You are on page 1of 17

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/353107671

Is it the perceived stress scale (PSS) Undimimensional and invariant? A


Bifactor analysis in Mexican adults

Article in Current Psychology · July 2021


DOI: 10.1007/s12144-021-02067-x

CITATIONS READS

11 653

5 authors, including:

Arturo Juárez García Cesar Merino


Universidad Autónoma del Estado de Morelos University of San Martín de Porres
125 PUBLICATIONS 948 CITATIONS 331 PUBLICATIONS 2,652 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

José Félix Brito-Ortíz Alisma Monroy


Autonomous University of Baja California Universidad Autónoma del Estado de Morelos
18 PUBLICATIONS 55 CITATIONS 2 PUBLICATIONS 27 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by José Félix Brito-Ortíz on 04 August 2021.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Current Psychology
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-02067-x

Is it the perceived stress scale (PSS) Undimimensional and invariant?


A Bifactor analysis in Mexican adults
Arturo Juárez-García 1 & César Merino-Soto 2 & José Félix Brito-Ortiz 3 & Martha Eugenia Nava-Gómez 3 &
Alisma Monroy-Castillo 4

Accepted: 1 July 2021


# The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2021Abstract

The perceived stress scale (PSS) has been widely used as a unidimensional global measure of stress despite its classical bi-
dimensional structure. However, a bifactor model seems to be the best structure alternative in terms of a possible method effect.
The few available studies on PSS validity (in both the 14 and 10 item versions) that test the bifactor model do not consider its
measurement invariance between occupational status and gender. The goal of this study was to test the bifactor structure and
measurement invariance between occupational status and gender in the PSS 14 and 10 item versions. 1149 Mexican adults
voluntarily answered the Spanish version of the PSS, 591 were students and 558 were health or education workers. Through a
SEM methodology, several models were tested (unidimensional, two factors, bifactor, positive and negative wording effect, and
random content). The results showed a bifactor model and a general factor with a subfactor for negative phrasing as the best for
both versions (14–10 items) (CFI > .998, RMSEA<.037). Measurement invariance for sex and occupational status was found.
The PSS-10 version showed slightly better indicators. In conclusion, PSS-10 and PSS-14 versions showed no evidence of gender
or occupational status bias. A bifactor structure was the best model in both versions; however, these findings suggest a clear
method effect in the PSS due to items with different directions (negative phrasing). Suggestions for future research and practical
implications are discussed.

Keywords Perceived stress scale . Bifactor . Validity . Mexican

Introduction study variable that has driven more inter and trans-
disciplinary efforts in the health sciences due to its
Stress is an emerging phenomenon that has captured the biopsychosocial complexity. It is associated with the most
interest of a wide diversity of disciplines. It may be the emerging public health diseases worldwide (e.g., cardio-
metabolic, immune, musculoskeletal, and mental diseases)
and has a salient role in overall mortality (Kiecolt-Glaser
* Arturo Juárez-García & Glaser 1999; Cohen, Janicki-Deverts & Miller, 2007,
arturojuarezg@hotmail.com Sapolsky, 2004). After the biological theory of Selye
(1976), the psychological theory of Lazarus and Folkman
César Merino-Soto
sikayax@yahoo.com.ar; cmerinos@usmp.pe (1984) earned its place as a way to conceive stress from a
more widely and necessary biopsychosocial perspective,
José Félix Brito-Ortiz adopting a transactional approach of the complex relation-
felix.brito@uabc.edu.mx
ship between people and their environment. This theory
Martha Eugenia Nava-Gómez considers three elements responsible for stress: the envi-
martha.nava40@uabc.edu.mx
ronment, the personal susceptibility of the individual, and
Alisma Monroy-Castillo mainly, the cognitive appraisal of events. For Lazarus and
alisma.monroy@uaem.mx Folkman (1984), stress is: “a particular relationship be-
1
tween the person and the environment that is appraised
Universidad Autónoma del Estado de Morelos, Cuernavaca, México
by the person as taxing or exceeding his or her resources
2
Universidad de San Martín de Porres, Lima, Peru and endangering his or her well-being” (p. 43); thus, it is
3
Universidad Autónoma de Baja California, Tijuana, México stated that stress is mainly a subjective phenomenon, a
4
Universidad Autónoma del Estado de Morelos, Cuernavaca, México pivotal feature to consider when measuring it.

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

The Perceived Stress Scale (PSS; Cohen, Kamarck, and 2 factors has been replicated by dozens of subsequent studies.
Mermelstein, 1983) is one of the most widely used tools in For example, in a systematic review of the psychometric prop-
clinical and non-clinical scenarios to measure perceived stress. erties of the PSS, Lee (2012) selected 18 reports spanning
It was developed based on Lazarus’ concept of cognitive ap- from 1988 to 2012 and found that practically all the studies
praisal but in terms of an experience of overall stress (not of reported a 2 factor-structure. It is important to note that in most
specific events). In the PSS model, people assess the degree of of these studies, Cronbach’s Alpha indices (α) evidenced ac-
control they have or do not have over unpredictable or unex- ceptable thresholds (>.70); however, these were reported as-
pected situations in their lives. This assessment includes the suming the unidimensionality of the scale without testing it.
perception of oppressive or burdensome situations, which in Likewise, other methodological assumptions were made with-
the absence of adequate coping resources, translates into un- out testing. This trend continues in other studies after the pub-
ease and discomfort. This agrees with the transactional and lication of Lee’s review (Gónzález, Rodríguez, and
cognitive perspective of stress (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Hernandez, 2013; Safaei & Shokri, 2014; Eklund,
Remor & Carrobles, 2001; Remor, 2006). Bäckström & Tuvesson, 2014; Smith, Rosenberg & Haight,
The original PSS consists of 14 items. 7 reversed (or neg- 2014, amongst others).
atively worded) items favoring control over threatening situa- In general, some limitations can be identified in many of the
tions, and 7 positively worded items focused on the loss of above-mentioned studies: a) reliability is estimated with
control and its consequences (Cohen et al., 1983). The scale Cronbach’s α coefficients without verifying the compliance of
was subsequently shortened; first to a 10-item (6 positive and their assumptions; b) the exploratory solutions are reached with
4 negative items), and later to a 4-item version (2 positive and common factor methods or with the “Little Jiffy” criterion,
2 negative items; Cohen & Williamson, 1988). The original Known as the Kaiser rule (Kaiser, 1970) (Principal Com-
PSS English version has been translated into several lan- ponents, eigenvalues equal to or greater than 1, and Varimax
guages, including Arabic (Almadi, Cathers, Mansour, & rotation); c) CFA is estimated with the maximum likelihood
Chow, 2012), German (Reis, Lehr, Heber & Ebert, 2019), (ML) function; d) invariance of the scale across groups is as-
Iranian (Safaei & Shokri, 2014), Korean (Lee & Jeong, sumed, making direct comparisons between them (e.g. sex, age,
2019), Greek (Michaelides, Christodoulou, Kkeli, Karekla & occupation, etc.); and e) there is no empirical evidence to sup-
Panayyioutu, 2016), French (Lesage, Berjot & Deschamps, port the use of total score with the use of suitable methods to
2012), Japanese (Mimura & Grifiths, 2004), Swedish directly examine the uni- or multi-dimensionality of the scale.
(Eklund, Bäckström & Tuvesson, 2014), Serbian Regarding the points discussed above, it should be noted
(Jovanović, & Gavrilov-Jerković, 2015), and Spanish that: a) the Cronbach’s Alpha coefficient (α) is suitable if
(Remor and Carrobles, 2001; Remor, 2006), amongst others. assumptions such as tau-equivalence, are verified and com-
These wide range of translations point to the far-reaching in- plied with (Trizano-Hermosilla, & Alvarado, 2016). The ome-
ternationalization of the PSS, and to the potential representa- ga coefficient (ω), is a more robust indicator that can better
tional cognitions of stress and their universality (Fetvadjiev & represent the reliable variance of the construct (McDonald,
Van de Vijver 2015). For information on all translations 1999); b) the use of the Little Jiffy criterion is currently the
please visit the author’s website: https://www.cmu.edu/ least recommended method to validate constructs and mea-
dietrich/psychology/stress-immunity-disease-lab/scales/ sures of latent variables (Brown, 2015; Lloret-Segura,
index.html. Ferreres-Traver, Hernández-Baeza, & Tomás-Marco, 2014;
The factor structure of the PSS scale was not presented in Park, Dailey & Lemus, 2002); c) although it is frequently
Cohen et al.’s seminal work (1983), but a later study (Cohen used, the ML function is sensitive to the breach of the multi-
& Williamson, 1988) provided 2 factors obtained via a prin- variate normality assumption, and applying a CFA – SEM
cipal component method. Surprisingly, the second factor, without this feature miscalculates the estimated parameters
which contained only the recoded or negatively worded items of the items and the latent covariance (Brown, 2015); d) it is
(items reflecting control over stress), was theoretically and well known that the scores of any scale could change depend-
methodologically underestimated. This factor was considered ing on the characteristics of a sample (e.g. sex or occupational
by the authors as “irrelevant”. They insist on the unidimen- status). When such variance is relevant to the latent variable
sionality of the scale and suggest the use of the total score. assessed, there is a bias that invalidates any differences orig-
However, this can be troublesome from a theoretical and psy- inally found between groups. Measurement invariance estima-
chometric point of view (Reis, et al., 2019), and it is worri- tion ensures that the meaning and levels that outline the con-
some given that the finding of a two-factor model is so struct are equivalent across groups. This can be directly tested
frequent. through differential item functioning (DIF), or multigroup
Based on substantive studies, including those of the origi- confirmatory factor analysis (MCFA) (Reis et al., 2019;
nal authors (Cohen & Janicki-Deverts, 2012; Cohen, Tyrrell, Sass, 2011). Finally, e) the recommended procedures that
& Smith, 1993; Cohen & Williamson, 1988), the structure of prove the uni- or multi-dimensionality are based on methods

