You are on page 1of 14

Journal of Educational Psychology © 2012 American Psychological Association

2013, Vol. 105, No. 1, 25–38 0022-0663/13/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0029692

Examining Dimensions of Self-Efficacy for Writing

Roger Bruning, Michael Dempsey, Courtney McKim


and Douglas F. Kauffman University of Wyoming
University of Nebraska, Lincoln

Sharon Zumbrunn
Virginia Commonwealth University

A multifactor perspective on writing self-efficacy was examined in 2 studies. Three factors were
proposed—self-efficacy for writing ideation, writing conventions, and writing self-regulation—and a
scale constructed to reflect these factors. In Study 1, middle school students (N ⫽ 697) completed the
Self-Efficacy for Writing Scale (SEWS), along with associated measures. Confirmatory factor analysis
(CFA) showed SEWS data fit the proposed 3-factor model well. In Study 2, a second CFA of data from
563 students from 2 high schools likewise showed good model fit. Scores based on the 3 writing
self-efficacy factors were examined in relation to students’ liking writing, self-reported writing grades,
and statewide writing assessment (SWA) scores. Results showed writing ideation and self-regulation
self-efficacy to be significantly more strongly related to liking writing than conventions self-efficacy but
less related than conventions self-efficacy to SWA scores. All 3 writing self-efficacy dimensions showed
moderate positive correlations with self-reported writing performance. Further analyses showed higher
levels for all 3 dimensions of writing self-efficacy for students in more advanced English/language arts
classes. Overall, results from the studies were interpreted as supporting multifactor models of writing
self-efficacy and the utility of closer ties between self-efficacy measures and domains being assessed.

Keywords: writing, self-efficacy, ideation, conventions, self-regulation

Few perceptions about the self are more important than self- variety of other writing-related variables (McCarthy, Meier, &
efficacy beliefs— confidence that one can perform successfully in Rinderer, 1985; Pajares, 2003, 2007; Pajares & Johnson, 1996;
a particular domain (Bandura, 1997, 2006). Willingness to engage Pajares, Miller, & Johnson, 1999; Pajares & Valiante, 1997, 2006;
in domain-related activities and persist when confronted with Shell, Colvin, & Bruning, 1995; Shell, Murphy, & Bruning, 1989;
difficulties or distractions are only two of self-efficacy’s important Zimmerman & Bandura, 1994). This prior research also has helped
correlates. Higher self-efficacy levels relate to multiple positive attune researchers and practitioners to writing’s motivational and
outcomes, including setting higher goals, using more effective self-regulatory dimensions (e.g., Bruning & Horn, 2000; Graham,
learning strategies, and having lower anxiety (Bong, 2006). Self- 2006; Hayes, 1996; Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 1999, 2002, 2007).
efficacy becomes especially critical when domain-related tasks are At the same time, however, it seems to have shed relatively little
demanding and motivational conditions are less than ideal. Writing light on the processes involved in the act of writing. With only a
is one such domain. few exceptions, writing self-efficacy measures have tended to
This article describes a new perspective on writing self-efficacy. atheoretically sample writing activities, tasks, and outcomes. We
It builds on a large body of prior research showing self-efficacy’s propose that aligning measures more explicitly with psychological
importance for successful writing performance and its ties to a and linguistic features of the writing process may provide oppor-
tunities to learn more about both self-efficacy for writing and
writing itself.
This article was published Online First August 13, 2012. The starting point for this study, therefore, was an assumption
Roger Bruning, Michael Dempsey, and Douglas F. Kauffman, Depart- that a multifactor portrayal of writing self-efficacy, as contrasted
ment of Educational Psychology, University of Nebraska, Lincoln; Court- with a more global sampling view, might better reflect self-
ney McKim, Department of Professional Studies, University of Wyoming; efficacy for writing. Based on models and research emphasizing
Sharon Zumbrunn, Foundations of Education Department, Virginia Com- idea translation and the role of language-related processes in
monwealth University. writing (e.g., Beers & Nagy, 2009; Bereiter & Scardamalia, 1987;
We want to thank members of the Nebraska Writing Research Group for Flower & Hayes, 1981, 1984; Hayes, 2012; Myhill, 2008;
their suggestions and feedback and express our deep appreciation to the
Schleppegrell, 2007) and research showing strong effects of strat-
administration and teachers of the Lincoln (NE) Public Schools, especially
Leslie Lukin, Karen Saunders, and David Smith, for their generous support
egy instruction on writing quality (e.g., Graham & Perin, 2007;
throughout the planning and conduct of this research. Harris, Graham, & Mason, 2006), we posited three classes of
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Roger Brun- activities as consistently involved in the writing act: (1) generating
ing, Center for Instructional Innovation, 209 Teachers College Hall, University ideas (ideation), (2) expressing those ideas using writing’s
of Nebraska, Lincoln, NE 68588-0384. E-mail: rbruning@unl.edu language-related tools (conventions), and (3) managing writing

25
26 BRUNING, DEMPSEY, KAUFFMAN, MCKIM, AND ZUMBRUNN

decisions and behaviors (self-regulation). This model guided con- Shell et al.’s self-efficacy scales were similar to those they had
struction of a writing self-efficacy measure. We then examined the used earlier but were adapted to fit this study’s younger partici-
model’s theoretical and practical implications in two studies, one pants. Writing skills self-efficacy was shown to predict writing
with middle school students and a second with high school stu- performance at all grade levels but, unlike writing task self-
dents. efficacy, did not increase significantly with grade level, suggesting
that writing self-efficacy gains are more related to students’ abil-
Prior Measures of Writing Self-Efficacy ities to successfully perform various writing tasks than to changes
in specific writing skills.
In this section we present examples of previous research on
writing self-efficacy that informed our current effort. Our primary
goal here is not to comprehensively review writing self-efficacy Research by Pajares and Associates
research, which has been done elsewhere (e.g., Klassen, 2002; In work beginning in the 1990s, Pajares and his associates (e.g.,
Pajares, 2003), but to illustrate how writing self-efficacy has been Pajares, 2003, 2007; Pajares, Britner, & Valiante, 2000; Pajares et
conceptualized and assessed. We focus specifically on the mea-
al., 1999; Pajares & Valiante, 1997, 1999, 2001, 2006) developed
sures utilized in these studies and on views of writing that each
what arguably has been the most comprehensive writing self-
seems to reflect, beginning with research by McCarthy and her
efficacy research program to date. In an early study, Pajares and
associates (1985).
Johnson (1994) found that writing skills but not writing task
self-efficacy predicted students’ actual performance in composing
Research by McCarthy and Associates essays, mirroring Shell et al.’s (1989) earlier findings. Subsequent
research by Pajares and Valiante revealed several additional find-
In one of the earliest empirical studies to explicitly reference
ings related to writing self-efficacy, including gender-related dif-
writing self-efficacy, McCarthy et al. (1985) studied the relation-
ferences, an inverse relationship to writing apprehension, and its
ship between college students’ writing self-efficacy and perfor-
independent and mediating effects on writing achievement (Pa-
mance. McCarthy et al.’s index of writing quality was raters’
jares, 2007; Pajares & Valiante, 1997, 1999, 2001).
judgments of 19 skills students used in writing expository essays;
Pajares and Valiante’s primary measure of writing self-efficacy
their measure of writing self-efficacy was students’ ratings of their
has been the Writing Self-Efficacy Scale (WSES; Pajares, 2007;
certainty about whether they could demonstrate these same skills.
As described by the authors, their measure’s items focused most Pajares & Valiante, 1999). As described by Pajares (2007), this
directly on writing mechanics, such as writing an essay without scale was constructed based on discussions of items previously
major spelling errors or run-on sentences, and accounted for ap- used by Shell et al. (1995, 1989) and rewriting them to make them
proximately 10%–15% of the variance in students’ actual writing appropriate across all grade levels. As reported in Pajares (2007),
scores. the WSES requires that students “provide judgments of their
confidence in their ability to successfully perform grammar, usage,
composition, and mechanical writing skills, such as correctly
Research by Shell and Associates punctuating a one page passage or organizing sentences into a
Following up on McCarthy et al.’s (1985) work, Shell et al. paragraph to clearly express a theme” (p. 240).
(1989) surveyed college students to examine relationships among In most of Pajares and his associates’ work, writing self-efficacy
writing self-efficacy, outcome expectancy beliefs, and writing has been treated as a unidimensional construct. Pajares (2007)
achievement. They gathered writing samples from each student suggested, however, that the WSES might reflect potentially sep-
written in response to the prompt “What do you believe to be the arable dimensions of writing self-efficacy and conducted explor-
qualities of a successful teacher?” These writing samples were atory factor analyses of WSES performance of students in Grades
judged analytically by independent raters blind to the students’ 4 through 11 (Pajares, 2007) showing this to be the case. Two
survey responses. factors emerged—the first reflecting basic skills (e.g., spelling,
Shell et al. (1989) measured writing efficacy with two subscales: using punctuation and verb tenses correctly) and a second tapping
a task subscale and a component skills subscale. The first consisted more complex composition skills (e.g., structuring paragraphs and
of items sampling self-efficacy for writing activities students essays, using topic sentences).
might conceivably perform (e.g., writing an essay or short story), Like Shell and colleagues’ tasks and skills measures previously,
while the second required efficacy judgments about writing-related Pajares’s basic and complex factors suggest the possibility of
skills (e.g., correctly spelling words, using parts of speech prop- parsing the construct of writing self-efficacy to good effect. At the
erly). Shell et al. found that although writing task self-efficacy did same time, it clearly is difficult to tie either of these researchers’
not predict writing performance, writing skills self-efficacy did, measures directly to models of writing (e.g., Bereiter & Scarda-
foreshadowing findings in both their own and others’ studies (e.g., malia, 1987; Flower & Hayes, 1984; Hayes, 1996, 2006) or to
Pajares et al., 1999; Pajares & Valiante, 2001) that writing self- potentially writing-relevant psychological and language-related
efficacy measures focusing on basic writing skills reliably predicts processes (e.g., Flower & Hayes, 1984; Myhill, 2008:
writing quality. Schleppegrell, 2007). As an illustration of how this connection has
In a subsequent study, Shell et al. (1995) studied grade- and been made for self-regulation and writing, we now turn to work by
achievement-level differences in fourth, seventh, and 10th graders’ Zimmerman and his associates. In the body of writing self-efficacy
writing self-efficacy. Short essays again were gathered from stu- research, Zimmerman’s framing of self-efficacy for self-regulation
dents, this time descriptions of their favorite television programs. as writers plan, write, and reflect on their writing (see Zimmerman
DIMENSIONS OF WRITING SELF-EFFICACY 27

