You are on page 1of 25

Accounting in Europe

ISSN: (Print) (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/raie20

The Effects of Key Audit Matters on the Auditor’s


Report’s Communicative Value: Experimental
Evidence from Investment Professionals and Non-
professional Investors

Annette Köhler, Nicole Ratzinger-Sakel & Jochen Theis

To cite this article: Annette Köhler, Nicole Ratzinger-Sakel & Jochen Theis (2020) The Effects of
Key Audit Matters on the Auditor’s Report’s Communicative Value: Experimental Evidence from
Investment Professionals and Non-professional Investors, Accounting in Europe, 17:2, 105-128,
DOI: 10.1080/17449480.2020.1726420

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/17449480.2020.1726420

Published online: 25 Feb 2020.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 3073

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 35 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=raie20
Accounting in Europe, 2020
Vol. 17, No. 2, 105–128, https://doi.org/10.1080/17449480.2020.1726420

The Effects of Key Audit Matters on the


Auditor’s Report’s Communicative Value:
Experimental Evidence from Investment
Professionals and Non-professional
Investors‡

ANNETTE KÖHLER*, NICOLE RATZINGER-SAKEL** and


JOCHEN THEIS †
*University of Duisburg-Essen, Duisburg, Germany, **University of Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany and †University
of Southern Denmark, Kolding, Denmark

A BSTRACT We investigate the effect of key audit matters (KAM) in the auditor’s report as required by the
new ISA 701. We consider investment professionals and non-professional investors in our experiments, in
which we test the communicative value of a KAM section relating to goodwill impairment. Our main results
show that in the condition in which the KAM section suggests that already small changes in the key
assumptions could eventually lead to a goodwill impairment (KAM negative condition), investment
professionals assess the economic situation of the company to be significantly better as compared to the
condition in which the KAM section suggests that only large changes in the key assumptions could
eventually lead to a goodwill impairment (KAM positive condition). In the additional analysis with non-
professional investors, we find that a KAM section has no communicative value, implying that non-
professional investors have difficulties with processing the information conveyed with KAM.

Keywords: communicative value; improved auditor’s report; key audit matters; trust model; investment
professionals

JEL Classification: M42

1. Introduction
A promising way to enhance the communicative value of the auditor’s report is to provide users
with additional insights into the auditor’s work by communicating so-called key audit matters
(KAM), i.e. ‘matters that, in the auditor’s professional judgment, were of most significance in
the audit […]’ (IAASB, 2015b, paragraph 11, a) in a separate section of the report. This
concept is reflected by the new International Standard on Auditing ISA 701 Communicating

*Correspondence address: Jochen Theis, University of Southern Denmark, Kolding Campus, Kolding 6000, Denmark

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

© 2020 European Accounting Association


106 A. Köhler et al.

Key Audit Matters in the Independent Auditor’s Report, which was published in January 2015 and
is effective for audits of financial statements ending on or after December 15, 2016.1 Referring to
these developments, the objective of this study is to experimentally examine the potential effect of
a separate KAM section in the auditor’s report as required by ISA 701 on the auditor’s report’s
communicative value for users.
Prior studies investigating the effect of a KAM or critical audit matters (CAM) section respect-
ively on the auditor’s report’s communicative value for users either look at aggregated capital
market reactions, or individual investors’ decisions or assessments.2 Existing empirical evidence
on aggregated market reactions rather suggests that investors do not find the expanded infor-
mation in the auditor’s report incrementally informative (Bédard, Gonthier-Besacier, & Schatt,
2014; Lennox, Schmidt, & Thompson, 2016). However, in a more nuanced analysis, Bédard,
Gonthier-Besacier, and Schatt (2015) find that the first-time disclosure of justifications of assess-
ments (JOA; conceptually equivalent to KAM/CAM) in French auditor’s reports is associated
with reduced information asymmetry for smaller firms. For individual investors’ decisions, Chris-
tensen, Glover, and Wolfe’s (2014) experiment documents that investors who receive a CAM
section are more likely to change their investment decision than are investors who receive a stan-
dard (i.e. old) audit report or investors who receive the same CAM section information in a man-
agement’s footnote. However, the effect of a CAM section is reduced when offering a resolution
paragraph. In an overall perspective, existing studies draw a rather inconclusive picture of the
potential effect of a separate KAM section on the auditor’s report’s communicative value for
users, and further research in this respect seems fruitful. Furthermore, prior studies have not
yet looked at the potential effect of different tendencies (positive vs. negative) of KAM on the
communicative value.
Consequently, in our study, we examine the potential effect of the additional information about
KAM in the auditor’s report by considering different tendencies of KAM on the communicative
value for investment professionals and non-professional investors. Our findings are useful for
standard setters and auditors by highlighting the importance of carefully considering how differ-
ent types of users perceive the message conveyed with the KAM section. This aspect is highly
relevant because the identification and communication of KAM are subject to the auditor’s pro-
fessional judgment.
To achieve our research purposes, we use a between-subjects experimental design among 89
investment professionals from Germany (82.28 percent), US, UK or Canada (10.12 percent),
and other countries around the world (7.60 percent). In additional analysis, we run a similar exper-
iment with 69 non-professional investors. We capture the auditor’s report’s communicative value
by two dimensions, the potential to change the user’s assessment of the company’s economic situ-
ation and the user’s confidence in making that assessment. With these two dimensions, we capture
the main assessments within investment professionals’ and non-professional investors’ analyses
being elements of investment decisions. We therefore assume that the communicative value of the
auditor’s report changes if user’s assessments in either of these two dimensions change.
In our experiments, we manipulate the auditor’s report by including a KAM section that relates
to goodwill impairment, whereby we refer to the KAM goodwill example in the illustrative audi-
tor’s report outlined by the IAASB in its exposure draft Reporting on Audited Financial State-
ments: Proposed New and Revised International Standards on Auditing (ISAs) (IAASB, 2013).
A review of the experience with the new auditor reporting requirements in the UK reveals that
goodwill impairment related risks rank number one among risks communicated as KAM
(FRC, 2016, p. 15).
We later argue in detail how the specific informational content of the KAM section affects, inter
alia, the levels of perceived openness and fairness, and potentially competence, which constitute
factors of perceived trustworthiness in the model of trust (see Figure 1). This in turn has the
Accounting in Europe 107

Figure 1. Model of trust – line of argument.

potential to affect our results as we differentiate between two content-related3 manipulations: (1)
A KAM section suggesting that already small changes in the key assumptions could eventually
lead to a goodwill impairment. We refer to this manipulation as a KAM section with a negative
tendency regarding the company’s economic situation, or, in short, KAM negative. (2) A KAM
section suggesting that only large changes in the key assumptions could eventually lead to a
goodwill impairment. We refer to this manipulation as a KAM section with a positive tendency
regarding the company’s economic situation, or, in short, KAM positive. The control group is
provided with the former ISA 700 auditor’s report (without separate KAM section).4 In this
regard, it is important to highlight that ISAs are silent on specific tendencies of KAM sections.
Hence, the communication of KAM does not necessarily imply a negative tendency. Correspond-
ingly, an in-depth analysis of the FRC (2015) review of the experience with the extended auditor’s
report reveals that the sections on goodwill impairment and other risks within the auditor’s reports
are not generally conveying negative tendencies (see for example section on risks due to capital
restructuring in the auditor’s report for New World Resources plc; New World Resources, 2014),
but also positive tendencies (see for example section on goodwill impairment risks in the auditor’s
reports for Pearson plc, Greggs plc, and GlaxoSmithKline plc; GlaxoSmithKline, 2014; Greggs,
2014; Pearson, 2014).
We base our hypotheses on the implications of trust literature and a model of trust, which we
propose to apply in our study in order to explain the potential effect of a KAM section in the audi-
tor’s report on the communicative value for users. In line with this theoretical framework, in our
main experiment, we find that in the KAM negative condition, investment professionals assess the
economic situation of the company to be significantly better as compared to the KAM positive
condition. Correspondingly, the descriptive results indicate that investment professionals’ confi-
dence in their assessment is higher in the KAM negative condition. Thus, based on a model of
trust, we conclude that the specific informational content of the KAM section affects, inter
alia, the levels of perceived openness and fairness, and potentially competence (i.e. factors of per-
ceived trustworthiness), eventually leading to unequal levels of trust which the investment pro-
fessionals associate with the auditor’s report. This divergence in perceived trustworthiness of
the auditor’s report will then also alter the perceived trustworthiness of the financial statements
and hence, investment professional’s assessments of the company’s economic situation and
108 A. Köhler et al.

investment professional’s confidence in making that decision. These results suggest that from an
investment professionals’ perspective, the KAM section with a positive tendency is rather ill-per-
ceived as a kind of appeasement given the challenges the auditor had faced during the audit
(implying, inter alia, lower levels of perceived openness and fairness), while the KAM section
with a negative tendency is rather well-perceived as a helpful signal that draws the investment
professionals’ attention to issues that they had not been aware of before (implying, inter alia,
higher levels of perceived openness and fairness and possibly also competence). Thus, neither
preparers nor audit committees or auditors need to fear that the disclosure of critical entity-
related information leads to negative implications; in contrast, investment professionals value
this information positively.
Contrary to our main analysis, our additional analysis with non-professional investors suggests
that a KAM section has no communicative value. In particular, we do not find significant differ-
ences for the two dimensions of communicative value or any other variable captured in our exper-
iment between groups. Hence, we conclude that non-professional investors have difficulties with
processing the information conveyed with a KAM section.
This study contributes to the auditor reporting literature in at least three ways. First, by using
two types of users, we show that investment professionals and non-professional investors react
differently to KAM sections embedded in a highly complex, and thus fairly realistic case. In
this regard, our findings seem helpful in explaining the mixed results of prior literature concerning
the effects of KAM. Second, to the best of our knowledge, this is the first study that examines the
potential effect of the additional information about KAM in the auditor’s report by considering
different tendencies of KAM on the communicative value for investment professionals and
non-professional investors. Third, by referring to a model of trust that theoretically explains
why different tendencies of KAM potentially lead to different investment professionals’ assess-
ments of aspects related to the communicative value, we employ an innovative approach in the
auditor reporting literature not used thus far.
The remainder of this study is structured as follows. After presenting previous research on the
matter we develop our hypotheses based on theoretical deliberations in section 2. Section 3
describes the main experimental design as well as the participating investment professionals.
Section 4 reports the results of the main experiment as well as robustness checks. Section 5
describes the additional analysis with non-professional investors and reports these results, and
section 6 concludes and outlines limitations of our study.

