You are on page 1of 6

1

Bi-manual Sensory Discrimination: A Kinesthetic


Study
Suhas Kakade, Subhasis Chaudhuri, Fellow, IEEE, and Abhishek Gupta

Abstract—The ability of humans to perceive and differentiate non-dominant hands. For a given reference signal, we define
kinesthetic sensory information significantly influences our daily ρ as the smallest detectable difference between sequentially
activities and motor control. This study examines the impact presented force signals. This threshold is computed as the
of asynchronous bi-manual discrimination tasks in comparison
to uni-manual discrimination tasks on kinesthetic perception. difference between 50% and 84% detection of stimulus [3],
Our study aims to reveal the relationship between kinesthetic [5]. In this case, we have used difference between the mag-
perception of haptic signals by examining perceptual thresholds nitude of force signal as a stimulus. In contrast to uni-
in three different scenarios using (i) the dominant hand, (ii) manual haptic perception, which uses only one hand, in this
the non-dominant hand, and (iii) both hands simultaneously to work, bi-manual haptic perception, which specifically involves
differentiate between two successive signals. Subjects exposed to
force signals in these three situations conveyed their perceptions simultaneously perceiving the distinct haptic force signals with
of alterations in signal magnitude after each trial. Subsequently, both hands is specifically investigated. Thus, this study aims
we applied psychometric functions to the collected responses to to determine whether the kinesthetic perception of individuals
determine perceptual thresholds. Our results indicate a substan- changes when using both hands, as opposed to one.
tial difference in threshold values between bi-manual and uni- The null hypothesis Ho and alternative hypothesis Ha
manual scenarios, with the bi-manual scenario exhibiting higher
thresholds, indicating inferior perceptual ability when both hands evaluated in this work are stated below.
are simultaneously utilized in two separate discrimination tasks. Ho : ρ for both hands during the bi-manual and uni-manual
Furthermore, our investigation reveals distinct perception thresh- perception of force signals are equal.
olds between the dominant and non-dominant hands, owing to Ha : ρ for both hands during the bi-manual and uni-manual
differences in the perceptual capability of the two hands. These perception of force signals are unequal.
findings provide substantial insight into how the nature of tasks
may alter kinesthetic perception, offering implications for the
To accomplish this, we gathered collectively over 72 h
development of haptic interfaces in practical applications. of data from 18 volunteers who perceived force signals
in three different phases with their dominant hand, non-
Index Terms—Kinesthetic perception, uni-manual haptic per-
ception, bi-manual haptic perception, perceptual threshold, Hu- dominant hand, and both hands simultaneously. They were
man performance. asked to differentiate among the magnitude of sequentially
presented force signals in each setting and their responses
were recorded. Additionally, we recorded the response time
I. I NTRODUCTION
of subjects perceiving changes in force. To determine ρ values

T OUCH is crucial to human perception as it allows us to


interact with the physical world. The ability to interpret
and discriminate force sensations using muscles, tendons, and
for force signals in bi-manual and uni-manual paradigms, we
subsequently applied psychometric functions [6]–[8] on the
collected responses, and the obtained ρ values are compared.
joints is known as kinesthetic haptic perception [1], [2]. This The key contributions of this paper are:
sensory experience allows us to recognize, differentiate, and • We developed an experimental protocol to study the
identify objects through touch. influence of asynchronous bi-manual discrimination tasks
Humans have perceptual constraints that limit their capa- on kinesthetic perception.
bility to recognize or discriminate such inputs. The absolute • We compared perceptual thresholds for asynchronous dis-
threshold (AL) is the smallest stimulus required for a person crimination tasks in bi-manual and uni-manual contexts.
to recognize the presence of a sensory input [3]. Usually, AL • The results demonstrated significantly higher ρvalues in
is considered as a stimulus strength that can be detected in the bi-manual setting, suggesting a reduced haptic percep-
50% of instances. Weber’s law of perception [4] is used to tion when using both hands. This discovery suggests that
define the threshold at which perception occurs. It stipulates processing information from two sources simultaneously
that a change in a signal will only be recognized if it exceeds might diminish sensitivity to haptic input.
a specific threshold compared to the prior stimulus. At a • The study also discovered that the perception thresholds
given stimulus intensity I, the relation between just noticeable for the dominant and non-dominant hands differed, in-
difference (JND) δI and Weber threshold d can be defined as dicating that the two hands may have different levels of
δI = dI (1) perceptual awareness.