& Kitsantas, 2007) seems to rest most clearly on a theory-based Toward an Alternative Perspective on Writing
account of writing processes. Self-Efficacy
Self-efficacy is a domain-specific construct, which means that
Research by Zimmerman and Associates there can be no all-purpose measure of self-efficacy. As Bandura
(2006) has stated, scales of perceived self-efficacy “must be tai-
In an influential early study, Zimmerman and Bandura (1994)
lored to the particular domain of functioning that is the object of
predicted that college students’ self-efficacy for regulating writing
interest” (p. 308). Pajares and Valiante (2006) similarly argued
activities along with self-efficacy for academic achievement and that measures of self-efficacy must reflect “an understanding of
grade-related goals would predict writing attainment beyond ver- both the domain under investigation and its different features, as
bal aptitude. Students in regular and advanced English composi- well as of the types of capabilities the domain requires and the
tion classes completed the authors’ Writing Self-Regulatory Effi- range of situations in which these capabilities might be applied” (p.
cacy Scale, consisting of 25 items on which students rated their 162).
perceived capability for successfully executing strategic aspects of In our judgment, these views strongly suggest the value of
writing such as planning and revising, responding to creative framing writing self-efficacy in ways that will yield information on
requirements of writing, and self-managing writing time and ac- writers’ judgments about successfully meeting writing’s psycho-
tivities. Principal components analysis showed the Writing Self- logical, linguistic, and behavioral challenges. Operationalizing
Regulatory Efficacy Scale represented a single factor. Among such a framework presumably should be directed at dimensions of
Zimmerman and Bandura’s findings were that students in the writing judged to be both theoretically and practically important.
advanced class had higher self-efficacy for managing writing ac- We sought to build on Zimmerman and colleagues’ research by
tivities. Self-efficacy scores also predicted the self-evaluative stan- tying our measure to a model of writing that separately highlights
dards students used and confidence in receiving higher grades. To writing’s cognitive and language-related aspects as well as its
Zimmerman and Bandura, these results not only indicated self- self-regulatory dimensions.
regulatory efficacy’s importance for writing but suggested their
measure’s potential use in making diagnostic assessments.
In subsequent research, however, Zimmerman and his associates Four Assumptions About the Act of Writing and
emphasized measures of self-efficacy closely tied to their experi- Writing Self-Efficacy
mental work, in which modeling and directing attention to writing
Four assumptions guided development of our writing self-
process goals were utilized to improve writing skills and writing
efficacy framework. The first, following the models and concep-
self-efficacy (e.g., Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 1999, 2002; see also
tions of Flower and Hayes and others (e.g., Chenoweth & Hayes,
Schunk & Zimmerman, 2007). Zimmerman and Kitsantas (1999,
2001; Flower & Hayes, 1980, 1981, 1984; Hayes, 1996, 2012;
2002), for instance, designed experimental interventions in which
McCutchen, Teske, & Bankston, 2008; Zimmerman & Kitsantas,
students learned to combine simple kernel sentences into a nonre-
2007) is that writing is a complex cognitive act generating high
petitive sentence and then were asked to make self-efficacy judg-
demands on working memory. As Flower and Hayes (1980) wrote
ments about whether they could successfully rewrite new kernel
in an oft-quoted description, a writer in the act of writing is “a
sentences. Thus, while these writing self-efficacy measures are
thinker on a full-time cognitive overload” (p. 33). Compared with
highly suited to their experimental purposes, they were not in-
conversation, for instance, which provides a great deal of contex-
tended to function as general assessments.
tual scaffolding for speech production and understanding, most
writing contexts provide much less support (Bruning & Horn,
Summary of Previous Writing Self-Efficacy Research 2000). Writing’s grammatical forms are highly formalized. Writers
also typically are displaced from their audience, requiring they
Writing self-efficacy research extending back into the mid- expend cognitive resources figuring out how to communicate with
1980s has illuminated relationships between writing self-efficacy an unseen reader.
and multiple writing-related variables (e.g., writing quality and A second assumption is that writing development advances
standards, level of writing apprehension), revealed groups in slowly. Writing requires coordination and integration of numerous
which writing self-efficacy may be lower or higher (e.g., students subskills, and the typically slow course of writing development
in different grades, boys or girls), and pointed to the possibility of reflects writers’ need to proceduralize knowledge at multiple cog-
self-efficacy varying by type of writing performance. Most writing nitive, metacognitive, and linguistic levels (e.g., Bereiter & Scar-
self-efficacy measures, however, have broadly sampled writing- damalia, 1987; Kellogg, 2008; McCutchen et al., 2008). For novice
related skills and tasks, making them less than ideal for yielding writers, for example, the activity of writing—whether retrieving
information about writers’ self-efficacy for specific dimensions of words from long-term memory or expressing ideas within appro-
writing. An exception has been Zimmerman’s work on self- priate syntactic frames—requires conscious attention to virtually
efficacy for writing self-regulation, which identified multiple ac- all levels of the composition process (e.g., choosing words, spell-
tivities tied to self-regulatory competence in writing (Zimmerman ing them correctly, putting them in sentences). For such writers,
& Bandura, 1994; Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 2007). Thus far, especially, working memory will be freed for essential tasks such
however, no models or measures have been developed that provide as idea generation and organization only when fluency is achieved
independent information on writers’ self-efficacy for meeting writ- on other levels (Chenoweth & Hayes, 2001).
ing’s cognitive and linguistic, as well as its self-regulatory de- Our third assumption, perhaps somewhat more conjectural than
mands. the first two, stems from our own writing experiences and obser-
28 BRUNING, DEMPSEY, KAUFFMAN, MCKIM, AND ZUMBRUNN

vations. Because writing typically is an extended, effortful act, we highly recursive and therefore do not posit a sequential ordering
propose that writers form strong impressions of their own writing for these domains.
experiences. These can range from judgments of their levels of
success on various tasks (e.g., the ability to choose appropriate
Writing Self-Efficacy: Ideation
words or satisfy a language’s conventions) to feelings of anxiety or
frustration. Certainly, writing-related outcomes (e.g., writing We argue that one likely dimension of writing self-efficacy is
grades) affect writing self-efficacy, as has been shown in numer- writers’ beliefs about their abilities to generate ideas, their ide-
ous studies (e.g., see Pajares, 2003), but we assume also that many ation. Writing cannot proceed without ideas. Idea generation has a
self-evaluative judgments about degree of success in meeting prominent place both in Flower and Hayes’s (1984) model of the
writing’s many challenges—and hence self-efficacy for writing— writing process and in Hayes’s (1996, 2006, 2012) later revisions
emerge during the writing process itself. of the original Flower and Hayes model. In all of these models,
Our fourth assumption also is conjectural—writers group their idea generation is portrayed as an ongoing process in working
writing-related experiences into psychologically meaningful cate- memory influencing all other parts of writing. Various dimensions
gories— but is grounded in the tendency to organize knowledge of ideation can be conceptualized as creating distinguishable
into schema-like conceptual structures (Schraw, 2006). Aggre- writing-related challenges. For instance, do I have ideas to write
gated over time, we argue, writers’ judgments about their writing about, are they good ideas, and can I find the right words to
become identifiable, stable dimensions of their sense of themselves express them?
as writers. While we do not dispute the validity of a generalized Idea generation is situated primarily within the domain of se-
writing self-efficacy, potentially interesting subcategories may be mantics (Cruse, 2004; Evans & Green, 2006; Langacker, 1987,
implied by questions such as these: Can I think of things to write 2008) and involves writers’ abilities to generate the content and
about? Can I express my ideas in writing? Can I keep myself on ordering of their thoughts. Although often thought of as tied
track as I write— by avoiding distractions and coping with my mostly to words (Cruse, 1986, 2004; Langacker, 2008; Read,
feelings as I’m writing? If they are, their measurement may hold 2000), semantic knowledge also exists in schema-like structures
further clues for building writing motivation and success. and is reflected in fluid use of connotative and denotative mean-
Overall, then, our portrayal of writing begins with the assump- ings. As will be seen subsequently, our writing ideation self-
tion, widely shared, that writing is a demanding process that only efficacy items focus on writers’ judgments of the availability,
slowly becomes automatized and fluent. We also assume that the quality, and ordering of their ideas.
writing process creates strong memorial impressions that writers
group into psychologically identifiable categories. With these as- Writing Self-Efficacy: Conventions
sumptions in mind, we now turn to our proposed dimensions of
writing self-efficacy and the rationales for each. If they do in fact A second dimension of our model is self-efficacy for writing
conventions, which refer to a set of generally accepted standards
reflect writers’ psychological realities and are represented ade-
for expressing ideas in writing in a given language. In English
quately in a writing self-efficacy measure, we would first expect
these would include agreed-upon ways to spell, punctuate, capi-
our empirical data to reflect the posited dimensions. If so, we then
talize, and structure sentences. We have drawn this label from the
could explore the extent to which these dimensions relate to other
writing literature; related terms include writing mechanics and
variables, such as liking writing and writing performance.
translation (Fayol, Alamargot, & Berninger, 2012; Flower &
Hayes, 1981). Conventions provide the frames within which writ-
A Model of Writing Self-Efficacy ers express their ideas successfully.
Although terms like conventions and mechanics seem to con-
We see the writing self-efficacy model described in this section note low-level writing processes, writing draws on a complex array
as consonant with writing process models emphasizing working of linguistically based knowledge for beginning and advanced
memory’s centrality (e.g., Flower & Hayes, 1984; Hayes, 2006, writers alike (e.g., Myhill, 2008; Schleppegrell, 2007). Whereas
2012), as well as with other portrayals of writing and writing speakers can rely on contextual support, writers mostly cannot,
development (e.g., Bereiter & Scardamalia, 1987; Bruning & requiring them to recognize and use “rules” of written expression.
Horn, 2000; Graham, 2006; Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 2007). The Self-efficacy for executing writing conventions can vary widely,
model has three focal dimensions tied to earlier researchers’ work just as using them can be highly automatized (e.g., as in a profes-
(e.g., Pajares, 2007; Shell et al., 1995; Zimmerman & Bandura, sional journalist’s writing) or require a conscious, effortful set of
1994; Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 2007). The first is ideation, which responses (e.g., a child’s attempt to “write a neat paper”; Graham,
we view as rooted in cognitive processes, primarily idea genera- 2006).
tion, and generally tied to the domains of semantics and schematic Using writing’s conventions begins early and develops along
knowledge (Schraw, 2006). The second is writing conventions, multiple dimensions. In English, for example, beginning writers
which in our operationalization refers to the specific articulation of must understand the alphabetic principle, that letters represent a
ideas into writing’s forms and is aligned with Flower and Hayes’ language’s sounds, and have phonemic awareness, the ability to
concept of translation (Flower & Hayes, 1980; Hayes, 2006, recognize separable sounds in spoken words (Ehri, 2005; Vellu-
2012). The third dimension is self-regulation, which we see as tino, Tunmer, Jaccard, & Chen, 2007). These understandings,
extending well beyond writing’s activities (see Zimmerman & along with knowledge of words’ spellings, orthography, are
Kitsantas, 2007) to its management, monitoring, and evaluation. needed to give writing meaning. Morphological awareness—
Consistent with most writing researchers, we view writing as knowing how a language ties sound and meaning and how mor-
DIMENSIONS OF WRITING SELF-EFFICACY 29