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

like bifactor modeling (Reise, Morizot, & Hays, 2007), and factors are maintained as orthogonal (correlated attributes –
there are few available psychometric studies on PSS using this non-correlated methods). Consistently, confirmatory factor
approach. analyses through structural equation modeling (CFA –
After Lee’s review (2012), there is another set of studies SEM) have allowed verifying the effect of negatively worded
where the analysis of measurement invariance and bifactor items (i.e., recoded items) together with positively worded
modeling have been applied to the PSS. For example, it was items. In general, this trend to share unique variance requires
recently observed that the scale is invariant or equivalent for to be modeled next to the substantive factors (Corwyn, 2000;
variables such as sex (Reis et al., 2019; Denovan et al., 2019); Horan, DiStefano, & Motl, 2003; Marsh, 1989). Some studies
language (Perera et al., 2017); type of participants (ie, clinical/ with other self-reported measures in Hispanic populations
non-clinical; Jovanović & Gavrilov-Jerković, 2015), passing have also raised this concern (Dominguez-Lara, & Merino-
time (Reis et al., 2019), or between athlete and non-athlete Soto, 2018; Tomas & Oliver, 1999; Tomás, Galiana,
students (Chiu, Lu, Lin, Nien, Hsu, & Liu, 2016). However, Hontangas, Oliver, & Sancho, 2013; Tomás, Sancho, Oliver,
no studies have analyzed the invariance across occupational Galiana, & Meléndez, 2012; Suárez-Alvarez, Pedrosa,
status, like professional employees (teacher and nurses) and Lozano, García-Cueto, Cuesta, & Muñiz, 2018). In evaluating
students, even though these groups have concentrated the ma- the effect of item wording, control strategies in structural
jority of validation studies with the PSS (Lee, 2012). equation modeling (SEM) have been established for years,
Furthermore, given the biological or gender differences in such as the multitrait-multimethod approach (MultiTrait-
experiencing stress in different contexts, it seems necessary MultiMethod: MTMM; Marsh, 1989; Marsh and Grayson,
to continue testing the invariance by sex in different samples, 1995). The most frequent use occurs with the Correlated
thereby preventing wrong generalizations and procuring a Trait-Correlated Method approach (CTCM; Marsh, &
suitable treatment for each group (Messing et al., 2003). Grayson, 1995) which can be independent or complementary
Some studies have used a hierarchical CFA as a better fit to the bifactor analysis in general, and to another commonly
option for the two-factor model of the PSS (Golden-Kreutz, applied method called Correlated Trait-Correlated
Browne, Frierson, & Andersen, 2004; Siqueira Reis, Ferreira Uniqueness (Marsh, 1989; Marsh & Bailey, 1991).
Hino, & Romelio Rodriguez Añez, 2010). However, it has Based on all of the above, the purpose of this study is to
long been observed that these two factors frequently covariate determine the factor structure of the Perceived Stress Scale
highly (r > .50), suggesting the need for bifactor modeling. (PSS) in the 10- and 14-item versions, through bifactor model-
Indeed, recent studies demonstrated that a bifactor structure ing with a multitrait-multimethod approach, and to analyze the
with a simultaneous general factor and two specific factors measurement invariance across sex and occupational status
(negative items and positive items in each factor) has been (workers and students) in a sample of Mexican adults.
superior to the two related factors model (Denovan et al.,
2019; Jovanović, & Gavrilov-Jerković, 2015; Lee, & Jeong,
2019; Park, & Colvin, 2019; Reis, Lehr, Heber, & Ebert, Method
2019; Reyna, Mola, & Correa, 2019; Wiriyakijja et al.,
2019; Wu, & Amtmann, 2013). Other studies have found a Population
better-fit model with a general factor and just one specific
subfactor which captures the negative items (i.e., recoded; A non-random sample of 1149 Mexican adult subjects from
Michaelides, Christodoulou, Kkeli, Karekla, & Panayiotou, two universities was included in this study. 558 participants
2016; Perera et al., 2017). These findings suggest that there were healthcare and education workers, and 591 were
is still no homogeneity in the psychometric bifactor structure healthcare students. All of them were residents of the States
of the PSS. Besides, it should be noted that practically all of of Morelos and Baja California and were invited to participate
these studies used only the 10-item version of the PSS, thus mainly by e-mail. The response rate was about 60%. All the
the bifactor structure with the original 14-item scale is still participants were enrolled after signing an informed con-
unknown, as is the case of a comparative evaluation including sent. This study was performed in line with the principles
both scales (see Table 1). of the Declaration of Helsinki. Approval was granted by the
It is important to note that the interpretation of bifactor Ethics Committees of the Health Sciences School at the
modeling in these studies has been oriented towards its sub- University of Baja California (2018/No F-PROMEP-38/
stantiality (theoretical aspect), rather than to the plausible ef- Rev-04) and the Center for Transdisciplinary Psychology
fect of the method caused by the wording of the items (meth- Research at the University of Morelos (2019/No.CEI-
odological aspect). Bifactor modeling may also be interpreted 210219-12). In addition to voluntary participation, the only
as a variant of the multitrait-multimethod approach (Markon, inclusion criterion was to be an adult over 18 years old. The
2019; Reise, Moore & Haviland, 2010), where the covariance exclusion criteria were not completing the questionnaire ful-
of the substantive factors is parametrized and the method ly, being under psychiatric treatment (detected through the

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

Table 1 Studies of bifactor structure with the PSS scale

Study Sample Size and Major Results Invariance Remarks


(Country) Type

Wu, & N = 446 Versions: PSS-10 and PSS-14 Not reported • PSS-10 better than PSS-14
Amtmann, Multiple Language: English • The use of a total score is suggested
(2013) Sclerosis Best model: bifactor (1 general
USA Patients and 2 specific factors, 6 positive
Country: USA and 4 negative items)
Jovanović & N = 157 Versions: PSS-10 Not reported • PSS-10 better than PSS-14
Gavrilov- Psychiatric Language: Serbian • The use of a total score is suggested
Jerković, patients Best model: bifactor (1 general
(2015) N = 165 and 2 specific factors, 6 positive
Serbia Non-clinical and 4 negative items)
adults
Michaelides N = 320 Versions: PSS-10 Not reported • The use of a total score is suggested
(2016) Adults in general
Language: Greek
Greece Best model: bifactor (1 general and
2 specific factors, 6 positive and
4 negative items)
Perera et al. N = 5176 Versions: PSS-10 Not reported • The calculation of one score using a General Factor and
(2017) Hispanic/Latinos Language: Spanish and English another using the positive subdimension recoded is
USA Best model: bifactor (1 general suggested, due to a method effect found.
and 2 specific factors, 6 positive
and 4 negative items)
Reis et al. N = 11,939 Versions: PSS-10 Invariance • The use of the total score and that of subfactors is
(2019) German adults Language: German across suggested, depending on the interest of the investigator
Germany N = 1862 Best model: bifactor (1 general groups: sex • The scale shows temporary and sex invariance.
German adults and 2 specific factors, 6 positive Temporary
and 4 negative items) invariance
Denovan et al. N = 524 Versions: PSS-10 Group: sex • The use of a total score is suggested
(2019) University Language: English • The scale shows measurement invariance by sex
United Students Best model: bifactor (1 general
Kingdom and 2 specific factors, 6 positive
and 4 negative items)
Lee, & Jeong, N = 250 Versions: PSS-10 Not reported • The use of a total score is suggested
(2019) University Language: Korean
Korea Students Best model: bifactor (1 general
and 2 specific factors, 6 positive
and 4 negative items)
Wiriyakijja, N = 260 Versions: PSS-10 Not reported • The use of a total score is suggested
et al. (2019) Odontology Language: English
United students Best model: bifactor (1 general
Kingdom and 2 specific factors, 6 positive
and 4 negative items)
Park, & Colvin N = 373 Versions: PSS-10 Not reported • The use of a total score is suggested
(2019). Military Language: Korean
South Korea personnel Best model: bifactor (1 general
and 2 specific factors, 6 positive
and 4 negative items)
Reyna et al. N = 279 Versions: Version PSS 10 and Not reported • The use of a total score is suggested
(2019) Adults in general PSS 14
Argentina Language: Spanish
Best model: bifactor (1 general
and 2 specific factors, 6 positive
and 4 negative items)

self-report item: “are you under any psychiatric treat- the participants were: average age was 39 years old (range
ment?”), or being identified as a multivariate outlier. After 23–72 years old, SD = 10), 74.3% were females and the
the exclusion criteria screening (see the pretrial and item most common academic level was undergraduate or gradu-
analysis section), the effective sample size was made up ate (51.9%), followed by middle school (20%), postgraduate
of 1093 participants. The demographic characteristics of (17.5%), and high school (10.6%).

