You are on page 1of 14

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/267753624

Ethnicity and Cultural Values as Predictors of the Occurrence and Impact of


Experienced Workplace Incivility

Article in Journal of Occupational Health Psychology · November 2014


DOI: 10.1037/a0038277 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
71 2,834

3 authors:

Jennifer L. Welbourne Ashwini Gangadharan


University of Texas - Pan American Kutztown University
29 PUBLICATIONS 957 CITATIONS 8 PUBLICATIONS 165 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Ana Marcie Sariol


University of South Dakota
12 PUBLICATIONS 312 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Jennifer L. Welbourne on 02 May 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Occupational Health Psychology © 2014 American Psychological Association
2015, Vol. 20, No. 2, 205–217 1076-8998/15/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0038277

Ethnicity and Cultural Values as Predictors of the Occurrence and Impact


of Experienced Workplace Incivility
Jennifer L. Welbourne, Ashwini Gangadharan, and Ana M. Sariol
University of Texas–Pan American

Workplace incivility is a subtle type of deviant work behavior that is low in intensity and violates workplace
norms of respect. Past research demonstrates the harmful impact of incivility on work attitudes and employee
wellbeing; however, little is known about how incivility is experienced by individuals of different ethnicities
and cultural orientations. In the current study, we compared the amount and impact of workplace incivility that
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

was experienced by Hispanic and white, non-Hispanic employees. Further, we examined whether cultural
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

dimensions of vertical and horizontal individualism and collectivism moderated the relationships between
workplace incivility and work and health outcomes. A sample of 262 university employees (50% Hispanic;
63% female) provided self-reports of experienced incivility, burnout, job satisfaction, and cultural values.
Although male Hispanic employees experienced more incivility, female Hispanic employees experienced less
incivility than non-Hispanic employees of the same gender. Hispanic employees displayed greater resilience
against the impact of incivility on job satisfaction and burnout, compared with non-Hispanic employees.
Additionally, employees with strong horizontal collectivism values (emphasizing sociability) were more
resilient against the impact of incivility on burnout, whereas employees with strong horizontal individualism
values (emphasizing self-reliance) were more susceptible to burnout and dissatisfaction when faced with
incivility. These findings suggest that employees’ ethnicity and cultural values may increase or decrease their
vulnerability to the impact of incivility at work.

Keywords: collectivism, culture, individualism, workplace incivility

Scholars have recently identified the importance of investigating counter and respond to incivility at work. We suggest that exam-
subtle forms of deviant behaviors in the workplace (Cortina, 2008). ining incivility through a wider ethnic and cultural lens will inform
Workplace incivility, described as “low-intensity deviant behavior research and practice. First, because cultural values play a key role
with ambiguous intent to harm the target, in violation of workplace in shaping our perceptions (Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Triandis,
norms for mutual respect” (Andersson & Pearson, 1999, p. 457), 1995), they may influence how incivility is perceived. Because
includes behaviors such as excluding a colleague from an important uncivil behaviors are subtle and of ambiguous intent, targets’
meeting, speaking condescendingly to someone, or addressing some- perceptions affect how incivility is interpreted (Pearson & Porath,
one unprofessionally. Despite its subtle nature, the negative conse- 2005, p. 9). Second, as highlighted in Selective Incivility Theory
quences of incivility on employees’ job attitudes (Cortina, Magley, (Cortina, 2008; Cortina, Kabat-Farr, Leskinen, Huerta, & Magley,
Williams, & Langhout, 2001; Lim, Cortina, & Magley, 2008) and 2013), members of ethnic minorities may be especially likely to
mental and physical health (Lim et al., 2008) have been established. encounter incivility, due to thinly veiled prejudiced attitudes in the
These impacts can be understood through a stressor-strain framework workplace. Finally, both ethnicity and cultural values have been
in which incivility is conceptualized as a chronic, low-intensity stres- linked to how people cope with and respond to stressors (Chun,
sor (e.g., Cortina et al., 2001; Cortina, 2008). Moos, & Cronkite, 2009; Montoro-Rodriguez & Gallagher-
Despite well-documented impacts of incivility on work attitudes Thompson, 2010); thus, incivility as a stressor may differentially
and behaviors, there is a paucity of research exploring how em- impact employees with different cultural backgrounds.
ployees of different ethnicities, with unique cultural values, en- In particular, we propose that greater understanding of how
Hispanic and white, non-Hispanic employees differ in their expe-
riences and response to workplace incivility will broaden theoret-
ical knowledge of incivility and its practical implications. Individ-
This article was published Online First November 3, 2014. uals of Hispanic origin, defined as those who indicate their origin
Jennifer L. Welbourne, Ashwini Gangadharan, and Ana M. Sariol, as Mexican, Puerto Rican, Cuban, Central or South American1
Department of Management, University of Texas–Pan American.
Ashwini Gangadharan is now affiliated with the Department of Business
Administration, Kutztown University. 1
Although each of these Hispanic subcultures has its own unique
We thank Paul Sale for providing support for our data collection process
characteristics (Marin & Marin, 1991; Romero, 2004), they also share
and Celina Esparza for her assistance in survey administration. some core cultural values and experiences (Gallo et al., 2009; Hofstede,
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Jennifer 1980; Sabogal et al., 1987; Stone-Romero, Stone, & Salas, 2003), suggest-
L. Welbourne, Department of Management, 1201 West University Drive, ing that the variability within this category does not negate the value of
University of Texas–Pan American, Edinburg, TX 78539. E-mail: identifying and studying broader characteristics of Hispanic culture (Ro-
welbournjl@utpa.edu mero, 2004).

205
206 WELBOURNE, GANGADHARAN, AND SARIOL

(U.S. Census Bureau, 2010), are expected to comprise one of every finding that collectivistic cultural values were associated with
three U.S. residents by 2060 (www.census.gov). Drawing from reduced initiation of incivility. Lim and Lee (2011), however,
Selective Incivility Theory (Cortina, 2008; Cortina et al., 2013), found that more than 90% of a sample of Singaporean workers
Hispanics, as a growing ethnic minority within the United States experienced incivility, which affected their work–life conflict,
may be expected to encounter higher levels of workplace incivility psychological distress, and satisfaction with supervisors and co-
than white, non-Hispanic employees. workers, suggesting that incivility is not only an individualistic
Additionally, we suggest that distinctions in cultural values phenomenon.
between Hispanic and white, non-Hispanic employees may con- Although these initial studies yield promising insights into the
tribute to differential resilience to incivility. Certain shared values experience of workplace incivility across cultures, we note several
have been found to emerge among individuals who have been issues that warrant future investigation. First, we suggest that there
socialized as Hispanic2 (e.g., Gallo, Penedo, Espinosa, de los is a need for research to more directly compare the prevalence and
Monteros, & Arguellos, 2009; Hofstede, 1980; Sabogal et al., impact of incivility across cultures or ethnic groups. Additionally,
1987; Stone-Romero, Stone, & Salas, 2003). In particular, two while Liu et al. (2009) examine how individualistic and collectiv-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

cultural values recognized to contribute to resilience, specifically, istic values relate to instigation of incivility, the relevance of these
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

values stressing interpersonal functioning (Plant & Sachs- values for employees who are targets of uncivil behavior warrants
Ericsson, 2004) and family (Menselson et al., 2008; Organista, further attention. Finally, as both studies focused on Asian em-
Organista, & Kurasaki, 2003), are emphasized within Hispanic ployees, there is a need for greater understanding of how incivility
culture (Cuéllar, Arnold, & Gonzalez, 1995; Gallo et al., 2009), is experienced by members of other ethnic groups, such as His-
potentially providing a source of resilience to incivility at work. In panic employees.
contrast, white, non-Hispanic employees are more likely to focus In the following sections, we examine how ethnicity and cultural
on independence of the self (Markus & Kitayama, 1991), rather dimensions of individualism– collectivism may relate to the prev-
than on connections to others; we suggest that this may reduce their alence and impact of experienced incivility at work. We first use
resources (e.g., social support) for effectively managing interpersonal Selective Incivility Theory (Cortina, 2008; Cortina et al., 2013) as
stressors (see Chun et al., 2009), thereby lessening their resilience a framework for proposing differences in the prevalence of inci-
to the impacts of incivility. vility between Hispanic and non-Hispanic employees. Second, we
To address these issues, we explored the roles of ethnicity and draw on resilience theory (Richardson, 2002) and research on
cultural values in relation experienced workplace incivility. The protective cultural factors (Mendelsohn et al.; Plant & Sachs-
goals of this research were to examine whether Hispanic and white Ericsson, 2004; Umaña-Taylor et al., 2011) to suggest that the
non-Hispanic employees differ in the amount of incivility that they impact of incivility on job satisfaction and burnout is weakened for
experience at work and their resilience to the impact of incivility Hispanic employees and strengthened for white, non-Hispanic
on job dissatisfaction and burnout, and whether cultural values employees. Finally, we discuss the broader cultural dimension of
moderate the relationship between incivility, job satisfaction, and individualism– collectivism (Hofstede, 1980; Triandis, 1995) as a
burnout. In the following sections we review the theoretical back- potential moderator of the relationships between incivility and job
ground and basis for our hypotheses. satisfaction and burnout.