2. Prior Studies and Hypotheses


2.1. Overview over Prior Studies
There is some research evidence regarding the effect of a KAM or critical audit matters (CAM)
section respectively within the five following domains:5 Auditor liability (e.g. Backof, Bowlin, &
Goodson, 2014; Brasel, Doxey, Grenier, & Reffett, 2016; Brown, Majors, & Peecher, 2015;
Gimbar, Hansen, & Ozlanski, 2016a; Kachelmeier, Rimkus, Schmidt, & Valentine, 2019;
Kachelmeier, Schmidt, & Valentine, 2015; Vinson, Robertson, & Cockrell, 2019), auditor judg-
ment (e.g. Asbahr & Ruhnke, 2019; Ratzinger-Sakel & Theis, 2019), managerial decision making
(e.g. Bentley, Lambert, & Wang, 2018; Klueber, Gold, & Pott, 2018), aggregated capital market
reactions (e.g. Bédard et al., 2014, 2015; Lennox et al., 2016), and individual investors’ decisions
or assessments (e.g. Christensen et al., 2014; Rapley, Robertson, & Smith, 2018; Sirois, Bédard,
& Bera, 2018).
The bulk of the preliminary behavioral evidence concerning the studies on auditor liability
suggests that CAM sections regarding subsequent litigation either reduce or do not influence
Accounting in Europe 109

auditor liability (Gimbar, Hansen, & Ozlanski, 2016b). However, other studies find that a CAM
can increase auditor liability under specific conditions. For example, Gimbar et al. (2016a) argue
that the impact of a CAM on auditor liability actually varies as a function of accounting standard
precision. Relatedly, Vinson et al. (2019) find that the subsequent removal of a CAM increases
auditor liability. Studies investigating the impact of a KAM section on auditor judgment
suggest that the new reporting requirement might come at the expense of auditor judgment per-
formance (Ratzinger-Sakel & Theis, 2019). Both Ratzinger-Sakel and Theis (2019) and Asbahr
and Ruhnke (2019) basically argue that the disclosure of a KAM can serve as a moral license for
an auditor to acquiesce to the accounting treatment preferred by the client. Another stream of be-
havioral literature investigates the impact of KAM reporting on different aspects of managerial
decision making. For example, Bentley et al. (2018) find that CAM disclosures cause strategic
managerial responses and may encourage risk-increasing activities. In contrast, the study con-
ducted by Klueber et al. (2018) suggests that including a KAM section in the auditor’s report
can function as a beneficial mechanism for enhancing managerial financial reporting quality if
the information precision of the KAM is high. Regarding aggregated market reactions, the empiri-
cal evidence generally shows that investors do not find the expanded information in the auditor’s
report incrementally informative (Bédard et al., 2014; Lennox et al., 2016). Nevertheless, in a
more in-depth analysis, Bédard et al. (2015) find that the first-time disclosure of justifications
of assessments (JOA) in French auditor’s reports is associated with reduced information asymme-
try for smaller firms. With respect to individual investors’ decisions, Christensen et al. (2014)
provide experimental evidence suggesting that investors who receive a CAM section are more
likely to change their investment decision than are investors who receive a standard (i.e. old)
audit report, or investors who receive the same CAM section information in a management’s foot-
note. The authors furthermore argue that the identified effect of a CAM section is reduced when
offering a resolution paragraph. The outlined core implications are mainly confirmed in a related
study conducted by Rapley et al. (2018). Furthermore, using eye-tracking technology, Sirois et al.
(2018) find that KAMs have an attention directing impact and hence affect individual investors’
information acquisition process.

2.2. Hypotheses Development


To capture the communicative value of the auditor’s report for users we refer to two dimensions
which constitute our dependent variables: The potential to change the user’s assessment of the
company’s economic situation, and the user’s confidence in making that assessment. We consider
these two dimensions as they directly reflect the main assessments within investment pro-
fessionals’ and non-professional investors’ analyses being part of investment decisions. We there-
fore assume that the communicative value of the auditor’s report changes if user’s assessments in
either of these two dimensions change. There is limited evidence yet on whether the inclusion of a
separate KAM section in the auditor’s report is linked to the communicative value of the auditor’s
report for users (Christensen et al., 2014). However, this research neither considers the two types
of users constituting the capital market nor the specific tendency of a KAM section. We believe
that both aspects have to be considered in order to understand how different types of users per-
ceive the message conveyed with the KAM section. This is of particular interest for two reasons.
First, the communication of KAM is not standardized, but subject to the circumstances of the
audit and the auditor’s professional judgment. This implies differences among KAM sections,
e.g. in informational content. Second, as we outline in the following, differences in informational
content are likely to alter, inter alia, the levels of perceived openness and fairness, and potentially
competence (i.e. factors of perceived trustworthiness), which in turn has the potential to affect our
110 A. Köhler et al.

results. Consequently, we differentiate between two manipulations of a KAM section relating to


goodwill impairment.
In a first manipulation, we generate a KAM section with a negative tendency regarding the
company’s economic situation by formulating the last sentence as follows: Already small
changes in the key assumptions used could give rise to an impairment of the goodwill balance
in the future; referred to as KAM negative. Literally, if the auditor outlines that already small
changes in the key assumptions could eventually lead to a goodwill impairment, he or she
signals a high risk that an impairment will occur in the future which then would negatively
affect net income and hence, the economic situation of the company.
In a second manipulation, we generate a KAM section with a positive tendency regarding the
company’s economic situation by formulating the last sentence as follows: Only large changes in
the key assumptions used could give rise to an impairment of the goodwill balance in the future;
referred to as KAM positive.6 Hence, if the auditor outlines that only large changes in the key
assumptions could eventually lead to a goodwill impairment, he or she signals a low risk that
an impairment will occur in the future and hence, a low risk that net income and in turn, the econ-
omic situation of the company will be affected. In order to gain access to the investment pro-
fessionals, we were compelled to keep the experimental materials as compact as possible, i.e.
to minimize the time effort for participants. We therefore could not include manipulation
check questions in our main experiment.
In many auditor reporting studies, hypotheses are derived from communications literature and
communication models (e.g. Hasan, Roebuck, & Simnett, 2003), or from a compilation of prior
findings (e.g. Gold, Gronewold, & Pott, 2012). Those studies often argue that changes in wording
of a report affect the addressees’ informational basis and thus alter the respective perceptions.
Based on prior literature and in line with the IAASB’s implicit objective, we expect that the
first dimension of communicative value, users’ assessment of the company’s economic situation,
is affected by the specific informational content of the KAM section.
Intuitively, it seems reasonable to expect the users’ assessment of an entity’s economic situ-
ation to be more negative if the auditor’s report includes a KAM section with a rather negative
tendency as compared to a KAM section with a rather positive tendency. Regarding the
second dimension of communicative value, i.e. the user’s confidence in the assessment of the
company’s economic situation, users should be more confident in their assessment of the com-
pany’s economic situation if the auditor’s report includes a KAM section at all – regardless of
the specific KAM tendency. That is because the informational basis for users’ assessment
becomes larger by the provision of KAM and hence, information asymmetry is reduced in
both conditions. However, the provision of directional predictions based on changes of
wording is challenging, because the transformation of words into a message is an extremely
complex cognitive process (see for example, Fiske, 1990). Based on the implications of trust lit-
erature and a model of trust, which we propose to apply in our study in order to explain the poten-
tial effect of a KAM section in the auditor’s report on the communicative value for users,
expectations change. Lewicki, McAllister, and Bies (1998) outline that the understanding why
people trust and how trust shapes (social) relations has been a major field of research for disci-
plines like psychology, sociology, political science, anthropology and economics (see also
Axelrod, 1984; Barber, 1983; Ekeh, 1974; Gambetta, 1988; Worchel, 1979). Furthermore, it is
argued that trust is essential for a healthy personality (e. g., Shaver & Hazan, 1994), provides
a foundation for interpersonal relationships as well as for cooperation (e. g., Rempel, Holmes,
& Zanna, 1985), and constitutes the basis for stability in social institutions and markets
(e. g. Williamson, 1974). Definitions of trust are manifold. Earlier definitions focus on individ-
uals’ confidence in other person’s intentions and motives (Mellinger, 1956; Read, 1962), while
Accounting in Europe 111

more recent research focuses on behavior (Hosmer, 1995; Lewicki et al., 1998). Mayer, Davis,
and Schoorman (1995, p. 712) define trust as the

willingness of a party to be vulnerable to the actions of another party based on the expec-
tation that the other will perform a particular action important to the trustor, irrespective of
the ability to monitor or control that other party.