The present research aims to determine the haptic perception II. P RIOR W ORK
threshold ρ for an asynchronous task in both the bi-manual Estimating JND and Weber threshold has been the subject
and uni-manual modes, examining both the dominant and of extensive investigation for many years. [9], [10] discuss the
2

estimation of JND on offline data in a uni-manual setting and ing a separate object. They observed that both bi-manual
their application in adaptive sampling. They examined the JND and uni-manual explorations are equally effective, indicating
for force signals in uni-manual conditions and found that it that inter-hemispheric communication does not reduce the
depends on the magnitude of the force exerted. They noted that ability for shape discrimination tasks. While successive bi-
for low force levels, JND was smaller, and as the force level manual exploration led to good shape discrimination, subjects
increased, the JND grew larger. In [11]–[13], Weber’s law is struggled when comparing shapes simultaneously. It suggests
applied to the perception-based data reduction method to lower that either interference between brain hemispheres or the
the packet rate in the teleoperation system. It is accomplished challenge of attending to multiple objects at once reduces
by discarding haptic samples that are undetectable by humans shape discrimination.
and only transmitting sampled data whose value exceeds [23] performed experimentation similar to the work carried
a specified threshold. [10], [14] examined the variation in out in this paper. They obtained JND for synchronous force
Weber threshold caused by the shift in force direction for the signals in the x-direction while the hands are moving. A
multidimensional signals. staircase approach was utilized to generate the random forces.
In [15], tests of manual dexterity and kinesthetic discrimina- They investigated how the dominant and non-dominant hands
tion were conducted to investigate a correlation between these perceive these force signals when they both are in simultane-
skills. The results revealed a significant positive correlation ous motion. A 3-AFC paradigm was used to determine JND.
(p < 0.05) between them. Thus, individuals with better manual The results indicated that when both hands were perceiving
dexterity tend to exhibit strong kinesthetic discrimination the haptic signals, their ability to detect changes in force
ability, and vice versa. Thus, strengthening one of these skills diminished. The statistical analysis demonstrated a strong level
can help improve the other. In many areas, like rehabilitation, of confidence in the significance of these findings for the non-
sports training, etc., where both manual dexterity and kines- dominant hand, whereas the confidence level for the dominant
thetic discrimination play critical roles, comprehending and hand was lower. Thus, they were unable to conclude for the
utilizing this relationship could be beneficial. dominant hand.
[16] investigated whether Weber’s law equally applies to In real-life scenarios, events often occur asynchronously.
simultaneous and non-simultaneous (successive) viewing con- Thus, in this work, we focused on an asynchronous discrim-
ditions. Weber carried out an experiment in [17] on successive ination task when the hands are at a particular position. The
viewing and found that the range of Weber fraction is less than method of constant stimuli is used for determining ρ. As each
4% for discriminating the length of lines. He also observed sample of the random force is an instance of a random variable
that Weber’s law applies to successive viewing conditions and uniformly distributed between 0.5 N to 2.5 N compared to a
not to simultaneous viewing conditions. Hence, both condition reference force of 1.5 N, it allows for better estimation of ρ as
gives different threshold values. compared to a fixed set of input stimuli. Also, as the trials are
To the best of our knowledge, very little research exists independent of each other, any learning or adaptation effects
on bi-manual and uni-manual exploration or discrimination that can arise in an adaptive approach like the staircase method
tasks of haptic perception. [18] investigated unilateral and can be minimized. Here, we observed with a high degree of
bilateral haptic discrimination of curved hand-sized surfaces. confidence that perception reduces during a bi-manual setting
The results showed that discrimination of curvature did not for both the dominant and non-dominant hand. However, our
follow Weber’s law, and unilateral discrimination was con- approach may consume a considerable amount of time as it
sistently better than bilateral, possibly due to the placement requires a large number of trials at multiple intensity levels.
order, which had a significant effect on the bilateral condition. The paper is organized as follows. Section III delves into the
[19] explored how hands interact when manipulating a shared experimental setup required to estimate perception thresholds.
object with specialized exoskeletons. Findings indicated that Section IV comprehensively analyzes and deliberates on the
force imbalances were linked to the movement direction and acquired findings. Section V contains the conclusions derived
handedness, with right-handers showing directional asymme- from the study.
tries related to movement direction, while left-handers dis-
played more precise control of the force applied between their
III. E XPERIMENTAL S ETUP
hands.
A study conducted in [20] estimated the JND for recog- In order to determine the perception threshold of kinesthetic
nizing the force magnitude difference between the human force stimuli, an experimental setup is described in this sec-
hand’s left and right index fingers using a contra-lateral tion. Fig. 1 illustrates the setup used for our haptic experiment.
force-matching paradigm. They obtained a JND of 10% and Two Phantom Omni haptic devices capable of generating
observed that subjects tended to underestimate the force at force sensations are connected to a separate personal computer.
high levels and overestimate it at low levels. Researchers The human subject holds the stylus of each of these devices in
examined the perception of weight when lifting virtual boxes their hand. The study is divided into three distinct phases. In
of varying weights using either one hand or both hands in each of these phases, different (left or right) or both hands
[21]. Results indicated that the weight feels lighter when both are used for perceiving force signals as described below.
hands are engaged in lifting. However, specific details of the presentation of force signals
In [22], subjects explored object shapes using either one and recording of responses are discussed in more detail in the
hand to compare two objects or both hands, each explor- subsection III-B.
3