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

Instrument specifying one factor to capture the method effect are more
recommended than the correlated uniqueness models
The Spanish version of the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS; (Conway, Lievens, Scullen, Lance, 2004; Fan, & Lance,
Cohen et al., 1983; Remor & Carrobles, 2001; González & 2017; Lance, Noble, & Scullen, 2002). Thereby, this study
Landero, 2007) was completed by all participants. It consists estimates the method effect by configuring the factors oriented
of 14 items, distributed in two factors on a Likert-type scale, towards negatively or positively worded items, through two
ranging from “never” (0) to “very often” (4). The scoring of basic substantive models: 1) the unidimensional model, where
items 4, 5, 6, 7, 9, 10, and 13 are recoded to generate a new all the items were modeled within a single factor, which rep-
score which indicates greater perceived stress. In the 10-item resents the global construct of stress; and 2) the related factors
version, 4,5, 12, and 13 items are removed. The questionnaires model, where the items are pooled in two factors linked to the
were administered at the workplace sites through the author’s wording of the items (positive and negative) (Fig. 1).
contacts, and some were sent by email. Additionally, we estimated another group of models ad-
dressed to demonstrate the method effect using correlated
Procedure traits–correlated methods (CTCM). They were specified as
follows: 3) the bifactor model, which contained a general fac-
Data Analysis tor Fg (with all the negative items recoded, and the rest of the
positive items) and two specific factors (one containing the
Potential Response Bias Multivariate outliers were examined negative items and the other with positive items); representing
using D2 distance (Mahalanobis, 1936). In order to have a also a model of correlated traits–correlated methods (Markon,
balance between Type I and Type II errors, and due to possible 2019; Reise, Moore, & Haviland, 2010); 4) a model of one
differences in data collection and its effects on the responses, substantive general factor (all of the items), and one factor
the significance level of D2 was preserved at 99% with the with the negatively worded items, and 5) a model of one
Bonferroni correction (Penny, 1996), this is at α = .01 (critical substantive factor (all of the items) and one factor with the
value, D2 ≥ 29.134). Also, individual variability was calculat- positively worded items. Both models 4 and 5, had the nega-
ed to detect response patterns associated with the careless or tively worded items recoded. These models are related to the
insufficient effort in responding (irv, greater or lesser models of correlated traits–correlated methods minus one
variability suggests random and/or careless responding; (CT–C[M–1]; Eid, Lischetzke, Nussbeck, & Trierweiler,
Meade, & Craig, 2012). Analyses were performed using the 2003), where one of the factors is considered as a reference
careless R program (Yentes, & Wilhelm, 2018). to interpret the general factor. Finally, according to
Schönberger and Ponsford (2010), the addition of one factor
Descriptive Characteristics of Items The descriptive character- in a model to improve its fit raises the risk of capitalizing on
istics of items were analyzed as well as their association with the parameters added, even if the added factor is not substan-
three demographic variables, (e.g. sex, age, and occupational tive. To corroborate the potential effect on the fit improvement
status). Glass rank-biserial correlations (King, Rosopa, & produced by the bifactor, another model was created 6): eight
Minium, 2011) and polyserial correlations were calculated to items were chosen randomly (four positively worded and four
assess these associations. We used the Rcompanion negatively worded) from the PSS to replace the method factor.
(Mangiafico, 2020), and polycor (Fox, 2019) programs, The fit deterioration in this model suggests the relevance of
respectively. the true method factors for positive and negative wording. Fig.
1 shows a graphic representation of the models tested.
Internal Structure The number of latent dimensions in the PSS The assessment of multidimensionality in the bifactor anal-
was firstly verified. The Exploratory Graph Analysis (EGA; ysis was made with several descriptive coefficients. Firstly,
Golino & Epskamp, 2017), a new technique based on net- with the explained common variance (ECV; Rodriguez
works, was then applied. In this case, 500 parametric boot- et al., 2016a, 2016b) in the general factor (ECVg), in the spe-
strap samples, and Glasso estimation of Gaussian graphical cific factors (ECVf), and the items (I-ECV; Stucky, Edelen,
models (Golino & Epskamp, 2017) were specified. Also, as Vaughan, Tucker, & Butler, 2014). Secondly, factor reliability
a complementary exploration of the number of factors, we was evaluated with the total reliable variance (ω) and the spe-
performed an optimum parallel analysis (Timmerman, & cific variance of each factor (ωh). Also, the degree of construct
Lorenzo-Seva, 2011). The EGAnet (Golino & Christensen, replicability was evaluated by the H coefficient (Hancock &
2020) and Factor (Ferrando, & Lorenzo-Seva, 2017) pro- Mueller, 2001). Thirdly, the absolute relative parameter bias
grams were used. indicator was used (ARPB; Rodriguez et al., 2016a, 2016b).
The methodological approach used was confirmatory fac- This quantifies the degree of metric difference of the items
tor analysis (CFA), within the structural equation modeling between a unidimensional model and the general factor of
technique (SEM). Conceptually and empirically, the models the bifactor model. Finally, factor determinacy (FD,

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

Fig. 1 Six Measurement Models tested in the PSS – 14 and PSS – 10. items. A: factor for randomly selected items. Residuals not shown. (The
Note. G general factor. P: factor for positive items. N: factor for negative number of items shown is only informative).
items. n1 … n4: negatively worded items. p1 … p4: positively worded

Gorsuch, 1983; Rodriguez et al., 2016a, 2016b) and the higher unconstrained model (i.e., configural or weak invariance) to
magnitude of these ratios (ECV > .70; ω ≥ .80; H ≥ .80; FD ≥ the model with constrained intercepts (i.e., metric or strict
.90) indicate a greater power of the general factor (Quinn, invariance). The fit assessment in these models was performed
2014; Rodriguez et al., 2016a, 2016b), except for ARPB using the differences in the fit ratios (Chen, 2007), including
(ARPB: between 10% and < 15%). These coefficients were ΔCFI (< .01), ΔRMSEA (< .015), and ΔSRMR (< .030). To assess
calculated through the Bifactor Indices Calculator in R pro- measurement invariance as an effect size measure, specific
gram (Dueber, 2020). measures were implemented for the degree of the impact on
Modeling of both substantive and method effect models the item, using the dmacs measure (Nye, & Drasgow, 2011),
were performed with WLSMV estimator (Muthén, du Toit, which is a standardized measure equivalent in the interpreta-
& Spisic, 1997) and polychoric inter-item correlations. This tion of standardized differences (Nye, Bradburn, Olenick,
was considered appropriate because of its efficacy to treat Bialko, & Drasgow, 2019).
categorical ordinal variables within SEM modeling (Li,
2016). All the models were assessed using approximate fit
indices, including CFI (≥ .95), RMSEA (≤ .05) and its 90% Results
confidence intervals, SRMR (≤ .05), and WRMR (≤ .90; Yu,
2002). These were implemented using the lavaan (Rosseel, Preliminary and Item Analysis
2012) and semTools (Jorgensen, Pornprasertmanit,
Schoemann, & Rosseel, 2020) packages in R. The evaluation In the preliminary analysis of the response biases, 60 subjects
of invariance was performed with the cumulative procedure of with a D2 between 29.73 and 52.29 (i.e., 5.2% of the initial
added constraints (Brown, 2015). For categorical variables, sample) were detected. They accounted for an individual var-
invariance estimation was performed according to Wu and iability out of the median (Md = .916), i.e., between irv = .196
Estabrook specifications (2016), that is, from the and irv = 1.988. The response patterns seemed to be

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

inconsistent and therefore they were removed, thus, the effec- by the unidimensional model with a method subfactor for
tive final sample consisted of 1093 participants. negative wording (items recoded) (1F + 1 fm neg), and then
Regarding the items, the hypothesis of univariate (CVM > the rest (Table 3).
6.00, p < .01) and multivariate (Royston’s – H = 1534.597, p < Table 4 shows the parameters for the items. For the
.01) normal distribution were not validated with the data. On bifactor model, the negatively worded items have a low
the other hand, a correlation between the items of the scale and validity for the general factor Fg, while the factor for the
three sociodemographic variables (sex, age, and occupation) positive items (Fpos) carries heterogeneous factor loadings
was found. This suggests that exposure to these conditions is with low to moderate magnitudes. Meanwhile, the specific
related to different response trends (Table 2) and demonstrates factor Fneg retained the highest factor loadings. This result
sensibility to statistical and practical significance. For in- is reflected in the fit indices of the bifactor model, where
stance, the average correlation between sex and the positively the power of Fneg was clear (I-ECV > .85, ω > .90, ωh >
worded items was lower (Mr = .028) than with the negatively .80, H > .90, and FD > .90), in contrast with Fg weak-
worded items (Mr = .192). This may show that females tend to ness. The fit indicators of the items (RPB and I-ECV) also
higher responses to the items emphasizing the maladaptive confirm that the items do not represent a strong general
stress response. In contrast, there was a higher linear covari- factor.
ance between age (Mr = −.231) and occupation (Mr = .288) In assessing measurement invariance across groups
for the positively worded items, in contrast with the correla- (Table 5), successive constraints did not produce great chang-
tions between age (Mr = .036) and occupation (Mr = .093) for es in the parameters of the items and there was no need to add
the negatively worded items. In general, younger people per- modifications to improve the fit and establish the presence of
ceive more stress, and the occupational status (worker vs. invariance. However, when comparing both groups (by sex
student) tends to show higher correlations, suggesting that and occupational status) invariance of the intercepts seemed to
the perception of stress in workers is higher than that of stu- be absent for the ΔRMSEA difference. However, the effect size
dents (Table 2). indicators (at the item levels) were negligible: dmacs between
1.07e-16 and 8.89e-17, indicating that invariance between
Modeling groups remains.

