You are on page 1of 5

Profile of Mood States 4057

studies of German workers, Mexican peasants, Definition


and other groups through extensive “interpretative
questionnaires.” By understanding the productive Mood may be defined as a short-term feeling state
orientation in terms of “character,” Fromm was that may fluctuate within minutes to days. In con-
able to unite the philosophical traditions on trast to emotions, moods are more transient, often
human attempts to encounter the world through unrelated to external events and have varying
knowledge and love, with an attempt to under- intensities (Berger and Motl 2000). Moods influ-
stand the healthy character structure, both in rela- ence our overall sense of well-being, and impact
tion to societal forces that might encourage or both behavior patterns and perceived health
hamper it and in terms of the individual’s forma- (Berger and Motl 1998). The Profile of Mood
tion of character within that wider social context. States (POMS) is a 65 item self-report psycholog-
ical instrument intended for use with adults age
18 and above. The POMS assesses short-term
References mood states which are understood to be transient
and frequently fluctuating. While the original
Friedman, L., & Schreiber, A. M. (2013). The lives of Erich
Fromm: Love’s prophet. New York: Columbia Univer-
POMS (McNair et al. 1981) in both long and
sity Press. short forms is still widely used, the scale was
Fromm, E. (1956). The art of loving. New York: Harper & revised in 2012 (Heuchert and McNair 2012).
Row. The revision of the scale (POMS 2-A), in addition
Fromm, E. (1968). The revolution of hope: Towards a
humanized technology. New York: Harper & Row.
to the 65 item adult version, featured the develop-
Fromm, E. (1972). Escape from freedom. New York: Avon ment of a 60 item adolescent version (POMS 2-Y)
Books. intended for youth 13–17 years of age as well as
Fromm, E. (1990). Man for himself: An inquiry into the corresponding short forms for adults and adoles-
psychology of ethics. New York: Henry Holt and
Company.
cents. The scale is available in both pencil and
Maccoby, M. (1994). The two voices of Erich Fromm: The paper and computerized administration formats.
prophetic and the analytic. Retrieved from http://www. The full-length version requires approximately
maccoby.com/Articles/TwoVoices.shtml. 10–15 min to complete with 5–10 min reportedly
Marcuse, H. (1966). Eros and civilization: A philosophical
inquiry into Freud. Boston: Beacon Press.
required for the short versions.

P
Introduction

Productivity The Profile of Mood States (POMS; McNair et al.


1981) is a multidimensional Likert self-report
▶ Generativity Versus Stagnation scale originally developed for assessing psychiat-
ric patients’ response to pharmacological and psy-
chotherapeutic treatment. The POMS, however,
rapidly became applied to sports and exercise
Profile of Mood States psychology as well as for assessing coping
among persons with chronic medical illness.
H. Russell Searight and Kaitlyn Montone While there are multiple versions of the instru-
Department of Psychology, Lake Superior State ment, the most commonly used form consists of
University, Sault Ste. Marie, MI, USA 65 adjectives (“tense,” “lively,” “worn out”).
Respondents rate each item on a “0” (not at all)
to “4” (extremely) scale reflecting the respon-
Acronym dent’s mood over a specific period of time. Com-
monly used time frames include Today, Right
POMS Now, and This Week. Other less commonly
4058 Profile of Mood States

