared chimpanzees. Taken together, the
data suggest that the lateralization of manual and speech systems of communication may date back as far as 5 million years ago.

Introduction that there are significantly more right- compared to left-


hand gestures while subjects verbally recall memory list
Clinical, experimental and developmental data all sug- items or describe narrative text (Foundas, Macauley,
gest that motor systems involved in the control of man- Raymer, Maher, Heilman & Rothi, 1995; Kimura, 1973).
ual and oral movements are lateralized in the left These data have been interpreted as evidence that the pro-
hemisphere (Kimura, 1993; Iverson & Thelen, 1999). In duction of speech activates motor areas within the left
humans, it has been well established that language com- hemisphere that results in preferential use of the right
prehension and production are lateralized in distinct hand in simultaneous gesticulation. Finally, develop-
regions within the left cerebral hemisphere (Corballis, mental studies indicate that as infants grow older there
1992). Although initial clinical and experimental studies is a progression toward increased preferential use of the
focused on speech comprehension and production, right hand in gestural communication (Blake, O’Rourke
recent findings in deaf populations who have learned to & Borzellino, 1994). Moreover, in infants, increased
use non-speech communication systems suggest that right-hand use in gestures is associated with increased
similar areas within the left hemisphere are involved in use of speech or vocalizations (Dalby, Gibson, Grossi &
comprehending and producing signs (Corina, Vaid & Schneider, 1980; Locke, Bekken, McMinn-Larson & Wein,
Bellugi, 1992; Grossi, Semenza, Corazza & Volterra, 1995).
1996; Kimura, 1993). These results suggest that func- Whether homologous asymmetries are present in the
tional asymmetries in communicative systems are not processing of communicative behavior by nonhumans
modality specific and that similar neurobiological sys- remains a topic of considerable debate, particularly as it
tems mediate manual and spoken languages. pertains to discussions of the neurobiology of language
Further evidence suggesting an association between origins (see Steklis & Raleigh, 1979a, 1979b; Bradshaw
left hemisphere dominance in manual and spoken com- & Rogers, 1993 for review). There are numerous reports
munication comes from the reports of manual asym- of left hemisphere specialization in the processing of
metries in mimetic gestures produced while subjects are species-specific sounds as well as other classes of acoustic
engaged in speech. For example, it has been reported stimuli (see Hopkins & Fernandez-Carriba, 2002 for

Address for correspondence: William D. Hopkins, Division of Psychobiology, Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Emory University, Atlanta,
Georgia, 30322, USA; e-mail: lrcbh@rmy.emory.edu or whopkins@berry.edu

© Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2003, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX 4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA.
DESC_013.fm Page 56 Tuesday, January 28, 2003 11:46 AM