Number of Dimensions For the 14-item version of the PSS, PSS-10 The results for this version (also shown in Tables 2, 3,
EGA yielded a result of two dimensions with items 4 and 5) were similar regarding the order of the better models
pooled in wording-based sets (Fig. 2). A possible factorial estimated (based on a WRMR). A closer inspection of the
complexity in items 2, 3, and 10 was also observed, sug- WLSMV-□2 test and its associated fit indices shows that these
gesting measurement ambiguity (items explained by more were comparatively better for the PSS-10 versus the PSS-14.
than one dimension). The parallel analysis also suggested Nevertheless, the fit was also poor for the models 1F, 2F, and
two factors. For the 10-item version of the PSS, the re- the random factor model (Frandom composed of items 2, 8, 14,
sults showed the same pattern on the number of latent 6, 9, and 10).
dimensions and the complexity of the items. Because of inadequate initial convergence to estimate the
bifactor model, the quasi-Newton L-BFGS-B (Byrd,
Model Fit and Measurement Invariance Lu, Nocedal, & Zhu, 1995) optimization algorithm with
optimal method = “L-BFGS-B” was performed. The param-
PSS-14 For the fit indicators presented in Table 3, the unidi- eters of the items presented in Table 4 show similar results
mensional model (1F) was not satisfactory at all. The two- in both the PSS-10 and PSS-14 in terms of the fit of the
dimension model was only partially satisfactory (due to the bifactor model and the size of the item factor loading.
discrepancy between its CFI, SRMR, and RMSEA). In con- Nevertheless, both items and fit indicators (EVC, ω,
trast, when considering the WRMR, both models showed a ARPB, H, and FD) tended to be slightly superior for the
WRMR >1.0 and cannot be considered satisfactory. At the PSS-10 compared with the PSS-14. In summary, this ver-
same time, when modeling method factors, the CFI was not sion of the PSS also confirms the weakness of an underlying
discriminative enough to identify the most acceptable model, general factor and a higher power of the factor associated
except for the model introducing a factor with random content with the negative wording (Fneg).
(1F + Fran). For this model (Fran consisted of items 1, 2, 3, 14, Regarding the invariance of the PSS-10, the changes in fit
4, 6, 9, and 13), all the fit indicators converged in showing a indicators did not show substantial evidence of lack of invari-
substantial decrease of the fit, and consequently, this model ance. The discrepancy that calls for attention occurred in
was also not considered adequate to the data. Overall, the set ΔRMSEA, but the size effect indicators (dmacs) also pointed
of models for the method effect got the best-fit indices. In out a negligible impact within a range similar to the results
order of relevance, the bifactor model was the best, followed of the PSS-14.

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

Table 2 Descriptive statistics and correlations of the PSS items (n = 1093)

M SD sk ku CMV Correlation: sex Correlation: age Correlation: occupational status (CI 95%)
(CI 95%) (CI 95%)

rg Inf. Sup. rps Inf. Sup. rg Inf. Sup.

pss1* 1.842 .956 .160 −.190 9.870 .000 −.070 .070 −.238 −.295 −.181 .286 .223 .347
pss2* 1.603 1.009 .371 −.289 8.755 .061 −.013 .145 −.310 −.367 −.253 .379 .321 .436
pss3* 2.344 1.028 .018 −.663 8.126 .049 −.024 .123 −.283 −.338 −.228 .355 .299 .411
pss8* 1.838 .932 .134 −.364 9.581 .036 −.037 .111 −.218 −.275 −.161 .265 .197 .332
pss11* 1.994 .955 .218 −.302 9.759 .086 .014 .163 −.290 −.345 −.235 .351 .292 .407
pss12 2.781 .944 −.379 −.553 9.352 .036 −.039 .116 −.101 −.161 −.041 .160 .101 .232
pss14* 1.853 1.027 .143 −.576 7.776 −.071 −.144 .003 −.183 −.242 −.125 .225 .164 .295
pss4 2.510 1.077 −.450 −.448 8.035 .171 .090 .236 −.042 −.102 .018 .176 .112 .249
pss5 2.501 1.049 −.433 −.416 8.433 .224 .156 .292 .022 −.040 .084 .138 .062 .204
pss6* 2.543 1.155 −.531 −.543 7.795 .216 .141 .280 .042 −.020 .104 .113 .038 .179
pss7* 2.468 1.105 −.434 −.458 7.380 .194 .123 .268 .049 −.011 .109 .088 .019 .159
pss9* 2.551 1.037 −.526 −.346 10.018 .253 .182 .314 .037 −.025 .099 .134 .067 .201
pss10* 2.114 .941 −.025 −.356 9.485 .118 .039 .186 .116 .056 .176 −.065 −.125 −.003
pss13 2.309 1.012 −.254 −.519 8.378 .171 .105 .224 .033 −.027 .093 .070 .008 .135

Note. sk: skew. ku: kurtosis. rg: Glass’s biserial correlation. rps: polyserial correlation. Sex: male (female = reference category) occupational status:
worker (student reference category). *Items for PSS-10 version

Discussion and Conclusions previous studies also have demonstrated (Table 1). It is nec-
essary to introduce another source of additional variability to
This study analyzed the psychometric properties of the PSS better understand the psychometric functioning of the PSS.
(10 and 14 items) and focused mainly on unraveling the prob- Here, the additional variability took the form of factors that
lem of its dimensionality. It was based on the internal structure have to be modeled to separate their effect from the global
and measurement invariance examinations as relevant pieces dimension. This effect was mainly observed in the reversed
of evidence for validity and adequate interpretations of the or negatively worded items relative to the construct.
scale. The PSS was originally designed as a tool for measuring Although the negative phrasing and recodification proce-
global perceived stress through the simple metrical sum of its dure have been unconditionally well accepted since the pivot-
items (Cohen et al., 1983; Cohen, & Williamson, 1988). al studies of the PSS, our findings suggest that it generates a
However, our results confirm a fundamental conclusion that method effect. Other authors have correctly identified and

Fig. 2 Dimensionality explored


by EGA approach (n = 1093)

PSS-14
PSS-10

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

Table 3 Fit of the models tested with the PSS – 14 and the PSS – 10 (n = 1093)

WLSMV-χ2 CFI RMSEA WRMR SRMR Ranking best fit


(gl)

PSS – 14
Substantive models
1) 1F 6478.932 .892 .276 6.639 .199 6
(77) (.270, .282)
2) 2F 404.186 .994 .063 1.658 .053 4
(76) (.057, .069)
Models for method effect
3) Bifactor 108.641 .999 .026 .86 .025 1
(63) (.017, .034)
4) 1F + 1 fm neg 209.306 .998 .043 .957 .037 2
(70) (.036, .049)
5) 1F + 1 fm pos 307.677 .996 .056 1.447 .045 3
(70) (.49, .062)
6) 1F + Frandom 3929.828 .935 .226 5.17 .156 5
(39) (.220, .232)
WLSMV-χ2 CFI RMSEA WRMR SRMR Ranking best fit
(gl)
PSS – 10
Substantive models
1F 3879.1000 .843 .317 6.755 .210 6
(.309, 326)
2F 204.191 .993 .068 1.55 .051 4
(34) (.059, .077)
Models for method effect
Bifactor 26.973 1.000 .009 .563 .018 1
(25) (.00, .026)
1F + 1 fm neg 70.989 .998 .034 .914 .028 2
(31) (.024, .045)
1F + 1 fm pos 187.707 .994 .071 1.486 .049 3
(29) (.061, .081)
1F + Frandom 2565.356 .897 .283 5.494 .171 5
(29) (.274, .292)