phemes can be combined (Kuo & Anderson, 2006; Nagy, it gets difficult? and Can I manage my frustrations when my
Berninger, & Abbott, 2006)—also is needed for communication of progress slows or stalls?
such concepts as tense, number, and possession. Putting words to
screen or paper reflects other important dimensions of conven- Summary of the Model and Introduction to the
tions. Spelling words correctly, for example, depends on knowl- Present Studies
edge in many linguistic categories. Other conventions lie literally
beyond words—in the domains of syntax and discourse. Each We have proposed three dimensions for a model of writing
written language has its own devices—ranging from word order to self-efficacy: self-efficacy for writing ideation, conventions, and
punctuation—for signaling writers’ intended meanings. Can writ- self-regulation. Writing’s ideational processes include generating
ers generate complete sentences and use a language’s markers and shaping the concepts, principles, and reasoning upon which
(e.g., capitalization) to clearly express what is meant? Can they writing depends. Success in using conventions, in contrast, seems
sequence and combine ideas, the target of the early instructional to draw more heavily on linguistic skills as writers choose from
approach called sentence combining (e.g., O’Hare, 1973), recently among a language’s words, syntactic forms, and discourse struc-
revisited by Saddler and Graham (Saddler, Behforooz, & Asaro, tures in expressing their ideas. The final category—self-efficacy
2008; Saddler & Graham, 2005)? Beyond the sentence level lie for writing self-regulation— can reasonably be identified with a
discourse structures such as those of narrative and exposition. writer’s self-management and affective control but also involves
Mature writing requires understanding not only of such structures’ judgments about cognitive and linguistic features as writing is
features and purposes but of ways to signal their presence (e.g., by being produced.
paragraphing, headers). In the studies reported here, we describe tests of this model with
As with ideation, we assume writers’ cumulative experiences two groups in which we expected writing-related efficacy judg-
with writing conventions to be reflected in self-efficacy judgments. ments to be well formed. First, in a study of middle school
In grappling with writing conventions, writers presumably gradu- students, we hypothesized that our writing self-efficacy data would
ally form a sense of their capabilities with this set of challenges be characterized by a three-factor structure consistent with our
and their likely future success in performing them. For instance, proposed framework. We used confirmatory factor analysis (CFA)
spelling self-efficacy both varies widely across individuals (e.g., to test this hypothesis. If the data reflected the hypothesized
Rankin, Bruning, & Timme, 1994) and correlates significantly framework, we then could test its generality with a sample of high
with various writing-related measures, including writing self- school students and examine potential relationships to variables
efficacy, predictions of writing success, and writing performance previously explored in writing self-efficacy research. These in-
itself (Klassen & Georgiou, 2008). cluded students’ actual and self-reported writing performance,
their liking of writing, and indicators of their general literacy-
related competence.
Writing Self-Efficacy: Self-Regulation
Self-efficacy for writing self-regulation is reflected in writers’ Study 1
confidence they can direct themselves successfully through writ-
ing’s many dimensions and subtasks (Zimmerman & Bandura, Method
1994; Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 2007). Having ideas to write about
and command of writing’s conventions are essential to writing but The goal of Study 1 was to test the adequacy of a three-factor
not all of it. Writing can be difficult and tedious. Self-regulatory model of writing self-efficacy. For this study, middle school stu-
skills are needed not only to generate productive ideas and writing dents completed the Writing Habits and Beliefs Survey (WHBS),
strategies but also to manage the anxieties and emotions that can which included the Self-Efficacy for Writing Scale (SEWS) self-
accompany writing. efficacy items. The WHBS, designed to yield information on
As operationalized by Zimmerman and Bandura (1994), writing students’ writing habits and motivations, also tapped students’
self-regulation includes activities providing a “start to finish” writing achievement goals (Kauffman et al., 2010; Zumbrunn,
portrayal of writing a paper. Among them are the ability to find Bruning, Kauffman, & Hayes, 2010); implicit beliefs about writing
suitable writing topics, start writing without difficulty, capture (Bruning, Kauffman, Haines, & Zumbrunn, 2012; White & Brun-
readers’ interest early on, adjust writing style for varied audiences, ing, 2005); and writing-related activities, habits, and attitudes
clarify sentences by rewriting them, get “unstuck” from writing (Zeleny & Yilmaz-Soylu, 2010: Zumbrunn, Kauffman, Hayes, &
problems, motivate oneself to write even when a topic is less than Yilmaz-Soylu, 2010). WHBS data relating to writing achievement
captivating, and find and correct grammatical errors. goals, implicit beliefs, and habits extend beyond the present arti-
Writers obviously need a variety of self-regulatory strategies to cle’s scope and are not reported here.
cope with such complexity. They need to coordinate syntactic and Our hypothesis was that data yielded by the self-efficacy mea-
semantic knowledge, control aspects of the writing process as sure would closely fit the three-factor model previously described.
diverse as getting started and avoiding distractions (e.g., Hidi & As a first step in testing model fit, we proposed a single-factor
Boscolo, 2006), and evaluate whether they are achieving their model as a contrasting case, based on an assumption that the data
goals. Like self-efficacy for ideation and conventions, self-efficacy would not reflect a multifactor structure. We then compared the fit
for self-regulation of writing likely varies greatly across writers of this single-factor model with the fit of our hypothesized three-
(Zimmerman & Bandura, 1994), with questions such as these factor model.
pointing to some of writing’s key self-regulatory challenges: Can Participants. Six hundred ninety-seven students from four
I get started writing? Can I keep my writing going, especially when middle schools in a midsize midwestern city completed the WHBS
30 BRUNING, DEMPSEY, KAUFFMAN, MCKIM, AND ZUMBRUNN