56 William D. Hopkins and Monica Cantero

review). Far fewer studies have examined asymmetries in & Leavens, 1998). Unfortunately, in the Hopkins and
the production of communicative signals. Left hemi- Leavens (1998) study, it is not clear whether the associ-
sphere asymmetries in the production of vocalizations ation between right handed gesturing and vocal beha-
have been reported in some non-mammalian species vior exists at a population level nor is it clear whether
such as birds and frogs (Bisazza, Rogers & Vallortigara, the effect is due to the referential or affective expression
1998; Bradshaw & Rogers, 1993); however, in primates, of the vocal behavior. Based on the existing literature
the evidence suggests right hemisphere asymmetries on lateralization in the production of vocalization in
in the production of vocalizations, particularly those nonhuman primates, two hypotheses can be proposed
with negative emotional valence (Hauser, 1993; Hook- about the relationship between vocal and gestural com-
Costigan & Rogers, 1998). These findings are consistent munication in chimpanzees. Specifically, if the produc-
with the general view that the primary function of prim- tion of vocalizations is mediated by the right hemisphere
ate vocalizations is in communicating affective rather (e.g. Hauser, 1993) then subjects that vocalize while ges-
than semantic or other linguistic information (but see turing should show a higher incidence of left- compared
Seyfarth & Cheney, 1997). In humans, the right hemi- to right-handed gesturing. In contrast, if the production
sphere plays a role in producing non-linguistic auditory of vocalizations is mediated by the left hemisphere, then
information while the left hemisphere is responsible right-handed manual gestures should be more prevalent
for speech and language production associated with when subjects are simultaneously vocalizing compared
semantics and grammatical information. to when they are not. Lastly, hand use for gesture could
In contrast to the functions of vocalizations in non- be independent of vocalization; therefore, no association
human primates, recent studies indicate that great apes between hand use for gestures and vocalization should
use manual gestures to point to otherwise unattainable be evident.
things and they alternate their gaze between the referent
and communicative recipient (Call & Tomasello, 1994;
Krause & Fouts, 1997; Leavens & Hopkins, 1998). More Methods
recent findings indicate that chimpanzees modify their
communicative behavior in accordance with the atten-
Subjects
tional state of the audience such that vocal and other
auditory signals are used to capture the attention of an The sample consisted of 73 chimpanzees housed at the
inattentive recipient. In contrast, when the audience is Yerkes National Primate Research Center (YNPRC)
attentive to the communicator, chimpanzees and other ranging in age from 7 to 44 years. There were 38 females
great apes will more frequently use manual gestures and 35 males, respectively. Within the female sample there
rather than auditory signals to communicate to the were 13 human-reared and 25 mother-reared subjects. In
recipient (Hostetter, Cantero & Hopkins, 2001; Toma- the male sample there were 23 human-reared and 12 mother-
sello, Call, Nagell, Olguin & Carpenter, 1994; but see reared chimpanzees. Mother-reared subjects were chim-
Theall & Povinelli, 1999). Although more research is panzees that were raised by their biological, conspecific
needed to characterize the different factors that influ- mother beyond 30 days of life. Human-reared subjects
ence communicative behavior in great apes, some studies were chimpanzees that were either rejected by their bio-
indicate that chimpanzees and other great apes produce logical mother or removed from their biological mother
manual gestures, and to a lesser extent vocalizations for experimental purposes and raised by humans. Human-
and other acoustic signals, that may be both intentional reared chimpanzees are raised in standard nursery-
and referential, notable prerequisites for the evolution of setting with other neonatal and infant chimpanzees until
language and speech in humans. Based on these findings, 3 years of age at which time they are integrated into multi-
the potential for antecedents to the evolution of left age, multi-sex chimpanzee social groups (see Bard, 1996
hemisphere lateralization in language and speech pro- for summary of rearing practices of human-raised chim-
duction in humans may lie in the expression of prefer- panzees at the YNPRC).
ential hand use for manual gestures in great apes, notably
chimpanzees.
Procedure
The purpose of the current study was to further exam-
ine the association between hand use for gestural com- The testing procedure was relatively straightforward and
munication and vocal behavior in chimpanzees. This was was similar to that used in previous studies on gestural
prompted by observations that right-hand use in gestural communication in this colony of chimpanzees (see Hop-
communication is found to be significantly higher when kins & Wesley, 2002; Leavens & Hopkins, 1998, 1999 for
the gestures are accompanied by a vocalization (Hopkins reviews). On each test trial, an experimenter approached

© Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2003


DESC_013.fm Page 57 Tuesday, January 28, 2003 11:46 AM

Laterality in gestural and vocal communication 57

the subject’s home cage and positioned themselves approx- rearing. At the end of each test trial, the focal subject
imately 1 m from the subject’s home cage and directly in was given the banana no matter what behaviors they
front of the focal chimpanzee. Every effort was made to exhibited during the 1-min test session.
ensure that the experimenters were positioned centrally Each chimpanzee was tested on 10 separate occasions
in front of the subjects so as to preclude any effects of separated by at least one day. The 10 test sessions were
positional bias on hand use. Moreover, the experimenter administered over a 6-month period between July and
approached the subject’s home cage from different direc- December 2000. Individual subjects were tested in their
tions across test trials to control for this potential extrane- home cages. Nearly all of the subjects lived in social
ous variable. After approaching, the experimenter offered groups ranging from 2 to 16 individuals. Thus, focal
the chimpanzee a banana that was previously out of sampling techniques were used with one subject from
sight from the chimpanzee. Prior to offering the banana, each cage serving as a subject on a given trial. Inter-rater
the banana had been hidden inside the laboratory coat reliability in coding the hand use and frequency of
pocket worn by the experimenter. Because the experi- vocalizations was established in 10% of the sample of
menter was approximately 1 m from the focal subject, subjects. Cohen’s kappa values were 0.994 and 0.850
the banana was out of their immediate reach and this when coding for hand use and the number of vocaliza-
condition stimulated the production of communicative tions produced. Both of these values are considered high
behaviors by the focal chimpanzee. The experimenter and indicative of good reliability.
held onto the banana throughout the entire duration of
the trial and did not move from the initial point from
Data analysis
which they offered the banana. The experimenter also
maintained eye contact with the subject throughout the The frequency data were summed across the 10 test sessions
duration of the trial in order to increase the probability and several different means of characterizing the data
of engaging the subjects to produce a communicative were used in this study. First, general descriptive ana-
behavior. lyses were performed on the overall communicative beha-
A second experimenter, standing directly behind the viors, irrespective of hand use. Three variables were
experimenter offering the banana, recorded on a sheet of derived including the total number of gestures (Total),
paper all the communicative behavior of the focal subject total number of gestures accompanied by a vocalization
for a 1-minute sampling period. The communicative beha- (Sum + Vocal) and the total number of gestures produced
vior was sampled for 1 minute in order to increase the without a vocalization (Sum + No-Vocal). In terms of
probability that the chimpanzees would be observed to laterality, handedness indices were derived by subtract-
gesture while either vocalizing or not. The second experi- ing the number of left-hand responses from the number
menter recorded all instances of either a gesture accom- of right-hand responses and dividing by the total number
panied by a vocalization or a gesture not accompanied of responses HI = [(R − L)/(R + L)]. A total handedness
by a vocalization. The second experimenter also made index was derived for all gestures (Sum-HI). In addition,
note of the hand used to gesture by the subject. A gesture handedness indices were derived for gestures that were
accompanied by a vocalization was only recorded when accompanied by a vocalization (Gesture + Vocal) and
the two events occurred simultaneously or within a 2-sec for those that were not (Gesture + No-Vocal). All ana-
interval. Moreover, only a single response was recorded lyses were done using parametric statistics with alpha set
although, in principle, more than one vocalization could to p < 0.05. Post-hoc analyses were performed using
be produced by a single gestural event. For example, if the Tukey’s HSD ( p < 0.05) unless otherwise noted.
focal chimpanzee gestured and then produced multiple
vocalizations in a sequential manner, then only a single
gesture + vocal event was recorded. However, if the chim- Results
panzee stopped gesturing by pulling their hand away from
the cage mesh and then initiated another gesture, another
Descriptive statistics
event was recorded. No attempt was made to characterize
the vocalizations in terms of their structure using spec- The chimpanzees produced a total of 2,468 gestures
trographic analysis. However, none of the vocalizations including 1,934 that were not accompanied by a vocaliza-
would be considered species-specific such as food calls or tion and 532 that were accompanied by a vocalization.
grunts and not all of the subjects were observed to vocalize. The mean number of gestures produced with and without
Rather, among the subjects that did vocalize, the calls a vocalization as a function of sex and rearing history
varied from individual to individual and appeared to be can be seen in Table 1. Overall, there were no significant
idiosyncratic sounds acquired in response to their captive sex or rearing differences in the total number of gestures

© Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2003


DESC_013.fm Page 58 Tuesday, January 28, 2003 11:46 AM

58 William D. Hopkins and Monica Cantero

Table 1 Mean number of gestures produced with and without


a vocalization

Variable Sum + Vocal Sum + No-Vocal

Sex
Males 8.01 24.09
Females 6.30 29.55
Rearing history
Human-Reared 3.22 28.24
Mother-Reared 11.09 25.40