designated time frames have included The Past Additionally, the standard version is not optimally
Three Minutes, Since Taking Medication, and The efficient for pre-post -test studies of mood
Past Month (McNair and Heuchert 2013). changes associated with exercise or athletic
The POMS’ 65 items represent six subscales competition.
assessing tension-anxiety, depression, anger-
hostility, fatigue, confusion-bewilderment, and
vigor-activity. The POMS-2 also includes an addi- Psychometric Properties
tional “friendliness” dimension which is reported
separately from the other six subscales and reflects Because the POMS assesses short-term emo-
mood states emerging in an interpersonal context tional states, test-retest reliability would be
(Heuchert and McNair 2012). In addition to expected to be modest at best. The POMS man-
subscale scores, a composite score, Total Mood ual reports test-retest data from one hundred psy-
Disturbance, is obtained by adding five of the chiatric outpatients. Stability coefficients from
negative affect subscales and subtracting the initial intake to “pretreatment” (median
vigor score, reflecting total mood disturbance. time = 20 days) range from .65 for vigor to .74
The TMD is interpreted as a global index of for depression-dejection. Six weeks after initiat-
distress. ing treatment, the sample demonstrated even
greater reductions in test-retest reliability rang-
ing from .43 for vigor to .53 for anger-hostility
POMS: Alternate Forms (McNair and Heuchert 2013).
Internal consistency reliability figures are sig-
The original POMS has three versions: the stan- nificantly higher with coefficient alphas ranging
dard 65 item scale, a short form, and a bipolar from .84 for confusion-bewilderment to .95 for
version. While there are several short forms in depression-dejection. (McNair and Heuchert
print, including an 11-item version without sub- 2013).
scales, a commonly used form features 30 items Multiple correlational studies provide support
retaining the six subscales of the longer version. for the POMS’ construct validity. Moderate to
The recent revision of the scale (POMS-2) high correlations have been found between
includes 35 item versions for both adults and POMS subscales and the total score on the Beck
adolescents. Depression Inventory with Vigor inversely related
The bipolar POMS also retains the six sub- to BDI scores (Nyenhuis et al. 1999). Correlations
scales but features 72 items representing a mood with the State- Trait Anxiety Scale also fall in the
state and its opposite: composed-anxious; moderately high range with POMS tension and
agreeable-hostile elated – depressed; confident- State Anxiety at .72 and Trait Anxiety at .70
unsure; energetic-tired; clearheaded-confused. (Nyenhuis, et al. 1999).
Ratings occur on a 0 (“much unlike this”) to Because the scale assesses dimensions that are
(“much like this”). The bipolar version is meant expected to fluctuate, many POMS studies mea-
to be used primarily in clinical settings (Lorr and sure mood associated with activities such as exer-
McNair 1980) and has recently been applied to cise or health-related indices. Among a sample of
assess response to interventions such as art ther- young adults, a 20-min walk (Perkins et al. 2011)
apy among acutely ill psychiatric inpatients (Chiu was associated with significant reductions in
et al. 2015). The unipolar POMS is the version POMS tension, depression, fatigue, anxiety, and
most commonly used – particularly in sports and confusion.
exercise psychology. Sleep quality, measured with the Pittsburgh
When the POMS was applied to health care Sleep Quality Index, indicated significant associ-
settings, the standard 65 item version seriously ations with POMS mood dimensions. Compared
taxed medically ill patients’ energy levels. with college students classified as “good sleepers”
Profile of Mood States 4059

“, poor sleepers” exhibited significantly greater POMS Norms


elevations on POMS depression, tension, anger,
confusion, and fatigue (Lund et al. 2010). Norms have been developed for several specific
As the POMS literature became well population and reveal age-related differences in
established, investigators used the scale to under- POMS mood states. Relative to a middle-aged
stand underlying mechanisms associated with adult sample (mean age = 44.00 years), a geriatric
mood changes. Among a group of pregnant sample (mean age = 68.1 years) exhibited signif-
women at 38 to 40 weeks gestation, POMS icantly lower scores on tension- depression,
depression scores were positively associated fatigue, and total mood disturbance (Nyenhuis
with testosterone levels immediately before and et al. 1999). However, further analyses indicated
several days after delivery (Hohlagschwandtner that this difference was attributable solely to dif-
et al. 2001). ferences between older and younger males with
Factor analytic studies provide support for the no age-related disparities among the women.
POMS as a multidimensional instrument Relative to adults 25 years and older, college-
Heuchert and McNair (2012) report that of six aged individuals (ages 18–24 years), exhibited
factor analytic studies, three analyses confirmed significantly higher scores on all the scales except
the six POMS dimensions. Confusion – bewilder- vigor on which a significantly lower mean score
ment, however, did not consistently emerge as a was obtained (Nyenhuis et al. 1999).
distinct factor corresponding to the subscale Because of its wide use with athletes, there was
(Heuchert and McNair 2012). In two independent concern that the published POMS norms based on
samples, consisting of psychiatric outpatients and college students (McNair et al. 1971) were not
a group of adults who were regular smokers, con- useful in discriminating mood states among ath-
firmatory factor analyses found three factors letes. A large sample of international and recrea-
corresponding to the POMS subscales – anger- tional athletes scored significantly higher on all
hostility, vigor-activity, and fatigue-inertia. How- the POMs dimensions suggesting that norms
ever, the remaining three subscales – tension- should be revised when applying the scale to
anxiety, depression-dejection, and confusion- athletes (Terry and Lane 2000).
bewilderment – loaded on a single dimension
(Norcross et al. 1984). The authors attributed
this pattern to high interscale correlations, the The Iceberg Profile and Competitive P
distinctive characteristics of the samples, and Athletes
social desirability. In a Japanese sample, five fac-
tors emerged with confusion and depression- The use of the POMS to discriminate between
dejection loading onto a single factor elite and less successful athletes was a popular
(Yokoyama et al. 1990). research topic in the late 1970s and early 1980s.
Short POMS versions (30 or 37 items), fea- Morgan (1985) described the POMS “iceberg pro-
turing six subscales developed through factor file” which was touted as a key discriminator
analyses, have exhibited equal or superior levels between successful and less successful athletes.
of internal consistency reliability. The brief Characterized by high scores on vigor and low
POMS exhibits a more readily interpretable fac- scores on the negative mood subscales, this
tor structure resulting from the elimination of POMS configuration was presented as a key dis-
items which do not load on a specific factor as criminator between athletes who qualified for elite
well as items which appear redundant teams such as national Olympic teams compared
(Bourgeois et al. 2010). Five of the six POMS with others who try out but are not selected. Suc-
subscales also emerged as distinct dimensions in cessful athletes were those who were selected for
a confirmatory factor analysis (Bourgeois the US Olympic team or who had won multiple
et al. 2010). varsity letters. Data were presented for elite
4060 Profile of Mood States