Workplace Incivility and Cultural Influences Prevalence of Incivility Among Hispanic


Workplace incivility is characterized by its violation of norms of and White Non-Hispanic Employees
respect and courtesy, low intensity, and ambiguous intent (Ander- According to Selective Incivility Theory (Cortina, 2008; Cortina
sson & Pearson, 1999; Pearson et al., 2000, 2001). Between 70% et al., 2013), incivility, particularly that which is subtle and am-
to 96% of employees from various industries report experiencing biguous, may selectively target ethnic minorities (as well as wom-
incivility in recent years (Cortina et al., 2001; Cortina & Magley, en); thus, incivility aimed at these groups can be viewed as a form
2009; Pearson & Porath, 2009). of modern discrimination reflecting subtle prejudices. Cortina
Targets of workplace incivility frequently report lowered job (2008; Cortina et al., 2013) suggests that in today’s workplace,
satisfaction (Cortina et al., 2001; Lim et al., 2008; Penney & which prohibits more blatant forms of racism and sexism, the
Spector, 2005; Miner-Rubino & Reed, 2010) and greater intentions ambiguity of uncivil behaviors allows instigators to express subtle
to quit (Cortina et al., 2001; Lim et al., 2008; Miner-Rubino & prejudices in ways that escape detection, because of the ease with
Reed, 2010; Pearson et al., 2000, 2001). Additionally, incivility which uncivil behaviors can be attributed to factors other than
has been linked to poorer psychological health (Cortina et al., gender or race. Cortina (2008; Cortina et al, 2013) further proposes
2001; Lim et al., 2008) and burnout (Miner-Rubino & Reed, that when ethnicity and gender intersect (e.g., an employee is both
2010), and indirectly to physical health through the psychological female and a member of an ethnic minority), a type of “double
distress it causes (Lim et al., 2008). jeopardy” (Beal, 1970; Epstein, 1973) occurs, such that the em-
Studies examining workplace incivility have tended to focus on ployee will be at heightened risk for experiencing incivility.
the experiences of White, non-Hispanic employees from predom-
inately individualistic cultures. However, two recent studies ex-
2
plore this phenomenon through a broader cultural scope. Liu, Chi, Stone-Romero et al. (2003) assert that Hispanics within the United
States frequently retain these traditional cultural values, such as collectiv-
Friedman, and Tsai (2009) examined the relationship between ism, emphasis on family, and motivation to maintain positive interpersonal
cultural values and instigation of incivility among employees from relationships (Hofstede, 1980), rather than fully assimilating to the pre-
collectivistic (Taiwan) and individualistic (United States) cultures, dominant culture within the United States.
CULTURE AND WORKPLACE INCIVILITY 207

Although support for Selective Incivility Theory has been found encompasses a motivation to maintain pleasant, conflict-free as-
among samples of female employees (Cortina et al., 2001; Cortina sociations with others (Gallo et al., 2009). Familism, a value
et al., 2002; Cortina et al., 2013) and African American employees promoting attachment and loyalty to family (Schwartz, 2007), is
(Cortina et al., 2013), the question of whether Hispanic employees, also identified as a core value within Hispanic culture, that is fairly
and in particular, Hispanic women, experience higher levels of robust across different Hispanic subcultures, generations, and lev-
incivility than non-Hispanic employees has yet to be examined. els of acculturation.
However, studies have demonstrated that Hispanic employees In contrast, white, non-Hispanic cultural values tend to highlight
experience other differential treatment in comparison to white, independence from others, rather than connection with others
non-Hispanic employees in the workplace (e.g., Cianni & Rom- (Markus & Kitayama, 1991); thus, there is less emphasis on the
berger, 1995; Dreher & Cox, 1996). Further, because we concep- importance of interpersonal functioning (Plant & Sachs-Ericsson,
tualize incivility as a stressor, we note that the broader stress 2004) and family (Sabogal et al., 1987). As a result, white, non-
literature finds that members of ethnic minorities are likely to Hispanic employees may be less likely to experience the resilience
experience heightened levels of stressors (Farley, Galves, Dickin- that has been associated with these qualities. Additionally, a cul-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

son, & Perez, 2005; Rodriguez-Calcagno & Brewer, 2005), in part tural emphasis on independence from others may be isolating
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

due to heightened discrimination faced by these groups (Clark, (Singelis et al., 1995) and result in less social support (Chun et al.,
Anderson, Clark, & Williams, 1999; James, 1994). Based on the 2009), potentially decreasing resilience to stressors, such as inci-
previous theory and research, we predict the following: vility.
Although growing research supports that aspects of culture may
Hypothesis 1: Hispanic Employees will report higher levels of provide resilience against the impact of stressors, there has been
experienced incivility than non-Hispanic employees. little investigation of these culturally specific protective mecha-
Hypothesis 2: The relationship between ethnicity and experi- nisms within the domain of occupational stress. Here we examine
enced incivility will be moderated by gender, such that His- them in relation to workplace incivility, which has been concep-
panic women will report the highest levels of incivility. tualized as a low-grade occupational stressor (Cortina, 2008; Cor-
tina & Magley, 2009; Cortina et al., 2001). Because the cultural
values held by Hispanic individuals are expected to provide more
Cultural Resilience and Incivility
resilience against stressors than those held by white, non-Hispanic
We draw from the resilience literature to explain how ethnicity individuals, we propose that incivility will be less strongly related
and cultural values might affect how individuals cope with inci- to burnout and low job satisfaction for Hispanic employees than
vility and its negative impacts. Resilience can be understood as a for white non-Hispanic employees.
positive adaptation to stress (Luthar & Cicchetti, 2000; Tugade &
Fredrickson, 2004; Fletcher & Sarkar, 2013; Richardson, 2002). Hypothesis 3: The relationship between workplace incivility
Richardson (2002) suggests that individuals may develop protec- and (a) burnout and (b) job satisfaction is moderated by
tive factors from social, ecological, and spiritual sources, which ethnicity, such that the relationship between these variables is
enable them to overcome stressors. Culture can be a source of weaker among Hispanic than white non-Hispanic employees.
resilience that protects individuals against the negative impacts of
stressors. In particular, scholars suggest that the values or coping
Individualism–Collectivism and Experienced Incivility
styles associated with certain cultures may provide a protective
influence with regard to the health impacts of stressors (Case & The aspects of culture (interpersonal functioning, focus on fam-
Robinson, 2003; Garcia Coll, Akerman, & Cicchetti, 2000; ily) identified as contributing to resilience in face of stressors are
Menselson et al., 2008; Palloni & Morenoff, 2001; Umaña-Taylor ones that are generally valued in collectivistic and de-emphasized
et al., 2011) and have called for additional research on the adaptive in individualistic cultural traditions (Markus & Kitayama, 1991).
aspects of culture under conditions of stress or risk (Garcia Coll et The individualism– collectivism dimension represents one of the
al., 2000). In particular, cultural emphases on sociability and most fundamental distinctions made between cultures. Collectiv-
family may contribute to resilience in overcoming stressors. Re- ists tend to have interdependent self-construals; they view them-
searchers have posited that cultures that emphasize strong inter- selves in terms of relationships with others, and are influenced by
personal functioning have increased resilience against the mental social norms and a desire for group harmony (Hofstede, 1980;
health impacts of stressors (Menselson et al., 2008; Plant & Sachs- Singelis et al., 1995; Triandis, 1995). Both family integrity (e.g.,
Ericsson, 2004). Similarly, cultural values that promote attachment agreement with statements such as children should live at home
and loyalty to family can provide increased social support to buffer with their parents until they get married) and sociability are
against the impact of stressors on mental health (Gil-Rivas et al., encompassed within collectivistic values (Triandis et al., 1986;
2003; Menselson et al., 2008; Organista et al., 2003). Triandis, Chan, Bhawuk, Iwao, & Sinha, 1995). In contrast, indi-
Although these constructs are not specific to (or characteristic of vidualists tend to have independent self-construals; they view
all) individuals of Hispanic ethnicity, research suggests that values themselves as unique and autonomous from others, and are influ-
focusing on sociability and family tend to be emphasized within enced by personal goals and preferences (Hofstede, 1980; Singelis
Hispanic culture. For instance, the importance of sociability is et al., 1995; Triandis, 1995). Individualistic values center on
highlighted in the Hispanic values of personalismo (Cuéllar, Ar- competition, self-reliance, and distance from one’s in-groups (Sin-
nold, & Maldonado, 1995) and simpatía (Gallo et al., 2009); gelis et al., 1995).
personalismo reflects a drive to relate to others in a warm, personal The United States and Western European countries are typically
way (Cuéllar, Arnold, & Maldonado, 1995), whereas simpatía characterized as individualistic cultures (Hofstede, 1980), whereas
208 WELBOURNE, GANGADHARAN, AND SARIOL