seating capacity of 6 people. Normal laboratory activities were


permitted during the experiment, including group discussion
and movement near the door. A distance of about 40 cm
is maintained between the subject’s shoulders and the haptic
devices. During the experiment, subjects’ arms and wrists were
fixed at a position while perceiving the force signals. The
experiments were implemented on Intel I3 processor-based
personal computers operating at a frequency of 3.4 GHz using
C++ programming language in the Visual Studio 2019.
Fig. 1. Experimental setup for an asynchronous bi-manual task. A. Haptic Device
In order to generate the kinesthetic force signals, we utilized
the Phantom Omni [24] haptic device. The device has six
• Phase I - Dominant hand:
degrees of freedom for versatile control. HAPI [25], an open-
In the first phase, subjects utilize their dominant hand,
source software platform was used alongside this device to
the one typically more skilled in tasks to perceive force
provide a robust foundation for signal generation and manipu-
signals. In this phase, they grasp the stylus of one of
lation. The Phantom Omni could collectively exert a maximum
the haptic devices with their dominant hand. In each
force of 3.3 N along the x, y, and z directions at an update rate
trial, they are presented with two separate force signals
of 1 kHz. It has a detachable stylus, which can be held like a
in quick succession on the stylus. They may indicate
pen, as shown in Fig. 1. The stylus features two programmable
whether they can detect the difference in magnitude
buttons, one of which is bright grey and the other is dark grey.
between these two signals.
These buttons can be used to initiate particular tasks or record
• Phase II - Non-dominant hand:
responses during interactions.
In the second phase, subjects switch to their non-
dominant hand to sense the haptic signals, grasping the B. Stimuli Characteristics
stylus of any haptic device with their non-dominant hand.
Our experimental stimuli revolved around piece-wise con-
Once again, during each trial, they receive two distinct
stant force signals. During each trial of the experiment,
force signals in rapid succession on the stylus and may
subjects will encounter two distinct force signals one after
determine whether they can distinguish a difference in
another in quick succession, which can be likened to the
magnitude between the two signals.
sensation of force acting upon their hands. Both signals are
• Phase III - Engaging both hands:
unidirectional and precisely aligned with the z-direction of
This phase is equivalent to conducting both phases I and
the haptic device. The forces are applied either towards or
II of the experiment together. In this phase, subjects feel
away from the subject, creating a sensation akin to forces
separate forces on both hands at the same time. This
being exerted onto or away from the subject’s hand. Initially, a
phase involves holding the stylus of both haptic devices
reference force F1 is established as a baseline for comparison.
simultaneously, one in each hand. During each trial, each
It is always constant with a magnitude of 1.5 N. The ensuing
personal computer generates two different haptic signals.
force signal F2 may then deviate from this reference signal,
These signals are applied to the respectively connected
thereby offering subjects distinct experiences that could be
haptic devices. The subjects will perceive these signals
perceptually discerned. F2 is always random in magnitude and
in such a way that their dominant hand receives two
uniformly distributed between 0.5 to 2.5 N.
consecutive signals from one haptic device while their
The force F1 is always of a 5 s duration, as shown in Fig. 2.
non-dominant hand simultaneously but asynchronously
However, the duration of F2 is dependent on the response of
receives the other two signals from the second haptic
the subject. It varies from a minimal value to a maximum
device. Subjects will independently assess whether they
duration of 5 s. If the subject reports a change in the signal
detect any changes in the signals experienced by each of
level, the F1 for the next trial will start immediately, ending
their hands.
F2 for the current trial at that instance. The maximum duration
Their response can fall into any of these four cases:
of 5 s is decided from past experience considering the time
1) Detected a change in the magnitude of respectively required to acquire the signal as stated in [10], [26]. The per-
perceived force signals by both hands. ception of force could change at the points where magnitude
2) Detected a change in the magnitude of force signals varies from one level to another between force signals. These
for the dominant hand but not for the non-dominant meticulously designed force signals played a crucial role in
hand. exploring kinesthetic force perception thresholds.
3) Detected a change in the magnitude of force signals
for the non-dominant hand but not for the dominant C. Methodology
hand. Subjects had a thorough introduction to the haptic device
4) Did not detect a change in the magnitude of respec- before initiating the experiment. To help them become com-
tively perceived force signals by both hands. fortable using the haptic device, considerable training was pro-
The experiment was carried out in a laboratory with a vided. Subsequently, subjects received clear explanations about
4