Similarly, Currall and Judge (1995, p. 151) define trust as ‘an individual’s behavioral reliance on
another person under a condition of risk’. Johnson-George and Swap (1982, p. 1306) suggest that
‘willingness to take risks may be one of the few characteristics common to all trust situations’ and
hence, that ‘there is something of importance to be lost’ (Mayer et al., 1995, p. 712).
An own body of research focuses on understanding and measuring trust (e.g., Cummings & Bro-
miley, 1996; Currall & Judge, 1995; Kramer, 1999). Butler (1991) derives a conditions of trust
inventory based on a compilation of factors utilized in prior studies (see also Mishra, 1996;
Sitkin & Roth, 1993, who use very similar factors in their studies). Drawing from those
studies, Mayer et al. (1995) propose a model of (organizational) trust. Based on their model,
the authors make three propositions all of which are of importance for our study (see Mayer
et al., 1995, for the following argumentation). First, it is argued that trust for a trustee will be a
function of the trustee’s perceived ability, benevolence, and integrity (and also of the trustor’s
propensity to trust, which is of minor relevance for our approach). Ability captures the trustee’s
competence, benevolence his or her loyalty, openness, receptivity and availability, and integrity
aspects like his or her discreetness and fairness. Thereby, each factor captures unique elements of
trustworthiness. Second, it is proposed that the effect of integrity on trust will be most salient early
in the relationship prior to the development of meaningful benevolence data. And third, the effect
of perceived benevolence on trust will increase over time as the relationship between parties
develops.
According to the model, the level of trust in the trustee will – in combination with the perceived
risk of the situation – drive the trustor’s attitude towards risk taking in the relationship with the
trustee. Finally, the observation of outcomes in the specific situation of trust (was the trustee in
fact trustworthy?) will influence the perceived trustworthiness and might alter the level of trust
and consequently the trustor’s attitude towards risk taking in the relationship with the trustee.
Trustworthiness also affects, monitors, and guides individuals’ actions and attitudes in their inter-
actions (Kasper-Fuehrera & Ashkanasy, 2001). Specifically, perceived trustworthiness reduces
suspicion and increases openness toward the trustor (Shinners, 2009; Szulanski, Cappetta, &
Jensen, 2004). Jones (1996, p. 5) argues, that ‘to trust someone is to have an attitude of optimism
about’ that person. Other studies find that trust has a positive effect on perceived accuracy of
information provided (Benton, Gelber, Kelley, & Liebling, 1969; Roberts & O’Reilly, 1974)
and a negative effect on the perceived probability of loss (Nooteboom, Berger, & Noorderhaven,
1997). To summarize, ‘the effects of trust on attitudes and perceptions have been found to be
fairly consistent and positive’ (Langfred, 2004, p. 385).
Drawing from the outlined conceptions of trust, we argue that the communication between
auditor and user based on the auditor’s report constitutes a situation of trust. The users base
their investment decisions (in case of investors) or their analyses (in case of financial analysts/
investment professionals) on financial statements and auditor’s reports. Users rely on the trust-
worthiness of financial statements providers and auditors, with their money or reputation being
at stake. Decisions and analyses will be driven by perceived assurance provided by the auditor
and the (resulting) perceived credibility of the financial statements, or – in other words – by
their trustworthiness. Consequently, differences in perceived trustworthiness related to the audi-
tor’s report will also alter the perceived trustworthiness of the financial statements.
112 A. Köhler et al.

In this study, we apply the outlined model of trust to our experimental setting. We argue that the
two different KAM sections applied in this study (KAM positive vs. KAM negative) affect, inter
alia, the levels of perceived openness and fairness, and potentially competence (i.e. factors of per-
ceived trustworthiness) and hence lead to unequal levels of trust which users associate with the
auditor’s report. Furthermore, we argue that this in turn leads to different levels of trust in, i.e.
credibility of, the financial statements. Because of the impact that trust has on all kinds of percep-
tions, attitudes, and assessments (as outlined before), we assume that the different levels of trust in
the financial statements lead to different user’s assessments of the company’s economic situation
and different user’s confidence in making the assessments. The preceding line of argument is
illustrated in Figure 1.
The Appendix outlines the wording of the KAM section with positive and negative tendency.
We expect that the specific differences in informational content mainly trigger the following
drivers of perceived trustworthiness of the trustee (see Figure 1): competence, loyalty, openness,
fairness and promise fulfillment. The other drivers being part of the model of trust (receptivity,
availability, consistency and discreetness) rather imply direct and/or repetitive interaction
between trustee and trustor and therefore do not match our setting very well. Specifically, we
argue that the KAM section with positive tendency might in fact be ill-perceived as a kind of
appeasement given the challenges the auditor had faced during the audit. This would imply,
inter alia, lower levels of perceived openness and fairness. On the contrary, a KAM section
with negative tendency might be well-perceived as a helpful signal that draws the users’ attention
to issues that they had not been aware of before. This would imply, inter alia, higher levels of
perceived openness and fairness (and possibly also competence). Consequently, a KAM
section with negative tendency leads to a significantly higher level of trust in the trustee
(auditor) as compared to a KAM section with positive tendency. Hence, in line with the impli-
cations of trust literature, the trustor (user) is less sensitive to risk, less suspicious and more
open toward the auditor and associates higher levels of trust with the auditor’s report (see for
example, Guiso, Sapienza, & Zingales, 2008; Kim, Ferrin, & Rao, 2008; Klein & Shtudiner,
2015). This leads (1) to a higher perceived trustworthiness of the financial statements and, con-
sequently, (2) in line with the positive effect of trust on attitudes and perceptions of the trustor
consistently found in the other mentioned studies, to a better assessment of the company’s econ-
omic situation. In addition, it seems reasonable to assume that the user has more confidence in the
assessment when he or she is less sensitive to risk, less suspicious, more open, and perceives the
KAM section as an information sign that draws the attention to issues that he or she has not been
aware of before.
However, although we expect the outlined differences in the user’s assessment of the com-
pany’s economic situation, as well as in the user’s confidence in making that assessment, due
to different levels of trust, directional predictions based on theoretical deliberations seem ambi-
tious, not least because the model of trust has not been used in auditor reporting literature.
Based on the outlined theoretical implications and the argumentation, we therefore formally
state the following undirectional hypotheses:

HYPOTHESIS 1. Users assess the company’s economic situation different if the auditor’s
report includes a KAM section with a negative tendency compared to a KAM section with a
positive tendency.
HYPOTHESIS 2. Users’ confidence in their assessment of the company’s economic situ-
ation differs if the auditor’s report includes a KAM section with a negative tendency com-
pared to a KAM section with a positive tendency.
Accounting in Europe 113

3. Research Design
3.1. Instrument and Experimental Flow
In order to make our main experiment most accessible for investment professionals and to
increase the number of participations, we develop a web-based and a paper-and-pencil version
of our research instrument. The web-based participants receive an invitation email with a link
to the instrument. On the first page of the web-based instrument, participants are instructed to
carefully read the introduction on this page before working on the case study. Participants
learn that they will be provided with information concerning the Alpha Group and that they
will be asked for their assessments related to different economic issues and also for more
general questions. Participants are also instructed to base their assessments only on information
provided within the case study, that there is no possibility to receive further information concern-
ing the Alpha Group, to work on the case study by themselves and in the given order, and to
provide all required answers. Finally, we assure that responses will be analyzed on an aggregate
basis and that individual answers and personal information will be treated confidentially and only
used for research purposes. On the second page, all participants receive short background infor-
mation about the Alpha Group and comprehensive excerpts of the group’s annual report which
consist of a consolidated income statement, statement of cash flow, balance sheet, other financial
data (for two consecutive financial years) and notes according to International Financial Report-
ing Standards (IFRS). In addition, participants receive a full auditor’s report. All information
elements are arranged one below the other and participants can scroll up and down to process
the excerpts of the Alpha Group’s annual report.7
In order to allow for meaningful variation in the assessment of the economic situation of the
Alpha Group and the confidence in that assessment, i.e. in order to avoid large proportions of
answers on either end of the scales, we decided that the Alpha Group should neither be in financial
distress nor economically booming. Therefore, the Alpha Group is a financially stable group with
a significant decline in profits from the first to the second year (changes in operating income/profit
for financial year −93.16 percent/−104.26 percent) driven by, among other things, declining sales
revenue (−21.80 percent). We also believe that the decline in profits generally motivates partici-
pants to assess the provided information about the Alpha Group in more detail.8
As we manipulate a goodwill-related KAM section in the auditor’s report, the goodwill recog-
nized by the Alpha Group as well as the related note is of particular interest for our study. By
analyzing the respective balance sheet item and note, participants learn that the Alpha Group
recognizes a goodwill with a carrying amount of 5,107 T Euro (26.01 percent of non-current
assets/7.32 percent of total assets) and that the goodwill arose when the Beta AG (public
limited company) and the Gamma GmbH (limited liability company) were purchased and
merged with the Alpha Group. Furthermore, the note contains information about the impairment
test procedure in general and states that the findings of the impairment test indicate no need for
any impairment (in 2012).
Finally, the auditor’s report is presented to the participants. In the experimental groups we
oppose two auditor’s reports with different tendencies of the KAM section (as explained
above) based on the goodwill-related KAM example provided in IAASB (2013) (see IAASB,
2013, pp. 13–14, for original wording and the Appendix for wording of our manipulations). In
the control group, participants are presented with the former standard IAS 700 auditor’s report
(without KAM section).9
At the end of the described second page of the web-based experiment (below the auditor’s
report), participants are instructed to click the ‘Continue’ button and to answer the then following
questions. They are also informed that they have the possibility to return to the excerpts of the
annual report after reading the respective questions by using a ‘Back’ button. The two questions
114 A. Köhler et al.

on the third page of the experiment capture our dependent variables and constitute the experimen-
tal task (for further details regarding our dependent variables, see following section).
After answering the questions on the third page of the experiment, participants click on ‘Con-
tinue’ and open the fourth page, where they are informed that it is not possible to return to the
excerpts of the annual report anymore. Participants are then instructed to rate the relevance of
each element of the provided excerpts of the Alpha Group’s annual report (including auditor’s
report) for the assessment of the economic situation and to indicate how each of the elements
changed their confidence in their assessment on five-points Likert scales (from ‘not relevant at
all’/‘high decrease in confidence’ to ‘extremely relevant’/‘high increase in confidence’).
Finally, we gather demographic information on the fifth page and thank the participants for
their participation on the sixth page.
Procedures, instructions and case materials for the paper-and-pencil version of the experiment
are identical to the above described (analogously adapted where necessary). While we ensured
technically that participants work on the experiment in the described sequence and that revisions
of given answers are not possible in the web-based experiment, we split up the case material to
two envelopes which have to be opened and sealed in a specific sequence for the paper-and-pencil
experiment to implement similar controls.