Note. Models: unidimensional (1F), bifactor (2F), unidimensional with method factor - negative items (1F + 1fm neg), positive item method (1F + 1fm
pos) and random (1F + 1Frandom)

interpreted these same results (Michaelides, Christodoulou, Methodologically, the results of this study highlight the
Kkeli, Karekla, & Panayiotou, 2016; Perera et al., 2017). It fact that one of the factors traditionally interpreted as a sub-
is worth pointing out that in a specific context of measurement stantive dimension of stress in the PSS, confirmed herein as a
where the construct is oriented towards the maladaptive expe- method factor. Furthermore, our findings call into question the
rience (i.e., perceived stress), the items worded in a irrelevance that the authors of the scale have conferred to the
countersense lose coherence with the conceptual and opera- separation of two factors (Cohen, & Janicki-Deverts, 2012;
tional definition of the original construct, which can be Cohen, Kamarak, & Mermelstein, 1983; Cohen, Janicki-
troublesome. Deverts, & Miller, 2007; Cohen, & Williamson, 1988). One
Interestingly, the dimension of the negatively worded items consequence of that is the limited validity of the interpretation
has received names such as “perception of control”, “coping when using a single score that incorporates irrelevant variance
skills”, “resilience”, “perceived self-efficacy”, among others. of the construct. It should be noted that the total variance and
All these constructs are assumed as the automatic theoretical factor loadings of the negatively worded items were higher,
opposites of perceived stress (and therefore re-coded), adopting thus highlighting the positive experience of respondents in-
a view of two poles of a continuum in the stress process. This stead of a perception of maladaptation, as is expected in any
contrasts with the theoretical literature that tends to interpret stress measure.
these factors as completely independent constructs, even Our strategy confirms that the implementation of the
though they are, to some extent, negatively related to stress bifactor model served as a method of separation of two
(Orkaizagirre-Gomara, Sánchez, Ortiz & Ortiz, 2020). sources of variance. One due to the method (i.e., caused by

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

Table 4 Parameters of internal structure for the items of the PSS-14 and PSS-10 (n = 1093)

PSS-14 PSS-10

1F Bifactor 2F 1F + 1 fm neg 1F Bifactor 2F 1F + 1 fm neg

Fg Fg Fpo Fne RPB I-ECV Fpo Fne Fg Fne Fg Fg Fpo Fne RPB I-ECV Fpo Fne Fg Fne

PSS1 .466 .595 .376 .217 .714 .701 .701 .586 .718 .039 .184 .997 .703 .702
PSS2 .527 .749 .415 .297 .765 .838 .837 .661 .901 −.017 .266 1.000 .840 .840
PSS3 .444 .466 .586 .046 .387 .730 .732 .573 .685 .201 .164 .921 .720 .722
PSS8 .413 .47 .511 .122 .458 .692 .691 .535 .592 .482 .096 .601 .690 .690
PSS11 .433 .452 .53 .043 .421 .690 .688 .551 .619 .294 .110 .816 .682 .681
PSS12 .188 .06 .638 2.145 .008 .442 .444 – – – – – – –
PSS14 .428 .489 .474 .124 .515 .683 .683 .545 .603 .380 .096 .716 .682 .682
PSS4r .827 .189 .838 3.374 .048 .850 .137 .845 – – – – – – –
PSS5r .891 .236 .878 2.77 .067 .906 .171 .892 – – – – – – –
PSS6r .904 .292 .876 2.096 .100 .923 .209 .899 .845 .229 .893 2.690 .062 .915 .220 .895
PSS7r .832 .333 .79 1.5 .150 .857 .243 .821 .800 .258 .834 2.101 .087 .872 .249 .836
PSS9r .879 .281 .851 2.13 .098 .896 .199 .874 .835 .221 .866 2.778 .061 .890 .214 .869
PSS10r .565 .442 .441 .277 .501 .579 .331 .495 .531 .346 .480 .535 .342 .595 .335 .484
PSS13r .662 .245 .656 1.7 .122 .700 .162 .682 – – – – – – – –
Fpo – – – – – 1.0 – – – – – – – – 1.0 – –
Fne – – – – – .245 1.0 – – – – – – – – .293 1.0 – –
ECV .286 .504 .871 – – .450 .931 .516 .149 .896 – – .577 .903
ω .897 .925 .873 .937 – – .921 .936 .908 .879 .895 – – .902 .895
ωh – .377 .468 .819 – – .517 .875 .638 .089 .801 – – .676 .807
ARPB – 1.203 – – – – 1.790 .902 – – .974
H .950 .785 .723 .934 – – .885 .944 .893 .379 .905 – – .881 .907
FD .825 .784 .959 – – .939 .973 .938 .664 .957 – – .937 .958

Note. Fg: general factor. Fpo: factor for positive items. Fne: factor for negative items (required for recodification). RPB: relative bias of the parameter. I-
ECV: explained common variance for the item. ECV: explained common variance. ω and ωh: omega reliability ratio. H: replicability ratio. FD: factor
determinacy. Models: 1F (unidimensional), Bifactor, 2F (two dimensions), 1F + Fm neg (unidimensional with method factor –negative items). Only the
most relevant 4 models are shown

the wording of the items), and the other within the unidimen- general, a fit of any model may improve because of the addi-
sional interpretation of the PSS, represented by the general tion of parameters, but by using the procedure suggested by
factor (Fg). Although this approximation is similar to that of Schönberger & Ponsford (2010) in this work, the replicable
previous studies where the bifactor technique has been used in effect of the method factor founded in PSS was indeed con-
the PSS, the interpretation should not necessarily be oriented sistently superior. This effect seems to be more important than
towards the theoretical substantivity of its contents. A unidi- the effect of any possible substantive or relevant factor. Other
mensional measure of these contents can also represent vari- internationally well-known measures have also shown a rep-
ance when there are items worded in both senses (i.e., positive licable effect of the method, such as the Rosenberg Self-
and negative). Literature about the effect of item wording in an Esteem Scale (Lima, & Souza, 2019) or the General Health
opposite sense to the construct has indicated that this problem Questionnaire – 12 (Molina, Rodrigo, Losilla, & Vives,
is rather usual (Conway et al., 2004; Corwyn, 2000; 2014).
Dominguez-Lara, & Merino-Soto, 2018; Horan et al., 2003; There are practical implications in the assessment of the
Lindwall et al., 2012; Molina et al., 2014; Suárez-Alvarez, PSS. All of them are related to the vulnerable validity of its
et al., 2018: Tomás & Oliver, 1999; Tomás et al., 2013; unidimensional approach. For instance, when cut-offs are sug-
Tomás et al., 2012). gested for normative use (Cohen & Williamson, 1988;
Although the negative wording is used to reduce acquies- González Rodríguez & Hernández, 2013), the consideration
cence bias, the detected variance for this method factor was of variance that does not belong to the construct generates
relevant beyond the simple false addition of factors. In biased assessments, especially if the levels of reliability within

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

Table 5 Measurement invariance for PSS-14 and PSS-10 (n = 1093)

Sex group Occupational status group

WLSMV - χ2 CFI ΔCFI RMSEA ΔRMSEA SRMR ΔSRMR WLSMV - χ2 CFI ΔCFI RMSEA ΔRMSEA SRMR ΔSRMR
(gl) (90% IC) (gl) (90% IC)

PSS – 14
Configuration 268.90** .998 – .041 – .049 – 313.73 .998 – .048 – .047 –
(140) (.034, .048) (140) (.041, .055)
Threshold 291.60** .998 0.00 .037 0.00 .040 0.01 369.85 .997 0.00 .047 0.00 .047 0.00
(168) (.030, .044) (198) (.40, .053)
Loadings + threshold 564.40** .994 0.00 .061 −0.02 .052 −0.01 460.73 .996 0.00 .052 0.01 .050 0.00
(187) (.055, .067) (187) (.046, .058)
Intercept 891.26** .989 −0.01 .080 −0.02 .051 0.00 911.54 .990 −0.01 .081 0.03 .051 0.00
(200) (.074, .085) (200) (.075, .086)
PSS – 10
Configuration 95.24** .999 – .031 – .031 – 114.79 .998 – .040 – .037 –
(62) (.018, .043) (62) (.028, .051)
Threshold 116.46** .999 .00 .028 .00 .031 .00 158.52 .997 .00 .041 −.00 .037 .00
(82) (.015, .039) (82) (.032, .051)
Loading + threshold 297.82** .992 .00 .063 −.03 .047 −.01 215.01 .996 .00 .049 −.00 .043 −.00
(94) (.055, .071) (94) (.040, .057)
Intercept 497.06** .984 .00 .084 −.02 .045 .00 472.92 .987 .00 .081 −.03 .044 −.00
(103) (.076, .091) (103) (.074, .089)