(a) (b)
Fig. 2. Force stimuli used in the experiment. (a) Generated force signals in the absence of the subject’s feedback. (b) Alteration in the signal following the
subject’s feedback.

the experiment’s purpose and objectives, and any questions The state of these signals again changes to the corresponding
they had were thoroughly addressed. In order to ensure the F2 at t5 and t6 , respectively. Depending upon signal variation,
focused engagement of subjects, they were instructed to wear the dominant hand detected signal change at t7 ; however, the
headphones and close their eyes during experimentation. These non-dominant hand could not detect such change in the signal
precautions were taken to reduce any outside distractions that experienced by it. Consequently, variations in response times
might have affected their perception during the experiment. could cause the signals experienced by both hands to change
During each phase of the experimentation, 500 trials were asynchronously.
conducted. In each trial of phase I, subjects hold the stylus The sequence of operations followed during experimenta-
using their dominant hand. The causal force signals, F1 and tion is mentioned in the algorithm 1.
F2 , were presented consecutively. Subjects were instructed to
provide feedback whenever they noticed a change in force Algorithm 1 Experimental Procedure
magnitude. Subjects typically experience variations in the Step 1: Hold a stylus in the dominant hand for an uni-manual
magnitude of the force when the signal state changes. If the experience.
subject sensed the variation, the next trial was started imme- Step 2: Initiate the experiment at a convenience.
diately after the subject’s feedback was registered. However, Step 3: Record the position of the stylus before presenting the
if the subject did not perceive any change in the magnitude of force signal.
force signals, F2 lasts for the complete 5 s duration before the Step 4: Generate forces and apply them to the stylus one after
start of the next trial. The same procedure was then repeated another.
for phases II and III. Step 5: Ask the subject, “Whenever you perceive any change
In phase III of the experiment, the force signals generated in the force, press any button”.
by two different computers were sent to the connected haptic Step 6: If subject responds, capture response time of the
device. Both computers ran at the same frequency and were subject.
programmed to generate similar signals. However, as the Step 7: Repeat steps 4 to 6 for 500 samples.
signals were generated using different computers, they were Step 8: Repeat steps 2 to 7 for the other hand and finally for
independent of each other. To begin the experiment simultane- both hands for a bi-manual experience.
ously, the subjects were instructed to click any of the buttons
on each stylus. As shown in Fig. 3, the signals perceived by
the dominant and non-dominant hands started with F1 at the
same time instance t1 , and transitioned to respective F2 at t2 . D. Data Collection
Subjects independently responded to signal variation at t3 for Proper ethical permission was obtained from the Institute
authorities to involve human volunteers in the experiment. For
the input stimuli discussed in the previous subsection III-B,
data has been collected from 18 subjects. The data consists of
responses of subjects with response time. All the individuals
participated in the experiment voluntarily; they were not paid
any compensation. They gave written consent before starting
the experiment. These individuals were within the age group
of 24-37, comprising 14 males and 04 females. Among the 18
subjects, 14 were right-handed, whereas 04 were left-handed.
Among these individuals, 15 subjects were unfamiliar with the
haptic device, whereas 03 subjects were regular users of the
Fig. 3. Asynchronous force signals perceived by the dominant hand (DHS)
(in red) and the non-dominant hand (NHS) (in green) in a bi-manual scenario. haptic device. None of the subjects suffered from any neuro-
physiological disorder.
the dominant hand and at t4 for the non-dominant hand, start- If the experiment is carried out for a longer duration in
ing the next trial with F1 for the respective hands immediately. a single sitting, there may be a possibility of perceptual
5