Asian, Latin American, and African cultures tend to demonstrate higher face of stressors (Menselson et al., 2008; Plant & Sachs-Ericsson,
levels of collectivism (Hofstede, 1980). Although there are cross- 2004). Additionally, Triandis and Gelfand (1998) suggest that HC
cultural differences in individualism and collectivism, these values values, because of their emphasis on developing caring bonds with
also show considerable variation within any given culture (Trian- others, may be associated with receiving greater social support.
dis, 1994). For example, although Hispanic cultures in general are Social support has been demonstrated as an effective buffer against
based in stronger collectivistic roots than Anglo-American cultures a wide range of stressors (Cortina, 2004; Van Emmerik, 2002);
(Gomez, 2003; Marin & Triandis, 1985), there is still a great deal further, both emotional and organizational social support have
of variability in the degree to which individuals within each culture been linked to improved functioning in the face of workplace
endorse collectivistic and individualistic values. The recognition incivility (Miner, Settles, Pratt-Hyatt, & Brady, 2012). Therefore,
that these values coexist in every culture and that individuals we suggest that employees with strong HC values may demon-
within a particular culture can be arrayed along these cultural strate greater resilience against the impact of incivility on burnout
dimensions (Triandis et al., 1998) leads us to suggest that (regard- and job satisfaction.
less of ethnicity) the degree to which individuals adhere to collec-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tivistic and individualistic cultural values will influence their re- Hypothesis 4: The relationship between workplace incivility
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

silience to experienced incivility at work. and (a) burnout and (b) job satisfaction will be moderated by
levels of HC, such that the relationship between these vari-
ables will be weaker for employees who endorse high levels of
Vertical and Horizontal Dimensions of HC values.
Individualism–Collectivism
Whereas HC emphasizes sociability and interdependence, VC
Scholars have proposed that different varieties of collectivism emphasizes conformity and deference to one’s ingroup (Singelis et
and individualism may exist, and therefore, a simple dichotomy is al., 1995; Shavitt et al., 2006). Both theoretically and empirically,
not capable of fully capturing the complexity of these values VC has been linked to familism. Conceptually, both VC and
(Shavitt, Lalwani, Zhang, & Torelli, 2006; Singelis et al., 1995; familism share an emphasis on the prioritization of family over self
Triandis, 1995; Triandis & Gelfand, 1998). Accordingly, Triandis (Schwartz, 2007). VC correlates with familism scores (Schwartz,
(1995) distinguished between vertical and horizontal varieties of 2007) and is often operationalized through items which measure
individualism and collectivism, creating a four-category typology sacrifice to family (Triandis & Gelfand, 1998; Singelis et al.,
that includes vertical individualism (VI), horizontal individualism 1995). As previously discussed, strong family bonds can be a
(HI), vertical collectivism (VC), and horizontal collectivism (HC). source of resilience against stressors (Gil-Rivas et al., 2003; Hol-
Although both types of individualism focus on independence of leran & Waller, 2003; Menselson et al., 2008; Organista et al.,
the self from others, VI emphasizes status and competition to 2003; Umaña-Taylor et al., 2011). Further, VC values are based in
distinguish oneself from others, whereas HI emphasizes autonomy an acceptance of inequalities and expectations that one should
through unique characteristics and self-reliance. Similarly, al- sacrifice personal goals for the good of one’s group (Singelis et al.,
though both types of collectivism are characterized by interdepen- 1995). Employees who hold this combination of beliefs may be
dence with others, vertical collectivists embrace status differences more likely to tolerate or accept uncivil behavior from workgroup
within their in-groups, with value placed on self-sacrifice and members for the purpose of maintaining the integrity of their
compliance to one’s in-group, whereas horizontal collectivists group. Therefore, we predict that VC values will weaken the
emphasize sociability in an “equal status” framework. impact of incivility on burnout and job satisfaction.
There is growing consensus with regard to the value of applying
this more fine-tuned framework of horizontal-vertical dimensions Hypothesis 5: The relationship between workplace incivility
of individualism– collectivism to understand cultural influences on and (a) burnout and (b) job satisfaction will be moderated by
behavior and perception (see Nelson & Shavitt, 2002; Sivadas, levels of VC, such that the relationship between these vari-
Bruvold, & Nelson, 2008; Singelis et al., 1995; Shavitt et al., ables will be weaker for employees who endorse high levels of
2006). Singelis et al. (1995) suggest that vertical-horizontal dis- VC values.
tinctions may be especially significant for understanding how
people respond to and manage interpersonal conflicts. Accord- Vertical–Horizontal Dimensions of Individualism
ingly, we propose that use of this distinction may be particularly
and Resilience to Incivility
relevant to understanding the way that targets manage and respond
to workplace incivility. In the following section, we develop Whereas collectivistic cultural values are linked to interdepen-
specific hypotheses regarding the degree to which horizontal and dence, individualistic cultural values are associated with indepen-
vertical dimensions of individualism– collectivism provide a dence (Hofstede, 1980; Singelis et al., 1995; Triandis, 1995).
source of resilience in overcoming incivility. Because individualism focuses on autonomy, this cultural value
may be associated with a lack of social resources in times of stress
Vertical–Horizontal Dimensions of Collectivism (Chun et al., 2009). In particular, Singelis and colleagues (1995)
suggest that HI’s emphasis on self-reliance and “doing one’s own
and Resilience to Incivility
thing” may carry a risk of social isolation. We propose that this
HC has been characterized by an emphasis on sociability, co- may exacerbate the impact of interpersonal stressors, such as
operation, and maintaining caring relationships (Shavitt et al., workplace incivility, such that individuals with HI values may
2006). This parallels cultural emphases on interpersonal relation- have fewer social resources to turn to for support when facing
ships that have been purported to contribute to resilience in the these challenges. Further, we suggest that the alienation associated
CULTURE AND WORKPLACE INCIVILITY 209

with incivility (e.g., Vickers, 2006) may be compounded for em- Hispanic, 7.5% white non-Hispanic, 1.1% Asian, .8% Black/Afri-
ployees who are already experiencing greater social isolation, can American, .5% American Indian or Alaskan Native, and .4%
because of their HI cultural values. Based on these arguments, we indicating two or more races (quickfacts.census.gov). Data that we
propose that strong HI values will strengthen the negative impact collected to assess the acculturation of Hispanic respondents3
of incivility on burnout and job satisfaction. indicated that 26% of the Hispanic respondents in our sample were
strongly assimilated to the United States culture (based on their
Hypothesis 6: The relationship between workplace incivility classification on the Brief Acculturation Rating Scale for Mexican
and (a) burnout and (b) job satisfaction will be moderated by Americans (ARSMA)-II (Bauman, 2005), whereas 72% were clas-
levels of HI, such that the relationship between these variables sified as “bicultural” or holding aspects of both cultures.
will be stronger for employees who endorse high levels of HI
values. Procedure
Although VI shares with HI an emphasis on independence, it Participants were invited to participate in this research via
focuses primarily on competitive values and has not been explic- e-mails sent to faculty and staff university listservs. Employees
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

itly linked to values of self-reliance that might be associated with received an initial e-mail requesting their participation in an anon-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

social isolation. Further, although it shares with VC an emphasis ymous survey on workplace attitudes and behaviors; the e-mail
on acceptance of inequalities, VI does not encompass values of informed them that they would receive a link to the survey in the
family integrity proposed to provide protective influences of VC. upcoming weeks. Two weeks later, they received another e-mail
Therefore, we do not expect this dimension of individualism to which contained the link to the survey with instructions for com-
either strengthen or weaken the relationship of incivility with job pletion. Two weeks later, a reminder e-mail was sent containing
satisfaction and burnout. Although we do not make specific pre- the link to the survey. The survey assessed participants’ responses
dictions for VI, we will test its moderating role in an exploratory on workplace incivility, burnout, and job satisfaction, as well as
way. vertical and horizontal dimensions of individualism and collectiv-
ism.
Method Measures
Workplace incivility. Workplace incivility was measured
Participants with the 7-item Workplace Incivility Scale (WIS) (Cortina et al.,
Data were collected from faculty and staff at a public university 2001). Cronbach’s alpha in the current sample indicated a reliabil-
located in the southern United States near the border of Mexico. A ity of 0.94. Sample items include Have you been in a situation
total of 266 faculty and staff completed an anonymous online where any of your superiors or coworkers ignored or excluded you
survey. Four respondents with missing data on primary outcome from professional camaraderie? and Have you ever been in a
measures were removed from the sample and excluded from data situation where any of your superiors or coworkers paid little
analysis. Our final sample included 262 employees (63% female) attention to your statement or showed little interest in your opin-
comprising 42% faculty/instructors, 24% executive staff, 28% ion? Participants were asked to think about the last 6 months at
technical, clerical, and service staff, and 6% who indicated other their job when responding to these items, and to indicate the
types of university positions. The average age of the participants frequency of these events using a 7 point scale ranging from never
was 44.9 years (SD ⫽ 11.8), and the average tenure of the to very frequently.
participants in their current position was 8.4 years (SD ⫽ 8.5). Job satisfaction. We assessed Job satisfaction using a 6-item
Our sample of faculty and staff respondents comprised 132 version of the Brayfield and Rothe (1951) overall job satisfaction
Hispanic employees (50%), 108 white non-Hispanic employees measure. The measure contains six items, with response options on
(41%), 13 Asian employees (5%), and 3 Black/African American a 7-point scale ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree.
employees (1%). This varied across job type, such that the major- Cronbach’s alpha for the sample was 0.90. Sample items include I
ity of faculty in our sample were white, non-Hispanic (65%), feel fairly well satisfied with my present job and Most days I am
whereas the majority of staff in our sample were Hispanic (70%). enthusiastic about my work.
More specifically, faculty respondents were composed of 65% Burnout. We assessed burnout using a 21-item burnout scale
white, non-Hispanic, 25% Hispanic, 9% Asian, and 1% Black/ developed by Pines and Aronson (1988) to measure elements of
African American employees, and staff respondents were com- physical, mental, and emotional exhaustion at work. The Cron-
posed of 27% white, non-Hispanic, 70% Hispanic, 1.5% Asian, bach’s alpha for burnout was 0.97. Participants indicated the
and 1.5% Black/African American employees. This sample is frequency (on a 7-point scale ranging from never to always) with
generally consistent with the demographics of the university at which they experienced feelings such as rejected, and trapped.
which we collected our data: the faculty is made up of 46% white, Responses were collected with 7-point Likert scales, ranging from
non-Hispanic, 35% Hispanic, 9% Asian, and 2% Black/African never to always.
American employees, and the university staff is composed of 13% Individualism/Collectivism. We assessed cultural values of
white non-Hispanic, 83% Hispanic, 2% Asian, and 1% Black/ individualism and collectivism using the 16-item scale developed
African American employees. We note that the ethnic/racial di-
versity in our sample, as well as within the university as a whole, 3
Because the items of Brief ARSMA-II are specifically designed to
is greater than that found in the broader community. The popula- assess acculturation among Mexican American individuals, it was admin-
tion of the county in which this university is located is 90.7% istered only to participants who indicated Hispanic ethnicity.
210 WELBOURNE, GANGADHARAN, AND SARIOL

