You are on page 1of 21

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/257541825

Analyzing the Role of Social Norms in Tax Compliance Behavior

Article in Journal of Business Ethics · July 2012


DOI: 10.1007/s10551-012-1390-7

CITATIONS READS

159 2,534

3 authors:

Donna D. Bobek Amy M. Hageman


University of South Carolina Kansas State University
35 PUBLICATIONS 985 CITATIONS 35 PUBLICATIONS 1,050 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Charles F. Kelliher
University of Central Florida
19 PUBLICATIONS 222 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Amy M. Hageman on 31 January 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Analyzing the Role of Social Norms in Tax
Compliance Behavior

Donna D. Bobek, Amy M. Hageman &


Charles F. Kelliher

Journal of Business Ethics

ISSN 0167-4544
Volume 115
Number 3

J Bus Ethics (2013) 115:451-468


DOI 10.1007/s10551-012-1390-7

1 23
Your article is protected by copyright and
all rights are held exclusively by Springer
Science+Business Media B.V.. This e-offprint
is for personal use only and shall not be self-
archived in electronic repositories. If you wish
to self-archive your article, please use the
accepted manuscript version for posting on
your own website. You may further deposit
the accepted manuscript version in any
repository, provided it is only made publicly
available 12 months after official publication
or later and provided acknowledgement is
given to the original source of publication
and a link is inserted to the published article
on Springer's website. The link must be
accompanied by the following text: "The final
publication is available at link.springer.com”.

1 23
Author's personal copy
J Bus Ethics (2013) 115:451–468
DOI 10.1007/s10551-012-1390-7

Analyzing the Role of Social Norms in Tax Compliance Behavior


Donna D. Bobek • Amy M. Hageman •

Charles F. Kelliher

Received: 1 August 2011 / Accepted: 30 May 2012 / Published online: 2 August 2012
 Springer Science+Business Media B.V. 2012

Abstract The purpose of this study is to explore with results of this study further clarify the important role that
more rigor and detail the role of social norms in tax social norms have with regard to taxpayers’ compliance
compliance. This study draws on Cialdini and Trost’s behavior.
(The Handbook of Social Psychology: Oxford University
Press, Boston, MA, 1998) taxonomy of social norms to Keywords Tax compliance  Taxpayers  Social norms
investigate with more specificity this potentially decisive
(Alm and McKee, Managerial and Decision Economics,
19:259–275, 1998) influence on tax compliance. We test Introduction
our research hypotheses regarding the direct and indirect
influences of social norms using a hypothetical compliance In the United States, the Internal Revenue Service (the gov-
scenario with 174 experienced taxpayers as participants. ernmental agency responsible for tax collection, IRS) estimates
Factor analysis of the social norm questions successfully that it loses over $450 billion annually due to taxpayer non-
identified four distinct social norm constructs, in line with compliance (www.irs.gov). The majority of this deficiency
Cialdini and Trost (1998). Results of the path analysis is due to under-reporting of the individual income tax. While
show that individuals’ standards for behavior/ethical the IRS expends significant resources on enforcement, it
beliefs (personal norms) as well as the expectations of acknowledges that increasing voluntary compliance is the key
close others (subjective norms) directly influence tax to reducing the tax gap (US Department of the Treasury 2009).
compliance decisions, whereas general societal expecta- Nevertheless, traditional economic models of compliance,
tions (injunctive norms) and other individuals’ actual which primarily incorporate detection and enforcement vari-
behavior (descriptive norms) have an indirect influence. ables (for a review see Andreoni et al. 1998), suggest that
This shows that social norms have important direct as well compliance is much higher than would be expected based on
as indirect influences on tax compliance behavior. We also the current audit and penalty rates. Over a decade ago, Andreoni
investigate a number of attitudinal variables that may et al. (1998) called for more research exploring the ‘‘diverse
be related to social norms and taxpayer compliance. The psychological, moral and social influences’’ (p. 885) on com-
pliance behavior, to help explain and understand the current
higher-than-expected level of compliance. While researchers
D. D. Bobek (&)  C. F. Kelliher
have begun to answer this call (e.g., Alm et al. 1999; Bobek and
Dixon School of Accounting, University of Central Florida,
P.O. Box 161400, Orlando, FL 32816-1400, USA Hatfield 2003; Wenzel 2004, 2005; Bobek et al. 2007; Torgler
e-mail: Donna.Bobek@bus.ucf.edu 2007; Blanthorne and Kaplan 2008), the need for additional
C. F. Kelliher research remains critical (Weisbach and Plesko 2007).
e-mail: Charles.Kelliher@bus.ucf.edu Accounting researchers studying non-economic influ-
ences on tax compliance have traditionally focused on
A. M. Hageman
individual psychological variables such as moral beliefs
Department of Accounting, Kansas State University,
109 Calvin Hall, Manhattan, KS 66506-0506, USA and attitudes (e.g., Reckers et al. 1994; Hanno and Violette
e-mail: HagemanA@ksu.edu 1996; Kaplan et al. 1997; Bobek and Hatfield 2003).

123
Author's personal copy
452 D. D. Bobek et al.

Meanwhile, some economists have tried to control for the taxpayer compliance, maintenance and enhancement of
influence of social norms in models of tax compliance these norms is essential to a stable tax revenue source. In
behavior (e.g., Alm et al. 1999; Pommerehne et al. 1994; this study, we extend prior research on social norms and tax
Scholz and Pinney 1995). However, these economic studies compliance by developing a comprehensive model of the
do not specify precisely what these social norms are; direct and indirect influences of social norms, and testing
instead, they model a variable that affects compliance in a this model using experienced taxpayers. We investigated
manner consistent with a strong effect from a nebulous the effect of social norms using a hypothetical compliance
outside social influence. Several recent studies have begun scenario with 174 experienced taxpayers as participants. As
to examine how social norms influence tax compliance detailed in Bobek et al. (2011),2 factor analysis of the
behavior (e.g., Wenzel 2004, 2005; Bobek et al. 2007; social norm questions successfully identified four distinct
Blanthorne and Kaplan 2008), but the need for additional social norm constructs, in line with Cialdini and Trost
clarity remains. Further, Torgler (2002) argues, ‘‘when (1998). All four social norm constructs were significantly
working with social norms, we have the difficulty of correlated with tax compliance decisions. Our results pro-
specifying their exact meaning’’ (p. 663). vide support for our predictions that the influence of social
In this study, we synthesize these prior findings by norms is complex and the constructs themselves are
drawing on social norms theory to clarify both the defini- interrelated. Specifically, our final path analysis results
tion and the influence of social norms on tax compliance show that only individuals’ standards for one’s own
behavior. Cialdini and Trost (1998, p. 152) characterize behavior (personal norms) and the perceptions of the
social norms as ‘‘rules and standards that are understood by expectations of close others (subjective norms) directly
members of a group, and that guide and/or constrain social influenced tax compliance decisions. Perceptions of gen-
behavior without the force of laws’’. They identify four eral societal expectations (injunctive norms) and others’
categories of influences encompassed in this definition: actual behavior (descriptive norms) had only an indirect
general societal expectations of behavior (injunctive influence on tax compliance behavior. Further, our results
norms), expectations of valued others for one’s own show that social norms are more influential than any of the
behavior (subjective norms), one’s own expectations for additional control variables (e.g., detection risk, fairness
proper behavior/ethical beliefs (personal norms), and perceptions) included in the path analysis.
standards that develop out of observation of others’ This article makes several contributions to the taxation
behavior (descriptive norms). Therefore, they include not and ethics literature. First, we provide a comprehensive
only outside social influences, but also an individual’s review of prior research that has studied the influence of
personal moral (or ethical) beliefs in their definition of social norms on tax compliance. Based on this review and
social norms.1 Cialdini and Trost’s (1998) social norms theory, we clarify
Alm et al. (1999) concluded, in an experimental eco- the role of social norms by developing a comprehensive
nomics setting, that social norms play a crucial role in model of their influence on tax compliance behavior.
taxpayers’ compliance decisions, observing that when the Second, we also consider how social norms influence each
social norm tended toward non-compliance, compliance other, thus modeling the indirect role that specific types of
dropped to almost zero. This finding suggests that, if social social norms have on tax compliance behavior. While
norms are a dominant influence on the current level of personal norms appear to be a primary influence on com-
pliance intentions, we also demonstrate that each of the
other types of social norms are interrelated, and that each
1
Other researchers have described social norms in a variety of of the social norm constructs provides opportunities to
similar ways. For example, Hechter and Opp (2001) refer to two types influence tax compliance. Third, we also identify certain
of definitions of social norms; oughtness norms or norms that entail attitudinal variables that are related to social norms; finding
moral imperatives and behavioral regularities which are less
that personal norms are strongly related to financial pres-
restrictive and are based on what is observed. From this perspective,
injunctive, subjective, and personal norms would all be viewed as sure is of concern given the current uncertain economic
oughtness norms. Descriptive norms, on the other hand, are what is environment. Fourth, our results may be helpful to poli-
observed: the behavioral regularity. Furthermore, Plant and Devine cymakers and tax researchers who are interested in
(1998) refer to internal and external motivations to engage in
improving compliance. In particular, with this compre-
behavior. They refer to internal motivations as personal standards of
behavior or how one personally believes they should respond—a hensive model of social norms and attitudinal variables,
concept very similar to Cialdini and Trost’s (1998) personal norm future researchers can assist policymakers by testing
construct. External motivations on the other hand are the commu-
nity’s ‘‘standards or guidelines’’ (p. 819) for proper behavior. This
2
concept is similar to Cialdini and Trost’s (1998) injunctive norm Bobek et al. (2011) provide details regarding the scale development
construct. In this article, we choose to rely on Cialdini and Trost’s of the social norm constructs and test other methodological issues.
(1998) taxonomy due to its comprehensive nature. They do not test hypotheses regarding social norms.