3.2. Dependent Variables


In line with Monroe and Woodliff (1993), we argue that the communicative value of the auditor’s
report changes due to the inclusion of a KAM section if these additional insights into the auditor’s
work change users’ beliefs. The two dimensions with which we capture the auditor’s report’s
communicative value constitute main judgments within investment professionals’ and non-pro-
fessional investors’ analyses on which investment decisions are built. Bonner (1999, p. 385)
defines judgments as ‘forming an idea, opinion, or estimate about an object, an event, a state,
or another type of phenomenon.’ Thereby, judgments often take the form of an evaluation of a
current state of affairs or the future. Decisions, on the contrary, are defined as

making up one’s mind about the issue at hand and taking a course of action. Decisions typi-
cally follow judgments and involve a choice among various alternatives based on judg-
ments about those alternatives and, possibly, preferences for factors such as risk and
money. (Bonner, 1999, p. 385)

In summary, while judgments reflect beliefs and are mainly driven by the underlying set of infor-
mation, decisions are possibly also driven by other factors. Because it is difficult to account for
these diverse factors that possibly influence decisions, behavioral studies often rely on judgments
as their main dependent variable(s) (e.g. Ashton & Ashton, 1988; Johnson, Theis, Vitalis, &
Young, 2019; Theis, Yankova, & Eulerich, 2012).10 With regard to the context of our study,
Coram, Mock, Turner, and Gray (2011) for example investigate the communicative value of
the auditor’s report and capture financial analysts’ value judgments regarding the reporting
company. To facilitate the participation in our experiment, we decided to simplify the experimen-
tal task and (1) ask participants ‘to assess the economic situation of the Alpha Group based on the
provided excerpts of the annual report including the auditor’s report’ (with an 11-points Likert
response scale and endpoints labeled extremely negative/extremely positive), as this constitutes
a core assessment underlying any valuation judgment of financial analysts (question and scale
derived from Theis et al., 2012).11
Furthermore, the confidence a person has, can have a significant impact on that persons’ assess-
ments and decisions (e.g. Estes & Hosseini, 1988; Phadnis, Caplice, Sheffi, & Singh, 2015).
Accounting in Europe 115

Specifically, an investor’s willingness to invest will increase with the confidence he or she has in
the assessment of the economic situation of the target firm.12 In addition, the value of an audit
mainly lies in an increase of the quality of financial statement information (e.g. Coram et al.,
2011; Mautz & Sharaf, 1961). In this regard, there is comprehensive empirical evidence that
financial statement users have more confidence in audited financial information (e.g. Abdel-
khalik, 1993; Watts & Zimmerman, 1983). Consequently, to better understand how the commu-
nicative value of the auditor’s report changes due to the inclusion of a KAM section, we (2) also
ask participants ‘How confident are you in your assessment of the economic situation of the Alpha
Group based on the provided excerpts of the annual report including the auditor’s report?’ and
again provide an 11-points Likert scale (endpoints labeled not confident at all/absolutely confi-
dent) for their answers.

3.3 Participants in the Main Experiment


For the main experiment, we gained access to the CFA Institute (CFA – Certified Financial
Analyst) with more than 123,000 members in 145 countries and to the DVFA (German Associ-
ation of Financial Analysts) with more than 1,400 members in Germany. For the web-based
experiment, the invitation email is sent out to a CFA Institute survey pool (with members from
all over the world) and to all DVFA members. The paper-and-pencil experiment is conducted
during several training sessions at the DVFA headquarters with investment professionals by
one of the authors in turn.
We derive 14 participants from the CFA Institute (web-based) and 24/51 participants from the
DVFA (web-based/paper-and-pencil) subjects pool (7 more web-based observations were deleted
due to discontinuation of participation), yielding the final sample of 89 participants analyzed
below (we do not identify further need to exclude individual observations from the analysis).13
The average age of our participants is 37.66 years. 79.75 percent of the participants are male,
82.28 percent of the participants come from Germany, 10.12 percent from the USA, UK or
Canada (decreasing order) and 7.60 percent from other countries around the world. 81.40
percent of the participants work as investment professionals for on average 10.53 years. Of
those participants working as investment professionals, 26.67 percent are bankers, 21.33
percent are (sell- or buy-side) financial analysts, 18.67 percent are asset managers, 5.33
percent are investment bankers, 5.33 percent are consultants, 4.00 percent are funds managers
and 18.67 percent work in none of the outlined professions. Most participants mainly work
with equity investments (35.92 percent), corporate bonds (21.36 percent) or sovereign bonds
(12.62 percent). Our participants’ experience with personal capital market investments is moder-
ate to considerable (with an average of 3.45 on a 5-points Likert scale with endpoints labeled no
experience/extensive experience).
Investment professionals are, broadly defined, persons who develop, manage or monitor
finance and investment products, make investment decisions or provide respective advice, or
analyze credit, solvency or other financial risks (DVFA, 2019). Investment professionals
mainly work in positions as (sell-/buy-side) analyst, asset manager, (investment) banker, consult-
ant, financial experts in insurance and other companies. (CFA Institute, 2019; DVFA, 2019).
Unlike in very restricted professions such as the audit profession, the overall population of invest-
ment professionals is difficult to circumscribe. This is because a ‘financial professional may use
various titles whether or not he or she is registered or licensed with a regulatory authority. […
Furthermore the] same financial professional may register in more than one capacity’ (SEC,
2019, p. 1). To nevertheless gain an impression on whether our sample of investment pro-
fessionals seems representative of the underlying population, we compared participant character-
istics with a set of other experimental studies using investment professionals as well (e.g.
116 A. Köhler et al.

Abdellaoui, Bleichrodt, & Kammoun, 2013; Cipriani & Guarino, 2009; Cohn, Engelmann, Fehr,
& Maréchal, 2015; Jin, Drozdenko, & DeLoughy, 2013). Core demographic characteristics like
age, gender, and work experience are fairly consistent across the reviewed studies and comparable
with our study. Less homogenous across studies is the distribution of affiliations. However, just as
in our study, (investment) bankers and analysts typically constitute the largest subsamples. Sum-
marizing, we do not see a reason to believe that our sample is significantly biased as compared to
the population regarding relevant participant characteristics.

4. Results
4.1. Descriptive Analysis for Main Experiment with Investment Professionals
Figure 2 outlines the results of our descriptive analysis for the two dependent variables. The
excerpts of the annual report of the Alpha Group in combination with the former ISA 700

Figure 2. Descriptive results for dependent variables (main experiment).


Accounting in Europe 117

Table 1. Descriptive results for the main experiment.

Coding [Endpoints of Group


Variables scale]
Former KAM KAM
ISA 700 Negative Positive
Mean / SD (Number of observations)
Assessment of Economic Situation 1–11 [extremely negative; 4.32 / 1.80 5.16 / 2.18 4.03 / 1.60
extremely positive] (25) (31) (32)

Confidence in Assessment of 1–11 [not confident at all; 6.20 / 2.65 6.48 / 2.31 6.00 / 2.16
Economic Situation absolutely confident] (25) (31) (31)

Relevance Auditors’ Report for 1–5 [not relevant at all; 2.38 / 0.94 2.61 / 1.20 2.25 / 0.80
Assessment of Economic extremely relevant] (26) (31) (32)
Situation

auditor’s report (control group) lead to a mean investment professionals’ assessment of the econ-
omic situation of 4.32 and to a mean investment professionals’ confidence in this assessment of
6.20. Hence, investment professionals tend to assess the economic situation of the Alpha Group to
be slightly negative and tend to be relatively confident in their assessment. In the KAM negative
experimental group, investment professionals’ assessments of the economic situation are more
positive with higher associated confidence in comparison to the control group, leading to
means of 5.16 and 6.48, respectively. On the contrary, for the KAM positive experimental
group, we find more negative assessments of the economic situation (4.03) with lower associated
confidence (6.00) in comparison to the control group. Summarizing, the descriptive analysis
suggests that there is a considerable difference in means for both dependent variables between
the experimental groups. Furthermore, especially the KAM negative manipulation leads to con-
siderable reactions to that information (see also Table 1).
We also ask participants to indicate the relevance of individual elements of the annual report for
the assessment of the economic situation and change in confidence due to individual elements.
Differences in means between groups are small and there is no obvious pattern. In general, the
income statement seems to be the most relevant source of information for the investment pro-
fessionals, followed by the cash flow statement and the balance sheet (across-groups; untabu-
lated). The relevance of the auditor’s report for the assessment of the economic situation is
fairly low for all groups. Interestingly, although not statistically significant, mean assessments
for the relevance of the auditor’s report follow the pattern we observe for our dependent variables:
the relevance is lowest for the KAM positive and highest for the KAM negative group (see Table
1). This makes sense in light of the theoretical framework we apply, because information can only
be relevant if it is trustworthy.