by Triandis and Gelfand (1998). Consistent with Triandis and ethnic group (n ⫽ 16) or chose not to report their ethnicity (n ⫽ 6)
Gelfand (1998), horizontal and vertical dimensions of individual were excluded from those analyses. For all other analyses that we
and collectivism were assessed. HC (4 items; ␣ ⫽ .70) included report, the full data set was used.
statements such as To me, pleasure is spending time with others
and I feel good when I cooperate with others. VC (4 items; ␣ ⫽ Ethnicity and Amount of Experienced Incivility
.78) contained items such as Family members should stick to-
gether, no matter what sacrifices are required and It is my duty to We predicted that Hispanic employees would experience greater
take care of my family, even when I have to sacrifice what I want. levels of incivility than white Non-Hispanic employees (H1), and
For HI (4 items; ␣ ⫽ .82), sample items included I rely on myself that this effect would be moderated by gender such that female
most of the time; I rarely rely on others and I’d rather depend on Hispanic employees would experience the highest level of incivil-
myself than others. Finally, VI (4 items; ␣ ⫽ .67) included state- ity (H2). We tested these hypotheses by conducting an Analysis of
ments such as Competition is the law of nature and It is important Covariance (ANCOVA) in which we controlled for job type. Job
that I do my job better than others. All items were rated on 7-point type was controlled for in the analysis,4 because it was associated
with the amount of incivility that employees experienced (F ⫽
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

scales ranging from strongly disagree to strongly agree.


4.29, p ⬍ .05). The ANCOVA indicated no main effects of gender
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Demographic variables. Participants reported their gender,


age, and ethnicity. They were also asked to indicate the type of job [Females: M ⫽ 2.3; Males: M ⫽ 2.5; F(1, 227) ⫽ 2.49, ns] or
they held (selecting from categories of professor, associate profes- ethnicity [Non-Hispanics: M ⫽ 2.3; Hispanics: M ⫽ 2.4; F(1,
sor, assistant professor, lecturer/instructor, executive/administra- 227) ⫽ 1.79, ns] on the amount of incivility experienced by
tive staff, technical staff, clerical/secretarial staff, service/mainte- employees. A statistically significant interaction, F(1, 227) ⫽
nance staff, or other) and the length of time they had been 10.04, p ⫽ .01, between gender and ethnicity on incivility indi-
employed in that position. cated that among males, Hispanic employees (M ⫽ 2.6) experi-
Acculturation. Participants who self-identified as Hispanic enced more incivility than non-Hispanic employees (M ⫽ 2.0),
were additionally asked to complete the Brief ARSMA-II (Bau- F(1, 83) ⫽ 4.93, p ⫽ .03, whereas, among females, Hispanic
man, 2005), a measure of acculturation that has been shown to employees experienced less incivility (M ⫽ 2.3) than non-Hispanic
demonstrate adequate reliability and validity (Bauman, 2005). This employees (M ⫽ 2.8), F(1, 147) ⫽ 4.39, p ⫽ .04. Thus, Hypoth-
scale is a shortened version of the ARSMA-II (Cuéllar, Arnold, & eses 1 and 2 were not supported.
Maldonado, 1995), which was a refinement of the ARSMA (Cuel-
lar, Harris, & Jasso, 1980); both of these scales are among the most Does Ethnicity Moderate the Impact of Incivility on
frequently used measures to assess acculturation among Hispanic Burnout and Job Satisfaction?
populations in the United States (Thomson & Hoffman-Goetz,
2009). The Brief ARSMA-II is aimed at assessing acculturation of We predicted that employees of Hispanic ethnicity would ex-
Mexican American persons (although it also has some applicabil- perience a weaker association between incivility and burnout
ity for other Hispanic groups; Cuéllar, Arnold, & Maldonado, (H3a) and job satisfaction (H3b) than White, Non-Hispanic em-
1995); therefore, it was a good fit for measuring acculturation of ployees. We conducted moderated regression analyses (Aiken &
Hispanic participants in our region at the border of Mexico. The West, 1991) to examine our hypotheses regarding the role of
Brief ARSMA-II contains two subscales to assess Mexican Ori- ethnicity in moderating the impact of incivility on job satisfaction
entation (6 items) and Anglo Orientation (6 items) of respondents and burnout (see Table 2). Because of the meaningful interaction
as orthogonal constructs. Items are measured on a 5-point scale of ethnicity and gender on incivility found in the previous analysis,
(not at all to extremely often/nearly always) with a focus on we also included gender in our tests of the current hypotheses.
language use and preferences (e.g., I speak English; I enjoy speak- Following Aiken and West (1991), all predictor variables were
ing Spanish), media preferences (e.g., I enjoy English language transformed into standardized z scores, and interaction terms were
movies; I enjoy reading in Spanish), and social interaction (e.g., computed. Main effects of experienced incivility, ethnicity, and
My friends are Anglo). Subscale scores are combined to compute gender were entered into the regression in Step 1. In the second
an overall acculturation score, which is classified at 5 levels step, two-way interaction terms between ethnicity, gender, and
ranging from highly assimilated to maintaining traditional cultural incivility were entered into the regression. For all statistically
values from one’s country of origin. significant interaction effects, means were estimated and plotted at
high (⫹1 SD) and low (⫺1 SD) levels of each of the terms of the
interaction, and simple slope analyses were computed to interpret
Results the interaction.
Means, standard deviations, intercorrelations, and reliability co- Consistent with prior research, incivility predicted lower job
efficients for all variables were computed (see Table 1). Composite satisfaction (␤ ⫽ ⫺.72, p ⫽ .002) and greater burnout (␤ ⫽ .92,
scores were created for all variables containing multiple items. The p ⬍ .01). Supporting Hypothesis 3, the relationship between inci-
Cronbach’s alpha for the VI scale (␣ ⫽ .67) was slightly lower vility and job satisfaction was moderated by ethnicity (␤ ⫽ .13,
than accepted standards. All other scales demonstrated acceptable p ⬍ .05), with the pattern of this interaction (See Figure 1)
reliability. We conducted Analysis of Covariance (ANCOVA) and indicating that the negative relationship between incivility and job
moderated hierarchical regression analyses to test our hypotheses.
Because our first set of research questions (H1–H3) focused spe- 4
We note that the same pattern of results and statistical significance was
cifically on comparison of Hispanic and white, non-Hispanic em- found when we computed this analysis without the inclusion of job type as
ployees, participants who self-identified as a member of another a covariate.
CULTURE AND WORKPLACE INCIVILITY 211

Table 1
Means, Standard deviations, Internal Consistency Reliability, and Intercorrelations

Variable Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

1. Job satisfaction 5.48 1.41 .90


2. Burnout 2.84 1.11 ⫺.58ⴱⴱ .97
3. Incivility 2.47 1.40 ⫺.42ⴱⴱ .62ⴱⴱ .94
4. Horizontal individualism 5.50 1.11 ⫺.11 .08 .03 .82
5. Vertical individualism 3.56 1.13 .02 .05 .02 .30ⴱⴱ .67
6. Horizontal collectivism 5.71 .74 .08 ⫺.19ⴱⴱ ⫺.12ⴱ .02 ⫺.18ⴱ .70
7. Vertical collectivism 5.15 1.22 .01 ⫺.07 ⫺.19ⴱⴱ ⫺.02 .15ⴱ .31ⴱ .78
Note. Coefficient alpha reliabilities are displayed in the diagonal.