produced by the chimpanzees. The total number of gestures


Figure 1 The mean HI values for the overall number of
produced with and without a vocalization was also com- gestures (Sum-HI), number of gestures produced with a
pared using a mixed design analysis of variance. Sex and vocalization (Gesture + Vocal) and the number of gestures
rearing history served as between-group variables while produced without a vocalization (Gesture + No-Vocal). All
Sum + Vocal and Sum + No-Vocal served as repeated were significantly different from zero, even after correcting for
measures. This analysis revealed a significant interaction Type I error using Bonferonni’s correction procedure. In
between communicative response and rearing history addition, the HI values for the Gesture + Vocal responses were
F(1, 69) = 4.79, p < 0.01. Post-hoc analysis indicated that significantly higher than the HI values produced for the
mother-reared chimpanzees produced more Sum + Vocal Gesture − Vocal responses, as revealed by a paired samples
responses compared to human-reared subjects. No sig- t-test.
nificant differences between mother- and human-reared
chimpanzees were found for the Sum + No-Vocal measure.
In short, mother-reared produced more gestures that accompanied by a vocalization (Gesture + No-Vocal)
were accompanied by a vocalization than human-reared were analyzed using two separate 2 (sex) × 2 (rearing
chimpanzees. history) analyses of covariance (ANCOVA). The cov-
ariate was the Sum + Vocal and Sum + No-Vocal values
from the initial analysis. ANCOVA was used to statist-
Laterality effects
ically control for differences in the total number of
One-sample t-tests were performed on the handedness responses produced by mother- and human-reared chim-
scores to assess population-level biases in hand use. panzees and its potential influence on the handedness
For the Sum-HI t(72) = 3.92, p < 0.001, Gesture + Vocal index values. For the Gesture + Vocal data, significant
t(50) = 4.53, p < 0.001 and Gesture + No-Vocal t(72) = main effects for sex F(1, 46) = 5.58, p < 0.03 and rearing
2.92, p < 0.01 measures, significant right-hand popula- history F(1, 46) = 6.23, p < 0.02 were found. Females
tion biases were found. In the next analysis, whether the (Mean = 0.71) had higher HI values then males (Mean =
magnitude in right-hand use differed within subjects that 0.21) and human-reared chimpanzees (Mean = 0.74)
gestured while vocalizing or not was assessed. Within the had higher HI values than mother-reared chimpanzees
entire sample of chimpanzees, 51 subjects produced ges- (Mean = 0.19). For the Gesture + No-Vocal responses,
tures both with and without vocalizations. This allowed no significant main effects for rearing history or sex were
for a within-subject comparison of the influence of vocal found.
behavior on the expression of hand preference in gestural
communication. For this analysis, the Gesture + Vocal and
Comparisons with other measures of hand use
Gesture + No-Vocal handedness scores were compared
using a paired samples t-test. The analysis revealed a The results of the previous analyses on the Gesture +
significant difference with the Gesture + Vocal handed- Vocal and Gesture + No-Vocal measures indicated that
ness scores significantly higher than the Gesture + human-reared chimpanzees were more right-handed
No-Vocal handedness scores t(50) = 3.51, p < 0.01 (see than mother-reared individuals. What is not clear is
Figure 1). whether this effect generalizes to other motor tasks or is
specific to gestural communication. Specifically, it could
be argued that these effects are due to the fact that the
Sex and rearing effects of lateralized hand use
chimpanzees are being reared in a human designed,
For this analysis, the handedness index values for the right-handed world (see Collins, 1985). If this explana-
responses accompanied by (Gesture + Vocal) or not tion accounts for the observed findings then the rearing

© Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2003


DESC_013.fm Page 59 Tuesday, January 28, 2003 11:46 AM

Laterality in gestural and vocal communication 59

differences should be evident for measures of hand use pushed the tube back out of their home cage through
that are not communicative in function. In contrast, if the cage mesh.
the rearing effect is specific to gestures accompanied by For all measures, a minimum of 25 responses was
a vocalization then the importance of human rearing on collected in each subject. As with the gesture data, hand-
the development of laterality would be limited to the edness indices were derived following the procedure
expression of communicative behavior. described above. Positive values reflected right-hand
To test this hypothesis, the handedness scores for biases and negative values reflected left-hand biases. The
human- and mother-reared chimpanzees were com- absolute value reflected the strength in hand preference.
pared for three other measures of hand preference includ- Separate 2 (sex) × 2 (rearing history) ANOVAs for each
ing simple reaching (REACH), coordinated bimanual handedness measure failed to reveal any significant
actions (TUBE) and bimanual feeding (FEED). The main effects or interactions. Thus, the effect of human
behavioral tasks used to assess hand preferences in the rearing on lateralized hand use is specific to gestural
chimpanzees have been described in detail elsewhere communication.
(Hopkins, 1993, 1994, 1995a, 1995b). Listed below is a
brief description of each task and the general procedure
used to assess hand preference. Discussion