4.2. ANOVA for Main Experiment with Investment Professionals


Table 2, Panel A outlines the results of the ANOVA applied to analyze the data for the assessment
of the company’s economic situation. First, group means for the former ISA 700/KAM positive/
KAM negative condition differ significantly (p = 0.0529). A post-hoc mean comparison test
reveals that the mean investment professionals’ assessment of the economic situation of the
Alpha Group is significantly more positive in the KAM section with a negative tendency as com-
pared to the KAM section with a positive tendency condition (p = 0.057/0.063/0.056 based on
118 A. Köhler et al.

Table 2. Results for the ANOVA (main experiment).

Prob >
Source Sum of Squares Degrees of Freedom Mean Square F F
Panel A: Dependent variable “assessment of economic situation”
Between Groups 21.386338 2 10.693169 3.04 0.0529
Within Groups 298.602298 85 3.51296822
Total 319.988636 87 3.6780303
Bartlett’s Test for Equal Variances: chi2(2) = 2.9704 Prob > chi2 = 0.226
Mean Difference
[Significance (p-value) Former ISA 700 KAM Negative
Bonferroni/Scheffe/Sidak]
KAM Negative 0.84129 [0.296 / –
0.254 / 0.268]
KAM Positive −0.28875 [1.00 / −1.13004 [0.057 /
0.847 / 0.918] 0.063 / 0.056]
Prob >
Source Sum of Squares Degrees of Freedom Mean Square F F
Panel B: Dependent variable “confidence in assessment of economic situation”
Between Groups 3.712610 2 1.856305 0.33 0.7167
Within Groups 471.741935 85 5.549905
Total 475.454545 87 5.464995
Bartlett’s Test for Equal Variances: chi2(2) = 1.1694 Prob > chi2 = 0.557

Bonferroni/Scheffe/Sidak adjustment of confidence intervals). Hence, we can support our first


hypothesis H1. While the descriptive analysis revealed that the investment professionals’ confi-
dence in the assessment of the economic situation in the KAM section with a negative tendency
as compared to the KAM section with a positive tendency condition is considerably higher, the
difference is insignificant in an ANOVA analysis (Table 2, Panel B). Hence, we cannot support
our second hypothesis H2.
In light of our findings and referring to the model of trust, we argue that the KAM section with
positive tendency might in fact be ill-perceived as a kind of appeasement given the challenges the
auditor had faced during the audit. This would imply, inter alia, lower levels of perceived open-
ness and fairness. On the contrary, a KAM section with negative tendency might be well-per-
ceived as a helpful signal that draws the users’ attention to issues that they had not been aware
of before. This would imply, inter alia, higher levels of perceived openness and fairness (and
possibly also competence). Consequently, a KAM section with negative tendency leads to a sig-
nificantly higher level of trust in the trustee (auditor) as compared to a KAM section with positive
tendency. Hence, in line with the implications of trust literature, the trustor (user) is less sensitive
to risk, less suspicious and more open toward the auditor and associates higher levels of trust with
the auditor’s report (see for example, Guiso et al., 2008; Kim et al., 2008; Klein & Shtudiner,
2015). This leads (1) to a higher perceived trustworthiness of the financial statements and, con-
sequently, (2) in line with the positive effect of trust on attitudes and perceptions of the trustor
consistently found in the other mentioned studies, to a better assessment of the company’s econ-
omic situation. In addition, it seems reasonable to assume that the user has more confidence in the
assessment when he or she is less sensitive to risk, less suspicious, more open, and perceives the
KAM section as an information sign that draws the attention to issues that he or she has not been
aware of before.
Accounting in Europe 119

4.3. Robustness Checks


To validate our findings, we utilize ANCOVA and regression techniques and, inter alia, include
participants’ work experience and participants’ indication of the relevance of the auditors’ report
for the assessment of the economic situation of the company as control variables (results untabu-
lated). Specifically, the ANCOVA confirms a significant difference between group means (KAM
positive/KAM negative/Former ISA 700) for the assessment of the economic situation of the
company (p = 0.049), while none of the two control variables exhibit a significant influence.14
To further investigate the influence of the two different types of KAM (KAM positive/KAM
negative) in presence of the two control variables, we run a regression with the assessment of
the economic situation of the company as dependent variable, a binary coded independent vari-
able reflecting the type of KAM (0 = KAM positive; 1 = KAM negative),15 and again participants’
work experience as well as participants’ indication of the relevance of the auditors’ report for the
assessment of the economic situation of the company as control variables. Regression results
confirm the significantly positive impact of the KAM negative condition on the mean investment
professionals’ assessment of the economic situation of the Alpha Group, as compared to the KAM
positive condition (p = 0.029). In the regression, participants’ work experience has no (p = 0.302)

Table 3. Constructs, factors, indicators, and respective questions in the additional analysis.
Construct Factor Indicator Question
Ability Competence x11 My level of confidence that the information provider is
technically competent at the critical elements of his or
her job is …
x12 My level of confidence that the information provider has
an acceptable level of understanding of his or her job
is …
x13 My level of confidence that the information provider will
be able to do his or her job in an acceptable manner is

x14 My level of confidence that the information provider will
make well thought out decisions about his or her job
is …
Benevolence Loyalty x21 My level of confidence that the information provider is
on my side is …
x22 My level of confidence that the information provider acts
in my best interest is …
Openness x23 My level of confidence that the information provider
shares all known and relevant information about
important issues even if there is a possibility that the
information might jeopardize my interest in the Alpha
Group is …
x24 My level of confidence that the information provider
openly addresses difficulties is …
x25 My level of confidence that the information provider
provides me with precise information is …
Integrity Fairness x31 My level of confidence that the information provider will
treat me fairly is …
Promise Fulfillment x32 My level of confidence that I can rely on what the
information provider tells me is …
Trust Trust y11 The level of trust between the information provider and
myself is …
Scales: [nearly zero;
near 100%]
120 A. Köhler et al.

and participants’ indication of the relevance of the auditors’ report for the assessment of the econ-
omic situation of the company has a marginally significant positive (p = 0.084) influence. In
summary, our robustness checks confirm the reported findings.

5. Additional Analysis
In order to understand whether the findings of our main experiment with investment professionals
also hold for the other important user group, namely non-professional investors, we conduct an
additional analysis. We basically repeated our main experiment with students who can be used
as surrogates for the non-profession investors (e.g. Asare & Wright, 2012; Ashton & Kramer,
1980). Contrary to the main experiment with investment professionals, we did not face any
time constraints. Consequently, we had the possibility to include questions with which we
capture constructs, factors and indicators of the model of trust (see Table 3 for details, and the
respective questions). Furthermore, we asked questions to validate our manipulations, as well
as additional questions.16 For the additional analysis, we use students attending advanced
accounting classes at two major German universities (73.91 percent of the participants are
enrolled in a master, and the remainder in a bachelor program). The mean age of our 69 partici-
pants is 24.95 years, and females represent 42.19 percent of the sample.17

Table 4. Descriptive results and results for the ANOVA (additional analysis).
Panel A: Descriptive results for the additional analysis
Group
Variables Coding [Endpoints of scale]
Former ISA KAM KAM
700 Negative Positive
Mean / SD (Number of Observations)
Dependent Variables
Assessment of Economic 1–11 [extremely negative; 6.09 / 2.26 5.03 / 2.08 5.18 / 2.40
Situation extremely positive] (11) (30) (28)
Confidence in 1–11 [not confident at all; 7.45 / 2.30 7.47 / 2.15 7.57 / 2.01
Assessment of absolutely confident] (11) (30) (28)
Economic Situation
Panel B: ANOVA (dependent variable “assessment of economic situation”)
Source Sum of Squares Degrees of Freedom Mean Square F Prob > F
Between Groups 9.321447 2 4.660724 0.93 0.4010

Within Groups 331.9829 66 5.030044

Total 341.304348 68 5.019181


Bartlett’s Test for Equal Variances: chi2(2) = 0.5895 Prob > chi2 = 0.745
Panel C: ANOVA (dependent variable “confidence in assessment of economic situation”)
Source Sum of Squares Degrees of Freedom Mean Square F Prob > F
Between Groups 0.195294 2 0.097647 0.02 0.9784

Within Groups 295.051082 66 4.470471

Total 295.246377 68 4.341858


Bartlett’s Test for Equal Variances: chi2(2) = 0.2881 Prob > chi2 = 0.866
Accounting in Europe 121

First of all, an in-depth analysis of the results reveals no noticeable response pattern for any of
the captured questions and indicators (as an excerpt, Table 4, Panel A provides descriptive stat-
istics for our two dependent variables only). Furthermore, based on ANOVA we do not find sig-
nificant differences between groups for our two dependent variables (see Table 4, Panel B and C).
Basically, this would imply that a KAM section has no communicative value for non-professional
investors. As the non-professional investors participating in the additional analysis were con-
fronted with a complex, coherent case, we conclude that non-professional investors are not
able to perceive the message conveyed with a KAM section if it is embedded in a highly
complex, coherent, and hence realistic case.