Correlation is significant at p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ Correlation is significant at p ⬍ .01.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

satisfaction was stronger for non-Hispanic employees (␤ ⫽ ⫺.49) (⫺1 SD) levels of each of the terms of the interaction, and simple
than for Hispanic employees (␤ ⫽ ⫺.30). The interaction between slope analyses were computed to interpret the interaction.
incivility and ethnicity on burnout approached statistical signifi- HC moderated the impact of incivility on burnout (␤ ⫽ ⫺.15,
cance (␤ ⫽ ⫺.10, p ⫽ .056), with the pattern of the interaction (see p ⬍ .01). The plotted interaction (See Figure 3) shows that the
Figure 2) indicating that the relationship between incivility and relationship between incivility and burnout was weaker among
burnout was slightly stronger for non-Hispanic (␤ ⫽ .71) than employees with high HC values (␤ ⫽ .53, p ⬍ .01), as compared
Hispanic participants (␤ ⫽ .57). Gender did not moderate these with employees with low HC values (␤ ⫽ .83, p ⬍ .01). These
relationships. findings support Hypothesis 4a, suggesting that HC values buffer
the impact of incivility on burnout. However, HC did not moderate
Do Cultural Values Buffer the Impact of Incivility on the relationship between incivility and job satisfaction (␤ ⫽ .03,
Burnout and Job Satisfaction? ns); thus, Hypothesis 4b was not supported. VC did not moderate
the relationship between incivility and job satisfaction (␤ ⫽ ⫺.02,
t tests indicated that Hispanic participants scored higher than ns) or the relationship between incivility and burnout (␤ ⫽ .04, ns),
white non-Hispanic participants on HC, t(235) ⫽ ⫺2.12, p ⬍ .05; suggesting that this cultural value does not increase resilience
M ⫽ 5.8 vs. 5.4 and VC, t(235) ⫽ ⫺5.83, p ⬍ .01; M ⫽ 5.6 vs. against the impact of incivility. Thus, Hypothesis 5 was not sup-
4.7. We conducted moderated hierarchical regression analyses ported.
(Aiken & West, 1991) to examine our hypotheses regarding the HI values moderated the relationship between incivility and job
role of cultural values in buffering the impact of incivility on job satisfaction (␤ ⫽ ⫺.20, p ⬍ .01) and the relationship between
satisfaction and burnout (see Table 3). All predictor variables were incivility and burnout (␤ ⫽ .18, p ⬍ .01). The plotted interaction
transformed into standardized z-scores, and interaction terms were (See Figure 4) shows that employees with high HI values experi-
computed. Main effects of experienced incivility, HI, HC, VI, and ence a stronger negative relationship (␤ ⫽ ⫺.82, p ⬍ .01) between
VC were entered into the regression in Step 1. In the second step, incivility and job satisfaction than employees who score lower on
the two-way interaction terms between incivility and each cultural HI (␤ ⫽ ⫺.25, p ⬍ .06). Similarly, Figure 5 demonstrates that
value (e.g., Incivility ⫻ HC; Incivility ⫻ VC; etc.) were entered employees with high HI cultural values experience a stronger
into the regression. For all statistically significant interaction ef- relationship between incivility and burnout (␤ ⫽ .89, p ⬍ .01) than
fects, means were estimated and plotted at high (⫹1 SD) and low employees who score lower on HI (␤ ⫽ .48, p ⬍ .01). Thus, in
support of Hypothesis 6, these results suggest that the cultural
value of HI strengthens the negative impacts associated with
Table 2
Moderated Regression Results With Job Satisfaction and
Burnout as Dependent Variables

Dependent variables
Job satisfaction Burnout
Predictors Model 1 Model 2 Model 1 Model 2

Ethnicity ⫺.08 .23 ⫺.02 ⫺.36⫹


Incivility ⫺.37ⴱⴱ ⫺.72ⴱⴱ .61ⴱⴱ .92ⴱⴱ
Gender .10 .13ⴱ .00 ⫺.03
Incivility ⫻ Ethnicity .13ⴱ ⫺.10⫹
Incivility ⫻ Gender .32 ⫺.29
Gender ⫻ Ethnicity ⫺.30 .33
R2 .15 .17 .38 .40
⌬R2 .15ⴱⴱ .02⫹ .38ⴱⴱ .02⫹
Note. Standardized regression coefficients are displayed.

p ⬍ .06. ⴱ p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. Figure 1. Interaction of incivility and ethnicity on job satisfaction.
212 WELBOURNE, GANGADHARAN, AND SARIOL

Figure 3. Interaction of incivility and horizontal collectivism on burnout.


This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Figure 2. Interaction of incivility and ethnicity on burnout.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

interpret and respond to uncivil behavior at work. In this research,


incivility. As expected, VI did not moderate the relationships we compared the prevalence and impact of incivility experienced
between incivility and job satisfaction (␤ ⫽ .05, ns); however, by Hispanic and white non-Hispanic employees. Additionally, we
unexpectedly, the effect of VI as a moderator of incivility and examined whether cultural values of horizontal and vertical indi-
burnout approached significance (␤ ⫽ ⫺.11, p ⫽ .06). As plotted vidualism and collectivism moderated the relationship between
in Figure 6, employees with high VI values demonstrated a slightly incivility, burnout, and job satisfaction. We explore our findings
weaker relationship between incivility and burnout (␤ ⫽ .57, p ⬍ below.
.01), compared with employees with low VI scores (␤ ⫽ .80, p ⬍
.01). Prevalence of Workplace Incivility Among Hispanic
and Non-Hispanic Employees
Discussion
In contrast to our predictions based in Selective Incivility The-
The goal of this research was to investigate ethnicity and cul- ory (Cortina, 2008; Cortina et al, 2013), there was no main effect
tural values in relation to workplace incivility, a subtle form of of ethnicity on experienced incivility, contradicting prior research
workplace mistreatment often conceptualized as an occupational that suggests Hispanic individuals are at higher risk for stressful
stressor (Cortina, 2008). Past research recognizes the role of eth- events as a result of discrimination (Farley et al., 2005; Rodriguez-
nicity and culture in shaping perceptions (Markus & Kitayama, Calcagno & Brewer, 2005). This finding might be interpreted in
1991; Triandis, 1995), as well as responses to stressors (Chun et light of the value of simpatía that characterizes Hispanic culture.
al., 2009; Montoro-Rodriguez & Gallagher-Thompson, 2010), Placing value on interacting with others in a congenial way may
suggesting the value of examining incivility within a broader reduce conflict in interpersonal relationships (Stone-Romero et al.,
cultural context. Further, increased diversity in the United States 2003), therefore reducing experiences of incivility. Further, the
workforce (Toossi, 2012) underscores the need for better under- direction of the interactive effect of ethnicity and gender on
standing of how members of various ethnic and cultural groups incivility was opposite to our prediction, such that Hispanic fe-
males reported experiencing less (rather than more) incivility,
compared with white, non-Hispanic females or Hispanic males.
Table 3 This result contradicts Selective Incivility Theory (Cortina, 2008;
Moderated Regression Results With Job Satisfaction and Cortina et al., 2013), as well as previous studies that provided
Burnout as Dependent Variables support for the double jeopardy hypothesis (Buchanan & Fitzger-
Dependent variables
Job satisfaction Burnout
Predictors Model 1 Model 2 Model 1 Model 2
ⴱ ⴱⴱ
HI ⫺.15 ⫺.18 .08 .11ⴱ
VI .10 .13ⴱ ⫺.01 ⫺.05
HC .10 .10 ⫺.14ⴱ ⫺.14ⴱ
VC ⫺.10 ⫺.12 .09 .09
Incivility ⫺.38ⴱⴱ ⫺.37ⴱⴱ .61ⴱⴱ .60ⴱⴱ
Incivility ⫻ HI ⫺.20ⴱⴱ .18ⴱⴱ
Incivility ⫻ HC .03 ⫺.15ⴱ
Incivility ⫻ VI .05 ⫺.11⫹
Incivility ⫻ VC ⫺.02 .04
R2 .17 .20 .40 .44
⌬R2 .17ⴱⴱ .03ⴱ .40ⴱⴱ .04ⴱⴱ
Note. Standardized regression coefficients are displayed. Figure 4. Interaction of incivility and horizontal individualism on job

p ⬍ .06. ⴱ p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. satisfaction.
CULTURE AND WORKPLACE INCIVILITY 213

ployees of different ethnic backgrounds may respond differently to


an uncivil work environment.
Although the current research focused on differences in resil-
ience attributable to cultural values, we recognize that other factors
might also contribute to reduced impact of incivility on satisfaction
and burnout among Hispanic employees. For example, Hispanic
participants may experience (at work or in general) greater expo-
sure to stressors, particularly related to discrimination; as a result,
they may have developed more effective coping responses for
handling uncivil behaviors. Indeed, Raver and Nishii (2010) pro-
pose that increased exposure to multiple forms of mistreatment
may result in adaptation to the mistreatment resulting in weakened
outcomes. Additionally, participants may have varied in how they
Figure 5. Interaction of incivility and horizontal individualism on burn-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