runners, wrestlers, and rowers who had been activities reported to be enjoyable and in which
selected for Olympic competition or who had there is minimal interpersonal competition
earned multiple collegiate athletic letters, with (Berger and Motl 2000). The association between
some evidence that successful athletes in these exercise intensity and mood is not straightfor-
sports were more likely to exhibit this distinct ward. While high intensity activity appears to be
POMS configuration. Morgan also noted that optimal from a cardiovascular perspective,
these successful athletes scored lower than the improved mood is only achieved with moderate
population norms for negative affect (Morgan exercise intensity. For example, experienced
and Johnson 1978; Morgan and Pollock 1977; cyclists exercising at 69% of maximum heart
Terry 1995). However, the ability of various rate demonstrated significant improvement on
POMS subscales to actually predict successful the POMS dimensions of anger, vigor, fatigue,
performances was between 70% and 80%. While and confusion while higher intensity was associ-
intriguing, subsequent studies failed to find that ated with no changes in mood and maximal inten-
the POMS accurately discriminated between dif- sity (work out to the point of exhaustion) was
fering levels of athletic expertise (Terry 1995). linked to increased negative mood (Motl
The iceberg profile’s generalizability is further et al. 1996).
challenged by the finding that mood states associ- A practical question is the length of time neces-
ated with success vary by sport. For example, sary to produce positive POMS changes. The asso-
successful karate and cross-country running per- ciation appears to follow an inverted U pattern.
formance has been associated with elevated levels While short/brief activity of 5 to 10 minutes has
of POMS anger (Terry 1995). Additionally, within shown some association with positive mood
seemingly homogeneous groups of athletes, there change, the effect size appears to be relatively
is a good deal of variability. Terry (1995) found small while “overload” training has been associ-
that approximately 25% of successful athletic per- ated with either the absence of mood improvement
formers did not exhibit the iceberg profile and that or deterioration in mood. In terms of improving
over half of unsuccessful performers did exhibit mood, optimal exercise duration appears to be of
POMS icebergs. about 20–30 min (Berger and Motl 1998).
Finally, competition appears to diminish the
emotional benefits since mood is highly depen-
Exercise and Mood dent upon competition outcome. While winners
exhibited improved mood on all six of the POMS
The POMS has been widely used in research exam- subscales, members of losing teams exhibited
ining the effect of exercise on mood. The scale’s declines in vigor and increases in anger. These
widespread acceptance has led to multiple studies negative effects on mood continued to be present
attempting to determine the types and parameters two hours after the competition ended (Berger and
of exercise associated with improved mood. Activ- Motl 1998; Hassman and Blomstrand 1995).
ities involving regular abdominal breathing such as
yoga, meditation, and Tai chi have shown similar
patterns of mood benefits as established aerobic Use of the POMS in Medical Settings
activities such as walking or swimming (Berger
and Owen 1992; Berger and Motl 2000; Jin Because of its sensitivity to emotional changes,
1992). Routinized activates such as yoga, jogging, the POMS has been used in multiple studies of
and swimming appear to have more consistent patients’ responses to both conventional and alter-
mood benefits than sports with less predictability native medical intervention for serious and /or
requiring outward attention such as fencing (Berger chronic illness. Most POMS studies investigate
and Owen 1988) or possibly, basketball. coping with a cancer diagnosis as well as the
While perhaps predictable, desirable POMS impact of various types of psychosocial interven-
mood changes are associated with exercise tions among patients with cancer histories.
Profile of Mood States 4061