123
Author's personal copy
Role of Social Norms 453

Table 1 Summary table of social norm constructs


Descriptive norms Injunctive norms Subjective norms Personal norms

Description What one perceives that other The perception of what most A person’s perception about Self-based standards or
people do in a given people think others should what those who are expectations for behavior
situation. Watching others do in a given situation. They important to him think he (i.e., what an individual
provides information about specify what should be done should do in a given believes he/she should do)
what is ‘‘normal’’ in a novel and are the moral rules of situation. They are one’s in a given situation. These
or ambiguous situation the group perceptions of the standards arise from
injunctive norms held by internalized values
the people whose opinions
matter most
Social goal Effective action—the desire Building and maintaining Building and maintaining Managing self-concept
being to be accurate in one’s social relationships social relationships
achieved by choices and behaviors
conforming
to norm
When will Most likely to use evidence of They motivate behavior by When people are motivated to Enforced through the
they matter? others’ behavior to decide promising social rewards or comply with the norms of anticipation of self-
most effective course of punishments. Need not be ‘‘referent’’ others enhancement or self-
action when the situation is expressed in order to direct deprecation (‘‘self-
novel, ambiguous, or behavior. These norms are reinforcing’’)
uncertain, and especially more influential when they
when the source of are made salient
reference is similar to us
Adapted from Bobek et al. (2007)

interventions (such as educational programs, media pro- constrain social behavior without the force of law’’ (Cial-
grams, advertising campaigns, or other measures) aimed dini and Trost 1998, p. 152). The four different social norm
at changing social norms.3 Furthermore, because the constructs identified by Cialdini and Trost are injunctive
perceived detection risk is linked to both social norm norms, descriptive norms, subjective norms, and personal
constructs as well as tax compliance itself, policymakers norms. As summarized in Table 1,4 Descriptive norms are
may wish to consider how the perception that cheating is standards that develop out of observation of how others
widespread affects the justifiability of tax non-compliance. actually behave in particular situations. Injunctive norms,
Finally, this study also contributes to the social psychology on the other hand, specify what should be done and are
literature by explicitly considering the inter-relationships therefore the moral rules of the group. Subjective norms
among the norm constructs. relate specifically to the expectations of important others
The remainder of this article is organized as follows. In (e.g., family, friends, coworkers, and so forth). Finally,
the next section, theory and prior research are discussed personal norms are one’s own expectations for behavior,
and our hypotheses are developed. Next, we discuss our which may arise in part from the internalization of
method for testing the hypotheses, and then report our injunctive norms.5 Injunctive, subjective, and personal
results. Finally, we draw conclusions and offer suggestions norms all relate to the expectations of certain groups or
for future research. individuals, or, described another way, what one believes

4
This table is adapted from a similar table presented in Bobek et al.
Prior Research and Hypotheses Development (2007) and is based on Cialdini and Trost’s (1998) extensive
discussion of social norm theory in the Handbook of Social
Theory of Social Norms Psychology.
5
Personal norms are another term for personal ethical beliefs/moral
obligation, etc. There has been much prior research that has
Social norms are defined as ‘‘rules and standards that are
investigated taxpayer ethics related to tax compliance (e.g., Reckers
understood by members of a group, and that guide and/or et al. 1994; Kaplan et al. 1997; Henderson and Kaplan 2005). In this
study, we do not discuss or consider the underlying ethical theories
that relate to individuals’ ethical judgments, as this is not our focus.
3
In fact, Nina Olson, the National Taxpayer Advocate in the US, in We are keenly interested in how social norms (including personal
remarks made at the American Taxation Association Midyear norms) affect taxpayer compliance because we believe that, while it
Meeting on March 4, 2011, indicated that her office is currently may be difficult to change someone’s ethical orientation or level of
considering an educational tour throughout the country in an effort to moral development, researchers and policymakers may have an
improve voluntary compliance. opportunity to influence compliance through social norms.

123
Author's personal copy
454 D. D. Bobek et al.

he/she ought to do in a given situation (Hechter and Opp becomes increasingly lax, eventually cheating becomes so
2001; Kallgren et al. 2000). Descriptive norms are merely lucrative that any one person is willing to suffer the social
one’s perception of what others actually do. stigma even if no one else cheats. Once one taxpayer
In addition to refining these social norm constructs, cheats, it causes an epidemic of cheating. The new equi-
Cialdini and colleagues (e.g., Kallgren et al. 2000) also librium is somewhat ‘‘sticky’’ and substantial efforts by the
emphasize that a particular social norm is unlikely to taxing authority are required to improve compliance.
influence an individual’s behavior unless it is salient (or Davis et al. (2003) found a similar result and also point
‘‘focal’’) at the time of the behavior. Social norms research out that compliance equilibriums are ‘‘sticky’’ in both
in social psychology has clearly established that social directions. Another analytical model, by Korobow et al.
norms guide action in ‘‘meaningful ways’’ (Schultz et al. (2007), showed that compliance behavior was also influ-
2007). This research has led to social norms marketing enced by communication among taxpayers. Specifically,
campaigns designed to influence a variety of behaviors Korobow et al. (2007) found that when enforcement rates
including alcohol use among college students (e.g., Clapp are low, tax evasion actually increases when agents
et al. 2003), environmental conservation (e.g., Goldstein (taxpayers) share information regarding their neighbors’
et al. 2008), and tobacco use among teenagers (e.g., Brown compliance decisions.
and Moodie 2009). Despite the experimental evidence Porcano and Price (1993) found that survey respondents
showing the importance of social norms on behavior, the were more likely to comply if a social sanction (cheaters
success of these marketing campaigns has been mixed, being publicized in the newspaper) supplemented
perhaps in part because they have focused on targeting the enforcement efforts and this effect increased as the likeli-
wrong type of norms.6 These studies suggest that under- hood of being caught (the audit rate) increased. In an
standing the influence of social norms and the relationships experimental economics setting, Alm et al. (1999) found
between these constructs is important for changing that when the social norm (the results of a vote on the
behavior. enforcement regime) tended toward non-compliance,
almost all subjects cheated. However, when ‘‘cheap talk’’7
Prior Tax Research on Social Norms was allowed prior to voting, subjects generally voted in
favor of stronger enforcement mechanisms, which then led
While recent social psychology research has focused on to more compliance.
changing social norms to influence behavior, the trend in While the studies just discussed concentrate on external
prior tax compliance research has been on measuring social societal influences, as depicted in Table 2, other studies
norms as a way of explaining compliance behavior. Due in have focused on the personal and/or subjective norm con-
part to a lack of precision in defining the social norm struct (e.g., Hanno and Violette 1996; Kaplan et al. 1997;
constructs, this prior research leaves open questions Bobek and Hatfield 2003). Hanno and Violette (1996) and
regarding the effect of social norms on tax compliance. Bobek and Hatfield (2003) tested two different versions of
In Table 2, we chronologically summarize prior tax Ajzen’s (1991) Theory of Planned Behavior. In both of
compliance research that has included a ‘‘social norm’’ these studies, moral obligation (personal norms) and the
variable. As is evident from inspection of Table 2, while a opinions of important others (subjective norms) influenced
number of studies have considered the role of social norms compliance decisions. Kaplan et al. (1997) found that
on tax compliance, none has done so in a comprehensive moral development was significantly correlated with
manner. Further, many prior studies (e.g., Cowell 1990; compliance and influenced the effectiveness of educational
Porcano and Price 1993; Alm et al. 1999; Davis et al. 2003) communications designed to improve compliance. The
have used terms similar to ‘‘social norm’’ to mean one or results of Blanthorne and Kaplan (2008) also suggest that
more of the social norm constructs discussed above, individual ethical beliefs (personal norms) are particularly
without a precise definition of which type of norm is important, as they found that social (subjective) norms
actually affecting behavior. This is particularly true of directly influence ethical beliefs, which in turn affect
several instructive analytical studies. For example, Cowell compliance behavior, but did not discover a direct rela-
(1990) shows that a ‘‘social stigma’’ variable serves to tionship between subjective norms and tax compliance
increase compliance as long as there is a reasonable level
of enforcement by the taxing authority. If enforcement 7
Cheap talk refers to the ability of subjects to informally discuss
specific aspects of the experiment. It should be noted that most
6
For example, Schultz et al. (2007) report that focusing attention on experimental economics studies use neutral terminology. For exam-
descriptive norms (i.e., what others do) regarding average energy ple, instead of words like ‘‘tax’’ and ‘‘audit’’ they use terms like ‘‘fee’’
usage, may actually increase energy usage of those with below and ‘‘check’’ (Alm et al. 1999). Thus, any effect from a social norm
average usage. They show that adding a message focused on social construct arises from the experimental setting and not due to the
approval/disapproval (i.e., injunctive norm) eliminates this effect. subjects tapping into their own social norms of tax compliance.