**p < .01

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

the heterogeneity of the factors of the PSS scale are not con- Authors’ Contributions AJG and CMS conceived the idea and design the
sidered (Reis et al., 2019). study, performed analyses, and drafted the manuscript. FBO, MNG, and
AMC were in charge of the implementation of the research logistics,
In conclusion, a two-dimension format of both Spanish
contributed and make suggestions to the design and the analysis of the
versions of the PSS instrument (10- and 14-item) does not results, and reviewed the manuscript.
show bias across sexes or occupational status. However, this
Spanish version of the PSS scale fits better to a bifactor model
as has happened with the English (Wu & Amtmann, 2013), Funding No funds, grants, or other support was received for this study.
Serbian (Jovanovic et al., 2015), Greek (Michaelides, 2016),
German (Reis et al., 2019), Korean (Park and Colvin, 2019) Data Availability Database is available in this repository: https://osf.io/
m4uzt/?fbclid=IwAR3L9oF6BsPxcqJ6uP_r5w5xYyvuKpwIr-
versions, and even in other Latin samples (Reyna et al., 2019). _TNiga91-m2sY1a3-NB638ZO0
Unlike what these previous studies suggest about the preferred
use of a total score, a more careful assessment of the uni- or Code Availability N/A
multi-dimensional interpretation seems necessary, which
should be psychometrically justified (Reis et al., 2019). Declarations
Based on the method effects found, we suggest performing
more research to determine the advantages of using only the Ethics approval The project was approved by the ethics committee of
contents of the positively worded items (towards the experi- the Health Sciences School at the Universidad Autónoma de Baja
California, with registration number F-PROMEP-38/Rev-04 and the
ence of stress). This approach is needed to avoid the method ethics committee of the Center for Transdisciplinary Psychology
effect of the negative items and will be closer to the theoretical Research at the Universidad Autónoma del Estado de Morelos
and operational framework of stress, as it happens with other (2019/No.CEI-210219-12).
more recent and promising self-reported stress instruments
(Amirkhan, 2018). Consent to participate Informed consent was obtained from all individ-
ual participants included in the study.
If the PSS is used for research, then we suggest applying an
adjustment for the negative wording (in the opposite sense to Consent for publication Any kind of individual information of partici-
the experience of stress). Nevertheless, the interpretation of pants was included in the manuscript. Participants agreed and were in-
the general score of the PSS seems weakened by the need of formed that the global report of the study would have academic purposes
modeling the method effect at calculating the scores, and thus and would be submitted in a scientific journal.
failing to obtain a valid measure. Although this would be a
Conflict of interests The authors have no relevant financial or non-
reasonable analytical option for the investigator, we can con- financial interests to disclose. On behalf of all authors, the corresponding
fidently say that it would be an important challenge for the author states that there is no conflict of interest.
non-research practitioner, with insufficient training in multi-
variate analysis skills needed in the use of specialized soft-
ware. This challenge will also occur in the context of small References
sample sizes, where the applicability of structural equation
models is not reliable. Almadi, T., Cathers, I., Mansour, A. M. H., & Chow, C. M. (2012). An
One limitation of the study is the absence of a homoge- Arabic version of the perceived stress scale: Translation and valida-
tion study. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 49(1), 84–89.
neous and representative populated-based sample. https://doi.org/10.1037/t25566-000.
Nevertheless, the sample size relative to the magnitude of Amirkhan, J. H. (2018). A brief stress diagnostic tool: The short stress
the expected effects was reasonable. Given the time con- overload scale. Assessment, 25(8), 1001–1013. https://doi.org/10.
straints of participants and organizations, we decided to apply 1177/1073191116673173.
a short questionnaire, so another limitation was the lack of Bono, R., Blanca, M. J., Arnau, J., & Gómez-Benito, J. (2017). Non-
normal distributions commonly used in health, education, and social
external criteria that would allow assessing the convergent sciences: A systematic review. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1602.
and divergent validity of the factors and items of the PSS. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01602.
However, this study was firstly intended to contribute to the Brown, T. (2015). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research
unsolved problems of the scale’s dimensional structure, so we (2nd ed.). Guilford Press.
Byrd, R. H., Lu, P., Nocedal, J., & Zhu, C. (1995). A limited memory
can use objective criteria (e.g biomarkers) in future studies
algorithm for bound constrained optimization. SIAM Journal
with smaller samples. Concerning the items, the non- Scientific Computing, 16(5), 1190–1208. https://doi.org/10.1137/
normality of their distribution was notable, yet not unusual 0916069.
in the investigation of psychosocial constructs (Bono, Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of
Blanca, Arnau, & Gómez-Benito, 2017; Micceri, 1989), espe- measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 14, 464–
504. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510701301834.
cially in categorical variables. This was not a problem that Chiu, Y. H., Lu, F. J. H., Lin, J. H., Nien, C. L., Hsu, Y. W., & Liu, H. Y.
limited our conclusions since the application of the WLSMV (2016). Psychometric properties of the perceived stress scale (PSS):
and the polychoric correlations attenuated its possible effect. Measurement invariance between athletes and non-athletes and

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

construct validity. PeerJ, 4, e2790. https://doi.org/10.7287/peerj. Golden-Kreutz, D. M., Browne, M. W., Frierson, G. M., & Andersen, B.
preprints.2268v1. L. (2004). Assessing stress in cancer patients: A second-order factor
Cohen, S., & Janicki-Deverts, D. (2012). Who ' s stressed? Distributions analysis model for the perceived stress scale. Assessment, 11(3),
of psychological stress in the United States in probability samples 216–223. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191104267398.
from 1983, 2006, and 2009. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, Golino, H. F., & Epskamp, S. (2017). Exploratory graph analysis: A new
42(6), 1320–1334. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2012. approach for estimating the number of dimensions in psychological
00900.x. research. PLoS One, 12(6), e0174035 journal.pone.0174035.
Cohen, S., & Williamson, G. M. (1988). Perceived stress in a probability Golino, H., & Christensen, A. P. (2020). EGAnet: Exploratory graph
sample of the United States. In S. Spacapan & S. Oskamp (Eds.), analysis: A framework for estimating the number of dimensions in
The Claremont Symposium on Applied Social Psychology. The so- multivariate data using network psychometrics. R package version
cial psychology of health (p. 31–67). Sage Publications, Inc.. 0.9.5.
Cohen, S., Janicki-Deverts, D., & Miller, G. E. (2007). Psychological González, M., & Landero, R. (2007). Factor structure of the perceived
stress and disease. Journal of the American Medical Association, stress scale (PSS) in a sample from Mexico. The Spanish Journal of
298(14), 1685–1687. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.298.14.1685. Psychology, 10(1), 199–206.
Cohen, S., Kamarak, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of González, M. T., Rodríguez, M. N., & Hernández, R. L. (2013). The
perceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 24(4), perceived stress scale (PSS): Normative data and factor structure
385–396. for a large-scale sample in Mexico. The Spanish Journal of
Cohen, S., Tyrrell, D. A., & Smith, A. P. (1993). Negative life events, Psychology, 16, E47. https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2013.35.
perceived stress, negative affect, and susceptibility to the common Gorsuch, R. L. (1983). Factor analysis. Lawrence Erlbraum Associates.
cold. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(1), 131– Hancock, G. R., & Mueller, R. O. (2001). Rethinking construct reliability
140. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.64.1.131. within latent variable systems. In R. Cudeck, S. du Toit, & D.
Conway, J. M., Lievens, F., Scullen, S. E., & Lance, C. E. (2004). Bias in Sörbom (Eds.), Structural equation modeling: Present und future—
the correlated uniqueness model for MTMM data. Structural A Festschrift in honor of Karl Joreskog (pp. 195–216).
Equation Modeling, 11, 535–559. https://doi.org/10.1207/ Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International.
s15328007sem1104_3. Horan, P. M., DiStefano, C., & Motl, R. W. (2003). Wording effects in
Corwyn, R. F. (2000). The factor structure of global self-esteem among self-esteem scales: Methodological artifact or response style?
adolescents and adults. Journal of Research in Personality, 34(4), Structural Equation Modeling, 10(3), 435–455. https://doi.org/10.
357–379. https://doi.org/10.1006/jrpe.2000.2291. 1207/S15328007SEM1003_6.
Denovan, A., Dagnall, N., Dhingra, K., & Grogan, S. (2019). Evaluating
Jorgensen, T. D., Pornprasertmanit, S., Schoemann, A. M., & Rosseel, Y.
the perceived stress scale among UK university students:
(2020). semTools: Useful tools for structural equation modeling. R
Implications for stress measurement and management. Studies in
package version 0.5–3. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=
Higher Education, 44(1), 120–133. https://doi.org/10.1080/
semTools
03075079.2017.1340445.
Jovanović, V., & Gavrilov-Jerković, V. (2015). More than a (negative)
Dominguez-Lara, S., & Merino-Soto, C. (2018). Efectos de método en el
feeling: Validity of the perceived stress scale in Serbian clinical and
Inventario de Depresión Estado-Rasgo (IDER): Un análisis SEM.
non-clinical samples. Psihologija, 48(1), 5–18. https://doi.org/10.
[method effects in the trait-state Depresion inventory]. Avances en
2298/psi1501005j.
Psicología Latinoamericana, 36(2), 253–267. https://doi.org/10.
Kaiser, H. F. (1970). A second generation little jiffy. Psychometrika, 35,
12804/revistas.urosario.edu.co/apl/a.4151.
401–415. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02291817.
Dueber, D. (2020). BifactorIndicesCalculator: bifactor indices calculator.
R package version 0.2.0. Recuperado de: https://CRAN.R-project. Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Glaser, R. (1999). Chronic stress and mortality
org/package=BifactorIndicesCalculator among older adults. Journal of the American Medical Association,
Eid, M., Lischetzke, T., Nussbeck, F. W., & Trierweiler, L. I. (2003). 282(23), 2259–2260. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.282.23.2259.
Separating trait effects from trait-specific method effects in King, B. M., Rosopa, P. J., & Minium, E. W. (2011). Statistical reason-
multitrait-multimethod models: A multiple-indicator CT-C(M-1) ing in the behavioral sciences. John Wiley & Sons.
model. Psychological Methods, 8, 38–60. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Lance, C. E., Noble, C. L., & Scullen, S. E. (2002). A critique of the
1082-989X.8.1.38. correlated trait-correlated method and correlated uniqueness models
Eklund, M., Bäckström, M., & Tuvesson, H. (2014). Psychometric prop- for multitrait-multimethod data. Psychological Methods, 7(2), 228–
erties and factor structure of the Swedish version of the perceived 244. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989x.7.2.228.
stress scale. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 68(7), 494–499. https:// Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping.
doi.org/10.3109/08039488.2013.877072. Springer publishing company.
Fan, Y., & Lance, C. E. (2017). A reformulated correlated trait-correlated Lee, B., & Jeong, H. I. (2019). Construct validity of the perceived stress
method model for multitrait-multimethod data effectively increases scale (PSS-10) in a sample of early childhood teacher candidates.
convergence and admissibility rates. Educational and Psychological Psychiatry and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 29, 76–82. https://
Measurement, 77(6), 1048–1063. https://doi.org/10.1177/ doi.org/10.1080/24750573.2019.1565693.
0013164416677144. Lee, E. H. (2012). Review of the psychometric evidence of the perceived
Ferrando, P. J., & Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2017). Program FACTOR at 10: stress scale. Asian Nursing Research, 6(4), 121–127. https://doi.org/
Origins, development and future directions. Psicothema, 29(2), 10.1016/j.anr.2012.08.004.
236–241. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2016.304. Lesage, F. X., Berjot, S., & Deschamps, F. (2012). Psychometric proper-
Fetvadjiev, V., & Van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2015). Measures of personality ties of the French versions of the perceived stress scale.
across cultures. In G. J. Boyle, D. H. Saklofske, & G. Matthews International Journal of Occupational Medicine and
(Eds.), Measures of personality and social psychological constructs Environmental Health, 25(2), 178–184. https://doi.org/10.2478/
(pp. 752-776). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Elsevier. https://doi. S13382-012-0024-8.
org/10.1016/B978-0-12-386915-9.00026-7. Li, C. H. (2016). Confirmatory factor analysis with ordinal data:
Fox, J. (2019). polycor: Polychoric and polyserial correlations. R package Comparing robust maximum likelihood and diagonally weighted
version 0.7-10}. Available in: https://CRAN.R-project.org/ least squares. Behavior Research Methods, 48(3), 936–949. https://
package=polycor. doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0619-7.