attributed uncivil behavior. For instance, Hispanic participants


out.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

may have been more likely to relate the incivility that they expe-
rienced to their ethnicity, compared with non-Hispanic white par-
ald, 2008; Berdahl & Moore, 2006; Barnum, Liden, & DiTomaso, ticipants, potentially impacting their responses to incivility. Al-
1995); however, it is consistent with a recent study (Raver & though these factors were not assessed here, they are valuable
Nishii, 2010) that found no evidence of double jeopardy for directions for future work.
general workplace harassment.
Although these findings may indeed reflect actual differences in Cultural Values and Resilience to Incivility
incivility directed at these targets, alternatively, they may reflect
To better understand the role of specific cultural values in
differences in targets’ perceptions of incivility (i.e., female His-
buffering or exacerbating the impacts of incivility, we examined
panic employees may have a higher threshold for perceiving
whether vertical and horizontal dimensions of collectivism and
incivility). For example, Lim and Lee (2011) suggest that cultural
individualism moderated the relationships between incivility and
differences in the salience of gender power differences may con-
job satisfaction and burnout. Our results suggest that HC values,
tribute to their finding that females reported less incivility than
which emphasize sociability and warm interpersonal relationships
males in a sample of Singaporean employees. Specifically, within
(Shavitt et al., 2006), increase resilience to burnout associated with
this culture, males may expect more respectful treatment and
incivility. We suggest that HC values may be protective against
therefore have lower thresholds for incivility. This explanation
burnout, because they facilitate the formation of strong social
may also be relevant to the current study, as Hispanic culture is
support networks (Triandis & Gelfand, 1998). Previous work
characterized as adhering to strong gender roles (Cuéllar, Arnold,
demonstrates beneficial effects of emotional support in the context
& Gonzalez, 1995). We suggest that future research examine more
of incivility (Miner et al., 2012).
directly whether different thresholds for incivility are associated
Interestingly, protective aspects of HC did not extend to job
with ethnicity and whether instigators of incivility are less likely to
satisfaction. To interpret this result, we suggest that job satisfac-
direct incivility at targets that are Hispanic and female.
tion may be more closely aligned with individualistic than collec-
tivistic cultural values. Defining oneself through attitudes and
Ethnicity and Resilience to Incivility traits is characteristic of individualistic cultural orientations. How-
As predicted, Hispanic employees, compared with White non- ever, persons with collectivistic orientations tend to define them-
Hispanic employees, experienced less burnout and reduction in job selves through relationships and group memberships, rather than
satisfaction in response to incivility. The greater resilience expe- personal attitudes and traits (Triandis & Gelfand, 1998); thus, job
rienced by Hispanic employees compared with white, non- satisfaction, as an attitude toward one’s job, may not be as relevant
Hispanic employees in our study is consistent with studies that of an outcome for them. This interpretation is consistent with our
have reported protective cultural influences associated with values
focused on social functioning and family (Case & Robinson, 2003;
Garcia Coll et al., 2000; Menselson et al., 2008; Palloni & More-
noff, 2001; Umaña-Taylor et al., 2011). These values are empha-
sized within Hispanic culture (Cuéllar, Arnold, & Gonzalez, 1995;
Gallo et al., 2009; Plant & Sachs-Ericsson, 2004), potentially
contributing to greater resilience against incivility, whereas white,
non-Hispanic participants are more likely to hold values focusing
on independence and autonomy (Markus & Kitayama, 1991),
which are less likely to provide (and may even reduce) resilience
to incivility’s impact. The current study is one of the first to
investigate culturally specific protective mechanisms within the
domain of occupational stress and in relation to work-related
outcomes such as job satisfaction, Further, these findings extend
the workplace incivility literature by providing evidence that em- Figure 6. Interaction of incivility and vertical individualism on burnout.
214 WELBOURNE, GANGADHARAN, AND SARIOL

finding that individualistic values (specifically, HI) moderated the & Gelfand, 1998). Additionally, Chen and colleagues (1997) pro-
relationship between incivility and job satisfaction, whereas col- pose that horizontal dimensions are more relevant to explaining
lectivistic values did not. Given that past research (e.g., Case & interpersonal phenomenon (e.g., incivility5), whereas vertical di-
Robinson, 2003; Garcia Coll et al., 2000; Menselson et al., 2008; mensions are more appropriate for research on intergroup phenom-
Palloni & Morenoff, 2001; Umaña-Taylor et al., 2011) has pri- enon; this may also account for the differential effects for hori-
marily focused on cultural resilience in relation to physical and zontal and vertical dimensions found here. More broadly, this
mental health impacts, we suggest that researchers further inves- pattern of results underscores the importance of examining vertical
tigate the degree to which protective benefits of HC generalize to and horizontal dimensions of individualism and collectivism sep-
work outcomes. arately, as argued by previous researchers.
VC values emphasize acceptance of status differences, as well
as compliance and sacrifice to one’s ingroup (Shavitt et al., 2006; Practical Implications
Singelis et al., 1995). VC has been linked conceptually and em-
pirically to the cultural dimension of familism (Schwartz, 2007; Our results suggest that Hispanic males and non-Hispanic fe-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Triandis & Gelfand, 1998), which has been shown to provide males were especially likely to experience incivility at their jobs.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

resilience in response to stressors. Contrary to prediction, VC Thus, we encourage managers to be aware that members of these
values did not weaken the impact of incivility on burnout or job groups may be particularly likely to be targeted by incivility and to
satisfaction. One reason may be that whereas we based our hy- consider appropriate interventions at both the individual and or-
potheses on the protective benefits associated with familism, many ganizational levels. Given the differential effects between the
studies have operationalized familism in terms of family warmth groups examined here, we suggest that discussions of culture,
and support (e.g., Gil-Rivas et al., 2003; Holleran & Waller, 2003; ethnicity, and gender may enhance the effectiveness of employee
Umaña-Taylor et al., 2011). In contrast, VC emphasizes elements trainings or interventions designed to curb incivility. Our findings
of duty and sacrifice (e.g., to family). These particular components also demonstrate that employees with different cultural values may
of familism may not provide the same type of protection when vary in their vulnerability to incivility. Specifically, employees
facing stressors. Alternatively, the protective influence of family- from individualistic cultures who hold values associated with
related cultural values may not extend to the subtle workplace self-reliance (HI) may be more susceptible to the negative impacts
stressors examined here. For instance, Lim and Lee (2011) found of incivility on their job attitudes and wellbeing. Awareness of this
that perceived family support did not buffer Singaporean employ- will allow employers to be more attuned to the needs of employees
ees against the impact of incivility, but instead appeared to exac- whose cultural values may place them at greater risk for these
erbate its negative consequences for employees. impacts. In particular, organizations might consider providing ad-
Individuals with strong HI values experienced a greater associ- ditional support networks for these employees, given that their
ation between incivility, burnout, and reduced job satisfaction. values of self-reliance might be isolating for them if they experi-
Consistent with our predictions, this suggests that employees who ence incivility. Additionally, the finding that individuals with
endorse HI values, which emphasize self-reliance and indepen- strong HC values experienced less burnout when faced with inci-
dence (Singelis et al., 1995), are more vulnerable to the negative vility suggests that organizations focus on fostering employee
strains associated with workplace incivility. We suggest that this is values that emphasize sociability and interconnection with others.
attributable to the combination of experiencing a potentially iso-
lating phenomenon [incivility] (Vickers, 2006) while at the same Limitations and Future Directions
time holding cultural values that may place one at risk for greater
social isolation (Singelis et al., 1995). Future research might test Although this study provides initial insights into how cultural
more directly whether individuals with HI values are less likely to values and ethnicity relate to incivility, it has limitations. Cross-
obtain social support when facing incivility. sectional data limit our ability to infer causality in the relationships
Interestingly, our analyses show a nonsignificant trend (p ⫽ .06) examined here. Additionally, the use of single-source, self-report
of VI values, which emphasize status and competition (Singelis et methodology increases the threat of common method variance.
al., 1995), buffering the impact of incivility on burnout. Why However, because incivility and cultural values are experienced
might individuals who value status and competition be less likely directly by the respondent and not always observable by others, we
to experience burnout when targeted by incivility at work? One believe they are most appropriately assessed through self-report,
possibility is that they may be more accepting of incivility, if they rather than by more objective measures or observer ratings. Ad-
see uncivil acts as simply part of a competitive playing field at ditionally, because interaction effects are less likely to be artifacts
work, or a way to get ahead. Although the current study focuses of common method variance (Evans, 1985; Siemsen, Roth, &
specifically on subtle workplace deviance, we encourage future Oliveira, 2010), we suggest that the moderating effects of ethnicity
work to further explore the potential for protective cultural mech- and culture found here cannot be fully accounted for in this way.
anisms within individualistic cultures. Our sample focused solely on university employees and was
Overall, our findings suggest that vertical and horizontal dimen- limited to a single workplace. However, because academia has
sions of individualism and collectivism relate uniquely to incivility been identified as an occupation in which employees are likely to
and its associated outcomes. In general, we found that horizontal encounter incivility (Morrissette, 2001), we suggest that this was
dimensions were most crucial to buffering or strengthening the
relationship between incivility and work and health outcomes; we 5
Although incivility may be selectively targeted based on group mem-
attribute this to the fact that these cultural values relate more bership, we note here that more broadly, incivility is considered an inter-
directly to the presence or absence of support networks (Triandis personal stressor.
CULTURE AND WORKPLACE INCIVILITY 215