123
Table 2 Summary of prior tax compliance research on social norms
Types of norm studied and description of variable

Study Type of analysis Social Descriptive Injunctive Subjective Personal norms Summary of findings
norm norms norms norms
construct
specified?
Role of Social Norms

Cowell (1990) Analytical No Model included a ‘‘social stigma’’ variable that increased
compliance. Relied on a reasonable level of enforcement.
Compliance equilibrium ‘‘sticky’’
Porcano and Price Experimental—178 US No A ‘‘social stigma’’ variable was used. The possibility of tax
(1993) taxpayers evaders’ names being reported in the newspaper
significantly increased compliance
Hanno and Violette Experimental—73 continuing Yes Expectations 2 items (fulfill duty Both social norms variables influenced compliance, personal
(1996) education students of family as citizen and fulfill norms more important
members moral and ethical
influenced obligations)
compliance
Alm et al. (1999) Experimental Yes Results of a vote on When the results of the vote tended toward non-compliance,
economics—151 enforcement almost all subjects cheated. Allowing ‘‘cheap talk’’ prior to
undergraduate students level voting improved social norms and increased compliance.
Experiment was conducted using neutral (i.e., non-tax)
terminology
Davis et al. (2003) Analytical No Model including ‘‘social norm’’ variable increased
compliance. Compliance equilibriums are sticky in both
directions
Bobek and Hatfield Experimental—108 students Yes Opinion of 1 item (I think it Both subjective norms and moral obligation (i.e., personal
(2003) and 70 adults important would be morally norms) influenced compliance
others wrong to …)
Wenzel (2004) Experimental—1,306 Yes 3-Item scale 3-Item scale Both social norm constructs influenced compliance.
Australian taxpayers However, personal norms were more important and the
effect of injunctive norms was mediated by how strongly
Author's personal copy

subjects’ identified with ‘‘being Australian’’


Wenzel (2005) Experimental—1,036 Yes 3-Item scale 3-Item scale Longitudinal study showed that both social norm constructs
Australian taxpayers affect compliance, however injunctive norms are partially
mediated by personal norms
Bobek et al. (2007) Experimental—76 students Yes Factor Factor scores of Factor scores Factor scores of 6-item Cross-country differences in compliance explained by
from Australia, 54 (45) scores of 3-item scale of 6-item scale (combined with injunctive, subjective, and personal norms. However, were
taxpayers from the US 2-item scale subjective norms) not able to tease out separate constructs for subjective and
(Singapore) scale (combined personal norms
with
personal
norms)
Korobow et al. (2007) Analytical No When enforcement rates are low, knowledge of neighbors’
compliance decisions decreases compliance
Blanthorne and Kaplan Experimental—355 US Yes Factor scores Factor scores Subjective norms influenced underreporting ethics (i.e.,
(2008) taxpayers of 3-item of 5-item scale personal norms), but not compliance. Underreporting
scale ethics affected compliance
455

123
Author's personal copy
456 D. D. Bobek et al.

decisions. None of these particular studies examined the rewards. Thus, when individuals are faced with a new,
relationship between broader social norms (injunctive and ambiguous situation, they are likely to observe the actions of
descriptive) and tax compliance. others in the same situation to help make an ‘‘accurate’’
Finally, several recent studies have explored whether per- decision. Over time, these societal actions may become
sonal norms and broader social norms influence tax compli- socially sanctioned behaviors (Cialdini and Trost 1998; see
ance behavior. Wenzel (2004) studied the influence of both also Crandall 1988). Furthermore, taxpayer descriptive norms
injunctive norms and personal norms. In his study, conducted may also influence subjective norms. Just as societal actions in
in Australia, personal norms were more important than general (such as the proportion of the population that wears a
injunctive norms. In addition, the importance of injunctive seat belt, or that properly reports all cash earnings) are likely to
norms was mediated by how strongly the respondents iden- influence injunctive norms, these same actions are also likely
tified with ‘‘being Australian.’’ In a follow-up longitudinal to influence perceptions of what one’s family or friends
study of Australian citizens, Wenzel (2005) established that believe one ‘‘should’’ do in a situation. Using the same rea-
personal and injunctive norms influenced tax compliance soning, it is likely that descriptive norms could influence one’s
behavior, but that the influence of injunctive norms was par- personal norms as well. Further, Hechter and Opp (2001)
tially mediated by the effect of personal norms. Finally, Bobek suggest that oughtness norms (like injunctive, subjective, and
et al. (2007) demonstrated that differences in social norms personal norms) often emerge from behavioral regularities
partially explain cross-country differences in tax compliance (i.e., descriptive norms). Thus our first hypothesis predicts:
intentions. Their study also established that the social norms of
H1 Taxpayers’ descriptive norms toward tax compliance
‘‘close others’’ (both the individual moral beliefs and the
will influence their injunctive, subjective, and personal
norms of important others) played the most important role in
norms toward tax compliance.
explaining tax compliance intentions. However, they were not
able to tease out separate constructs for personal and sub- Injunctive norms may also exert influence on subjective
jective norms. and personal norms. In fact, subjective norms may be viewed
Nearly, all prior tax research that has included a social norm as a narrower category of injunctive norms. Thus, the
construct has identified a significant influence on tax com- injunctive norms of society toward tax compliance behavior
pliance from these constructs. However, there has been a lack may influence the expectations of close friends or family
of specificity regarding the identity, emergence, and trans- members, particularly if these referents have similar opinions
mission of these norms. Alm and McKee (1998, p. 270) to society at large. Injunctive norms may also influence per-
concluded that ‘‘social norms play an important, perhaps a sonal norms. According to Schwartz (1977), individuals
decisive role in tax compliance.’’ Furthermore, Wenzel (2004, develop their self-based standards of behavior (personal
p. 215) suggested that greater refinement of the social norm norms) from the societal expectations of behavior that are
constructs and their role on tax compliance behavior was gleaned through social interactions (injunctive norms). Indi-
needed to understand their potentially ‘‘substantial impact.’’ viduals may initially follow injunctive norms to avoid social
stigma, but over time, may continue to follow such norms for
Hypotheses Development internal reasons, such as enhancing their self-image. This
leads to the second hypothesis:
We begin our hypotheses development with the theoretical
H2 Taxpayers’ injunctive norms toward tax compliance
premise that the larger influences the smaller. In other
will influence their subjective and personal norms toward
words, general societal norms may exert influence on one’s
tax compliance.
own personal norms; however, at any given point in time it
is implausible that the influence goes in the opposite Finally, subjective norms may also influence personal
direction. With that in mind, the following discussion norms. Just as individuals may internalize societal values in
provides theoretical justification for our hypotheses about forming their personal standards of acceptable behavior,
the inter-relationships between the different social norm they may also internalize the values of close others. Indi-
constructs. Figure 1 depicts our complete model and viduals that are surrounded by family members and friends
summarizes our hypotheses. who support taxpayer compliance are also more likely to
Descriptive norms arise from observing individuals’ exhibit ethical beliefs that support this behavior (e.g.,
behavior in specific situations. Conformity to descriptive Blanthorne and Kaplan 2008). We therefore predict that:
norms is more likely when individuals desire to behave
H3 Taxpayers’ subjective norms toward tax compliance
‘‘correctly’’ in a novel situation. On the other hand, injunctive
will influence their personal norms toward tax compliance.
norms refer to general societal expectations of appropriate
behavior; conforming to injunctive norms typically arises out The extent to which each of the different social norm
of a desire to build social relationships and achieve social constructs directly influence tax compliance decisions

123
Author's personal copy
Role of Social Norms 457

H5

H1
Subjective
Norms
H2 H5

H3
Descriptive H1 Injunctive H5 Tax Compliance
Norms Norms Intentions

H1
H2
Personal H4, H5
Norms

H5

Fig. 1 Hypothesized relationships. Dashed lined arrows represent alternative hypotheses