fatigue, which will impact the perception threshold. Hence,


the subjects were not allowed to sit for a duration longer than
30 min. Therefore, multiple sessions with each subject were
necessary to collect comprehensive data. The overall duration
required to perform the experiment was approximately 4 h per
subject.

IV. R ESULTS
To understand the distribution of the force signal F2 , the
complete spectrum of values representing F2 for all the Fig. 5. Response of a right-handed subject.
subjects is gathered. A cumulative frequency plot of these
values, as illustrated in Fig. 4 displays a ramp-like shape, TABLE I
ρ AND Tr FOR A RIGHT- HANDED SUBJECT. H ERE , DH STANDS FOR
confirming that F2 is uniformly distributed. DOMINANT HAND AND NH STANDS FOR NON - DOMINANT HAND .

Experimentation Phase AL ρ Tr (ms)


I - DH 0.2157 0.0604 378.6604
II - NH 0.2457 0.0774 408.8196
III - DH 0.2483 0.0958 394.3995
III - NH 0.2836 0.1124 416.3575

Fig. 4. Cumulative frequency of F2 experienced by all the subjects.

For each of the subjects, we apply the psychometric func-


tion [8] to the responses gathered during each phase of the
experimentation to obtain AL and ρ. The response of one
of the right-handed subjects is depicted in Fig. 5. The x-axis
represents an absolute difference between the forces F1 and Fig. 6. Response of a left-handed subject.
F2 , whereas the y-axis represents the probability of successful
perception. The illustration demonstrates that this right-handed
individual readily discerns signal changes when conducting an similar pattern is also observed in a bi-manual case. However,
experiment exclusively with their dominant hand. However, there is a difference between Tr of the uni-manual and the
their perception experiences a minor decline when performing bi-manual setting for both hands.
the same experiment with their non-dominant hand. During
phase III of the experiment, when both hands are engaged in TABLE II
ρ AND Tr FOR A LEFT- HANDED SUBJECT
the tasks, the perception further decreases for both hands, with
the dominant hand exhibiting a slightly superior perceptual Experimentation Phase AL ρ Tr (ms)
capability compared to the non-dominant hand. The corre- I - DH 0.3650 0.1120 408.1397
sponding ρ values for each of these phases for the same subject II - NH 0.3428 0.1116 404.5294
are shown in Table I. From these, it is observed that AL, ρ, and III - DH 0.3896 0.1553 421. 7670
average response time Tr are smaller for the dominant hand III - NH 0.4385 0.1415 421.2940
as compared to the non-dominant hand in both uni-manual as
well as bi-manual perception. It indicates that the dominant In a similar manner, from the recorded responses of each
hand has better perception than the non-dominant hand. It is of the subjects, the values of AL and ρ are obtained.
also observed that ρ and Tr have been increased during the
bi-manual tasks as compared to the uni-manual tasks, thus 1) Statistical inference: The comparison of ρ for all the
degrading the perception of both hands. subjects is carried out using paired t-tests for all three phases of
Similarly, Fig. 6 and Table II show the response of one experimentation as described in Table III. For all four cases of
of the left-handed subjects. Here, we see the same pattern as comparison, we obtained p-values smaller than 0.05, indicating
observed in the case of the right-handed subject in the uni- the difference in the mean of threshold values of the respective
manual case. Also, the perceptual capability of both hands cases of comparisons.
reduces during a bi-manual discrimination task. However, in Further, we carried out a comparison between the dominant
phase III of the experiment, the non-dominant hand slightly hand and non-dominant hand perception thresholds using the
performed better than the dominant hand. In the uni-manual Repeated Measures Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) test for
case, Tr is observed to be nearly equal for both hands. A bi-manual and uni-manual perception of force signals. For a
6