during the spring semester. The sample included all students survey administration to make the measure more suitable for
enrolled in eighth grade English/language arts (ELA) classes. In students in middle and high school.
the overall pool of Study 1 participants, of those reporting their The final version of the 16 SEWS items can be seen in Table 1.
grade level (n ⫽ 694), 692 (99.7%) were eighth graders, one was Five items were designed to represent idea generation, five con-
a seventh grader, and one a ninth grader. Of those reporting gender ventions, and six writing self-regulation. Following methods sug-
(n ⫽ 692), 327 (47.3%) were boys and 365 (52.7%) girls. Mean gested by Bandura (2006) and utilized by other self-efficacy re-
reported age (n ⫽ 678) was 13.8 years; modal age was 14 (70.4%). searchers (Pajares, Hartley, & Valiante, 2001; Shell et al., 1989),
Overall, approximately 62% of the participating students reported participants rated their self-efficacy on each item on a 0 to 100
their racial– ethnic status as Caucasian, 10% as African American, scale ranging from no confidence to complete confidence. Analy-
8% as Latino/Latina, 5% as Asian/Pacific Islander, and 15% as ses of SEWS’ item performance and factor structure were the
multiracial or another ethnicity. Five hundred eighty-two (83.5%) primary goals of this study.
reported that English was the primary language spoken at home; Permission for conducting the study was obtained from the
110 students (16.5%) reported languages other than English pri- university institutional review board, the school’s district office,
marily being spoken there. The proportions of students participat- and principals of the participating schools. A letter from the senior
ing in free or reduced lunch programs in the four participating researchers informing parents/guardians about the study and its
schools were 76%, 66%, 51%, and 23% (overall proportion in purposes was distributed to ELA teachers and sent home with their
district middle schools ⫽ 38.1%). students. It stated that students would be completing a writing-
Measures related survey (the WBHS) in their ELA classes that would take
Self-Efficacy for Writing Scale (SEWS). SEWS consists of 15–20 min to complete, that results were confidential, and that
16 items corresponding to the three posited categories of writing- participation in the study was voluntary and would have no effect
related experience: ideation, conventions, and self-regulation. on their student’s grades or relationships with teachers or the
Some of these items, primarily from the ideation and conventions school. No students declined participation.
categories, had been previously utilized in a study of college Surveys were administered during class time in the students’
students (Dempsey, Bruning, & Kauffman, 2010), in which our assigned classrooms by their teachers. The teachers read general
goal was to obtain a general estimate of writing self-efficacy. We instructions for completing the survey, which indicated that there
also examined items from Pajares (2007), Zimmerman and Ban- were no correct or incorrect answers and that students should raise
dura (1994), and those of Shell et al. (1995, 1989) as we began to their hands if they had any questions. While there was no time
formulate and operationalize the framework for the current study. limit for completing the survey, the instructions to teachers indi-
As the framework was further refined through discussions within cated that “when most of the students had finished,” they could
our research team and in consultations with school personnel, a suggest that all students try to finish “in the next minute or two.”
new, expanded set of items reflecting the hypothesized three-factor Feedback received from teachers indicated that students completed
structure was created and pilot-tested with a second set of college the survey comfortably within the 20-min time frame. When
students. A near-final set of items was again reviewed by school finished, students returned their surveys to their teachers, who
personnel, who suggested changes in wording and methods of forwarded them on to the district office.

Table 1
Final Model Parameters for a Three-Factor Model of Writing Self-Efficacy: Middle School Students

Factor and item Factor loading Standard error Standardized value

Ideation
1. I can think of many ideas for my writing. 1.000 0.000 0.799
2. I can put my ideas into writing. 0.978 0.041 0.823
3. I can think of many words to describe my ideas. 1.018 0.041 0.838
4. I can think of a lot of original ideas. 0.991 0.043 0.800
5. I know exactly where to place my ideas in my writing. 0.928 0.042 0.770

Conventions
6. I can spell my words correctly. 1.000 0.000 0.636
7. I can write complete sentences. 0.920 0.058 0.728
8. I can punctuate my sentences correctly. 1.281 0.073 0.835
9. I can write grammatically correct sentences. 1.381 0.077 0.869
10. I can begin my paragraphs in the right spots. 0.843 0.062 0.595

Self-regulation
11. I can focus on my writing for at least one hour. 1.000 0.000 0.803
12. I can avoid distractions while I write. 0.984 0.042 0.806
13. I can start writing assignments quickly. 0.964 0.041 0.816
14. I can control my frustration when I write. 0.742 0.043 0.634
15. I can think of my writing goals before I write. 0.826 0.042 0.701
16. I can keep writing even when it’s difficult. 0.803 0.039 0.734
DIMENSIONS OF WRITING SELF-EFFICACY 31

Results examine relationships of its dimensions to other variables in this


new sample.
We first sought to determine goodness of fit of the SEWS data
to a single-factor model and to our proposed three-factor model.
The criteria and standards used to judge model fit (see Hu & Study 2
Bentler, 1999) included the comparative fit index (CFI; values
above 0.95 indicate very good fit, and those at or above 0.90
indicate reasonable fit; Bentler, 1990), Steiger’s root-mean-square Method
error of approximation (RMSEA; values below 0.05 indicate a Overview. Data for Study 2 were gathered from high school
very good fit, and those at or below 0.10 indicate a reasonable fit; students in the same school system as students in Study 1. Like
Steiger, 1990), and the standardized root-mean-square residual Study 1 participants, Study 2 participants—mostly 11th graders—
(SRMR; ⬍0.05; Hu & Bentler, 1999). This constellation of fit completed the comprehensive survey of writing habits, the WHBS.
statistics conforms to recommended strategies for evaluating fit of The majority of these students would take the state’s statewide
structural models (Hu & Bentler, 1999). writing assessment (SWA) approximately one month later. Our
We first fit the data from SEWS to the single-factor model. The goals in Study 2 were to (1) use CFA to test the generalizability of
fit was unacceptable, ␹2(104) ⫽ 1,750.205, p ⬍ .001, CFI ⫽ the three-factor self-efficacy for writing model and (2) contingent
0.750, RMSEA ⫽ 0.151, SRMR ⫽ 0.094, indicating that this on the CFA findings replicating those at the middle school level,
model did not adequately explain the data’s structure. We then fit examine SEWS factors’ relationships to other variables, which
the data to the hypothesized three-factor model. The fit this time included liking writing, SWA performance, self-reported writing
was acceptable, ␹2(101) ⫽ 439.152, p ⬍ .001, CFI ⫽ 0.949, grades, and English/language arts (ELA) class enrollment. As in
RMSEA ⫽ 0.069, SRMR ⫽ 0.046, and was significantly better Study 1, the WHBS tapped additional student data (e.g., achieve-
than the one-factor model, ⌬␹2(3) ⫽ 1,311.053, p ⬍ .001. The ment goal orientation, implicit beliefs about writing) not reported
fitted data, which encompassed all 16 items, was consistent with here.
the proposed model, so no further models were tested. Factor Participants. Five hundred sixty-three 11th and 12th graders
loadings are presented in Table 1. in two public high schools in a midwestern city completed the
Mean ratings of self-efficacy were 70.46 (SD ⫽ 20.49), 79.31 survey during the spring semester. The four middle schools from
(SD ⫽ 16.44), and 61.31 (SD ⫽ 23.26) for the ideation, conven- which Study 1 participants were drawn served as feeder schools
tions, and self-regulation subscales, respectively. Distributions for for these two schools. Of participants reporting their grade level
each showed significant (p ⬍ .001) levels of negative skewness (n ⫽ 557), 520 (93.4%) were 11th graders and 37 (6.6%) were
(– 0.804, –1.450, and – 0.513, respectively; all SEs ⫽ 0.093), 12th graders; of those reporting gender (n ⫽ 553), 292 (52.8%)
indicating that student ratings of writing self-efficacy tended to- were boys and 261 (47.2%) girls. Mean reported age (n ⫽ 537)
ward higher levels of the rating scale and were not normally was 16.76 (SD ⫽ 0.77), with a modal age of 17. Overall, approx-
distributed. The three scales showed significant positive correla- imately 70% of the participating students were Caucasian, 7%
tions with one another. The correlation between ideation and African American, 6% Latino/Latina, 6% Asian/Pacific Islander,
self-regulation was strong (r ⫽ .718), while conventions was and 10% multiracial or of another ethnicity. Four hundred eighty
moderately correlated with both ideation (r ⫽ .526) and self- (87%) of the participants reported that English was the primary
regulation (r ⫽ .463). Reliabilities for each of the factor-related language spoken at home, while 75 students reported languages
subscales also were high. Alpha for the five items related to the other than English primarily being spoken there. Proportions of
ideation factor was 0.903, for the five conventions items 0.847, students participating in free or reduced lunch programs in the
and for the six self-regulation items 0.884. participating schools were 45% and 22% (overall proportion in
district high schools ⫽ 31.0%). SWA scores in the school’s
database that could be matched to our survey data set were avail-
Discussion
able for 470 students. These students’ demographics mirrored
The purpose of Study 1 was to test the extent to which responses those in the overall survey sample.
to SEWS’ items fit the proposed model of self-efficacy of writing. English class enrollment. All participants were enrolled in
Results revealed evidence for SEWS’ capture of three dimensions one of four ELA courses in their high schools: General English
of self-efficacy for writing— generating writing ideas; expressing (GE), Composition (Comp), American Literature and Composition
them within writing’s linguistic forms; and managing the behav- (ALC), and Advanced Placement Language and Composition
ioral, mental, and emotional challenges of writing. Although dif- (APLC). General English is the most basic of the classes. As
ferences in levels of self-efficacy cannot be judged directly from described in information provided by school personnel, GE is
mean ratings of different item types, there plainly were some designed for juniors and seniors needing further work in develop-
sizeable differences by category, with these middle school students ing language and composition skills needed to meet the school’s
on average rating their confidence highest for carrying out writing graduation demonstration requirement. The class focuses on build-
conventions and lowest on their ability to manage writing’s self- ing writing skills through narrative and expository writing; stu-
regulatory dimension. dents study a variety of American fiction and nonfiction, as well as
Based on Study 1 results consistent with the proposed three- magazines and newspaper articles. They also receive instruction in
factor model, we then turned to a new sample— high school study skills and strategies. Thirty-seven (6.6%) of our participants
students—to test the model’s generality. If high school students’ were enrolled in GE, 12 of whom reported a language other than
performance also was consistent with the model, we then would English being spoken at home.
32 BRUNING, DEMPSEY, KAUFFMAN, MCKIM, AND ZUMBRUNN