remains an open question. In one viewpoint, tax compli- psychology research (which has primarily focused on obser-
ance is at least initially, a private, unobservable action and vable, rather than un-observable behavior) which of these
thus could be immune to direct outside social influence. constructs is most important with regard to tax compliance.
Some social psychologists (e.g., Schwartz 1977) posit that Based on this discussion, we present two alternative
personal norms are the only type of norm that directly hypotheses regarding the influence of the different types of
influences behavior. Further, Cialdini and Trost (1998) note social norms on tax compliance decisions. Our fourth
that some researchers have found less conformity with hypothesis predicts that only personal norms will directly
injunctive and descriptive social norms when behavior was influence tax compliance decisions, while our fifth
private as opposed to publicly observed. Alternatively, hypothesis predicts that each of the social norm constructs
Cialdini and colleagues (e.g., Cialdini and Trost 1998; identified by Cialdini and Trost (1998) will influence
Cialdini and Goldstein 2004) argue that different types of individuals’ tax compliance decisions:
norms may be mutually activated in a situation. Thus, other
H4 Taxpayers’ personal norms toward tax compliance
types of social norms may influence behavior beyond
will directly influence their tax compliance decisions; the
personal norms. Despite the relatively private nature of tax
relationship between tax compliance and subjective norms,
compliance behavior, it is possible that these other norm
injunctive norms, and descriptive norms toward tax com-
constructs are also influential.
pliance will only be indirect (through personal norms).
An additional argument in favor of multiple norms influ-
encing tax compliance behavior relates to the uniqueness of H5 Taxpayers’ personal norms, subjective norms, injunc-
the tax compliance setting. Taxpayers and policymakers alike tive norms, and descriptive norms toward tax compliance will
view the ‘‘fairness’’ of the tax system as extremely important directly influence their tax compliance decisions.
(see Slemrod and Bakija 2008). One facet of tax fairness is the
desire for oneself and others to pay their ‘‘fair share’’ of taxes.
There are two avenues toward achieving this fair distribution Research Methods
of tax burdens. First is the structure of the tax system itself,
which few citizens can directly influence; second, however, is Participants
one’s own level of compliance with the system. This is one
way in which social norms are likely to be important to tax- Experienced taxpayers were used for this study. Partici-
payers’ compliance decisions. If taxpayers believe ‘‘every- pants were obtained by having students, in exchange for
body cheats’’ (descriptive norms), society thinks it is ‘‘okay to extra credit, recruit subjects who were ‘‘non-accounting
cheat’’ (injunctive norms), and/or their friends/family think it students at least 25 years old that are US citizens or resi-
is ‘‘okay to cheat’’ (subjective norms), then they are not only dents that have filed a federal income tax return.’’8 This
more likely to cheat, but are also more likely to believe it is
8
okay to cheat (personal norms). As detailed earlier, the con- Two instructors of an introductory accounting course at a large
clusion of prior tax research is that one or more of the above southeastern university in the spring and summer of 2008 offered
extra credit to their students for providing the names, mailing address,
social norm constructs are indeed related to tax compliance. It and email addresses of eligible participants that had expressed an
is not clear from prior tax research, or from prior social interest in participating in the study. Each student could provide no

123
Author's personal copy
458 D. D. Bobek et al.

recruiting technique resulted in 470 potential participants. items assessing demographic information (e.g., age,
Participants completed the study questionnaire either in income, education, political philosophy, etc.).
online or hard copy format (the questionnaires contained
identical content and participants were randomly assigned Measurement of Variables
to one of the two format conditions).9 Of the 470 potential
participants, 223 individuals responded and the responses The primary dependent variable is tax compliance intentions,
of 218 individuals met the study’s criteria. This resulted in measured as the response to, ‘‘Do you think that if you were in a
a response rate of 47 %. However, as our study’s hypoth- similar situation as Larry [the hypothetical actor in the sce-
eses were tested using path analysis, the elimination of nario], YOU would deduct the ADDITIONAL $2,000?’’
subjects with missing data values was necessary (Schu- Participants responded using a 7-point Likert-type scale,
macker and Lomax 2004). Participants that neglected to ranging from ‘‘7 = very unlikely’’ to ‘‘1 = very likely,’’ such
respond to at least one of the questionnaire items were that higher values indicated greater compliance. The mean
eliminated from the final sample, resulting in a useable (standard deviation) of this response was 5.11 (2.17), indicat-
sample of 174 responses (usable response rate of 37 %). ing that most participants believed that they would be some-
Table 3 reports the sample demographics and compares what likely to comply with the tax law (and were somewhat
the sample to the national population. Our sample was unlikely to deduct the additional automobile expense).10 This
older (70 % between the ages of 45 and 64) and included measure concerns participants’ evaluation of a hypothetical
more females (57 %) than males. Participants also had dilemma and thus is a measure of their compliance intentions.
higher income and were better educated than the national Concerns that behavioral intentions might not map directly to
population. The majority of participants (68 %) did not taxpayers’ actual behavior in a situation are partially mitigated
prepare their own tax returns. by the fact that a joint investigation of actual and hypothetical
tax evaders (Webley et al. 2001) indicated that both groups had
Experimental Task and Procedures similar attitudes and motivations.
Multiple items were designed to measure each of the four
Participants responded to a hypothetical tax compliance types of social norms of tax compliance. The response scale
dilemma in this study. The dilemma was adapted from for the majority of these items was a 7-point Likert-type scale,
Kaplan et al. (1997) and Bobek and Hatfield (2003), who and each category included items measuring the norms of tax
used a similar scenario. To increase participants’ ability to compliance in general and toward the specific hypothetical tax
relate to the hypothetical taxpayer and reduce the ‘‘better scenario. Items were coded with higher values indicating more
than average’’ effect that individuals often exhibit when favorable norms toward tax compliance.
evaluating a comparison target (Alicke et al. 1995), the In addition to demographic items, participants respon-
scenario included additional contextual details. The text of ded to items measuring individual differences in attitudes
this tax compliance dilemma is presented in the Appendix. and perceptions that may also be related to tax compliance
After reviewing the hypothetical tax compliance sce- intentions and are common to other tax compliance studies.
nario, participants then answered items regarding what they For example, we included items measuring the perceived
and others would do if they were in a similar position. They probability of detection to control for differences in par-
next responded to items measuring their social norms of ticipants’ perceptions regarding the opportunity to be non-
tax compliance (including personal, subjective, injunctive, compliant (e.g., Bobek and Hatfield 2003; Blanthorne and
and descriptive norms) for the specific scenario. Partici- Kaplan 2008), and a measure of risk aversion to control for
pants also responded to items measuring their general their likelihood of taking advantage of the opportunity
social norms of compliance for the four types of social (e.g., Christensen and Hite 1997). We measured inclusive
norms, as well as items measuring their general attitudes identification, as this variable was found to be related to the
and perceived fairness of income tax. Finally, we included influence of the injunctive norms of Australians (Wenzel
2004). We also included items measuring participants’
perceptions of the fairness of the income tax system, and
Footnote 8 continued
more than two names and if more than one name was provided the
individuals were required to be from different households. We 10
By comparison, participants viewed that ‘‘Larry’’ (the hypothetical
emphasized that the responses were confidential, and that the par- taxpayer in the scenario) would be slightly unlikely to comply (mean
ticipants would never be associated with the study or have individual of 3.29), as would the average taxpayer (mean 3.03). Thus,
responses reported. The extra credit points awarded to students were participants were significantly more likely to believe that they would
tied to their identification of eligible participants, rather than to par- be more compliant than would other taxpayers (p \ .001). Further-
ticipants’ actual completion of the study. more, 47 % of participants indicated that it was ‘‘very unlikely’’ that
9
A copy of the questionnaire can be made available by contacting they would take the additional deduction, indicating that the
the authors. remaining half of participants might not be completely compliant.

123
Author's personal copy
Role of Social Norms 459

Table 3 Sample demographics First, we generated a sample of 36 scale items based on an


Sample (n = 174) a
2007 National population b extensive literature search of prior tax compliance studies
that had measured social norms. We categorized these
Age items in terms of the four types of social norms from the
Under 25 = 0 % 20–24 = 10 % Cialdini and Trost (1998) framework. The scale was
25–44 = 22 % 25–44 = 38.5 % refined by using reliability analysis and factor analysis
45–64 = 70 % 45–64 = 34.5 % (principal components analysis with Promax rotation) to
65? = 8 % 65? = 17 % eliminate items with low item-to-total correlations or low
Family income item loadings. Table 4 reports the results of the final factor
\$25,000 = 3 % \$25,000 = 17 % analysis, including the item loadings for each of the mea-
$25,000–$49,999 = 15 % $25,000–$49,999 = 24 % sures included in the final scales. For the analysis of the
$50,001–$74,999 = 19 % $50,000–$74,999 = 20.5 % effect of social norms on tax compliance behavior, these
$75,000–$99,999 = 22 % $75,000–$99,999 = 14.5 % four correlated factor scores for each participant were
Over $100,000 = 41 % Over $100,000 = 24 % retained as independent variables.
Gender
Male = 43 % Male = 49 % Correlation Analysis
Female = 57 % Female = 51 %
Education Table 5 reports descriptive statistics and a correlation matrix
Did not complete Did not complete for the dependent variable, social norm variables, and the
High school = 0.5 % High school = 16 % additional attitudinal and demographic variables. All four of
High school graduate = 6 % High school graduate = 30 % the social norm constructs are strongly correlated with tax
Some college = 29 % Some college = 27 % compliance intentions, as taxpayers with stronger social
College graduate = 38 % College graduate = 27 % norms are more likely to report that they would comply (all
Post-graduate study = 26.5 % Post-graduate study = 10 %
p \ .001). Furthermore, all of the social norm constructs are
Prepare Your Own Tax Return?
highly correlated with each other (again, all p \ .001).
Yes = 32 %
Several other interesting findings emerge from the cor-
relation analysis. First, none of the demographic variables
No = 68 %
is significantly correlated with tax compliance intentions.
a
Numbers are expressed as a percentage of the total sample of However, in addition to social norms, tax compliance
respondents providing information for each demographic question
b
intentions are also correlated with fairness perceptions,
US Census Bureau: Age represents percentage of adults over age 19
in each category; family income numbers represent the percentage of
perceived probability of disallowance, attitude toward
families in each category; education numbers represents percentage of additional money, familiarity with the deduction, and risk
persons over 25 in each category preferences. These variables are also strongly correlated
with many of the social norm constructs; furthermore, the
their attitude toward additional money, as similar variables perception of the overall audit rate is also strongly corre-
were significantly related to tax compliance intentions in lated with injunctive norms. We include the effects of the
prior research (e.g., Bobek and Hatfield 2003). Finally, we non-social norms variables as control variables in the
also measured how often participants had taken a deduction model used to test the hypothesized relationships.
for automobile expenses. We included this variable
because, based on social norm theory, descriptive norms Hypothesis Testing with Path Analysis—Effect
may be particularly salient when faced with an unfamiliar of Social Norms
or novel situation (see Table 1). Refer to the Appendix for
a discussion of the measurement of these individual and Our study’s hypotheses predict that social norms will have
attitudinal control variables. a direct and indirect relationship on tax compliance
intentions. We therefore study both the relationships
among the social norm constructs as well as the effect of
Results these constructs on tax compliance behavior. We use path
analysis to test the relationships among these constructs.
Social Norm Scale Path analysis is a modeling technique that simultaneously
estimates multiple regression equations; it is an extension
Our construction of a scale for assessing taxpayer social of multivariate regression that allows for the simul-
norms followed the guidelines of Mason and Levy (2001) taneous testing of any number of independent and depen-
and is described in greater detail in Bobek et al. (2011). dent variables. However, the hypothesized relationships