TABLE III [5] W. M. B. Tiest and A. M. Kappers, “Cues for haptic perception of
C OMPARISON OF ρ USING PAIRED T- TEST FOR α = 0.05. H ERE , M compliance,” IEEE transactions on haptics, vol. 2, no. 4, pp. 189–199,
STANDS FOR THE GROUP MEAN OF THE PERCEPTUAL THRESHOLD AND SD 2009.
FOR THE CORRESPONDING STANDARD DEVIATION . [6] M. A. Garcı́a-Pérez, R. Alcalá-Quintana, R. L. Woods, and E. Peli,
“Psychometric functions for detection and discrimination with and
Comparison of ρ between t-value p-value Confidence Interval without flankers,” Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, vol. 73, pp.
DH in uni-manual (M = 0.1929, SD = 0.0792)
and bi-manual (M = 0.2354, SD = 0.0915) scenario
2.5369 0.02128 [0.007155, 0.07785] 829–853, 2011.
NH in uni-manual (M = 0.2342, SD = 0.0914)
3.0467 0.00729 [0.0127, 0.06988]
[7] F. A. Wichmann and N. J. Hill, “The psychometric function: I. fitting,
and bi-manual (M = 0.2755, SD = 0.0978) scenario sampling, and goodness of fit,” Perception & psychophysics, vol. 63,
DH (M = 0.1929, SD = 0.0792) and NH
(M = 0.2342, SD = 0.0914) in uni-manual scenario
4.1305 0.0006991 [0.02023, 0.06247] no. 8, pp. 1293–1313, 2001.
DH (M = 0.2354, SD = 0.0915) and NH
2.973 0.008532 [0.01165, 0.06862]
[8] K. Zchaluk and D. H. Foster, “Model-free estimation of the psychometric
(M = 0.2755, SD = 0.0978) in bi-manual scenario function,” Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, vol. 71, no. 6, pp.
1414–1425, 2009.
[9] O. Dabeer and S. Chaudhuri, “Analysis of an adaptive sampler based
significance level α = 0.05, we obtained a p-value of 0.000005 on weber’s law,” IEEE transactions on signal processing, vol. 59, no. 4,
pp. 1868–1878, 2010.
and F ratio of 11.9754. From the ANOVA based statistical [10] S. Chaudhuri and A. Bhardwaj, “Kinesthetic perception,” A Machine
analysis, it is evident that the means of all four groups are Learning Approach, 2018.
statistically different. Thus, for the considered experimental [11] P. Hinterseer, S. Hirche, S. Chaudhuri, E. Steinbach, and M. Buss,
“Perception-based data reduction and transmission of haptic data in
setup, our assertion that the bi-manual and uni-manual percep- telepresence and teleaction systems,” IEEE Transactions on Signal
tion threshold differs from each other is statistically supported. Processing, vol. 56, no. 2, pp. 588–597, 2008.
As seen in Tables I and II, there seems to be variation [12] V. Gokhale, J. Nair, S. Chaudhuri, and S. Kakade, “Network-aware
adaptive sampling for low bitrate telehaptic communication,” in Haptics:
in Tr for individual subjects. Thus, we performed a similar Science, Technology, and Applications: 11th International Conference,
analysis for Tr using paired t-test. It is observed that there EuroHaptics 2018, Pisa, Italy, June 13-16, 2018, Proceedings, Part II
is no significant difference between the means of respective 11. Springer, 2018, pp. 660–672.
[13] S. Kakade and S. Chaudhuri, “Perceptually compressive communication
Tr . We used a Repeated Measures ANOVA test to further of interactive telehaptic signal,” in International Conference on Human
evaluate the variations in Tr between the dominant and non- Haptic Sensing and Touch Enabled Computer Applications. Springer,
dominant hands for bi-manual and uni-manual perception of 2020, pp. 480–488.
[14] H. Pongrac, P. Hinterseer, J. Kammerl, E. Steinbach, B. Färber,
force signals. For α = 0.05, a p-value of 0.3300 and an U. Muenchen, and T. Muenchen, “Limitations of human 3d force
associated F ratio of 1.1706 were obtained from the analysis. discrimination,” Human-Centered Robotics Systems, vol. 2006, 2006.
The statistical tests did not find significant differences between [15] I. L. Elfant, “Correlation between kinesthetic discrimination & manual
dexterity.” The American Journal of Occupational Therapy: Official
the four cases, making it inconclusive whether the dominant Publication of the American Occupational Therapy Association, vol. 31,