Composition, in which 31 participants (5.5%) were enrolled (11 ing them to create a persuasive essay. SWA procedures in-
reporting a language other than English spoken at home), is the volved a first day, in which students received the prompt and
next-level class. This class is intended to help students acquire planned their response, and a second, in which they completed
skills in narrative, expository, and persuasive writing through their writing (Dappen, Isernhagen, & Anderson, 2008). In the
guided practice, which focuses on writing processes and products administration in which our participants were involved, each of
and includes study of the students’ own and others’ writings. Study the approximately 22,000 essays generated statewide at each
of the English language and effective research techniques also are grade level was then hand-scored in a central location in the
included. This course can be taken as an alternative to meet the state in a 3-day session by practicing teachers familiar with
writing graduation demonstration requirement. student writing at the grade level being assessed. Each student’s
The majority of our participants (n ⫽ 323, 57%) were enrolled writing sample was judged by two trained raters, who first
in American Literature and Composition classes. Of these, 40 reviewed each paper based on the analytic criteria of the 6-Trait
reported that a language other than English was spoken at home. Writing Model (Spandel, 2005)—ideas and content, organiza-
Students in these classes study literature of the Americas reflecting tion, word choice, sentence fluency, conventions, and voice—
a multicultural society and its history. They read and discuss short
and then assigned global ratings on a 1– 4 scale. Thus, all
stories, novels, folklore, drama, poetry, essays, and biographies.
participants in the SWA, including ours, received a rating that
Emphasis in written composition is placed on expository form and
was the sum of the two individual ratings and ranged from 2– 8.
style and on critical responses to literature and film.
Reliabilities for the SWA scoring procedure generally have
Finally, just over 30% (n ⫽ 172) of our participants were
been acceptable. For the 2007 SWA, for instance, 49% of rater
enrolled in Advanced Placement Language and Composition,
pairs produced exact categorical matches, and 48% of raters
the most advanced of the classes. Twelve of these students
reported that English was not the primary language spoken at placed papers in adjacent categories; only 3% of papers re-
home. This course is designed to help students become skilled quired adjudication (Nebraska Department of Education, 2007).
readers of prose written in a variety of rhetorical styles and SWA scores for the students in our high school sample were
skilled writers who compose for a variety of purposes. Writing made available for our research through a blinded process in
and reading activities are designed to help students become which the school system’s evaluation personnel matched stu-
aware of interactions among a writer’s purposes, audience, and dents’ SWA scores provided by the state to our survey data and
subjects, as well as how language conventions and resources then forwarded an anonymous data set back to us.
make writing effective. Procedures. Permission for conducting Study 2 was obtained
Measures from our university’s institutional review board, the school’s dis-
Self-Efficacy for Writing Scale (SEWS). The SEWS measure trict office, and the participating schools’ principals. A letter
administered to the high school students in Study 2 was identical informing parents/guardians of the study and its purposes was
to that given to the middle school students in Study 1, consisting distributed to the schools’ ELA teachers and sent home with the
of 16 items representing self-efficacy for performing in three students. It stated that students would complete a 15- to 20-min
writing-related dimensions: ideation, conventions, and self- writing-related survey in class, that results were confidential, and
regulation. As in Study 1, the high school students rated their that participation in the study was voluntary and would have no
writing self-efficacy on a 0 to 100 scale (Bandura, 2006). effect on student grades or relationships with teachers or the
Liking Writing Scale (LWS). Four Likert-type items were school. No students declined participation.
constructed to provide general information about the extent of In addition to completing the SEWS and LWS as part of the
students’ positive attitudes about writing. These items were (1) “I WHBS, students also reported which of the four high school ELA
enjoy writing,” (2) “I don’t like to write” (reverse-coded), (3) courses they were enrolled in; their writing-related grades in ELA
“writing is fun,” and (4) “I feel bad when I write” (reverse-coded). and other classes; and their age, gender, and ethnicity. Surveys
Students rated items on a 5-point scale, ranging from strongly were administered during class time in the students’ assigned ELA
disagree to strongly agree. CFA results revealed a good fit be- classrooms by their teachers. As in Study 1, teachers gave students
tween the LWS data and a one-factor solution, ␹2(2) ⫽ 4.72, p ⫽ general instructions for responding to the survey and told them
.0942, CFI ⫽ 0.998, RMSEA ⫽ 0.049, SRMR ⫽ 0.012, and item there was no time limit. When students finished, they returned the
analysis showed good reliability for the LWS (␣ ⫽ .831).
completed surveys to their teachers, who forwarded them to the
Self-reported writing performance. Students reported their
school district office.
writing-related grades in two school-related contexts for the cur-
rent year: (1) on writing assignments in their ELA classes and (2)
on writing assignments in other classes. For each, they were asked Results
to indicate if their current-year writing assignment grades were “all
or mostly all” A⫹ or A, B⫹ or B, C⫹ or C, D⫹ or D, or F. Each Tests of model fit. We tested the new data’s fit to the three-
self-reported grade category was coded on a 5-point scale (A⫹ or factor model observed in Study 1, again employing the generally
A ⫽ 5, B⫹ or B ⫽ 4, etc.) and the two scores combined to create accepted criteria used to judge model fit (see Hu & Bentler, 1999);
a single writing performance index (␣ ⫽ .774). these included the comparative fit index (CFI), Steiger’s (1990)
Statewide writing assessment (SWA). Approximately one root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the stan-
month after completing the WHBS, which included the above dardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR; Hu & Bentler, 1999).
measures, most of these same students took part in the state’s The fit of SEWS’ 16 items to the three-factor model in the high
statewide writing assessment (SWA), writing to a prompt ask- school data was acceptable, ␹2(101) ⫽ 361.489, p ⬍ .001, CFI ⫽
DIMENSIONS OF WRITING SELF-EFFICACY 33

0.953, RMSEA ⫽ 0.069, SRMR ⫽ 0.045.1 Because this model higher than did GE students. On SEWS self-efficacy for writing
reflected both theoretical predictions and current data, no further self-regulation subscale, APLC students scored higher than did GE
models were tested. and Comp students. On the LWS, APLC students indicated sig-
Loadings of SEWS items on the three factors are presented in nificantly more positive feelings about writing than did GE stu-
Table 2. Mean ratings of writing self-efficacy were 73.56 (SD ⫽ dents. APLC and ALC students reported receiving significantly
18.99), 84.39 (SD ⫽ 14.43), and 62.63 (SD ⫽ 23.02) for the better grades on their classroom-related writing than did students
ideation, conventions, and self-regulation subscales, respectively. in GE and Comp groups. Finally, students in the APLC and ALC
As in Study 1, subscale distributions were negatively skewed classes scored significantly higher on the SWA than did the Comp
(skewness ⫽ –1.018, –1.677, and – 0.589, respectively; SEs ⫽ students, who in turn scored significantly better than did the GE
0.104, 0.103, and 0.104, respectively; ps ⬍ .001). The three factors students.
showed medium to high correlations with one another. The corre-
lation between writing ideation self-efficacy and self-regulatory
Discussion
self-efficacy again was the strongest (r ⫽ .707, p ⬍ .001), while
self-efficacy for writing conventions was moderately correlated The purposes of Study 2 were to (1) use CFA to test the
with self-efficacy for ideation (r ⫽ .530, p ⬍ .001) and self- generalizability of the three-factor self-efficacy for writing model
regulation (r ⫽ .440, p ⬍ .001). Alphas for the writing ideation, in a new sample of students and, assuming good model fit, to (2)
conventions, and self-regulation self-efficacy subscales were examine SEWS factors’ relationships with other writing-related
high— 0.923, 0.858, and 0.874, respectively—paralleling Study 1 constructs. Consistent with Study 1’s findings with middle school
findings. students, Study 2 results for high school students supported a
Relationship of SEWS to other variables. Table 3 shows multifactor conceptualization of writing self-efficacy, with high
correlations among SEWS subscales, the LWS, self-reported writ- school students’ responses to the three SEWS subscales—self-
ing grades, and student SWA performance. Each of the three efficacy for writing ideation, writing conventions, and writing
SEWS subscales showed significant positive correlations to the self-regulation— closely matching the proposed three-factor
LWS (p ⬍ .001). While the correlations between ideation (r ⫽ model. Positive relationships among SEWS subscale scores also
.487) and self-regulation (r ⫽ .497) subscale scores with the LWS were noted. Self-efficacy for writing ideation and self-regulation
did not differ from one another (Meng, Rosenthal, & Rubin, 1992), shared nearly 50% of their variance (r ⫽ .707), while self-efficacy
both were significantly greater (p ⬍ .001) than the relationship for conventions again had significantly less in common with either
between the SEWS conventions scale and the LWS (r ⫽ .225). of these dimensions (rs ⫽ .530 and .440, respectively). Our mea-
Each of the three SEWS subscales also showed significant (p ⬍ sure of writing affect, the LWS, was considerably more strongly
.001) moderate positive correlations with self-reported writing related to self-efficacy for meeting the ideational and self-
performance (rs ranging from .357 to .404). The relationships of regulatory challenges of writing (rs ⫽ .487 and .497, respectively)
SEWS subscales to SWA performance were considerably more than to self-efficacy for conventions (r ⫽ .225). These findings
variable, however, with positive but relatively low relationships suggest a commonality between confidence for managing writ-
between SEWS ideation and SWA scores (r ⫽ .203) and between ing’s cognitive and metacognitive dimensions that has stronger ties
self-regulation and SWA scores (r ⫽ .206), compared with a to feelings about writing than confidence in one’s ability to carry
significantly higher (p ⬍ .001) relationship between SEWS con- out writing’s conventions.
ventions and SWA scores (r ⫽ .378). Self-reported writing per- Significant differences were present in the high school group in
formance in school was moderately related (r ⫽ .332, p ⬍ .001) to patterns of relationships of SEWS subscale scores with self-
scores on the SWA. reported writing grades and SWA scores. As can been seen in
Table 4 presents mean scores for the SEWS measure, the LWS, Table 3, correlations are moderate, positive, and roughly equiva-
for self-reported writing performance, and SWA performance as a lent between the SEWS ideation, conventions, and self-regulation
function of ELA class enrollment. Potential differences by class dimensions and self-reported writing performance (rs ⫽ .357,
enrollment on each of the SEWS dimensions, the LWS, and the .404, and .394, respectively). For SWA performance, however, this
performance measures were tested by means of one-way analyses approximate level of relationship to the writing self-efficacy sub-
of variance (ANOVAs). Because of the multiple tests of signifi- scales was present only for writing conventions (r ⫽ .378). Al-
cance, evidence of heterogeneity of variance in some tests, and though significant, the relationships between self-efficacy for writ-
uneven ns among the groups, alpha levels for all tests and follow- ing ideation and SWA scores (r ⫽ .203) and self-efficacy for
ups were set at p ⫽ .01. ANOVAs showed that mean scores on all writing self-regulation and SWA scores (r ⫽ .206) were signifi-
variables differed significantly by group (see Table 4), with ratings cantly lower, with each sharing less than 5% of variance with
on all categories of beliefs (SEWS ideation, conventions, and SWA performance. Liking writing had only a small relationship
self-regulation variables; LWS scores) and performance scores (r ⫽ .133) to SWA performance, with less than 2% of variance
(self-reported writing grades, SWA scores) trending higher at each shared.
higher level of ELA enrollment. Follow-up tests using Tukey’s Overall, the relationships noted in this study between the writing
honestly significant difference analysis showed that APLC stu- beliefs measures (i.e., self-efficacy, liking writing) and SWA per-
dents scored significantly higher in SEWS self-efficacy for writing
ideation than all other groups, while GE students scored signifi- 1
The model fit statistics were essentially the same when tested with and
cantly lower than all other groups. On the SEWS self-efficacy for without students reporting a language other than English as the primary
writing conventions scale, students in ALC and APLC classes language spoken at home. The model reported here is based on all students
scored higher than did those in Comp classes, who in turn scored in the pool.
34 BRUNING, DEMPSEY, KAUFFMAN, MCKIM, AND ZUMBRUNN