an appropriate setting for our research. Further, although our work Berdahl, J. L., & Moore, C. (2006). Workplace harassment: Double jeop-
aimed to increase understanding of how Hispanic and white, ardy for minority women. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 426 – 436.
non-Hispanic employees differ in their experience of incivility, our http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.91.2.426
sample lacked representation of other ethnic groups. As noted Brayfield, A. H., & Rothe, H. F. (1951). An index of job satisfaction.
earlier, values emphasizing relationships and family are not unique Journal of Applied Psychology, 35, 307–311. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
to persons of Hispanic origin; in particular, similar values emerge h0055617
Buchanan, N. T., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (2008). Effects of racial and sexual
in African and Asian cultures (Schwartz et al., 2010). Thus, it will
harassment on work and the psychological well-being of African Amer-
be informative to examine the buffering effects of cultural values
ican women. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 13, 137–151.
within other ethnic groups. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1076-8998.13.2.137
It is possible that the location in which we conducted our Case, M. H., & Robinson, W. L. (2003). Interventions with ethnic minority
research influenced our findings. In contrast to other locations populations: The legacy and promise of community psychology. In G.
within the United States, individuals of Hispanic ethnicity are a Bernal, J. E. Trimble, A. K. Burlew, & F. T. L. Leong (Eds.), Handbook
numeric majority (rather than a minority) in the border region in of racial and ethnic minority psychology (pp. 573–590). Thousand Oaks,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

which we collected our data (www.census.gov). This may have CA: Sage. http://dx.doi.org/10.4135/9781412976008.n29
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

affected intergroup relationships, potentially reducing selective Chen, C., Meindl, J., & Hunt, R. (1997). Testing the effects of vertical and
incivility targeted at Hispanic employees. However, because in- horizontal collectivism: A study of reward allocation preferences in
creased incivility was reported by male Hispanic employees (in China. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 28, 44 –70. http://dx.doi
spite of being a majority in the population), we suggest that the .org/10.1177/0022022197281003
research context cannot fully account for these results. Being in a Chun, C., Moos, R., & Cronkite, R. (2009). Culture: A fundamental context
location where the majority of the population is Hispanic may also for the stress and coping paradigm. In P. Wong & L. Wong (Eds.),
Handbook of multicultural perspectives on stress and coping (Interna-
strengthen the degree to which Hispanic employees hold values
tional and Cultural Psychology) (pp. 29 –53). New York, NY: Springer.
that are traditionally emphasized within their culture. In this case,
Cianni, M., & Romberger, B. (1995). Perceived racial, gender, and ethnic
the effects of cultural resilience that are observed within this
differences in access to developmental experiences. Group & Organi-
sample may be stronger than in another research context. Finally, zation Management, 20, 440 – 459. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
because of proximity to Mexico, it is likely that a majority of our 1059601195204004
Hispanic participants were of Mexican background (although we Clark, R., Anderson, N. B., Clark, V. R., & Williams, D. R. (1999). Racism
did not directly assess country of origin). As a result, our research as a stressor for African Americans. A biopsychosocial model. American
does not acknowledge the variability that may occur across differ- Psychologist, 54, 805– 816. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.10
ent Hispanic subcultures. We suggest that future research take a .805
more nuanced approach to examine whether employees coming Cortina, L. M. (2004). Hispanic perspectives on sexual harassment and
from different Hispanic subcultures (e.g., Central American, Cu- social support. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 570 –
ban, etc.) have differential experiences of workplace incivility. 584. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0146167203262854
Cortina, L. (2008). Unseen injustice: Incivility as modern discrimination in
organizations. The Academy of Management Review, 33, 55–75. http://
Conclusion dx.doi.org/10.5465/AMR.2008.27745097
Cortina, L., Kabat-Farr, D., Leskinen, E., Huerta, M., & Magley, V.
In spite of increased diversity in the workforce, there has been
(2013). Selective incivility as modern discrimination in organiza-
sparse attention to ethnicity and cultural values in the experience tions. Journal of Management, 39, 1579 –1605. http://dx.doi.org/
of workplace incivility. The current study sheds light on how 10.1177/0149206311418835
ethnicity and cultural values relate to the incidence of workplace Cortina, L. M., Lonsway, K. L., Magley, V. J., Freeman, L. V., Col-
incivility and its impact on work attitudes and wellbeing. These linsworth, L. L., Hunter, M., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (2002). What’s gender
findings highlight the value in examining workplace incivility got to do with it? Incivility in the federal courts. Law & Social Inquiry,
through a broader cultural lens in order to better understand 27, 235–270. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1747-4469.2002.tb00804.x
employees’ vulnerability and resilience to workplace incivility. Cortina, L. M., & Magley, V. J. (2009). Patterns and profiles of response
to incivility in the workplace. Journal of Occupational Health Psychol-
ogy, 14, 272–288. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0014934
References Cortina, L. M., Magley, V. J., Williams, J. H., & Langhout, R. D. (2001).
Aiken, L., & West, S. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting Incivility in the workplace: Incidence and impact. Journal of Occupa-
interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. tional Health Psychology, 6, 64 – 80. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1076-
Andersson, L., & Pearson, C. (1999). Tit for tat? The spiraling effect of 8998.6.1.64
incivility in the workplace. Academy of Management Review, 24, 425– Cuéllar, I., Arnold, B., & Gonzalez, G. (1995). Cognitive referents of accul-
471. turation: Assessment of cultural constructs in Mexican Americans. Journal
Barnum, P., Liden, R. C., & DiTomaso, N. (1995). Double jeopardy for of Community Psychology, 23, 339 –356. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1520-
women and minorities: Pay differences with age. Academy of Manage- 6629(199510)23:4⬍339::AID-JCOP2290230406⬎3.0.CO;2-7
ment Journal, 38, 863– 880. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/256749 Cuéllar, I., Arnold, B., & Maldonado, R. (1995). Acculturation Rating
Bauman, S. (2005). The reliability and validity of the Brief Acculturation Scale for Mexican Americans-II: A revision of the original ARSMA
Rating Scale for Mexican Americans-II for children and adolescents. scale. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 17, 275–304. http://dx
Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 27, 426 – 441. http://dx.doi .doi.org/10.1177/07399863950173001
.org/10.1177/0739986305281423 Cuellar, I., Harris, L., & Jasso, R. (1980). An acculturation scale for
Beal, F. M. (1970). Double jeopardy: To be black and female. Detroit, MI: Mexican American normal and clinical populations. Hispanic Journal of
Radical Education Project. Behavioral Sciences, 2, 199 –217.
216 WELBOURNE, GANGADHARAN, AND SARIOL

Dreher, G. F., & Cox, T. H., Jr. (1996). Race, gender, and opportunity: A Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 355–366. http://dx.doi.org/
study of compensation attainment and the establishment of mentoring 10.1037/0022-006X.76.3.355
relationships. Journal of Applied Psychology, 81, 297–308. http://dx.doi Miner, K., Settles, I., Pratt-Hyatt, J., & Brady, C. (2012). Experiencing
.org/10.1037/0021-9010.81.3.297 incivility in organizations: The buffering effects of emotional and orga-
Epstein, C. F. (1973). Black and female: The double whammy. Psychology nizational support. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 42, 340 –372.
Today, 3, 57– 61, 89. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2011.00891.x
Evans, D. S. (1985). Entrepreneurial Choice and Success. Washington, Miner-Rubino, K., & Reed, W. (2010). Testing a moderated mediational
DC: U. S. Small Business Administration. model of workplace incivility: The roles of organizational trust and
Farley, T., Galves, A., Dickinson, L. M., & Perez, M. J. (2005). Stress, group regard. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 40, 3148 –3168.
coping, and health: A comparison of Mexican immigrants, Mexican- http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00695.x
Americans, and non-Hispanic whites. Journal of Immigrant Health, 7, Montoro-Rodriguez, J., & Gallagher-Thompson, D. (2009). The role of
213–220. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10903-005-3678-5 resources and appraisals in predicting burden among Latina and non-
Fletcher, D., & Sarkar, M. (2013). Psychological resilience - A review and Hispanic white female caregivers: A test of an expanded socio-cultural
critique of definitions, concepts and theory. European Psychologist, 18, model of stress and coping. Aging & Mental Health, 13, 648 – 658.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

12–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000124 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13607860802534658