The findings from this study indicate that preferential


Bimanual feeding (FEED)
use of the right hand for gestures is significantly enhanced
Each afternoon, the primates housed at the YRPRC when the gestures are accompanied by a vocalization,
receive fruits and vegetables as part of their daily diet. particularly among chimpanzees raised by humans. This
Each subject usually receives 2 oranges, 1 banana, some effect cannot be attributed to an overall influence of
celery stalks and/or carrots. Once retrieving the food, the human rearing on hand preference because differences
subjects typically move to a seating place and consume in handedness were not found for other measures of
the food. The chimpanzees typically hold the extra hand use. Moreover, hand use for gestures did not posit-
pieces of food with one hand and feed with the opposite ively correlate with any other measures of hand use.
hand. Hand use was recorded when the subjects were Based on previous studies examining the association
feeding with one hand for a minimum duration of 3 seconds between vocal production and manual gesticulation in
and the nonfeeding hand was holding the remaining humans, the evidence of increased right-hand use in ges-
portions of food. The dominant hand was recorded as tures that are accompanied by a vocalization suggest
the one feeding. that the left hemisphere is controlling both the manual
and vocal communication systems by chimpanzees. If
there was bilateral or right hemisphere control of the
Simple reaching (REACH)
vocalizations then right-hand gestures should have been
For this measure, a piece of food (a peanut or raisin) was inhibited or reduced in the chimpanzees. We believe this
randomly thrown into the subject’s cage. The subjects is the first evidence of asymmetries in the production of
had to locomote to the location and pick up the food. intentional, referential communicative signals by nonhu-
The hand used to pick up the food was recorded. A trial man primates.
was counted only when the subjects maintained a tri- It could be argued that the differences in handedness
pedal posture while reaching (i.e. one upper limb and scores between mother- and human-reared chimpanzees
two hind limbs were on the floor). are due to a sampling bias in total hand use because the
mother-reared chimpanzees produced significantly more
gestures accompanied by a vocalization than human-
Coordinated bimanual tube task (TUBE)
reared chimpanzees. Although this explanation cannot
Poly-vinyl-chloride tubes (24 to 31 cm long, 2.5 cm wide) be ruled out, we believe it is unlikely for three reasons.
with peanut butter smeared on the inside were given to First, the total number of overall responses does not
the subjects in their home cage. The digit and hand used correlate with the handedness scores derived for gestures
to remove the peanut butter was recorded each time the produced with (r = −0.026, df = 49, n.s.) or without a
subjects inserted their finger, removed peanut butter vocalization (r = 0.087, df = 71, n.s.). If sampling bias
from the tube and placed their finger in their mouth. explained the results then positive correlations should
Observations continued until the subjects stop showing have been evident. Second, the ANCOVA did not reveal
interest in the tube (usually when they have eaten all the a significant influence of the covariate (total number of
peanut butter), dropped it for at least 10 seconds, or responses) on the handedness scores. Third, the mean

© Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2003


DESC_013.fm Page 60 Tuesday, January 28, 2003 11:46 AM

60 William D. Hopkins and Monica Cantero

handedness scores for gestures produced with (Mean = Acknowledgements


0.460; Adjusted Mean = 0.461) and without a vocaliza-
tion (Mean = 0.186; Adjusted Mean = 0.185) were nearly This research was supported by NIH grants NS-29574,
identical whether or not the total number of responses NS-36605 and RR-00165 to the Yerkes Regional Primate
was used to adjust the handedness scores. Research Center. We thank Michael J. Wesley and
What is unclear from this study is why the human- Autumn Hostetter for assistance in data collection. The
reared chimpanzees were more right handed than the Yerkes Center is fully accredited by the American Asso-
mother-reared chimpanzees when gestures were accom- ciation for Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care.
panied by a vocalization. One explanation may be that APA guidelines for the ethical treatment of animals were
the human-reared chimpanzees use their vocalizations in adhered to during all aspects of this study.
conjunction with their gestures for different functions
compared to mother-reared chimpanzees. In some previ-
ous research with many of the same chimpanzees as those References
used in this study, it has been found that human-reared
chimpanzees were more likely to exhibit gaze alternation Bard, K.A. (1996). Responsive care: Behavioral intervention
between a referent and human experimenter than mother- for nursery-reared chimpanzees. (Available from the Jane
reared chimpanzees (Leavens & Hopkins, 1998). It may be Goodall Institute, Ridgefield, CT, 06877.)
that the human-reared chimpanzees are using their vocal- Bisazza, A., Rogers, L.J., & Vallortigara, G. (1998). The
izations in a functionally distinct way that has an influence origins of cerebral asymmetry: a review of evidence of
on their hand use. Specifically, the human-reared chimpan- behavioral and brain lateralization in fishes, reptiles, and
zees may be attempting to use their vocalizations in a amphibians. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 22,
referential manner while the mother-reared chimpanzees 411–426.
are using their vocalizations to communicate affective. Blake, J., O’Rourke, P., & Borzellino, G. (1994). Form and
The referential use of the vocalizations by the human- function in the development of pointing and reaching ges-
tures. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 195–203.
reared chimpanzees may induce left hemisphere activa-
Bradshaw, J.L., & Rogers, L. (1993). The evolution of lateral
tion, which results in greater use of the right hand for asymmetries, language, tool-use and intellect. San Diego: Aca-
gestures accompanying these calls. No attempt was demic Press.
made to characterize the type of vocalizations made by Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (1994). Production and comprehen-
the chimpanzees nor was any attempt made to code for sion of referential pointing by orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus).
the affective valence of the vocalizations. Future studies Journal of Comparative Psychology, 108, 307–317.
should focus on this potential relationship between the Cantalupo, C., & Hopkins, W.D. (2001). Asymmetrical Broca’s
functional use of vocalizations and hand use. area in great apes. Nature, 414, 505.
In conclusion, the results indicate that preferential use Collins, R.L. (1985). On the inheritance of direction and
of the right hand is enhanced when accompanied by a degree of asymmetry. In S. Glick (Ed.), Cerebral lateraliza-
vocalization in chimpanzees, particularly in those raised by tion in nonhuman species (pp. 56–71). Orlando: Academic
Press.
human subjects. The preferential use of the right hand
Corballis, M.C. (1992). The lopsided brain: Evolution of the
for gestural communication did not correlate with other generative mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
measures of hand use and therefore does not appear to Corina, D.P., Pizner, H., Bellugi, U., Feinberg, T., Dowd, D.,
reflect a bias associated with other motor functions (also & O’Grady-Batch, I.L. (1993). Dissociation between linguistic
see Hopkins & Wesley, 2002). This indirectly supports and non-linguistic gestural system: a case of compositionality.
the theoretical notion that lateralization for referential Brain and Language, 43, 414–447.
communication may have a unique neurobiological rep- Corina, D.P., Vaid, J., & Bellugi, U. (1992). The linguistic basis
resentation in the brain compared to other behavioral, for left hemisphere specialization. Science, 255, 1258–1260.
motor and emotional functions. What brain areas may Dalby, J.T., Gibson, D., Grossi, V., & Schneider, R.D. (1980).
be associated with asymmetries in communication is not Lateralized hand gesture during speech. Journal of Motor
clear. With the recent evidence of left hemisphere neuro- Behavior, 12, 292–297.
Foundas, A.L., Macauley, B.L., Raymer, A.M., Maher, L.M.,
anatomical asymmetries in the peri-sylvian fissure region
Heilman, K.M., & Rothi, L.J. (1995). Gesture laterality in
as well as in the inferior frontal lobe, these appear to aphasic and apraxic stroke patients. Brain and Cognition, 29,
be ideal regions for future consideration (Cantalupo & 204–213.
Hopkins, 2001; Gannon, Holloway, Broadfield & Braun, Gannon, P.J., Holloway, R.L., Broadfield, D.C., & Braun,
1998; Hopkins, Marino, Rilling & MacGregor, 1998; A.R. (1998). Asymmetry of chimpanzee planum temporale:
Hopkins, Pilcher & MacGregor, 2001; Yeni-Komshian humanlike pattern of Wernicke’s brain language area homo-
& Benson, 1976). logue. Science, 279, 220–222.

© Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2003


DESC_013.fm Page 61 Tuesday, January 28, 2003 11:46 AM

Laterality in gestural and vocal communication 61

Geschwind, N., & Levitsky, W. (1968). Human brain: left–right Iverson, J.M., & Thelen, E. (1999). Hand, mouth and brain: the
asymmetries in the temporal speech region. Science, 151, dynamic emergence of speech and gesture. Journal of Consci-
186 –187. ousness Studies, 6, 19–40.
Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns in Kimura, D. (1973). Manual activity during speaking. I. Right-
adaptation. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. handers. Neuropsychologia, 11, 45–50.
Grossi, G., Semenza, C., Corazza, S., & Volterra, V. (1996). Kimura, D. (1993). Neuromotor mechanisms in human commun-
Hemispheric specialization for sign language. Neuropsycho- ication. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
logia, 34, 737–740. Krause, M.A., & Fouts, R.S. (1997). Chimpanzee (Pan troglo-
Hauser, M.C. (1993). Right hemisphere dominance in the dytes) pointing: hand shapes, accuracy, and the role of eye
production of facial expression in monkeys. Science, 261, gaze. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111, 330–336.
475–477. Leavens, D.A., & Hopkins, W.D. (1998). Intentional commun-
Hook-Costigan, M.A., & Rogers, L.J. (1998). Lateralised use ication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): a cross-sectional
of the mouth in production of vocalizations by marmosets. study of the use of referential gestures. Developmental Psycho-
Neuropsychologia, 36, 1265 –1273. logy, 34, 813–822.
Hopkins, W.D. (1993). Posture and reaching in chimpanzees Leavens, D.A., & Hopkins, W.D. (1999). The whole-hand
(Pan) and orangutans (Pongo). Journal of Comparative Psycho- point: the structure and function of pointing from a compar-
logy, 17, 162–168. ative perspective. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113,
Hopkins, W.D. (1994). Hand preferences for bimanual feeding 417–425.
in 140 captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): rearing and Leavens, D.A., Hopkins, W.D., & Bard, K.A. (1996). Indexical
ontogenetic factors. Developmental Psychobiology, 27, 395– and referential pointing in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).
407. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 110, 346–353.
Hopkins, W.D. (1995a). Hand preferences in juvenile chimpan- Locke, J.L., Bekken, K.E., McMinn-Larson, L., & Wein, D.
zees: continuity in development. Developmental Psychology, (1995). Emergent control of manual and vocal-motor activity
31, 619– 625. in relation to the development of speech. Brain and Language,
Hopkins, W.D. (1995b). Hand preferences for a coordinated 51, 498–508.
bimanual task in 110 chimpanzees: cross-sectional analysis. Seyfarth, R.M., & Cheney, D.L. (1997). Behavioral mechan-
Journal of Comparative Psychology, 109, 291–297. isms underlying vocal communication in nonhuman primates.
Hopkins, W.D., & Fernandez-Carriba, S. (2002). Laterality in Animal Learning & Behavior, 25, 249–267.
communicative behaviors in nonhuman primates: a critical Steklis, H.D., & Raleigh, M.J. (1979a). Behavioral and neuro-
analysis. In L. Rogers and R. Andrews (Eds.), Comparative biological aspects of primate vocalization and facial expres-
vertebrate lateralization (pp. 445 – 479). Oxford: Oxford Uni- sion. In H.D. Steklis & M.J. Raleigh (Eds.), Communication
versity Press. and behavior: An interdisciplinary series (pp. 257–282). New
Hopkins, W.D., & Leavens, D.A. (1998). Hand use and ges- York: Academic Press.
tural communication in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Jour- Steklis, H.D., & Raleigh, M.J. (1979b). Requisites for lan-
nal of Comparative Psychology, 112, 95–99. guage: interspecific and evolutionary aspects. In H.D. Steklis
Hopkins, W.D., Marino, L., Rilling, J., & MacGregor, L. & M.J. Raleigh (Eds.), Communication and behavior: An inter-
(1998). Planum temporale asymmetries in great apes as disciplinary series (pp. 283–314). New York: Academic Press.
revealed by magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). Neuro- Theall, L.A., & Povinelli, D. (1999). Do chimpanzees tailor
Report, 9, 2913–2918. their gestural signals to fit the attentional states of others?
Hopkins, W.D., Pilcher, D.L., & MacGregor, L. (2001). Syl- Animal Cognition, 2, 207–214.
vian fissure length asymmetries in primates revisited: a com- Tomasello, M., Call, J., Nagell, K., Olguin, R., & Carpenter,
parative MRI study. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 6, 1–12. M. (1994). The learning and use of gestural signals by young
Hopkins, W.D., & Wesley, M.J. (2002). Gestural commun- chimpanzees: a trans-generational study. Primates, 35, 137–
ication in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): the effect of situ- 154.
ational factors on gesture type and hand use. Laterality, 7, Yeni-Komshian, G.H., & Benson, D.A. (1976). Anatomical
19–30. study of cerebral asymmetry in the temporal lobe of humans,
Hostetter, A., Cantero, M., & Hopkins, W.D. (2001). Differ- chimpanzees, and rhesus monkeys. Science, 192, 387–389.
ential use of vocal and gestural communication in response
to the attentional state of a human experimenter. Journal of Received: 12 April 2001
Comparative Psychology, 115, 337–343. Accepted: 19 December 2001

© Blackwell Publishing Ltd. 2003

You might also like