Coping with cancer often requires managing assesses positive mood states. In exercise studies,
chronic pain. Compared with pain-free patients, reductions in the aversive mood scales such as
patients reporting higher levels of pain also depression or anger have been equated with
exhibited higher scores on POMS anxiety, anger, improved emotional status. In interpretation of
confusion, and total mood disturbance. Among changes in POMS scores, it is generally assumed
these patients with cancer, pain duration in hours that reductions in negative mood are the equiva-
and number of days of reported pain were moder- lent of increased positive mood. This assumption
ately correlated with POMS depression, fatigue, should be viewed with some tentativeness (Berger
confusion, and total mood disturbance (Glover and Motl 1988).
et al. 1995). The POMS has been useful in elucidating some
In a trial of mindfulness-based meditation for of the mechanisms underlying the positive asso-
cancer patients, a six-month follow-up found sig- ciation between certain types of exercise and
nificant reductions in POMS scales assessing neg- improved mood. It is also sensitive to more subtle,
ative mood (Carlson et al. 2001; Matchim and nonclinical mood changes associated with chronic
Armer 2007). Another complementary treatment, illness and successful coping as well as responses
music therapy, conducted over 10 weeks with to complementary and alternative therapies. The
oncology patients, was also associated with sig- POMS, despite its inability to discriminate elite
nificant improvements in POMS scores (Waldon from nonelite athletes, is well established in exer-
2001). Similarly, women who were breast cancer cise and sport psychology.
survivors demonstrated pre-post- test reductions
in POMS tension associated with a seven-week
yoga program (Culos-Reed et al. 2006). Cross-References
Traditional medical interventions have also
been associated with improved mood on the ▶ Beck Depression Inventory
POMS. Among patients receiving gabapentin for ▶ Construct Validity
diabetic neuropathy, as compared with the pla- ▶ Manic-Depressive Disorders
cebo group, those receiving the drug demon- ▶ State-Trait Anxiety Inventory
strated significant reductions on three of the ▶ Test-Retest Reliability
POMS subscales as well as on total mood distur-
bance (Backonja et al. 1998). P
Among patients who were HIV-positive, the References
POMS depression-dejection scale was found to
accurately classify HIV-positive patients with Backonja, M., Beydoun, A., Edwards, K. R., Schwartz, S. L.,
Fonseca, V., Hes, M., ... & Gabapentin Diabetic Neu-
and without major depressive disorder. The
ropathy Study Group. (1998). Gabapentin for the
POMS’ overall detection rate was 80% with a symptomatic treatment of painful neuropathy in
sensitivity of 55% and a specificity of 84%. Of patients with diabetes mellitus: a randomized con-
interest, the POMS depression scale’s classifica- trolled trial. Journal of the American Medical Associa-
tion, 280(21), 1831–1836.
tion accuracy was the same as the clinically
Berger, B. G., & Motl, R. W. (2000). Exercise and mood:
established Beck Depression Inventory A selective review and synthesis of research employing
(Patterson et al. 2006). the profile of mood states. Journal of Applied Sport
Psychology, 12(1), 69–92.
Berger, B. G., & Owen, D. R. (1988). Stress reduction and
mood enhancement in four exercise modes: Swim-
Conclusion ming, body conditioning, hatha yoga, and fencing.
Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 59(2),
The POMS, originally developed for use in psy- 148–159.
Berger, B. G., & Owen, D. R. (1992). Mood alteration with
chiatric settings, has limitations when applied to
yoga and swimming: Aerobic exercise may not be
the nonclinical populations in which it is widely necessary. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75(3 suppl),
used. Of the six subscales, only one, vigor, 1331–1343.

You might also like