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

Lima, T., & Souza, L. (2019). Rosenberg self-esteem scale: Method effect Unpublished manuscript. Recuperado de: https://www.statmodel.
and gender invariance. Psico-USF, 24(3), 517–528. https://doi.org/ com/download/Article_075.pdf
10.1590/1413-82712019240309. Nye, C. D., & Drasgow, F. (2011). Effect size indices for analyses of
Lindwall, M., Barkoukis, V., Grano, C., Lucidi, F., Raudsepp, L., measurement equivalence: Understanding the practical importance
Liukkonen, J., & Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C. (2012). Method effects: of differences between groups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96,
The problem with negatively versus positively keyed items. Journal 966–980. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022955.
of Personality Assessment, 94, 196–204. https://doi.org/10.1080/ Nye, C. D., Bradburn, J., Olenick, J., Bialko, C., & Drasgow, F. (2019).
00223891.2011.64593. How big are my effects? Examining the magnitude of effect sizes in
Lloret-Segura, S., Ferreres-Traver, A., Hernández-Baeza, A., & Tomás- studies of measurement equivalence. Organizational Research
Marco, I. (2014). El análisis factorial exploratorio de los ítems: Una Methods, 22(3), 678–709. https://doi.org/10.1177/
guía práctica, revisada y actualizada [exploratory item factor analy- 1094428118761122.
sis: A practical guide revised and up-dated]. Anales de Psicología/ Orkaizagirre-Gómara, A., Sánchez De Miguel, M., Ortiz de Elguea, J., &
Annals of Psychology, 30(3), 1151–1169. https://doi.org/10.6018/ Ortiz de Elguea, A. (2020). Testing general self-efficacy, perceived
analesps.30.3.199361. competence, resilience, and stress among nursing students: An inte-
Mahalanobis, P. C. (1936). On the generalised distance in statistics. grator evaluation. Nursing & Health Sciences.(22) 3, 529–538.
Sankhya A, 80, 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13171-019-00164-5. https://doi.org/10.1111/nhs.12689.
Mangiafico, S. (2020). rcompanion: Functions to support extension edu- Park, H. S., Dailey, R., & Lemus, D. (2002). The use of exploratory factor
cation program evaluation. R package version 2.3.25. Available in: analysis and principal components analysis in communication re-
https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=rcompanion search. eítems: Una guía práctica, revisada y actualizada. Anales
Markon, K. E. (2019). Bifactor and hierarchical models: Specification, de Psicología, 30(3), 1151–1169. https://doi.org/10.6018/analesps.
inference, and interpretation. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 30.3.199361.
15(1), 51–69. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050718- Park, S. Y., & Colvin, K. F. (2019). Psychometric properties of a Korean
095522. version of the perceived stress scale (PSS) in a military sample.
Marsh, H. W., & Bailey, M. (1991). Confirmatory factor analyses of BMC Psychology, 7(58), 58. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40359-019-
multitrait-multimethod data: A comparison of alternative models. 0334-8.
Applied Psychological Measurement, 15, 47–70. https://doi.org/10. Penny, K. I. (1996). Appropriate critical values when testing for a single
1177/014662169101500106. multivariate outlier by using the Mahalanobis distance. Applied
Marsh, H. W, & Grayson, D. (1995). Latent variable models of Statistics, 45(1), 73–81. https://doi.org/10.2307/2986224.
multitrait–multimethod data. In: R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equa- Perera, M. J., Brintz, C. E., Birnbaum-Weitzman, O., Penedo, F. J., Gallo,
tion modeling: Concept, issues, and applications (pp. 177–198). L. C., Gonzalez, P., Gouskova, N., Isasi, C. R., Navas-Nacher, E. L.,
Sage. Perreira, K. M., Roesch, S. C., Schneiderman, N., & Llabre, M. M.
Marsh, H. W. (1989). Confirmatory factor analysis of multitrait- (2017). Factor structure of the perceived stress Scale-10 (PSS)
multimethod data: Many problems Anda few solutions. Applied across English and Spanish language responders in the HCHS/
Psychological Measurement, 13, 335–361. https://doi.org/10.1177/ SOL sociocultural ancillary study. Psychological Assessment,
014662168901300402. 29(3), 320–328. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000336.
McDonald, R. P. (1999). Test theory: A unified treatment. : Lawrence Quinn, H. O. C. (2014). Bifactor models, explained common variance
Erlbaum Associates, Inc. (ECV), and the usefulness of scores from unidimensional item re-
Meade, A. W., & Craig, S. B. (2012). Identifying careless responses in sponse theory analyses. [doctoral dissertation, the University of
survey data. Psychological Methods, 17(3), 437–455. https://doi. North Carolina at Chapel Hill] https://doi.org/10.17615/t6ff-a088
org/10.1037/a0028085. Reis, D., Lehr, D., Heber, E., & Ebert, D. D. (2019). The German version
Messing, K., Punnett, L., Bond, M., Alexanderson, K., Pyle, J., Zahm, S., of the perceived stress scale (PSS-10): Evaluation of dimensionality,
Wegman, D., Stock, S. R., & de Grosbois, S. (2003). Be the fairest validity, and measurement invariance with exploratory and confir-
of them all: Challenges and recommendations for the treatment of matory bifactor modeling. Assessment, 26(7), 1246–1259. https://
gender in occupational health research. American Journal of doi.org/10.1177/1073191117715731.
Industrial Medicine, 43(6), 618–629. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajim. Reise, S. P., Moore, T. M., & Haviland, M. G. (2010). Bifactor models
10225. and rotations: Exploring the extent to which multidimensional data
Micceri, T. (1989). The unicorn, the normal curve, and other improbable yield univocal scale scores. Journal of Personality Assessment,
creatures. Psychological Bulletin, 105, 156–166. https://doi.org/10. 92(6), 544–559. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2010.496477.
1037/0033-2909.105.1.156. Reise, S. P., Morizot, J., & Hays, R. D. (2007). The role of the bifactor
Michaelides, M. P., Christodoulou, A., Kkeli, N., Karekla, M., & model in resolving dimensionality issues in health outcomes mea-
Panayiotou, G. (2016). Factorial structure of the perceived stress sures. Quality of Life Research, 16(1), 19–31. https://doi.org/10.
scale and implications for scoring. Revue Européenne de 1007/s11136-007-9183-7.
Psychologie Appliquée, 66(6), 309–316. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Remor, E. (2006). Psychometric properties of a European Spanish ver-
erap.2016.07.002. sion of the perceived stress scale (PSS). The Spanish Journal of
Mimura, C., & Griffiths, P. (2004). A Japanese version of the perceived Psychology, 9(1), 86–93.
stress scale: Translation and preliminary test. International Journal Remor, E., & Carrobles, J. A. (2001). Versión Española de la Escala de
of Nursing Studies, 41(4), 379–385. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Estrés Percibido (PSS-14): Estudio psicométrico en una muestra
ijnurstu.2003.10.009. VIH+.[Spanish versión of the perceived stress scale (PSS-14): A
Molina, J. G., Rodrigo, M. F., Losilla, J. M., & Vives, J. (2014). Wording psychometric study in a VIH+ sample] Ansiedad y estrés. 7(2–3),
effects and the factor structure of the 12-item general health ques- 195–201. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2002-15104-002
tionnaire (GHQ-12). Psychological Assessment, 26, 1031–1037. Reyna, C., Mola, D., & Correa, P. (2019). Escala de Estrés Percibido:
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036472. análisis psicométrico desde la TCT y la TRI. [Perceived Stress
Muthén, B.O, du Toit, S., & Spisic, D. (1997). Robust inference using Scale: Psychometric analysis from the TCT and TRI]. Ansiedad y
weighted least squares and quadratic estimating equations in latent Estrés, 25(2), 138–147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anyes.2019.04.
variable modeling with categorical and continuous outcomes. 003.