6. Conclusion and Limitations


With the new International Standard on Auditing ISA 701, the IAASB has recently introduced a
separate section in the auditor’s report that communicates KAM. The intention thereby is to
provide users with more information about the auditor’s work and, thus, to enhance the commu-
nicative value of the auditor’s report. In this study, we experimentally examine the potential effect
of a separate KAM section in the auditor’s report on its communicative value for users. Contrary
to prior literature, we do not only consider non-professional investors, but also investment pro-
fessionals in our experiment.
We find that in the condition in which the KAM section suggests that already small changes in
the key assumptions could eventually lead to a goodwill impairment (KAM negative), investment
professionals assess the economic situation of the company to be significantly better as compared
to the condition in which the KAM section suggests that only large changes in the key assumptions
could eventually lead to a goodwill impairment (KAM positive). Given that participants might per-
ceive a higher risk of impairment in the KAM negative vs. positive condition, our findings imply
that a possibly resulting more unfavorable assessment of the company’s economic situation in the
KAM negative condition is overcompensated by the positive mechanisms hypothesized based on
the model of trust. However, we acknowledge that we do not specifically account for the under-
lying reciprocity. Contrary to the findings of our main experiment, in our additional analysis
with non-professional investors, we observe that a KAM section has no communicative value.
Our study contributes to the auditor reporting literature in at least three ways. First, by using
two types of users, we show that investment professionals and non-professional investors react
differently to KAM sections embedded in a highly complex, and thus fairly realistic case. In
this regard, our findings seem helpful in explaining the mixed results of prior literature concerning
the effects of KAM.
Second, to the best of our knowledge, this is the first study that examines the potential effect of
the additional information about KAM in the auditor’s report by considering different tendencies
of KAM on the communicative value for investment professionals and non-professional inves-
tors. Our findings are useful for standard setters and auditors by highlighting the importance of
carefully considering how different types of users perceive the message conveyed with the
KAM section. This aspect is highly relevant because the identification and communication of
KAM are subject to the auditor’s professional judgment. According to our findings, neither pre-
parers nor audit committees or auditors need to fear that the disclosure of critical entity-related
information leads to negative implications. Rather, we encourage auditors to refrain from positive
tendencies in KAM paragraphs that might impair the perceived trustworthiness of the auditor as a
whole, and, by extension, the financial statements users’ assessment of the economic situation of
the audited firm. However, we would like to emphasize that negative tendencies of KAMs might
also fail in effectively conveying the messages intended by the auditor. A positive assessment of a
firm’s economic situation based on negative tendencies in KAMs (and, in turn, higher perceived
122 A. Köhler et al.

trustworthiness of the auditor) might contribute to the ‘expectation gap’. Therefore, our findings
indicate that auditors should choose the phrasing of the KAM section (in particular with regard to
evaluative statements) cautiously, considering that the signal sent to the financial statements users
might differ from the auditors’ original intent. Our results imply that (positive or negative) ten-
dencies in KAM reporting, inter alia, affect the perceived trustworthiness of the auditor –
however, trust in the auditor might be a necessary precondition for effectively conveying the
intended messages in KAM reporting in the first place.
Third, by referring to a model of trust that theoretically explains why different tendencies of
KAM potentially lead to different investment professionals’ assessments of aspects related to
the communicative value, we employ an innovative approach in the auditor reporting literature
not used thus far.
Our study is of course not without limitations. First, we employ an experimental approach in
which we manipulate the informational content of one KAM example in a specific setting. Con-
sequently, our findings mainly depend on our manipulations and the setting. However, it seems
reasonable to assume that the KAM example in the IAASB illustrative auditor’s report, from
which we carefully derive our manipulations, is of significant practical relevance and may also
be used by auditors as a general pattern. Furthermore, based on the implications of the model
of trust, it seems reasonable to assume that any KAM section with a (strong) positive or negative
tendency bears the risk of possibly unexpected users’ perceptions. Second, in order to gain access
to the investment professionals, we were compelled to keep the experimental materials as
compact as possible, i.e. to minimize the time effort for participants. We therefore have to
acknowledge that our main experiment lacks manipulation check questions. Third, we collected
the data for our study late 2013/early 2014, and hence, before KAM reporting became broadly
mandatory. As investment professionals are now more accustomed to such reporting, their reac-
tion today might be different. Fourth, we use students as surrogates for non-professional inves-
tors. While this has been done in previous experimental studies, our participants might differ
in relevant characteristics from the overall population of non-professional investors. Fifth, in
our experimental material, we are silent on the specific amount of the goodwill impairment.
Although we have no reason to believe that our results are biased as we hold this aspect constant
between groups, we want to acknowledge that investors’ reactions will generally depend on the
specific amount of a goodwill impairment. Sixth, while we chose a setting in which the Alpha
Group faced a significant decline in profits because we wanted to motivate participants to
assess the provided information more carefully, this framing might drive our results to some
degree. Seventh, our ability to investigate the relevance of cultural differences for our findings
is limited. Our paper provides several avenues for future research. One, considering the impact
of cultural differences in the context of the communicative value of a KAM section in more
detail could generate interesting insights. Two, repeating our experiment with a case containing
information on the specific amount of a goodwill impairment, and/or a different type of KAM
would increase the generalizability of the findings. Three, increasing number of participants
and including manipulation checks in an experiment with investment professionals, as well as
repeating the experiment with different surrogates for non-professional investors, could add
validity.

Notes
1
Examples for KAM include for instance: Goodwill, valuation of financial instruments, and effects of new accounting
standards (IAASB, 2013; IAASB, 2015b; see also IAASB, 2015a).
2
In addition, further research evidence regarding the effect of a KAM or critical audit matters (CAM) section respectively
exists within the domains of auditor liability, auditor judgment, and managerial decision making. A detailed overview
Accounting in Europe 123

over prior studies will be provided in the following section. The majority of this evidence is generated based on CAM.
However, CAM are considered to be conceptually equivalent to KAM. CAM reflect the PCAOB’s implementation of
enhancing the auditor’s reporting model in terms of communicating auditor insights to investors about critical audit
issues (for a detailed analysis from a more practical perspective, see Jermakowicz, Epstein, & Ramamoorti, 2018)
We use the term CAM whenever a study cited in the following refers to CAM. However, we use the term KAM
when we describe our own study as we refer to the concept of KAM.
3
Consequently, the focus of our research differs from other studies where presentation format is manipulated but infor-
mation content stays the same (see Libby & Emett, 2014, for a recent review of the effects of earnings presentation attri-
butes on manager and user behavior). Therefore, we carefully phrased our KAM manipulations so that they convey
different content and do not merely differ in terms of length.
4
We include the former ISA 700 as reference case for our analysis. In order to isolate the effect of the specific informa-
tional content of the KAM section in our experiment, we further include only one KAM in the auditor’s report.
5
For comprehensive reviews of the academic literature on KAM, see for example: Gold & Heilmann, 2018; Velte & Issa,
2019.
6
We stay as close as possible to the IAASB wording (IAASB, 2013) in phrasing our manipulations (compare original
wording in IAASB (2013, pp. 13–14), to the wording of our manipulations in the Appendix). As compared to the orig-
inal, we only adjust the last sentence of the KAM section for our manipulations. In order to create a KAM section with a
relatively strong negative tendency regarding the company’s economic situation (KAM negative), we add the word
“already” to the beginning of the original sentence. As outlined above, in comparison to the KAM negative condition
we replace “already small” by “only large” in the KAM positive condition. Furthermore, an in-depth comparison
between the IAASB wording of the KAM example with KAMs on goodwill impairment found in practice suggests
resembling structure and wording (see for example FRC, 2016, p. 16).
7
While the Alpha Group itself is fictitious, we are guided by the financial statements of a real German medium-sized
group within the solar industry in setting up the experimental case. To avoid that our participants recognize the
Alpha Group’s real counterpart, we multiply all (balance sheet, etc.) items with the same factor and change the industry
in which the Alpha Group operates from solar to industrial machinery. Furthermore, the case was carefully reviewed in
advance by several investment professionals for realism and understandability.
8
We want to highlight that the decline in sales of the Alpha Group is not necessarily indicative of a goodwill impairment,
as the case provides no information on the causes of the decline which, hence, is not necessarily related to the cash gen-
erating units standing behind the goodwill. Also, based on the case, there is no reason to assume that the decline in profits
is lasting. Finally, in the real financial statements that we used to develop our case, the change in sales and revenue was
identical to the case, and a goodwill impairment did de facto not occur.
9
We acknowledge that the participants in the control group naturally do not receive the information contained in the KAM
which provides additional insights from an auditor’s perspective.
10
However, because a clear differentiation between the two terms judgment and decision can be challenging, Bonner
(1999) argues that behavioral research often refers to the joint term JDM (Judgment and Decision Making) instead.
11
Other experimental studies investigating the communicative value of the auditor’s report capture participants’ invest-
ment decisions as the primary dependent variable (e.g., Christensen et al., 2014). Out of mentioned difficulties to
account for the diverse factors that possibly influence decisions, we decided to set our focus differently and rather
capture participants’ judgments.
12
However, greater confidence does not always lead to higher decision quality. Investigating the phenomenon of undue or
overconfidence in connection with investment decisions is not part of this study (for further reading, see for example
Lambert, Bessière, & N’Goala, 2012; Zacharakis & Shepherd, 2001).
13
We conducted the online experiment at the end of the year 2013 and collected the additional data in the training sessions
at the beginning of the year 2014.
14
The number of observations included in the ANCOVA is 61, and hence, lower than in the ANOVA reported as main
analysis (n = 88). This is because of missing values for the control variables included in the ANCOVA.
15
The number of observations included in the regression is 42, and hence, lower than in the ANCOVA reported (n = 61).
This is because we do not consider the former ISA 700 condition for regression purposes. With the regression, we yield a
R2 of 0.27.
16
The intention thereby was to provide additional empirical evidence for the line of argumentation (as illustrated in Figure
1) with a set of identical participants.
17
Prior behavioral research in accounting has used MBA students with relevant work experience (e.g., Elliott, Hodge,
Kennedy, & Pronk, 2007), as well as fairly unexperienced undergraduate students as surrogates for non-professional
investors (e.g., Pinsker, 2011). The students of our sample fall somewhere in between of MBA and undergraduate stu-
dents. Although the majority of our student participants are graduate students, it is common in Germany that students
enroll in a master program directly after finishing the bachelor program.
124 A. Köhler et al.

Disclosure Statement
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s).