123
Author's personal copy
460 D. D. Bobek et al.

Table 4 Factor analysis results


Statement descriptiona Mean Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4
response loading loading loading loading

How much of a moral obligation…do you 5.79 .716


feel to be completely honest…
I think it is acceptable to overstate tax 5.44 .758
deductions…(R)
Would you feel guilty if you took the 5.51 .888
additional $2,000 deduction?
Would you feel ashamed if you took the 5.17 .892
additional $2,000 deduction?
Would you feel justified in taking the 4.91 .809
additional $2,000 deduction? (R)
Would you feel pleased in taking the 5.23 .823
additional $2,000 deduction? (R)
Most people you know (would 4.47 .915
disapprove)
Your co-workers (would disapprove) 4.32 .835
Your family (would disapprove) 5.03 .901
Your friends (would disapprove) 4.58 .941
Your significant other (would disapprove) 5.26 .824
Most people would feel guilty if they took 3.74 .882
the additional $2,000 deduction
Most people would feel ashamed if they 3.52 .894
took the additional $2,000 deduction
Most people would be afraid they would 4.47 .717
get caught…(R)
…What percentage of taxpayers at your 7.35 (37 %) .741
income level deliberately pay less…(R)
…What percentage of US taxpayers at 8.14 (29 %) .727
your income level carelessly, but
unknowingly pay less…(R)
…What percentage of taxpayers at your 6.24 (48 %) .822
income level…would take the
additional deduction…(R)
…What percentage of the additional 6.30 (46 %) .689
$2,000…is the closest to the amount the
average US taxpayer would deduct? (R)
Percentage of variance explained 46 % 10 % 9% 7%
Factor description Personal norms Subjective norms Injunctive norms Descriptive norms
Factor loadings are Promax rotation scores
a
The personal, subjective, and injunctive social norm items were assessed on a 1–7 Likert-type scale. Descriptive norm items were assessed on a
1–11 Likert-type scale. Items were coded so that higher values indicated more favorable social norms toward tax compliance

depicted in Fig. 1 reflect a fully saturated model (i.e., a Fit indices show that our model is a good fit for the
theoretical model in which all paths are specified; see data.11
Schumacker and Lomax 2004). Thus, to test the relation- To further assess the overall fit of our final model, we
ships with path analysis, we also considered the influence compare the fit of our final model with a ‘‘naı̈ve’’ model that
of the non-social norms variables that emerged as impor-
tant in the correlation analysis. To develop a more parsi- 11
While the overall model v2 statistic is statistically significant, the
monious final model, we culled statistically insignificant ratio of the v2 statistic to the degrees of freedom is 1.82, less than the
paths pertaining to these control variables. Figure 2 and recommended 2.0 ceiling. The model’s comparative fit index (CFI) of
Table 6 depict the final path analysis model with social .964 exceeds the recommended cut-off of .90, and the root mean
square error of approximation (RMSEA) of 0.069 is below the
norms, tax compliance intentions, and the control variables. recommended threshold of .08.

123
Table 5 Descriptive statistics and correlation matrix
Role of Social Norms

Variable Mean S.D. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17

1.Tax compliance 2.89 2.17 1


intentions
2. Personal normsa 0 1 0.703 1
3. Subjective normsa 0 1 0.536 0.586 1
4. Injunctive normsa 0 1 0.335 0.485 0.452 1
5. Descriptive normsa 0 1 0.334 0.341 0.436 0.378 1
6. Unfair 14.47 4.91 20.200 20.18 -0.111 20.198 20.297 1
7. Disallowance 5.3 1.91 0.391 0.408 0.216 0.335 0.243 -0.080 1
8. Political 3.69 1.29 -0.119 -0.117 -0.020 -0.012 0.044 -0.086 0.057 1
9. Inclusive 12.96 2.06 -0.056 0.105 0.058 0.114 0.092 -0.043 0.049 -0.100 1
10. Money 2.11 1.58 20.51 20.633 20.352 20.254 20.268 0.200 20.281 0.129 -0.004 1
11. Familiarity 2.71 2.29 -0.136 20.231 0.046 -0.050 -0.109 0.179 0.021 -0.057 0.021 0.146 1
12. Risk 7.95 2.99 0.175 0.303 0.174 0.080 0.151 0.057 0.136 0.037 0.163 20.164 20.225 1
13. Age 48.87 11.46 0.112 0.129 0.096 0.012 0.132 -0.046 0.185 -0.145 0.082 -0.107 0.083 0.006 1
14. Gender 1.57 0.5 -0.095 -0.026 -0.110 0.068 -0.058 0.048 0.102 0.278 -0.060 0.185 -0.037 0.101 20.234 1
15. Income 3.83 1.21 0.050 -0.051 0.004 -0.074 0.102 -0.050 -0.018 -0.137 0.244 0.034 0.262 0.041 0.252 -0.129 1
16. Education 3.84 0.9 0.024 0.079 0.079 0.103 0.134 -0.091 0.065 -0.127 0.052 0.029 0.097 -0.012 0.096 -0.079 0.366 1
17. Return 1.68 0.47 -0.083 0.002 -0.033 -0.019 0.030 0.115 0.040 0.122 0.002 0.139 0.030 -0.029 0.061 0.176 0.043 -0.060 1
18. Audit 18.95 18.64 -0.096 0.041 -0.004 0.170 -0.092 0.028 0.128 0.148 0.158 0.013 -0.101 0.139 20.239 0.336 20.197 20.294 0.192
Author's personal copy

Tax Compliance Intentions are measured on a 7-point Likert-type scale, with 7 = ‘‘very unlikely’’ and 1 = ‘‘very likely’’ participants would evade taxes (cheat) by taking the additional deduction. Unfair is the sum of
three questions measuring perceived unfairness with higher values indicating more perceived unfairness (scale ranging from 3–21). Disallowance measures participants’ perceived likelihood of disallowance, and is
measured on a 1–7 scale with higher values indicating greater risk of detection. Political measures participants’ self-reported political philosophy, ranging from very conservative to very liberal (1–7 scale). Inclusive is
the sum of two measures of identification with the American community, with higher values indicating greater identification (2–14 scale). Money measures participants’ attitude toward having the additional money even
if it was illegal, measured on a 1–7 scale. Familiarity measures how often participants have deducted automobile expenses, ranging from never to very often (1–7 scale). Risk measures participants’ risk preferences
(1–11 scale), with higher values representing greater risk aversion. Age measures participants’ age in years. Gender is coded as ‘‘1’’ if male and ‘‘2’’ if female. Income is measured on a 1–5 scale, with higher values
representing greater income. Education is measured on a scale with five options, with higher values representing more education. Return is coded as ‘‘1’’ if participants prepared their last income tax return themselves
and ‘‘2’’ otherwise. Audit measures participants’ perception of the overall audit rate for individual income tax returns
a
Social norms factors are the factor scores obtained from the factor analysis described in Table 4. Factor scores are standardized with a mean (standard deviation) of 0 (1)
Bolded correlations are significant at p \ .05
461

123
Author's personal copy
462 D. D. Bobek et al.

Table 6 Path analysis results


Endogenous (dependent) variable Path Standardized T statistic
path coefficient

Descriptive norms (R2 of .1368) Unfairness ? descriptive norms -.280 -4.10


Probability of disallowance ? descriptive norms .221 3.19
Injunctive norms (R2 of .2444) Descriptive norms ? injunctive norms (H1) .336 5.18
Overall audit rate ? injunctive norms .171 2.59
Probability of disallowance ? injunctive norms .229 3.40
Subjective norms (R2 of .2903) Descriptive norms ? subjective norms (H1) .308 4.58
Injunctive norms ? subjective norms (H2) .337 5.05
Personal norms (R2 of .6075) Descriptive norms ? personal norms (H1) -.050 -0.92
Injunctive norms ? personal norms (H2) .197 3.46
Subjective norms ? personal norms (H3) .338 6.02
Probability of disallowance ? personal norms .172 3.23
Risk aversion ? personal norms .129 2.60
Familiarity ? personal norms -.137 -2.76
Attitude toward additional money ? personal norms -.424 -8.62
Tax compliance (R2 of .5077) Descriptive norms ? tax compliance (H5) .062 1.00
Injunctive norms ? tax compliance (H5) -.096 -1.47
Subjective norms ? tax compliance (H5) .204 3.05
Personal norms ? tax compliance (H4, H5) .543 8.89
Probability of disallowance ? tax compliance .143 2.38
Note: T statistics 1.96 (2.58) or over are statistically significant at p \ .05 (.01)