Table 2
Final Model Parameters for a Three-Factor Model of Writing Self-Efficacy: High School Students

Factor and item Factor loading Standard error Standardized value

Ideation
1. I can think of many ideas for my writing. 1.000 0.000 0.841
2. I can put my ideas into writing. 1.036 0.041 0.863
3. I can think of many words to describe my ideas. 1.052 0.042 0.848
4. I can think of a lot of original ideas. 0.986 0.042 0.820
5. I know exactly where to place my ideas in my writing. 1.016 0.043 0.826

Conventions
6. I can spell my words correctly. 1.000 0.000 0.645
7. I can write complete sentences. 0.829 0.059 0.703
8. I can punctuate my sentences correctly. 1.253 0.077 0.857
9. I can write grammatically correct sentences. 1.297 0.079 0.869
10. I can begin my paragraphs in the right spots. 0.919 0.070 0.643

Self-regulation
11. I can focus on my writing for at least one hour. 1.000 0.000 0.769
12. I can avoid distractions while I write. 0.983 0.053 0.768
13. I can start writing assignments quickly. 1.056 0.052 0.820
14. I can control my frustration when I write. 0.832 0.051 0.679
15. I can think of my writing goals before I write. 0.929 0.050 0.766
16. I can keep writing even when it’s difficult. 0.901 0.047 0.784

formance are relatively modest. At least two factors may have Writing self-efficacy beliefs of students across the four ELA
contributed to this. First, approximately a month passed between classes differed significantly on all three SEWS factors, showing
the administration of the writing beliefs measures and when stu- that student beliefs about performing capably in writing-related
dents took the SWA, which creates the potential for other factors tasks are tied to class placement along multiple dimensions. APLC
to moderate the relationship between confidence about perfor- students on average reported significantly higher self-efficacy for
mance and actual performance. Ideally, such measures should be writing ideation than did all other groups, for instance, while GE
administered contextually and in close temporal contiguity to the students reported lower self-efficacy for their ability to generate
performance. Second, the SEWS measure, while designed to yield ideas for writing than did students in all other classes. Other
scores for subdomains of writing performance, is not focused on differences were that APLC and ALC students had higher self-
specific writing tasks and genres. That is, SEWS provides infor- efficacy for writing conventions than did students in Comp and GE
mation about confidence for performing identifiable dimensions of classes, while APLC students judged their ability to self-regulate
the writing process, but in its present form it does not query their writing activities significantly more positively than did both
self-efficacy for performance on writing assessments or any other the GE and Comp students.
specific writing task or genre. Future research involving more If, as Bandura (1986) has argued, enactive mastery in any
targeted measures of writing self-efficacy designed to explore such domain is the most reliable route to building self-efficacy, differ-
relationships should prove valuable. ential experiences and success as writers seem likely to have

Table 3
Relationships Among SEWS Self-Efficacy Variables, Liking Writing, and Self-Reported and Test-Based Writing Performance for High
School Students

Statewide
Self-reported writing
SEWS: SEWS: Liking Writing writing assessment
Measure Conventions Self-regulation Scale (LWS) performance (SWA)a

SEWS: Ideation 0.530 (N ⫽ 555) 0.707 (N ⫽ 554) 0.487 (N ⫽ 553) 0.357 (N ⫽ 550) 0.203 (N ⫽ 457)
SEWS: Conventions 0.440 (N ⫽ 554) 0.225 (N ⫽ 557) 0.404 (N ⫽ 553) 0.378 (N ⫽ 460)
SEWS: Self-regulation 0.497 (N ⫽ 553) 0.394 (N ⫽ 551) 0.206 (N ⫽ 457)
Liking Writing Scale (LWS) 0.256 (N ⫽ 553) 0.133 (N ⫽ 462)
Self-reported writing performance 0.332 (N ⫽ 455)
Note. All correlations, with the exception of the relationship between the LWS and SWA (p ⫽ .003), are significant at p ⬍ .001. SEWS ⫽ Self-Efficacy
for Writing Scale.
a
Our sample included all students enrolled in 11th grade English/language arts classes at participating schools. Ns are lower for SWA comparisons because
not all of these students took the SWA. Demographic data showed that approximately 30% of these “missing” students were 12th graders enrolled in these
classes who could not have taken the SWA, which is given only to 11th graders. Correlations run with and without 12th graders produced nearly identical
results for all relationships.
DIMENSIONS OF WRITING SELF-EFFICACY 35

Table 4
Mean SEWS Writing Ideation, Conventions, and Self-Regulation Self-Efficacy; LWS Scores; Self-Reported Writing Performance; and
SWA Performance by High School Students’ ELA Class Enrollment

English/language arts class enrollment


GE (n ⫽ 37) Comp (n ⫽ 31) ALC (n ⫽ 323) APLC (n ⫽ 172) Effect
Measure M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) F p MSE size

SEWS: Ideation 62.05 (24.69) 69.40 (19.13) 72.39 (19.84) 78.84 (13.93) 10.22 .001 343.57 .053
SEWS: Conventions 64.66 (20.06) 72.17 (16.12) 84.65 (13.70) 90.19 (7.78) 49.15 .001 165.85 .211
SEWS: Self- regulation 53.71 (24.51) 53.73 (20.76) 60.60 (23.31) 69.66 (20.82) 9.95 .001 506.70 .052
Liking Writing Scale (LWS) 3.10 (0.90) 3.41 (0.78) 3.33 (0.95) 3.71 (0.85) 8.53 .001 0.822 .044
Self-reported writing performance 7.17 (2.06) 6.74 (1.48) 8.15 (1.40) 8.93 (1.08) 34.70 .001 1.875 .160
Statewide writing assessment (SWA) 3.95 (1.64) 4.86 (1.68) 5.98 (1.17) 6.73 (0.74) 61.59 .001 1.191 .287
Note. SEWS ratings ranged from 0 (no confidence) to 100 (completely confident), LWS scores from 1 to 5, self-reported writing performance scores from
2 to 10, and SWA scores from 2 to 8. Ns for the SWA analysis were 28, 14, 254, and 166 for the four groups, respectively. The effect size index is partial
␩2. SEWS ⫽ Self-Efficacy for Writing Scale; LWS ⫽ Liking Writing Scale; SWA ⫽ statewide writing assessment; ELA ⫽ English/language arts: GE ⫽
General English; Comp ⫽ Composition; ALC ⫽ American Literature and Composition; APLC ⫽ Advanced Placement Language and Composition; MSE ⫽
mean square error.