Supplemental data
Supplemental data for this article can be accessed https://doi.org/10.1080/17449480.2020.
1726420

ORCID
Jochen Theis http://orcid.org/0000-0003-3634-5193

References
Abdel-khalik, A. R. (1993). Why do private companies demand auditing? A case for organizational loss of control. Journal
of Accounting, Auditing & Finance, 8(1), 31–53.
Abdellaoui, M., Bleichrodt, H., & Kammoun, H. (2013). Do financial professionals behave according to prospect theory?
An experimental study. Theory and Decision, 74(3), 411–429.
Asare, S. K., & Wright, A. M. (2012). Investors’, auditors’, and lenders’ understanding of the message conveyed by the
standard audit report on the financial statements. Accounting Horizons, 26(2), 193–217.
Asbahr, K., & Ruhnke, K. (2019). Real effects of reporting key audit matters on auditors’ judgment and choice of action.
International Journal of Auditing, 23(2), 165–180.
Ashton, A. H., & Ashton, R. H. (1988). Sequential belief revision in auditing. The Accounting Review, 63(4), 623–641.
Ashton, R. H., & Kramer, S. S. (1980). Students as surrogates in behavioral research: Some evidence. Journal of Account-
ing Research, 8(1), 1–5.
Axelrod, R. (1984). The evolution of cooperation. New York: Basic Books.
Backof, A., Bowlin, K., & Goodson, B. (2014). The impact of proposed changes to the content of the audit report on
jurors’ assessments of auditor negligence. Retrieved from http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=
2446057
Barber, B. (1983). The logic and limits of trust. New Brunswick: Rutgers University Press.
Bédard, J., Gonthier-Besacier, N., & Schatt, A. (2014). Costs and benefits of reporting key audit matters in the audit report:
The French experience. Retrieved from https://www.isarhq.org/2014_downloads/papers/ISAR2014_Bedard_
Besacier_Schatt.pdf
Bédard, J., Gonthier-Besacier, N., & Schatt, A. (2015). Analysis of the consequences of the disclosure of key audit matters
in the audit report. Retrieved from http://www.hec.unil.ch/documents/seminars/dcc/1946.pdf
Bentley, J. W., Lambert, T. A., & Wang, E. Y. (2018). The effect of increased audit disclosure on managerial decision
making: Evidence from disclosing critical audit matters. Retrieved from https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?
abstract_id=3000978
Benton, A. E., Gelber, E., Kelley, H., & Liebling, B. (1969). Reactions to various degrees of deceit in a mixed-motive
relationship. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 12(2), 170–180.
Bonner, S. E. (1999). Judgment and decision-making research in accounting. Accounting Horizons, 13(4), 385–398.
Brasel, K., Doxey, M., Grenier, J. H., & Reffett, A. (2016). Risk disclosure preceding negative outcomes: The effects of
reporting critical audit matters on judgment of auditor liability. The Accounting Review, 91(5), 1345–1362.
Brown, T., Majors, T., & Peecher, M. (2015). The influence of evaluator evidence, a judgment rule, and critical audit
matters on assessments of auditor legal liability (University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign).
Butler, J. K. (1991). Toward understanding and measuring conditions of trust: Evolution of a conditions of trust inventory.
Journal of Management, 17(3), 643–663.
CFA Institute. (2019). Career development: Enhancing your employability. Retrieved from https://careers.accaglobal.
com/careers-advice/career-development/enhancing-your-employability/what-makes-a-finance-professional.html
Christensen, B. E., Glover, S. M., & Wolfe, C. J. (2014). Do critical audit matter paragraphs in the audit report change
nonprofessional investors’ decision to invest? Auditing: A Journal of Practice and Theory, 33(4), 71–93.
Cipriani, M., & Guarino, A. (2009). Herd behavior in financial markets: An experiment with financial market pro-
fessionals. Journal of the European Economic Association, 7(1), 206–233.
Cohn, A., Engelmann, J., Fehr, E., & Maréchal, A. A. (2015). Evidence for countercyclical risk aversion: An experiment
with financial professionals. American Economic Review, 105(2), 860–885.
Accounting in Europe 125

Coram, P. J., Mock, T. J., Turner, J. L., & Gray, G. L. (2011). The communicative value of the auditor’s report. Australian
Accounting Review, 58(3), 235–252.
Cummings, L. L., & Bromiley, P. (1996). The organizational trust inventory (OTI): development and validation. In R. M.
Kramer, & T. R. Tyler (Eds.), Trust in organizations: Frontiers of theory and research (pp. 302–331). Thousand
Oaks: Sage.
Currall, S. C., & Judge, T. A. (1995). Measuring trust between organizational boundary role persons. Organizational Be-
havior and Human Decision Processes, 64(2), 151–170.
DVFA. (2019). Membership. Retrieved from https://www.dvfa.de/verband/mitgliedschaft.html
Ekeh, P. P. (1974). Social exchange theory: The two traditions. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
Elliott, W. B., Hodge, F. D., Kennedy, J. J., & Pronk, M. (2007). Are M.B.A. Students a good proxy for nonprofessional
investors? The Accounting Review, 82(1), 139–168.
Estes, R., & Hosseini, J. (1988). The gender gap on Wall Street: An empirical analysis of confidence in investment
decision making. The Journal of Psychology, 122(6), 577–590.
Fiske, J. (1990). Introduction to communication studies. London: Routledge.
FRC. (2015). Financial reporting council: Extended auditor’s report: A review of experience in the first year. Retrieved
from https://frc.org.uk/Extended-auditors-reports.pdf.
FRC. (2016). Financial reporting council: Extended auditor’s report: A further review of experience. Retrieved from
https://www.frc.org.uk/Our-Work/Publications/Audit-and-Assurance-Team/Report-on-the-Second-Year-
Experience-of-Extended-A.pdf
Gambetta, D. (1988). Trust: Making and breaking cooperative relations. New York: Basil Blackwell.
Gimbar, C., Hansen, B., & Ozlanski, M. E. (2016a). The effect of critical audit matter paragraphs and accounting standard
precision on auditor liability. The Accouting Review, 91(6), 1629–1646.
Gimbar, C., Hansen, B., & Ozlanski, M. E. (2016b). Early evidence on the effects of critical audit matters on auditor liab-
ility. Current Issues in Auditing, Spring, A24–A33.
GlaxoSmithKline. (2014). Annual Report 2014. Retrieved from https://www.gsk.com/media/2710/gsk-annual-report-
2014-interactive.pdf
Gold, A., Gronewold, U., & Pott, C. (2012). The ISA 700 auditor’s report and the audit expectation gap – Do explanations
matter? International Journal of Auditing, 16(3), 286–307.
Gold, A., & Heilmann, M. (2018). The consequences of disclosing key audit matters (KAMs): A review of the academic
literature. Maandblad Voor Accountancy en Bedrijfseconomie, 93(1/2), 5–14.
Greggs. (2014). Greggs plc annual report and accounts 2014. Retrieved from https://corporate.greggs.co.uk/investors/
results-centre
Guiso, L., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2008). Trusting the stock market. The Journal of Finance, 63(6), 2557–2600.
Hasan, M., Roebuck, P. J., & Simnett, R. (2003). An investigation of alternative report formats for communicating mod-
erate levels of assurance. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory, 22(2), 171–187.
Hosmer, L. T. (1995). Trust: The connecting link between organizational theory and philosophical ethics. Academy of
Management Review, 20(2), 379–403.
IAASB. (2013). International auditing and assurance standard board: Exposure draft: Reporting on audited financial state-
ments: proposed new and revised International Standards on Auditing (ISAs) (New York: International Federation of
Accountants).
IAASB. (2015a). International auditing and assurance standard board: International Standard on Auditing 700 (Revised).
Forming an opinion and reporting on financial statements (New York: International Federation of Accountants).
IAASB. (2015b). International auditing and assurance standard board: International Standard on Auditing 701. Commu-
nicating key audit matters in the independent auditor’s report (New York: International Federation of Accountants).
Jermakowicz, E. K., Epstein, B. J., & Ramamoorti, S. (2018). CAM versus KAM – A distinction without a difference? The
CPA Journal, February, 34–40.
Jin, K. G., Drozdenko, R., & DeLoughy, S. (2013). The role of corporate value clusters in ethics, social responsibility, and
performance: A study of financial professionals and implications for the financial meltdown. Journal of Business
Ethics, 112(1), 15–24.
Johnson-George, C., & Swap, W. C. (1982). Measurement of specific interpersonal trust: Construction and validation of a
scale to assess trust in a specific other. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43(6), 1306–1317.
Johnson, J., Theis, J. C., Vitalis, A., & Young, D. (2019). The influence of firms’ emissions management strategy disclos-
ures on investors’ valuation judgments. Contemporary Accounting Research. forthcoming. Retrieved from https://
onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/1911-3846.12545.
Jones, K. (1996). Trust as an affective attitude. Ethics, 107(1), 4–25.
Kachelmeier, S. J., Rimkus, D., Schmidt, J. J., & Valentine, K. (2019). The forewarning effect of critical audit matter dis-
closures involving measurement uncertainty. Retrieved from https://ssrn.com/abstract = 2481284
126 A. Köhler et al.