H5: -.096 ns

Overall Audit .171*** Injunctive H2: .337*** Subjective


Rate Norms Norms

H5: .204***

.229***
H3: .338***
H1: .308*** Tax Compliance
Intentions
H1: .336*** H2: .197***

Personal H4, H5: .543***


Norms
-.280*** Descriptive
Unfairness
Norms H1: -.05 ns

.172***

.221***

*, p < .10, **, p < .05, ***, p< .01, ns= not significant
all tests two-tailed .129** -.137*** -.424***

Model Fit Statistics Probability of


Disallowance
Model Chi Square = 38.322 (df=21), ratio 1.82,
p = .0118
Attitude toward
Comparative FIT Index (CFI) = .964 Risk Aversion Familiarity
Additional Money
Root Mean Square Error of Approximation
(RMSEA) = .069

Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) = .967


.143**
Normed Fit Index (NFI) = .929

H5: 062 ns

Fig. 2 Test of hypothesized relationships

123
Author's personal copy
Role of Social Norms 463

assumes only direct paths between the social norm constructs path coefficient = .543, p \ .001) are each strongly sig-
and the tax compliance intentions. When we perform a path nificant, indicating that favorable subjective norms and
analysis on this naı̈ve model (the same as our theoretical personal norms both directly improve taxpayer compliance.
model but with no paths between any of the social norm Thus, Hypotheses 4 and 5 are each partially supported,
variables), the model fit is substantially worse than the model indicating that while tax compliance is largely a private
fit of our theoretical model as reported in Fig. 2. decision, the standards and approval of close family and
Hypothesis 1 predicts that descriptive norms will have a friends remain important.
direct influence on injunctive norms, subjective norms, and
personal norms. Two of these three relationships are sup- Control Variables
ported by the path analysis. The direct path from descrip-
tive norms to injunctive norms is statistically significant In addition to the hypothesized relationships, several inter-
(p \ .001) and strong (standardized path coefficient of esting results emerge from the attitudinal control variables.
.336). The path from descriptive norms to subjective norms First, several of the attitudinal variables demonstrated a sta-
is statistically significant (p \ .001) and strong (standard- tistically significant influence on descriptive and injunctive
ized path coefficient of .308) as well. However, the path norms. The path between perceptions of the overall audit rate
from descriptive norms to personal norms is not statisti- and injunctive norms (standardized path coefficient = .171,
cally significant. Thus, Hypothesis 1 is partially supported. p \ .001) was positive and significant, as a perception of a
Hypothesis 2 posits that injunctive norms will influence higher actual IRS audit rate led to more favorable perceptions
subjective norms and personal norms. This hypothesis is of societal expectations. Similarly, those that perceived higher
supported, as the direct path from injunctive norms to each degrees of unfairness in the tax system had less favorable
of these constructs is positive and statistically significant descriptive norms toward compliance (standardized path
(p \ .001). However, the standardized path coefficient is coefficient = -.280, p \ .001). Perceived probability of
larger in magnitude for the path between injunctive norms and disallowance affected both descriptive norms (standardized
subjective norms (standardized path coefficient of .337) than path coefficient = .221, p \ .001) and injunctive norms
the path between injunctive norms and personal norms (standardized path coefficient = .229, p \ .001). This rela-
(standardized path coefficient of .197). Hypothesis 3 addres- tionship indicates that if participants viewed that they would
ses whether subjective norms influence personal norms. The be able to ‘‘get away with’’ cheating on their tax return if
path from subjective norms to personal norms is positive and audited, they had less favorable social norms toward com-
statistically significant (p \ .001, standardized path coeffi- pliance. Blanthorne and Kaplan (2008) proposed a similar
cient = .338), thus supporting Hypothesis 3. relationship, and predicted that those with a greater opportu-
Hypotheses 4 and 5 offer alternative predictions for the nity to cheat would have less favorable social norms toward
effect of social norms on tax compliance decisions. complying. While a significant relationship did not emerge in
Hypothesis 4 predicts that because tax compliance is lar- their study, our results indicate that a perception that one can
gely a private decision, personal norms will be the only get away with cheating has a stronger influence on norms than
social norm construct to directly influence tax compliance. simply having the opportunity to cheat.
Alternatively, Hypothesis 5 predicts that each social norm Second, several attitudinal variables also influenced per-
construct may be mutually activated in a tax compliance sonal norms, including risk aversion (standardized path
situation and directly influence the tax compliance deci- coefficient = .129, p \ .05), perceived probability of disal-
sion. In the preceding correlation analysis, all four social lowance (standardized path coefficient = .172, p \ .001),
norm constructs demonstrated a strong, positive correlation attitude toward additional money (standardized path coeffi-
with tax compliance behavior (all correlations [.33, cient = -.424, p \ .001), and familiarity with the deduction
p \ .001). However, when also considering the relation- (standardized path coefficient = -.137, p \ .001). Taxpay-
ships among the social norm constructs themselves, a more ers that were more risk averse and viewed the probability of
complex relationship emerges. disallowance as higher had more favorable personal norms of
Neither the direct path between injunctive norms and tax tax compliance. However, taxpayers that were more likely to
compliance behavior nor between descriptive norms and agree that they would really enjoy reducing their tax bill by
tax compliance behavior is statistically significant. This $500, regardless of whether it was legal or not (attitude toward
indicates that the relationship between injunctive/descrip- additional money), as well as taxpayers that had past experi-
tive norms and tax compliance behavior is fully mediated ences in taking automobile deductions (familiarity) had less
by their relationship with the other social norm constructs. favorable personal norms. This influence of attitude toward
However, the path between subjective norms and tax additional money indicates that individuals’ financial situa-
compliance (standardized path coefficient = .204, p \ tion may play an important role in determining their internal
.001) and personal norms and tax compliance (standardized standards of appropriate tax compliance behavior. The

123
Author's personal copy
464 D. D. Bobek et al.

negative path from familiarity (i.e., prior experiences in taking Table 7 Compliers versus non-compliers
a similar deduction) to personal norms indicates that taxpay- Variable Tax compliance
ers’ experience may have reduced the stigma they might
otherwise feel toward a more ‘‘generic’’ illegal behavior, and Compliers Non-compliers
(n = 82) (n = 92)
may be an indication that they were not entirely compliant in
the past.12 Collectively, these results also indicate that social Tax compliance intentions 3.42 (1.68)*** 7.00 (.00)
norms are at least partially contextually based. Personal norms .5409 (.70)*** -.5252 (.97)
Finally, as shown in Fig. 2, the path analysis results Subjective norms .4533 (.77)*** -.4356 (1.01)
show that the four social norm constructs have a strong Injunctive norms .2802 (1.03)*** -.2707 (.91)
direct or indirect influence on tax compliance behavior. Descriptive norms .3162 (.96)*** -.2483 (.96)
Based on the standardized path coefficients, strong per- Unfair 13.49 (5.25)** 15.34 (4.44)
sonal norms toward tax compliance are the most influential Disallowance 5.87 (1.76)*** 4.79 (1.91)
factor on tax compliance decisions. In the path analysis, the Money 1.46 (.97)*** 2.70 (1.79)
only attitudinal variable to directly affect tax compliance Familiarity 5.68 (2.04)** 4.95 (2.46)
decisions was perceived probability of disallowance Risk 8.43 (3.12)** 7.52 (2.82)
(standardized path coefficient = .143, p \ .05); personal Audit 17.26 (17.30) 20.46 (19.72)
norms were more than three times more influential than this
Note: All variables are described in Table 5. Dichotomous groups are
attitudinal variable, and subjective norms were more created based on median-splitting the tax compliance intentions var-
influential as well. Similarly, the relationships between the iable. Each column displays mean (standard deviations) for each
social norm constructs were much stronger than the rela- group
tionships between social norms and attitudinal variables. * Difference is significant at \ p = .10
Together, these results establish the robustness of our ** Difference is significant at \ p = .05
comprehensive social norm model for explaining tax *** Difference is significant at \ p = .01
compliance intentions.13