played a role in the formation of these students’ self-efficacy writing—successfully translating ideas into linguistic forms. Al-
beliefs. For instance, students in the APLC and ALC classes had though some might underestimate the importance of “writing
significantly higher SWA scores than did the GE and Comp mechanics,” they in fact are a key to successful writing—the
students and reported receiving significantly better writing-related ability to express what one wants to say by putting the “world on
grades. As can be seen in Table 4, these performance differences paper” (Olson, 1994). Self-efficacy for self-regulation also seems
were substantial, especially between the least and most advanced to occupy at least some part of every writer’s judgments of him- or
groups. There also were considerable differences in attitudes to- herself as a writer. Individuals can form at least some conclusions
ward writing between the groups at the high and low ends of the about their capabilities for articulating writing goals, embarking on
class placement continuum (APLC and GE groups), although writing tasks, and keeping at writing, especially when it becomes
ratings of feelings about writing were moderately positive overall difficult.
in Study 2’s high school sample, averaging 3.43 on a 5-point scale. Analyses of these two sets of data show that middle and high
One possible source of self-efficacy differences was a significant, school students responded to the categories represented by SEWS’
␹2(3, N ⫽ 552) ⫽ 31.19, p ⬍ .001, disproportionality among the items in similar ways and provide initial evidence for the utility of
proportions of students reporting that a language other than Eng- measuring separable dimensions of writing self-efficacy. Students
lish was the primary language spoken in their home. These pro- enrolled in different levels of high school English/language arts
portions were 32% and 35% in the GE and Comp courses, respec- classes, for instance, showed significant differences in levels of
tively, compared with only 12% in the ALC and 7% in the APLC writing-related self-efficacy on each of the SEWS subscales,
classes. However, post hoc analyses showed no significant differ- which themselves showed varying relationships to different writ-
ences in SWA scores in any of the classes between students ing outcome measures. Self-efficacy for writing conventions and
reporting English and those reporting a language other than Eng- scores on a formal measure of writing, the SWA, also shared
lish as the primary language spoken at home. Future research significantly more variance than did self-efficacy for ideation and
explicitly focusing on such factors as language background and
self-regulation and the SWA. In contrast, the significantly greater
course-related experiences will be needed to provide a better
relationships between liking writing and self-efficacy for writing
account of such factors’ influence on writing self-efficacy.
ideation and writing self-regulation than between liking writing
and self-efficacy for writing conventions hint at the possibility of
General Discussion greater affect associated with writers’ confidence for thinking of
The two studies reported here establish an empirical foundation good ideas (ideation) and managing the writing process (self-
for multifactorial models of writing self-efficacy and for other regulation) than with believing they can capably execute writings’
writing self-efficacy measures based on such views. The logic conventions.
underlying this study’s three-part model, which divided the writing The SEWS measure may have potential utility for research in
act into ideational, expressive, and self-regulatory dimensions, is both theoretical and applied domains. Because it ties more explic-
that writers sort their views of their capabilities for carrying out itly than does general self-efficacy measures to dimensions of
writing-related tasks into recognizable categories. Such categories, writing performance yet samples them reasonably broadly, the data
we argue, can be both theoretically and practically meaningful. In it yields could be useful in studies of instructional interventions
Flower and Hayes’ models of writing (e.g., Flower & Hayes, 1981, and outcomes. Future studies using SEWS or another multifaceted
1984; Hayes, 1996, 2012), for example, writers are portrayed as measure of writing self-efficacy, for example, could extend earlier
calling on their topical and world knowledge to generate and studies of writing self-efficacy development by providing finer
organize writing-related ideas. We recognize a second category of grained data on developmental patterns (e.g., Pajares & Valiante,
self-efficacy beliefs as having more direct ties to the craft of 1999; Shell et al., 1995) and gender-related differences (Pajares et
36 BRUNING, DEMPSEY, KAUFFMAN, MCKIM, AND ZUMBRUNN

al., 1999; Pajares & Valiante, 1997, 2001) in writing self-efficacy. vary widely; within even a single domain (e.g., reading, mathe-
Because of their relative brevity and straightforwardness, such matics), a given model and associated measures will reflect only
measures may also lend themselves to evaluations of writing-based one of many potentially valid frameworks for representing
interventions or use in connection with standardized measures of domain-related self-efficacy. Nonetheless, we believe that the
writing performance. present study points to the possibility of advances in self-efficacy
Although we are optimistic about this approach thus far, we research through improved alignment of self-efficacy measures
recognize several limitations in the work reported here. First, we with theory-based models of student learning and performance.
do not view the three-part model operationalized in the current
studies as adequately representing writing’s complexities but as
only one of many potentially useful frameworks for measuring References
writing self-efficacy that could be proposed. In constructing the Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social
current model, we deliberately limited our perspective to self- cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
efficacy for accomplishing activities happening during the writing Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York, NY:
act. We excluded planning, for instance, because we judged that it Freeman.
does not occur with every writer and writing occasion. There Bandura, A. (2006). Guide to constructing self-efficacy scales. In F.
doubtless are also other important dimensions of writing (e.g., Pajares & T. Urdan (Eds.), Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents (pp.
revision) about which writers may hold self-efficacy beliefs that 307–337). Greenwich, CT: Information Age.
are both sensitive to differences in writers’ backgrounds or devel- Beers, S. F., & Nagy, W. E. (2009). Syntactic complexity as a predictor of
opmental levels and tied to writing success. adolescent writing quality: Which measures? Which genre? Reading and
Writing, 22, 185–200. doi:10.1007/s11145-007-9107-5
Second, this study does not address issues of genre or other
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psy-
contextual factors tied to writing self-efficacy. It seems highly chological Bulletin, 107, 238 –246. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238
probable that, for most writers, self-efficacy differs substantially Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1987). The psychology of written com-
by type of writing and writing contexts. For instance, there obvi- position. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
ously are many genres (e.g., poetry, narrative, exposition) and Bong, M. (2006). Asking the right question: How confident are you that
categories of content to write about (e.g., science, history, sports) you could successfully perform these tasks? In F. Pajares & T. Urdan
for which writers’ self-efficacy would vary widely. Third, in the (Eds.), Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents (pp. 287–303). Greenwich,
current study we examined writing self-efficacy’s connections to CT: Information Age.
only a relatively limited number of writing-related variables and Bruning, R., & Horn, C. (2000). Developing motivation to write. Educa-
theoretical issues. As we continue our research, however, we tional Psychologist, 35, 25–37. doi:10.1207/S15326985EP3501_4
expect to further probe relationships of different categories of Bruning, R., Kauffman, D., Haines, C., & Zumbrunn, S. (2012, April).
Implicit beliefs about writing, liking writing, and writing performance of
writing self-efficacy to other motivational and epistemological
8th graders, 11th graders, and college students. Paper presented at the
variables (e.g., goal orientation, implicit beliefs about writing), as Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association,
well as to developmental and gender-related differences in writing Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada.
self-efficacy. Chenoweth, N. A., & Hayes, J. R. (2001). Fluency in writing: Generating
Finally, as indicated above, the present studies provide only text in L1 and L2. Written Communication, 18, 80 –98. doi:10.1177/
minimal information about the sources of students’ writing self- 0741088301018001004
efficacy beliefs. As we move forward, however, we expect to tie Cruse, A. (1986). Lexical semantics. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
measures of self-efficacy to students’ writing experiences and to University Press.
interventions targeted at specific dimensions of the writing pro- Cruse, A. (2004). Meaning in language: An introduction to semantics and
cess. pragmatics. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
In spite of these limitations, we hope that the current research Dappen, L., Isernhagen, J., & Anderson, S. (2008). A statewide writing
assessment model: Student proficiency and future implications. Assess-
will suggest new approaches to investigating writing self-efficacy,
ing Writing, 13, 45– 60. doi:10.1016/j.asw.2008.04.001
along with potential ways of conceptualizing efficacy for other Dempsey, M., Bruning, R., & Kauffman, D. (2010). An empirical test of a
foundational skills (e.g., reading) or subject-matter areas (e.g., new model of self-efficacy for writing. Poster presented at the Annual
mathematics, science). In our view, self-efficacy research becomes Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, Denver,
stronger to the extent that it maps onto key processes and capa- CO.
bilities underlying successful performance in domains of interest. Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues.
We thus interpret Bandura’s (2006) and Pajares’s (1999) recom- Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 167–188. doi:10.1207/
mendations to make self-efficacy scales domain-specific as not s1532799xssr0902_4
only implying a need to link self-efficacy items to learners’ de- Evans, V., & Green, M. (2006). Cognitive linguistics: An introduction.
velopmental levels and specific contexts but as a call to specify the Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
domain-related cognitive, procedural, and self-regulatory subskills Fayol, M., Alamargot, D., & Berninger, V. W. (Eds.). (2012). Translation
of thought to written text while composing. New York, NY: Psychology
underlying successful performance in the domain.
Press.
Of course, this is much easier said than done. A basic challenge
Flower, L., & Hayes, J. R. (1980). The cognition of discovery: Defining a
is to get the level of specificity “just right”—neither so broad that rhetorical problem. College Composition and Communication, 31, 21–
we gain minimal understanding about learners’ self-efficacy for 32. doi:10.2307/356630
engaging in domain-critical tasks nor so specific that we learn little Flower, L., & Hayes, J. R. (1981). A cognitive process theory of writing.
about their confidence for engaging generally in a domain. An- College Composition and Communication, 32, 365–387. doi:10.2307/
other challenge is that theoretical models for different domains 356600
DIMENSIONS OF WRITING SELF-EFFICACY 37