Kachelmeier, S. J., Schmidt, J. J., & Valentine, K. (2015). The disclaimer effect of disclosing critical audit matters in the
auditor’s report (The University of Texas at Austin).
Kasper-Fuehrera, E. C., & Ashkanasy, N. M. (2001). Communicating trustworthiness and building trust in interorganiza-
tional virtual organizations. Journal of Management, 27(3), 235–254.
Kim, D. J., Ferrin, D. L., & Rao, H. R. (2008). A trust-based consumer decision-making model in electronic commerce:
The role of trust, perceived risk, and their antecedents. Decision Support Systems, 44(2), 544–564.
Klein, G., & Shtudiner, Z. (2015). Trust in others: Does it affect investment decisions? Quality & Quantity, 50(5), 149–
165.
Klueber, J., Gold, A., & Pott, C. (2018). Do key audit matters impact financial reporting behavior? Retrieved from https://
papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers. cfm?abstract_id=3210475
Kramer, R. M. (1999). Trust and distrust in organizations: Emerging perspectives, enduring questions. Annual Review of
Psychology, 50, 569–598.
Lambert, J., Bessière, V., & N’Goala, G. (2012). Does expertise influence the impact of overconfidence on judgment,
valuation and investment decision? Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(6), 1115–1128.
Langfred, C. W. (2004). Too much of a good thing? Negative effects of high trust and individual autonomy in self-mana-
ging teams. Academy of Management Journal, 47(3), 385–399.
Lennox, C. S., Schmidt, J. J., & Thompson, A. M. (2016). Is the expanded model of audit reporting informative to inves-
tors? Evidence from the UK (The University of Texas at Austin).
Lewicki, R. J., McAllister, D. J., & Bies, R. J. (1998). Trust and distrust: New relationships and realities. Academy of Man-
agement Review, 23(3), 438–458.
Libby, R., & Emett, S. A. (2014). Earnings presentation effects on manager reporting choices and investor decisions.
Accounting and Business Research, 44(4), 410–438.
Mautz, R. K., & Sharaf, H. A. (1961) The philosophy of auditing Lakewood Ranch: American Accounting Association.
Mayer, R. C., Davis, J. H., & Schoorman, F. D. (1995). An integrative model of organizational trust. Academy of Manage-
ment Review, 20(3), 709–734.
Mellinger, G. D. (1956). Interpersonal trust as a factor in communication. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 52(3),
304–309.
Mishra, A. K. (1996). Organizational responses to crisis: The centrality of trust. In R. M. Kramer & T. Tyler (Eds.), Trust
in organizations (pp. 261–287). Newbury Park: Sage.
Monroe, G. S., & Woodliff, D. (1993). The effect of education on the audit expectation gap. Accounting and Finance, 33
(1), 61–78.
New World Resources. (2014). New world resources plc annual report and accounts 2014. Retrieved from http://www.
newworldresources.eu/en/investors/reports/annual-reports-and-accounts
Nooteboom, B., Berger, H., & Noorderhaven, N. (1997). Effects of trust and governance on relational risk. Academy of
Management Journal, 40(2), 308–338.
Pearson. (2014). Pearson plc annual report and accounts. Retrieved from https://www.pearson.com/corporate/investors/
investor-information/reports-and-presentations.html
Phadnis, S., Caplice, C., Sheffi, Y., & Singh, M. (2015). Effect of scenario planning on field experts’ judgment of long-
range investment decisions. Strategic Management Journal, 36(9), 1401–1411.
Pinsker, R. (2011). Primacy or recency? A study of order effects when nonprofessional investors are provided a long series
of disclosures. Behavioral Research in Accounting, 23(1), 161–183.
Rapley, E. T., Robertson, J. C., & Smith, J. L. (2018). The effects of disclosing critical audit matters and auditor tenure on
investors’ judgments. Retrieved from https://ssrn.com/abstract=3294340
Ratzinger-Sakel, N. V. S., & Theis, J. C. (2019). Does considering key audit matters affect auditor judgment performance?
Corporate Ownership & Control, 17(Special Issue), 196–210.
Read, W. H. (1962). Upward communication in industrial hierarchies. Human Relations, 15(3), 3–15.
Rempel, J. K., Holmes, J. G., & Zanna, M. P. (1985). Trust in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
chology, 49(1), 95–112.
Roberts, K., & O’Reilly, C. (1974). Failures in upward communication in organizations: Three possible culprits. Academy
of Management Journal, 17(2), 205–215.
SEC. (2019). SEC-NASAA investor bulletin: Making sense of financial professional titles. Retrieved from https://www.
sec.gov/investor/alerts/ib_making_sense.pdf
Shaver, P. R., & Hazan, C. (1994). Attachment. In A. L. Weber, & J. H. Harvey (Eds.), Perspectives on close relationships
(pp. 110–130). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Shinners, E. (2009). Effects of the “what is beautiful is good” stereotype on perceived trustworthiness. UW-L Journal of
Undergraduate Research, XII, 1–5.
Sirois, L.-P., Bédard, J., & Bera, P. (2018). The informational value of key audit matters in the auditor’s report: Evidence
from an eye-tracking study. Accounting Horizons, 32(2), 141–162.
Accounting in Europe 127

Sitkin, S. B., & Roth, N. R. (1993). Explaining the limited effectiveness of legalistic “remedies” for trust/distrust. Organ-
ization Science, 4(3), 367–392.
Szulanski, G., Cappetta, R., & Jensen, R. J. (2004). When and how trustworthiness matters: Knowledge transfer and the
moderating effect of causal ambiguity. Organization Science, 15(5), 600–613.
Theis, J. C., Yankova, K., & Eulerich, M. (2012). Information order effects in the context of management commentary –
Initial experimental evidence. Journal of Management Control, 23(2), 133–150.
Velte, P., & Issa, J. (2019). The impact of key audit matter (KAM) disclosure in audit reports on stakeholders’ reactions: A
literature review. Problems and Perspectives in Management, 17(3), 323–341.
Vinson, J. M., Robertson, J. C., & Cockrell, R. C. (2019). The effects of critical audit matter removal and duration on
jurors’ assessments of auditor negligence. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory, 38(3), 183–202.
Watts, R., & Zimmerman, J. (1983). Agency problems, auditing and the theory of the firm: Some evidence. The Journal of
Law and Economics, 26(3), 613–633.
Williamson, O. E. (1974). Markets and hierarchies. New York: Free Press.
Worchel, P. (1979). Trust and distrust. In W. G. Austin, & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup
relations (pp. 174–187). Belmont: Wadsworth.
Zacharakis, A. L., & Shepherd, D. A. (2001). The nature of information and overconfidence on venture capitalists’
decision making. Journal of Business Venturing, 16(4), 311–332.

Appendix

Wording of the auditor’s report used for the ISA 700


[KAM negative/KAM positive] group
[Format differs from case material; accentuation for illustration purposes only]
INDEPENDENT AUDITOR’S REPORT
[Appropriate Addressee]
Report on the Financial Statements
We have audited the accompanying financial statements of the Alpha Group, which comprise
the statement of financial position as at December 31, 2012, and the statement of comprehensive
income, statement of changes in equity and statement of cash flows for the year then ended, and a
summary of significant accounting policies and other explanatory information.
Management’s Responsibility for the Financial Statements
Management is responsible for the preparation and fair presentation of these financial state-
ments in accordance with International Financial Reporting Standards, and for such internal
control as management determines is necessary to enable the preparation of financial statements
that are free from material misstatement, whether due to fraud or error.
Auditor’s Responsibility
Our responsibility is to express an opinion on these financial statements based on our audit. We
conducted our audit in accordance with International Standards on Auditing. Those standards
require that we comply with ethical requirements and plan and perform the audit to obtain reason-
able assurance about whether the financial statements are free from material misstatement.
An audit involves performing procedures to obtain audit evidence about the amounts and dis-
closures in the financial statements. The procedures selected depend on the auditor’s judgment,
including the assessment of the risks of material misstatement of the financial statements,
whether due to fraud or error. In making those risk assessments, the auditor considers internal
control relevant to the entity’s preparation and fair presentation of the financial statements in
order to design audit procedures that are appropriate in the circumstances, but not for the
purpose of expressing an opinion on the effectiveness of the entity’s internal control. An audit
also includes evaluating the appropriateness of accounting policies used and the reasonableness
of accounting estimates made by management, as well as evaluating the overall presentation of
128 A. Köhler et al.

the financial statements. We believe that the audit evidence we have obtained is sufficient and
appropriate to provide a basis for our audit opinion.
Opinion
In our opinion, the financial statements present fairly, in all material respects, the financial pos-
ition of Alpha Group as at December 31, 2012, and its financial performance and its cash flows for
the year then ended in accordance with International Financial Reporting Standards.
Key Audit Matters
Key audit matters are those matters that, in our professional judgment, were of most signifi-
cance in our audit of the consolidated financial statements. Key audit matters are selected
from the matters communicated with those charged with governance, but are not intended to rep-
resent all matters that were discussed with them. Our audit procedures relating to these matters
were designed in the context of our audit of the consolidated financial statements as a whole. Our
opinion on the consolidated financial statements is not modified with respect to any of the key
audit matters described below, and we do not express an opinion on these individual matters.
Goodwill
Under IFRSs, the Alpha Group is required to annually test the amount of goodwill for impair-
ment. This annual impairment test was significant to our audit because the assessment process is
complex and highly judgmental and is based on assumptions that are affected by expected future
market or economic conditions, particularly those in Europe. As a result, our audit procedures
included, among others, using a valuation expert to assist us in evaluating the assumptions and
methodologies used by the Alpha Group, in particular those relating to the forecasted revenue
growth and profit margins for the cash generating units Beta AG and Gamma GmbH. We also
focused on the adequacy of the Alpha Group’s disclosures about those assumptions to which
the outcome of the impairment test is most sensitive, that is, those that have the most significant
effect on the determination of the recoverable amount of goodwill.
[KAM negative]
Already small changes in the key assumptions used (see Alpha Group’s disclosures about
goodwill in Note 1) could give rise to an impairment of the goodwill balance in the future.
[KAM positive]
Only large changes in the key assumptions used (see Alpha Group’s disclosures about good-
will in Note 1) could give rise to an impairment of the goodwill balance in the future.
[Auditor’s signature]
[Date of the auditor’s report]
[Auditor’s address]

You might also like