Additional Analysis high and low levels of compliance by comparing the social
norms and attitudinal measures of these two groups. We
We performed two additional analyses to further investi- split the respondents into two groups based on the median
gate our results. First, following Hanno and Violette level of compliance (6.0 on a 7-point scale) and then cal-
(1996), we examine the characteristics of taxpayers with culated the mean (standard deviation) level of social norms
and attitudinal variables for those in the ‘‘high compliance’’
and ‘‘low compliance’’ groups (see Table 7). Confirming
the results of our path analysis, the largest differences
between compliers and non-compliers are due to different
12
Supplemental tests indicate that descriptive norms were not more levels of social norms.
influential for taxpayers with less familiarity with taking business Second, in untabulated analyses, we find that individuals
deductions for automobile expenses. However, descriptive norms with more favorable social norms (all four types) toward
were less (more) influential for older (younger) taxpayers, providing compliance are less likely to view the tax system as unfair,
some evidence that descriptive norms are more likely to be referenced
in novel situations. and are less likely to enjoy additional money even if it were
13
In supplemental analysis, we consider taxpayers’ perceptions of illegal. Further, those with more favorable personal and
the likelihood of compliance of Larry (the hypothetical actor in the subjective norms are more risk averse and those with more
scenario) and the average taxpayer. These alternative path analysis favorable injunctive and descriptive norms are more likely
models produce similar results, with the exception of several factors. to think the probability of audit is high. Demographic
First, personal norms and taxpayers’ perceived detection risk have no
influence on Larry or the average taxpayer’s perceived compliance. factors (e.g., education, age, income, etc.) were not related
Second, descriptive norms directly influence Larry and the average to social norms or tax compliance, with the exception that
taxpayer’s perceived compliance, while injunctive norms also directly those with more favorable descriptive norms tend to be
influence the perceived tax compliance of the average taxpayer. These more educated.
results suggest that, not surprisingly, personal norms only directly
influence one’s own actions, whereas descriptive norms have a
stronger influence on what people think that other people would do in Limitations
a situation. These findings provide additional construct validity
regarding the study’s social norm variables. Theoretically, personal The results of this study should be interpreted in light of the
norms should only be related to one’s own behavior, while descriptive
and injunctive norms directly relate to what one thinks the average study’s potential limitations. First, as this research dealt
taxpayer will do. with a type of illegal behavior, it is possible that

123
Author's personal copy
Role of Social Norms 465

respondents did not report their level of tax compliance We tested our research hypotheses by obtaining
intentions honestly. Furthermore, they reported their per- responses from experienced taxpayers regarding a hypo-
ceptions of the various social norms of tax compliance and thetical compliance dilemma. Our factor analysis results
thus some bias could be introduced. However, we also identified the four distinct social norm constructs consistent
measured participants’ propensity to overclaim to control with Cialdini and Trost’s (1998) taxonomy: personal,
for potential social desirability bias (Randall and Fernandes subjective, injunctive, and descriptive. While all four of
1991). Overclaiming did not affect the results for our study, these social norm constructs were significantly correlated
and it therefore is unlikely that the interpretation of the with tax compliance behavior, path analysis indicated that
results suffers from extensive social desirability bias.14 only subjective norms and personal norms directly affected
Furthermore, as part of an initial pilot test, we asked taxpayers’ compliance intentions. Descriptive and injunc-
respondents if they felt like could ‘‘answer the questions in tive norms were related to subjective and/or personal
the questionnaire truthfully (e.g., did you feel like your norms; however, they did not have a direct effect on tax
identity was sufficiently protected?)’’. All but one respon- compliance intentions.
dent responded ‘‘yes.’’ Thus, we conclude that in a setting where cheating is
Second, generalization of results should be done with possible, personal and societal norms do indeed play a
caution, as subjects were not obtained through a random ‘‘decisive role in tax compliance’’ (Alm and McKee 1998).
sampling method and thus may not be representative of the Based on the results of our comprehensive model, we also
population at large. In addition, due to their willingness to conclude that personal norms play a much stronger role
participate in the study, participants may have been more than any other type of social norm, potentially because in
cooperative than the average person. As our study exam- the US, tax returns and compliance decisions are not shared
ined the interrelationships between social norms and tax with the public. However, subjective norms still had a
compliance, though, results are unlikely to be affected by strong, direct influence, clarifying the finding of Bobek
this limitation. Our taxpayer sample was also more likely et al. (2007). Moreover, injunctive norms, while not
to be female, slightly older, better educated, and had a directly affecting tax compliance, demonstrated a strong
higher income than the US national population. However, influence on both personal and subjective norms; in turn,
as higher income taxpayers may have more opportunities to descriptive norms shaped both injunctive and subjective
cheat, understanding the factors influencing their tax norms. We also identify a number of attitudinal variables
compliance behavior is important. (e.g., fairness, risk preferences) that are related to social
norms and help clarify the factors that influence social
norms.
Conclusions and Future Research Our results also show that enforcement variables are
directly related to tax compliance intentions and three of
The purpose of this study was to clarify the influence of the four social norms constructs. The fact that enforcement
social norms on taxpayer compliance. By drawing on variables are related to the social norms constructs is
Cialdini and Trost’s (1998) taxonomy of social norms, we consistent with both the findings of analytical research
provide more specificity to what is meant when researchers (e.g., Cowell 1990 and Davis et al. 2003) and tax morale
attribute an effect to ‘‘social norms.’’ We synthesized the studies (e.g., Torgler 2001, 2007). Analytical studies sug-
findings of prior research with respect to this taxonomy and gest that favorable social norms (particularly descriptive
developed a comprehensive model that examined the direct norms) are dependent upon a reasonable level of enforce-
and indirect influences of the social norm constructs on tax ment and the tax morale literature has identified a negative
compliance behavior. In all, our study examines the influ- relationship between tax morale and the shadow economy.
ence of social norms on tax compliance with greater depth Furthermore, while actual audit rates for individual tax
and specificity than prior research. returns in the United States are somewhere around 1 %
(www.irs.gov, 2011), our study participants perceived that,
14
Overclaiming is a measure of social desirability bias that assesses on average, 19 % of all returns are audited—thus esti-
participants’ propensity to claim knowledge of false items. We used mating the overall audit rate at nearly 2,000 % higher than
the 10-item overclaiming scale developed by Randall and Fernandes
it actually is.15 This suggests that a significant portion of
(1991), in which participants indicated their familiarity with 25 items
(such as music, movies, etc.)—with 15 real items and 10 fake items. US tax revenue (all revenue not subject to third party
The overclaiming scale only includes responses to the 10 fake items.
While participants displayed some degree of overclaiming (knowl-
15
edge of the fake items), inclusion of the overclaiming scale did not There is also a significant difference (p \ .001) between genders
change any of the reported results. Thus, results did not appear to in audit rate perceptions. Male participants on average believed the
suffer from social desirability bias. For more details see Bobek et al. audit rate is around 12 %, while female participants thought it was
(2011). more than twice that much at almost 25 %.

123
Author's personal copy
466 D. D. Bobek et al.

informational reporting) is dependent upon both a false taxpayers and this may affect the likelihood that they pay the
sense of the audit rate and positive social norms toward tax. Some may be perceived to be ‘‘good’’ taxes (e.g., income
compliance. Understanding how external actions can taxes) while others may be perceived to be ‘‘bad’’ taxes (e.g.,
influence social norms may be critical to increasing and sales tax on internet purchases in the US). Future research
maintaining compliance levels and reducing the tax gap. should also consider different types of taxes (e.g., sales and
Thus, we believe that this study provides a springboard for use taxes, gift taxes), different tax laws with different pro-
future tax researchers to experiment with ways to improve pensities to cheat (e.g., earned income credit, home office
compliance norms and thereby improve tax compliance deduction), and examine broader samples.
behavior. The fact that social norms are such a strong influence
on tax compliance suggests that changing these norms is a Acknowledgments We thank participants at the 2010 AAA Annual
Meeting, 2010 ABO Research Conference and workshop participants
fruitful avenue to pursue for increasing tax compliance. This is at the University of Central Florida and Kansas State University for
echoed by Alm and Torgler (2011), who discuss the impor- their helpful comments. We are also very grateful for the helpful
tance of ethics to tax compliance and identify three different feedback from Robin Roberts, Michael Roberts (discussant at the
paradigms for increasing tax compliance. In addition to the AAA Annual Meeting), Alisa Brink (discussant at the ABO Research
Conference), Tanya Benford and two anonymous reviewers; and for
traditional enforcement paradigm (higher audit and penalty the assistance of Jillian Phillips and Marcye Hampton in the collec-
rates) and a service paradigm (simplifying the tax system, tion of the experimental data.
improving taxpayer education and taxpayer assistance), they
also emphasize the importance of a trust paradigm which they
argue is an ‘‘essential but largely neglected strategy’’ (p. 647). Appendix
They describe a trust paradigm as a ‘‘governmental-induced
change in the culture of paying taxes’’ (p. 647). The specific Text of Experimental Scenario
activities they recommend are consistent with a strategy that
targets social norms. For example they recommend using the Larry Brown is a 40-year-old married father of two boys.
‘‘mass media to reinforce tax compliance as the ethical form of He has his own small business and his wife, Laura, works
behavior’’ and ‘‘enlisting other organizations to promote part-time. In addition to working in his business, he is the
compliance, so that it is seen … that paying taxes is the coach for his son’s Little League baseball team.
accepted and ethical pattern of behavior [emphasis added]’’ Larry prepares his own income tax return. Larry uses his
(p. 647). Further, they recommend that the government ‘‘avoid personal automobile for both business and personal rea-
leading individuals to think cheating is ‘okay’’’ and argue that sons. The tax laws provide that automobile expenses are
a tax amnesty program is an example of sending a wrong deductible to the extent the automobile is used for business.
message to taxpayers. Moreover, educational and media In preparing his income tax return, Larry determines that
programs designed to strengthen social norms and tax com- the automobile was used 60 % for business. However,
pliance behavior are likely to be more cost effective than Larry also calculates if he FALSELY claimed it was used
enforcement variables, as hiring additional IRS auditors, 80 % for business, his deduction would increase by $2,000,
lawyers and judges to increase tax compliance is expensive. and he would save $500 in taxes.
While the usefulness of moral suasion (such as governmental
moral or normative appeals) on tax compliance remains an Measurement of Individual and Attitudinal Control
open question (e.g., Torgler 2004; Hasseldine et al. 2007), as Variables
does the usefulness of moral appeals compared to educational
campaigns or increased penalties (e.g., Sanders et al. 2008), Perceived probability of detection was measured with
we feel that the comprehensive picture provided by our study several different items. Two of the responses were on a
on the role of social norms on tax compliance may enable 7-point Likert scale with 1 = very unlikely and 7 = very
researchers to reexamine the effectiveness of moral suasion likely. The items were: ‘‘If you claimed the ADDITIONAL
and other governmental tactics. $2,000 deduction in automobile expenses, how likely do
Future researchers may also wish to examine how social you think it would be that you would be audited by the
norms are affected by specific governmental actions or policy. IRS?’’(mean response of 3.91), and ‘‘If you claimed the
Taxpayers may have a positive view of some ‘‘good’’ gov- ADDITIONAL $2,000 deduction and were audited by the
ernment programs (e.g., interstate highways, public trans- IRS, how likely is it the deduction would be disallowed?’’
portation, national parks, and public health and food safety) (mean response of 5.30). We also measured participants’
compared to other ‘‘bad’’ government programs that are often perceptions of the percentage of taxpayers who ‘‘DO
characterized by waste, fraud, and inefficiencies. These per- FUDGE on their taxes’’ that are caught by the IRS; this
ceptions, too, may affect social norms and tax compliance item was measured on an 11-point scale ranging from 0 %
behavior. Similarly, all taxes are not viewed the same by (1) to 100 % (11) (average response of 3.58 out of 11,