hand responds faster than the non-dominant hand in uni- no. 1, pp. 23–28, 1977.
manual versus the bi-manual conditions based on average [16] H. Ono, “Difference threshold for stimulus length under simultaneous
and nonsimultaneous viewing conditions,” Perception & Psychophysics,
response time. vol. 2, pp. 201–207, 1967.
[17] E. Weber, “Der tastsinn und das gemeinfühl: In: Handwörterbuch der
physiologie, vol 3 (wagner r, ed),” Braunschweig, Germany: Vieweg,
V. C ONCLUSION 1846.
The study investigated and compared the perceptual thresh- [18] A. M. Kappers and J. J. Koenderink, “Haptic unilateral and bilateral
discrimination of curved surfaces,” Perception, vol. 25, no. 6, pp. 739–
olds for bi-manual and uni-manual haptic perception. The 749, 1996.
statistical analysis showed that the threshold values were sub- [19] E. Galofaro, E. D’Antonio, N. Lotti, F. Patané, M. Casadio, and
stantially higher in the bi-manual haptic perception condition, L. Masia, “Bimanual motor strategies and handedness role in human-
robot haptic interaction,” IEEE Transactions on Haptics, 2023.
indicating a reduced ability to perceive haptic information [20] M. Raghu Prasad, S. Purswani, and M. Manivannan, “Force jnd for
while utilizing both hands simultaneously. The study also right index finger using contra lateral force matching paradigm,” in
discovered significant variations in perceptual threshold values ICoRD’13: Global Product Development. Springer, 2013, pp. 365–
375.
between dominant and non-dominant hands, showing varia- [21] C. Giachritsis, J. Barrio, M. Ferre, A. Wing, and J. Ortego, “Evaluation
tions in perceptual responsiveness between the two hands. of weight perception during unimanual and bimanual manipulation
These findings emphasize the significance of investigating hap- of virtual objects,” in World Haptics 2009-Third Joint EuroHaptics
conference and Symposium on Haptic Interfaces for Virtual Environment
tic perception in uni-manual and bi-manual settings. By better and Teleoperator Systems. IEEE, 2009, pp. 629–634.
comprehending such information, one can create interfaces [22] C. J. Dowell, J. F. Norman, J. R. Moment, L. M. Shain, H. F. Norman,
that provide users with better-optimized haptic feedback. F. Phillips, and A. M. Kappers, “Haptic shape discrimination and
interhemispheric communication,” Scientific Reports, vol. 8, no. 1, p.
377, 2018.
R EFERENCES [23] J. Awed, I. H. Elhajj, and N. Slobodenyuk, “Haptic force perception in
bimanual manipulation,” in Haptics: Perception, Devices, Mobility, and
[1] V. Hayward, O. R. Astley, M. Cruz-Hernandez, D. Grant, and G. Robles- Communication: International Conference, EuroHaptics 2012, Tampere,
De-La-Torre, “Haptic interfaces and devices,” Sensor review, vol. 24, Finland, June 13-15, 2012 Proceedings, Part II. Springer, 2012, pp.
no. 1, pp. 16–29, 2004. 1–6.
[2] C. Pacchierotti, D. Prattichizzo, and K. J. Kuchenbecker, “Cutaneous [24] “Omni device guide,” https://s3.amazonaws.com/dl.3dsystems.com/
feedback of fingertip deformation and vibration for palpation in robotic binaries/Sensable/UserGuide/Omni Device Guide.pdf, Feb 2013.
surgery,” IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, vol. 63, no. 2, [25] “Hapi manual for version 1.4.0,” https://h3d.org/wp-content/uploads/
pp. 278–287, 2015. 2021/02/H3DAPI 2.4/doc/HAPI\%20Manual.pdf, 2019.
[3] L. A. Jones and H. Z. Tan, “Application of psychophysical techniques [26] O. F. Nadjarbashi, Z. Najdovski, and S. Nahavandi, “Event-driven data
to haptic research,” IEEE transactions on haptics, vol. 6, no. 3, pp. transmission in variable-delay network,” in 2017 IEEE International
268–284, 2012. Conference on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics (SMC). IEEE, 2017,
[4] Weber’s law. [Online]. Available: https://www.britannica.com/science/ pp. 1681–1686.
Webers-law

You might also like