Flower, L., & Hayes, J. R. (1984). The representation of meaning in tary and middle school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98,
writing. Written Communication, 1, 120 –160. doi:10.1177/ 134 –147. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.134
0741088384001001006 Nebraska Department of Education. (2007). Nebraska Department of Ed-
Graham, S. (2006). Writing. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), ucation 2007 Statewide Writing Assessment for Grades 4, 8, and 11:
Handbook of educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 457– 478). Mahwah, Scoring Report. Lincoln, NE: Author.
NJ: Erlbaum. O’Hare, F. (1973). Sentence combining: Improving student writing without
Graham, S., & Perin, D. (2007). A meta-analysis of writing instruction for formal grammar instruction. Urbana, IL: NCTE.
adolescent students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 445– 476. Olson, D. R. (1994). The world on paper: The conceptual and cognitive
doi:10.1037/0022-0663.99.3.445 implications of writing and reading. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
Harris, K. R., Graham, S., & Mason, L. H. (2006). Self-regulated strategy University Press.
development for 2nd-grade students who struggle with writing. Ameri- Pajares, F. (2003). Self-efficacy beliefs, motivation, and achievement in
can Educational Research Journal, 43, 295–340. doi:10.3102/ writing: A review of the literature. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 19,
00028312043002295 139 –158. doi:10.1080/10573560308222
Hayes, J. R. (1996). A new framework for understanding cognition and Pajares, F. (2007). Empirical properties of a scale to assess writing self-
affect in writing. In C. M. Levy & S. Ransdell (Eds.), The science of efficacy in school contexts. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling
writing: Theories, methods, individual differences, and applications (pp. and Development, 39, 239 –249.
1–27). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Pajares, F., Britner, S. L., & Valiante, G. (2000). Relation between
Hayes, J. R. (2006). New directions in writing theory. In C. A. MacArthur, achievement goals and self-beliefs of middle school students in writing
S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. and science. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 406 – 422. doi:
28 – 40). New York, NY: Guilford Press. 10.1006/ceps.1999.1027
Hayes, J. R. (2012). Evidence from language bursts, revision, and tran- Pajares, F., Hartley, J., & Valiante, G. (2001). Response format in writing
scription for translation and its relation to other writing processes. In M. self-efficacy assessment: Greater discrimination increases prediction.
Fayol, D. Alamargot, & V. W. Berninger (Eds.), Translation of thought Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 33, 214 –
to written text while composing (pp. 15–25). New York, NY: Psychology 221.
Press. Pajares, F., & Johnson, M. J. (1994). Confidence and competence in
Hidi, S., & Boscolo, P. (2006). Motivation and writing. In C. A. MacAr-
writing: The role of writing self-efficacy, outcome expectancy, and
thur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research
apprehension. Research in the Teaching of English, 28, 313–331.
(pp. 144 –157). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Pajares, F., & Johnson, M. J. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in the writing of
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance
high school students: A path analysis. Psychology in the Schools, 33,
structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struc-
163–175. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6807(199604)33:2⬍163::AID-
tural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55. doi:10.1080/10705519909540118
PITS10⬎3.0.CO;2-C
Kauffman, D. K., Zhao, R., Dempsey, M., Zeleny, M., Wang, S., &
Pajares, F., Miller, M. D., & Johnson, M. J. (1999). Gender differences in
Bruning, R. (2010). How writing goals impact high school students
writing self-beliefs of elementary school students. Journal of Educa-
affect, self-efficacy beliefs, and achievement within writing-intensive
tional Psychology, 91, 50 – 61. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.91.1.50
high school courses. Poster presented at the Annual Meeting of the
Pajares, F., & Valiante, G. (1997). Influence of self-efficacy on elementary
American Educational Research Association, Denver, CO.
students’ writing. Journal of Educational Research, 90, 353–360.
Kellogg, R. T. (2008). Training writing skills: A cognitive developmental
Pajares, F., & Valiante, G. (1999). Grade level and gender differences in
perspective. Journal of Writing Research, 1, 1–26.
the writing self-beliefs of middle school students. Contemporary Edu-
Klassen, R. (2002). Writing in early adolescence: A review of the role of
self-efficacy beliefs. Educational Psychology Review, 14, 173–203. doi: cational Psychology, 24, 390 – 405. doi:10.1006/ceps.1998.0995
10.1023/A:1014626805572 Pajares, F., & Valiante, G. (2001). Gender differences in writing motiva-
Klassen, R., & Georgiou, G. (2008). Spelling and writing self-efficacy of tion and achievement of middle school students: A function of gender
Indo-Canadian and Anglo-Canadian early adolescents. International Mi- orientation? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26, 366 –381. doi:
gration and Integration, 9, 311–326. doi:10.1007/s12134-008-0068-6 10.1006/ceps.2000.1069
Kuo, L., & Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning Pajares, F., & Valiante, G. (2006). Self-efficacy beliefs and motivation in
to read: A cross-cultural perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41, writing development. In C. A. MacArthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald
161–180. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep4103_3 (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 158 –170). New York, NY:
Langacker, R. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar. Stanford, CA: Guilford Press.
Stanford University Press. Rankin, J. L., Bruning, R. H., & Timme, V. L. (1994). The development of
Langacker, R. (2008). Cognitive grammar. Oxford, England: Oxford Uni- beliefs about spelling and their relationship to spelling performance.
versity Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195331967.001.0001 Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8, 213–232. doi:10.1002/acp
McCarthy, P., Meier, S., & Rinderer, R. (1985). Self-efficacy and writing: .2350080303
A different view of self-evaluation. College Composition and Commu- Read, J. (2000). Assessing vocabulary. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
nication, 36, 465– 471. doi:10.2307/357865 University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511732942
McCutchen, D., Teske, P., & Bankston, C. (2008). Writing and cognition: Saddler, B., Behforooz, B., & Asaro, K. (2008). The effects of sentence-
Implications of the cognitive architecture for learning to write and combining instruction on the writing of fourth-grade students with
writing to learn. In C. Bazerman (Ed.), Handbook of writing research writing difficulties. Journal of Special Education, 42, 79 –90. doi:
(pp. 451– 470). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 10.1177/0022466907310371
Meng, X.-L., Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1992). Comparing correlated Saddler, B., & Graham, S. (2005). The effects of peer-assisted sentence-
correlation coefficients. Psychological Bulletin, 111, 172–175. combining instruction on the writing performance of more and less
Myhill, D. (2008). Towards a linguistic model of sentence development in skilled young writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 43–54.
writing. Language and Education, 22, 271–288. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.97.1.43
Nagy, W., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2006). Contributions of Schleppegrell, M. J. (2007). At last: The meaning in grammar. Research in
morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elemen- the Teaching of English, 42, 121–128.
38 BRUNING, DEMPSEY, KAUFFMAN, MCKIM, AND ZUMBRUNN

Schraw, G. (2006). Knowledge: Structures and processes. In P. A. Alex- Zeleny, M., & Yilmaz-Soylu, M. (2010). Writing habits and attitudes of
ander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (2nd adolescents: A look at the roles of technology-enhanced and traditional
ed., pp. 245–264). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. writing in adolescent lives. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2007). Influencing children’s self- National Reading Conference, Ft. Worth, TX.
efficacy and self-regulation of reading and writing through modeling. Zimmerman, B. J., & Bandura, A. (1994). Impact of self-regulatory influ-
Reading and Writing Quarterly, 23, 7–25. doi:10.1080/ ences on writing course attainment. American Educational Research
10573560600837578 Journal, 31, 845– 862.
Shell, D., Colvin, C., & Bruning, R. (1995). Developmental and ability Zimmerman, B. J., & Kitsantas, A. (1999). Acquiring writing revision skill:
differences in self-efficacy, causal attribution, and outcome expectancy Shifting from process to outcome self-regulatory goals. Journal of
mechanisms in reading and writing achievement. Journal of Educational Educational Psychology, 91, 241–250. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.91.2.241
Psychology, 87, 386 –398. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.87.3.386 Zimmerman, B. J., & Kitsantas, A. (2002). Acquiring writing revision and
Shell, D., Murphy, C., & Bruning, R. (1989). Self-efficacy and outcome self-regulatory skill through observation and emulation. Journal of Ed-
expectancy mechanisms in reading and writing achievement. Journal of ucational Psychology, 94, 660 – 668. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.94.4.660
Educational Psychology, 81, 91–100. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.81.1.91 Zimmerman, B. J., & Kitsantas, A. (2007). A writer’s discipline: The
Spandel, V. (2005). Creating writers through 6-trait writing assessment development of self-regulatory skill. In S. Hidi & P. Boscolo (Eds.),
and instruction. Boston, MA: Pearson. Motivation and writing: Research and school practice (pp. 51– 69). New
Steiger, J. H. (1990). Structural model evaluation and modification: An York, NY: Kluwer.
interval estimation approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25, Zumbrunn, S. K., Bruning, R. H., Kauffman, D. F., & Hayes, M. (2010).
173–180. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr2502_4 Explaining determinants of confidence and success in the elementary
Torrance, M., Thomas, G. V., & Robinson, E. J. (1996). Finding something writing classroom. Poster presented at the American Educational Re-
to write about: Strategic and automatic processes in idea generation. In search Association Annual Meeting, Denver, CO.
C. M. Levy & S. Ransdell (Eds.), The science of writing: Theories, Zumbrunn, S. K., Kauffman, D. F., Hayes, M., & Yilmaz-Soylu, M.
methods, individual differences and applications (pp. 199 –205). Mah- (2010). How writing achievement goals and self-efficacy relate to col-
wah, NJ: Erlbaum. lege writing goals and grade point averages. Paper presented at the
Vellutino, F. R., Tunmer, W. E., Jaccard, J. J., & Chen, R. (2007). American Educational Research Association Annual Meeting, Den-
Components of reading ability: Multivariate evidence for a convergent ver, CO.
skills model of reading development. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11,
3–32.
White, M. J., & Bruning, R. (2005). Implicit writing beliefs and their Received February 16, 2011
relation to writing quality, Contemporary Educational Psychology, 30, Revision received June 19, 2012
166 –189. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2004.07.002 Accepted June 27, 2012 䡲

Call for Papers: Special Issue


Ethical, Regulatory, and Practical Issues in Telepractice
Professional Psychology: Research and Practice will publish a special issue on recent ethical,
regulatory and practical issues related to telepractice. In its broadest definition the term telepractice
refers to any contact with a client/patient other than face-to-face in person contact. Thus, telepractice
may refer to contact on a single event or instance such as via the telephone or by means of electronic
mail, social media (e.g., Facebook) or through the use of various forms of distance visual
technology. We would especially welcome manuscripts ranging from the empirical examination of
the broad topic related to telepractice to those manuscripts that focus on a particular subset of issues
associated with telepractice. Although manuscripts that place an emphasis on empirical research are
especially encouraged, we also would welcome articles on these topics that place an emphasis on
theoretical approaches as well as an examination of the extant literature in the field. Finally,
descriptions of innovative approaches are also welcome. Regardless of the type of article, all articles
for the special issue will be expected to have practice implications to the clinical setting. Manu-
scripts may be sent electronically to the journal at http://www.apa.org/pubs/journals/pro/index.aspx
to the attention of Associate Editor, Janet R. Matthews, Ph.D.

You might also like