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Gallo, L. C., Penedo, F. J., Espinosa de los Monteros, K., & Arguelles, W. Morrissette, P. J. (2001). Reducing incivility in the university/college
(2009). Resiliency in the face of disadvantage: Do Hispanic cultural classroom. IEJLL: International Electronic Journal for Leadership in
characteristics protect health outcomes? Journal of Personality, 77, Learning, 5.
1707–1746. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00598.x Nelson, M., & Shavitt, S. (2002). Horizontal and vertical individualism and
Garcia Coll, C., Akerman, A., & Cicchetti, D. (2000). Cultural influences achievement values: A multimethod examination of Denmark and the
on developmental processes and outcomes: Implications for the study of United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33, 439 – 458.
development and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022102033005001
12, 333–356. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400003059 Organista, P. B., Organista, K. C., & Kurasaki, K. (2003). The relationship
Gil-Rivas, V., Greenberger, E., Chen, C., & Montero y López-Lena, M. between acculturation and ethnic minority health. In K. M. Chun, P. B.
(2003). Understanding depressed mood in the context of a family- Organista, & G. Marin (Eds.), Acculturation: Advances in theory, mea-
oriented culture. Adolescence, 38, 93–109. surement and applied research (pp. 139 –161). Washington, DC: Amer-
Gomez. (2003). The relationship between acculturation, individualism/ ican Psychological Association. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/10472-010
collectivism, and job attribute preferences for Hispanic MBAs. Journal Palloni, A., & Morenoff, J. D. (2001). Interpreting the paradoxical in the
of Management Studies, 40, 1089 –1105. hispanic paradox: Demographic and epidemiologic approaches. Annals
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences: International differences in of the New York Academy of Sciences, 954, 140 –174. http://dx.doi.org/
work-related values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2001.tb02751.x
Holleran, L. K., & Waller, M. A. (2003). Sources of resilience among Pearson, C., Andersson, L., & Porath, C. (2000). Assessing and attacking
Chicano/a youth: Forging identities in the borderlands. Child & Adoles- workplace incivility. Organizational Dynamics, 29, 123–137. http://dx
cent Social Work Journal, 20, 335–350. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/A: .doi.org/10.1016/S0090-2616(00)00019-X
1026043828866 Pearson, C., Andersson, L., & Wegner, J. (2001). When workers flout
James, K. (1994). Social identity, work stress, and minority workers’ convention: A study of workplace incivility. Human Relations, 54,
health. In G. P. Keita & J. J. Hurrell (Eds.), Job stress in a changing 1387–1419. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00187267015411001
workforce: Investigating gender, diversity and family issues (pp. 127– Pearson, C., & Porath, C. (2005). On the nature, consequences and reme-
145). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. dies of workplace incivility: No time for “nice”? Think again. The
Lim, S., Cortina, L. M., & Magley, V. J. (2008). Personal and workgroup Academy of Management Executive, 19, 7–18.
incivility: Impact on work and health outcomes. Journal of Applied Pearson, C., & Porath, C. (2009). The cost of bad behavior: How incivility
Psychology, 93, 95–107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.93.1.95 ruins your business- and what you can do about it. New York, NY:
Lim, S., & Lee, A. (2011). Work and nonwork outcomes of workplace Portfolio.
incivility: Does family support help? Journal of Occupational Health Penney, L., & Spector, P. (2005). Job stress, incivility, and counterproduc-
Psychology, 16, 95–111. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021726 tive work behavior (CWB): The moderating role of negative affectivity.
Liu, W., Chi, S., Friedman, R., & Tsai, M. (2009). Explaining incivility in Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 777–796. http://dx.doi.org/
the workplace: The effects of personality and culture. Negotiation and 10.1002/job.336
Conflict Management Research, 2, 164 –184. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ Pines, A., & Aronson, E. (1988). Career burnout: Causes and cures. New
j.1750-4716.2009.00035.x York, NY: Free Press.
Luthar, S. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2000). The construct of resilience: Impli- Plant, E. A., & Sachs-Ericsson, N. (2004). Racial and ethnic differences in
cations for interventions and social policies. Development and Psycho- depression: The roles of social support and meeting basic needs. Journal
pathology, 12, 857– 885. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400004156 of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 41–52. http://dx.doi.org/
Marin, G., & Marin, B. V. (1991). Research with Hispanic populations. 10.1037/0022-006X.72.1.41
Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Raver, J. L., & Nishii, L. H. (2010). Once, twice, or three times as harmful?
Marin, G., & Triandis, H. (1985). Allocentrism as an important character- Ethnic harassment, gender harassment, and generalized workplace ha-
istic of the behavior of Latin Americans and Hispanics. In R. Diaz- rassment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 236 –254. http://dx.doi
Guerrero (Ed.), Cross-cultural and national studies of social psychology .org/10.1037/a0018377
(pp. 85–114). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North-Holland. Richardson, G. E. (2002). The metatheory of resilience and resiliency.
Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58, 307–321. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224 – jclp.10020
253. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224 Rodriguez-Calcagno, M., & Brewer, E. (2005). Job stress among Hispanic
Menselson, T., Rehkopf, D. H., & Kubzansky, L. D. (2008). Depression professionals. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 27, 504 –516.
among Latinos in the United States: A meta-analytic review. Journal of http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0739986305280691
CULTURE AND WORKPLACE INCIVILITY 217

Romero, E. (2004). Latin American leadership: El Patron and El Lider Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectivism. New York, NY:
moderno. Cross Cultural Management, 11, 25–37. http://dx.doi.org/ Simon & Schuster.
10.1108/13527600410797828 Triandis, H. C., Botempo, R., Betancourt, H., Bond, M., Leung, K., Bernes,
Sabogal, F., Marin, G., Otero-Sabogal, R., Marin, B. V., & Perez-Stable, A., . . . de Montmollin, G. (1986). The measurement of etic and emic
E. J. (1987). Hispanic familism and acculturation: What changes and aspects of individualism and collectivism across cultures. Australian
what doesn’t? Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 9, 397– 412. Journal of Psychology, 38, 257–267. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/07399863870094003 00049538608259013
Schwartz, S. J. (2007). The applicability of familism to diverse ethnic Triandis, H., Chan, D., Bhawuk, D., Iwao, S., & Sinha, J. (1995).
groups: A preliminary study. The Journal of Social Psychology, 147, Multimethod probes of allocentrism and idiocentrism. International
101–118. http://dx.doi.org/10.3200/SOCP.147.2.101-118 Journal of Psychology, 30, 461– 480. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
Schwartz, S. J., Weisskirch, R. S., Hurley, E. A., Zamboanga, B. L., Park, I. J., 00207599508246580
Kim, S. Y., . . . Greene, A. D. (2010). Communalism, familism, and filial Triandis, H. C., Chen, X. P., & Chan, D. K. S. (1998). Scenarios for the
piety: Are they birds of a collectivist feather? Cultural Diversity and Ethnic measurement of collectivism and individualism. Journal of Cross-
Minority Psychology, 16, 548 –560. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021370 Cultural Psychology, 29, 275–289. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Shavitt, S., Lalwani, A., Zhang, J., & Torelli, C. (2006). The horizontal/ 0022022198292001
vertical distinction in cross-cultural consumer research. Journal of Triandis, H., & Gelfand, M. (1998). Converging measurement of horizon-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Consumer Psychology, 16, 325–342. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/ tal and vertical individualism and collectivism. Journal of Personality
s15327663jcp1604_3 and Social Psychology, 74, 118 –128. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-
Siemsen, E., Roth, A., & Oliveira, P. (2010). Common method bias in 3514.74.1.118
regression models with linear, quadratic, and interaction effects. Orga- Tugade, M. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Resilient individuals use
nizational Research Methods, 13, 456 – 476. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ positive emotions to bounce back from negative emotional experiences.
1094428109351241 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 320 –333. http://dx
Singelis, T., Triandis, H., Bhawuk, D., & Gelfand, M. (1995). Horizontal .doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.2.320
and vertical dimensions of individualism and collectivism: A theoretical Umaña-Taylor, A. J., Updegraff, K. A., & Gonzales-Backen, M. A. (2011).
and measurement refinement. Cross-Cultural Research: The Journal of Mexican-origin adolescent mothers’ stressors and psychosocial func-
Comparative Social Science, 29, 240 –275. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ tioning: Examining ethnic identity affirmation and familism as moder-
106939719502900302 ators. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 140 –157. http://dx.doi.org/
Sivadas, E., Bruvold, N., & Nelson, M. (2008). A reduced version of the 10.1007/s10964-010-9511-z
horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism scale: A four- U.S. Census Bureau. (2010). Overview of race and hispanic origin: 2010.
country assessment. Journal of Business Research, 61, 201–210. http:// Retrieved from http://www.census.gov/prod/cen2010/briefs/c2010br-02
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2007.06.016 .pdf
Stone-Romero, E. F., Stone, D. L., & Salas, E. (2003). The influence of Van Emmerik, H. (2002). Gender differences in the effects of coping
culture on role conceptions and role behavior in organisations. Applied assistance on the reduction of burnout in academic staff. Work & Stress,
Psychology, 52, 328 –362. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1464-0597.00139 16, 251–263. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/0267837021000034593
Thomson, M. D., & Hoffman-Goetz, L. (2009). Defining and measuring Vickers, M. (2006). Writing what’s relevant: Workplace incivility in public
acculturation: A systematic review of public health studies with His- administration- A wolf in sheep’s clothing. Administrative Theory &
panic populations in the United States. Social Science & Medicine, 69, Praxis, 28, 69 – 88.
983–991. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.05.011
Toossi, M. (2012). Labor force projections to 2020: A more slowly
growing workforce. Monthly Labor Review, 43– 64. Received February 6, 2014
Triandis, H. C. (1994). Culture and social behavior. New York, NY: Revision received September 8, 2014
McGraw-Hill Book Company. Accepted September 11, 2014 䡲

E-Mail Notification of Your Latest Issue Online!


Would you like to know when the next issue of your favorite APA journal will be available
online? This service is now available to you. Sign up at http://notify.apa.org/ and you will be
notified by e-mail when issues of interest to you become available!

View publication stats

You might also like