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Curr Psychol

Rodriguez, A., Reise, S. P., & Haviland, M. G. (2016a). Evaluating Timmerman, M. E., & Lorenzo-Seva, U. (2011). Dimensionality assess-
bifactor models: Calculating and interpreting statistical indices. ment of ordered polytomous items with parallel analysis.
Psychological Methods, 21(2), 137–150. https://doi.org/10.1037/ Psychological Methods, 16, 209–220. https://doi.org/10.1037/
met0000045. a0023353.
Rodriguez, A., Reise, S. P., & Haviland, M. G. (2016b). Applying Tomás, J., & Oliver, A. (1999). Rosenberg’s self-esteem scale: Two
bifactor statistical indices in the evaluation of psychological mea- factor or method effects. Structural Equation Modeling: A
sures. Journal of Personality Assessment, 98(3), 223–237. https:// Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 84–98. https://doi.org/10.1080/
doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2015.1089249. 10705519909540120.
Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: An R package for structural equation model- Tomás, J., Galiana, L., Hontangas, P., Oliver, A., & Sancho, P. (2013).
ing. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 1–36. https://doi.org/10. Evidencia acumulada sobre los efectos de método asociados a ítems
18637/jss.v048.i02. invertidos. [Acumulative evidence about the method effects by
Safaei, M., & Shokri, O. (2014). Assessing stress in cancer patients: inverted ítems]. Psicológica, 34, 365–381 https://www.redalyc.
Factorial validity of the perceived stress scale in Iran. Iranian org/pdf/169/16929535013.pdf.
Journal of Psychiatric Nursing, 2(1), 13–22 https://www.sid.ir/en/ Tomás, J., Sancho, P., Oliver, A., Galiana, L., & Meléndez, J. (2012).
journal/ViewPaper.aspx?id=372609. Efectos de método asociados a ítems invertidos vs. ítems en
Sapolsky, R. M. (2004). Why zebras don ' t get ulcers: The acclaimed negativo.[ method effects asocciated to inverted ítems vs negative
guide to stress, stress-related diseases, and coping-now revised and ítems]. Revista Mexicana de Psicología, 29(2), 105–115 https://
updated. Henry Holt paperbacks adn Company. www.redalyc.org/pdf/2430/243030190001.pdf.
Sass, D. A. (2011). Testing measurement invariance and comparing latent Trizano-Hermosilla, I., & Alvarado, J. M. (2016). Best alternatives reli-
factor means within a confirmatory factor analysis framework. ability in realistic conditions: Congeneric and asymmetrical mea-
Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 29, 347–363. https:// surements. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 769. https://doi.org/10.
doi.org/10.1177/0734282911406661. 3389/fpsyg.2016.00769.
Schönberger, M., & Ponsford, J. (2010). The factor structure of the hos- Wiriyakijja, P, Porter, S, Fedele, S, Hodgson, T., McMillan, R.,
pital anxiety and depression scale in individuals with traumatic brain Shephard, R., & Riordain, R. N. (2019). Validation of the HADS
injury. Psychiatry Research, 179(3), 342–349 https://doi.org/10. and PSS-10 and psychological status in patients with oral lichen
1016/j.psychres.2009.07.003. planus. Oral Diseases, 1, 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1111/odi.13220.
Selye, H. (1976). The stress concept. Canadian Medical Association
Wu, H., & Estabrook, R. (2016). Identification of confirmatory factor
Journal, 115(8), 718.
analysis models of different levels of invariance for ordered categor-
Siqueira Reis, R., Ferreira Hino, A. A., & Romélio Rodriguez Añez, C.
ical outcomes. Psychometrika, 81, 1014–1045. https://doi.org/10.
(2010). Perceived stress scale: Reliability and validity study in
1007/s11336-016-9506-0.
Brazil. Journal of Health Psychology, 15(1), 107–114. https://doi.
Wu, S. M., & Amtmann, D. (2013). Psychometric evaluation of the
org/10.1177/1359105309346343.
perceived stress scale in multiple sclerosis. International Scholarly
Smith, K. J., Rosenberg, D. L., & Haight, T. (2014). An assessment of the
Research Notices., 2013, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/608356.
psychometric properties of the perceived stress Scale-10 (PSS 10)
with business and accounting students. Accounting Perspectives, Yentes, R. D., & Wilhelm, F. (2018). careless: Procedures for computing
13(1), 29–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adiac.2014.09.005. indices of careless responding. R package version 1.1.3.
Stucky, B. D., Edelen, M. O., Vaughan, C. A., Tucker, J. S., & Butler, J. Yu, C. (2002). Evaluating cutoff criteria of model fit indices for latent
(2014). The psychometric development and initial validation of the variable models with binary and continuous outcomes (unpublished
DCI-A short form for adolescent therapeutic community treatment doctoral dissertation). University of California, Los Angeles, CA.
process. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 46(4), 516–521. Available in: http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download;
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsat.2013.12.005. jsessionid=890BA372486968AC957CF29C765395BA?doi=10.1.
Suárez-Alvarez, J., Pedrosa, I., Lozano, L. M., García-Cueto, E., Cuesta, 1.310.3956&rep=rep1&type=pdf
M., & Muñiz, J. (2018). Using reversed items in Likert scales: A
questionable practice. Psicothema, 30(2), 149–158. https://doi.org/ Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdic-
10.7334/psicothema2018.33. tional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Content courtesy of Springer Nature, terms of use apply. Rights reserved.


Terms and Conditions
Springer Nature journal content, brought to you courtesy of Springer Nature Customer Service Center GmbH (“Springer Nature”).
Springer Nature supports a reasonable amount of sharing of research papers by authors, subscribers and authorised users (“Users”), for small-
scale personal, non-commercial use provided that all copyright, trade and service marks and other proprietary notices are maintained. By
accessing, sharing, receiving or otherwise using the Springer Nature journal content you agree to these terms of use (“Terms”). For these
purposes, Springer Nature considers academic use (by researchers and students) to be non-commercial.
These Terms are supplementary and will apply in addition to any applicable website terms and conditions, a relevant site licence or a personal
subscription. These Terms will prevail over any conflict or ambiguity with regards to the relevant terms, a site licence or a personal subscription
(to the extent of the conflict or ambiguity only). For Creative Commons-licensed articles, the terms of the Creative Commons license used will
apply.
We collect and use personal data to provide access to the Springer Nature journal content. We may also use these personal data internally within
ResearchGate and Springer Nature and as agreed share it, in an anonymised way, for purposes of tracking, analysis and reporting. We will not
otherwise disclose your personal data outside the ResearchGate or the Springer Nature group of companies unless we have your permission as
detailed in the Privacy Policy.
While Users may use the Springer Nature journal content for small scale, personal non-commercial use, it is important to note that Users may
not:

1. use such content for the purpose of providing other users with access on a regular or large scale basis or as a means to circumvent access
control;
2. use such content where to do so would be considered a criminal or statutory offence in any jurisdiction, or gives rise to civil liability, or is
otherwise unlawful;
3. falsely or misleadingly imply or suggest endorsement, approval , sponsorship, or association unless explicitly agreed to by Springer Nature in
writing;
4. use bots or other automated methods to access the content or redirect messages
5. override any security feature or exclusionary protocol; or
6. share the content in order to create substitute for Springer Nature products or services or a systematic database of Springer Nature journal
content.
In line with the restriction against commercial use, Springer Nature does not permit the creation of a product or service that creates revenue,
royalties, rent or income from our content or its inclusion as part of a paid for service or for other commercial gain. Springer Nature journal
content cannot be used for inter-library loans and librarians may not upload Springer Nature journal content on a large scale into their, or any
other, institutional repository.
These terms of use are reviewed regularly and may be amended at any time. Springer Nature is not obligated to publish any information or
content on this website and may remove it or features or functionality at our sole discretion, at any time with or without notice. Springer Nature
may revoke this licence to you at any time and remove access to any copies of the Springer Nature journal content which have been saved.
To the fullest extent permitted by law, Springer Nature makes no warranties, representations or guarantees to Users, either express or implied
with respect to the Springer nature journal content and all parties disclaim and waive any implied warranties or warranties imposed by law,
including merchantability or fitness for any particular purpose.
Please note that these rights do not automatically extend to content, data or other material published by Springer Nature that may be licensed
from third parties.
If you would like to use or distribute our Springer Nature journal content to a wider audience or on a regular basis or in any other manner not
expressly permitted by these Terms, please contact Springer Nature at

onlineservice@springernature.com

View publication stats

You might also like