123
Author's personal copy
Role of Social Norms 467

corresponding to *25 %). Finally, we also measured it was illegal or not,’’ with 1 = strongly disagree and
participants’ perceptions of the percentage chance that their 7 = strongly agree (mean response of 2.11).
most recent federal income tax return would be audited Familiarity (how often participants had taken an auto-
(average response of 14 %) and their perceptions of the mobile deduction in the past) was measured on a 7-point
percentage of all returns audited annually (average Likert scale with 1 = never and 7 = very often (mean
response of 19 %). response of 2.71).
Including all items on a scale together did not result in a
sufficient level of scale reliability (Cronbach’s alpha of
0.388). Factor analysis indicated two distinct constructs:
the general probability of being audited, and whether References
participants would be able to ‘‘get away’’ with cheating
(disallowance). As ‘‘overall audit rate’’ and ‘‘perceived Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational
likelihood of disallowance’’ had the highest item loadings Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 55, 179–211.
Alicke, M. D., Klotz, M. L., Breitenbecher, D., Yurak, T. J., &
on the rotated factors, these variables were retained for
Vredenburg, D. S. (1995). Personal contact, individuation, and
additional analysis. the better-than-average effect. Interpersonal Relations and
Risk aversion was measured on an 11-point Likert scale Group Processes, 68(5), 804–825.
as, ‘‘If you want to claim a deduction, but the tax law is Alm, J., McClelland, G., & Schulze, W. (1999). Changing the social
norm of tax compliance by voting. KYKLOS, 52(2), 141–171.
unclear, how CERTAIN (as a percentage) would you want
Alm, J., & McKee, M. (1998). Extending the lessons of laboratory
to be that the IRS would allow the deduction before you experiments on tax compliance to managerial and decision
would deduct it?’’ with 1 = 0 % and 11 = 100 % (mean economics. Managerial and Decision Economics, 19, 259–275.
response of 7.95). Alm, J., & Torgler, B. (2011). Do ethics matter: Tax compliance and
morality. Journal of Business Ethics, 101, 635–651.
Inclusive identification (sense of pride in the American
Andreoni, J., Erard, B., & Feinstein, J. (1998). Tax compliance.
community) was measured with two items; each item used Journal of Economic Literature, 36(2), 818–860.
a 7-point Likert scale with 1 = do not agree at all and Blanthorne, C., & Kaplan, S. (2008). An egocentric model of the
7 = agree completely: ‘‘Being a member of the American relations among the opportunity to underreport, social norms,
ethical beliefs, and underreporting behavior. Accounting, Orga-
community is important to me’’ (mean response of 6.56)
nizations and Society, 33(7/8), 684–703.
and ‘‘I feel a sense of pride in being a member of the Bobek, D., & Hatfield, R. (2003). An investigation of the theory of
American community’’ (mean response of 6.40). When planned behavior and the role of moral obligation in tax
including these two items together on a scale, Cronbach’s compliance. Behavioral Research in Accounting, 15, 13–38.
Bobek, D. D., Hageman, A. M., & Kelliher, C. F. (2011). The social
alpha is 0.885; thus, the sum of these two items was used in
norms of tax compliance: Scale development, social desirability
the analyses. and presentation effects. Advances in Accounting Behavioral
Fairness was originally measured with six items, coded Research, 15, 37–66.
with higher values indicating greater degrees of perceived Bobek, D. D., Roberts, R. W., & Sweeney, J. T. (2007). The social
norms of tax compliance: Evidence from Australia, Singapore,
unfairness: ‘‘1. Federal income taxes are fair to people in
and the United States. Journal of Business Ethics, 74(1), 49–64.
my particular income situation,’’ ‘‘2. Federal income tax Brown, A., & Moodie, C. (2009). The influence of tobacco marketing
laws are fair to most people,’’ ‘‘3. Compared to other on adolescent smoking intentions via normative beliefs. Health
people like me, I pay more than my fair share of taxes,’’ ‘‘4. Education Research, 24(4), 721–733.
Christensen, A. L., & Hite, P. A. (1997). A study of the effect of
I think that the interest of people in my income group are
taxpayer risk perceptions on ambiguous compliance decisions.
well represented when Congress considers tax law chan- Journal of the American Taxation Association, 19(1), 1–18.
ges,’’ ‘‘5. I think the federal income tax system benefits the Cialdini, R., & Goldstein, N. (2004). Social influence: Compliance
rich and is unfair to the working man and woman,’’ and ‘‘6. and conformity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 591–621.
Cialdini, R., & Trost, M. R. (1998). Social influence: Social norms,
Many people fudge on their taxes because they think the
conformity, and compliance. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G.
tax laws are unfair to them.’’ However, including all six Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology. Boston:
fairness items on a scale does not result in acceptable scale Oxford University Press.
validity (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.68). Reliability analysis Clapp, J., Lange, J., Russell, C., Shillington, A., & Voas, R. (2003). A
failed norms social marketing campaign. Journal of Studies on
indicates that only items 1, 2, and 4 should be included in
Alcohol, 64, 409–414.
the scale (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.79). Thus, the fairness Cowell, F. A. (1990). Cheating the Government. Cambridge, MA:
variable used in the analyses was the sum of the responses MIT Press.
to these three items. Crandall, C. (1988). Social contagion of binge eating. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 588–598.
Attitude toward additional money was measured on a
Davis, J., Hecht, G., & Perkins, J. (2003). Social behaviors,
7-point Likert scale as the response to, ‘‘I would really enforcement, and tax compliance dynamics. The Accounting
enjoy reducing my tax bill by $500, regardless of whether Review, 78(1), 39–69.

123
Author's personal copy
468 D. D. Bobek et al.

Goldstein, N., Cialdini, R., & Griskevicius, V. (2008). A room with a Sanders, D. L., Reckers, P. M. J., & Iyer, G. S. (2008). Influence of
viewpoint: Using social norms to motivate environmental accountability and penalty awareness on tax compliance. Journal
conservation in hotels. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(3), of the American Taxation Association, 30(2), 1–20.
472–482. Scholz, J., & Pinney, N. (1995). Duty, fear, and tax compliance: The
Hanno, D. M., & Violette, G. R. (1996). An analysis of moral and heuristic basis of citizenship behavior. American Journal of
social influences on taxpayer behavior. Behavioral Research in Political Science, 39(2), 490–512.
Accounting, 8, 57–75. Schultz, P., Nolan, J., Cialdini, R., Goldstein, N., & Griskevicius, V.
Hasseldine, J., Hite, P., James, S., & Toumi, M. (2007). Persuasive (2007). The constructive, destructive, and reconstructive power
communications: Tax compliance enforcement strategies for of social norms. Psychological Science, 18(5), 429–434.
sole proprietors. Contemporary Accounting Research, 24(1), Schumacker, R. E., & Lomax, R. G. (2004). A beginner’s guide to
171–194. structural equation modeling. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Hechter, M., & Opp, K. (Eds.). (2001). Social Norms. New York: Associates.
Russell Sage Foundation. Schwartz, S. H. (1977). Normative influence on altruism. In L.
Henderson, B., & Kaplan, S. (2005). An examination of the role of Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology
ethics in tax compliance decisions. Journal of the American (Vol. 10, pp. 221–279). New York: Academic Press.
Taxation Association, 27(1), 39–72. Slemrod, J., & Bakija, J. (2008). Taxing ourselves: A citizen’s guide
Kallgren, C., Reno, R., & Cialdini, R. (2000). A focus theory of to the debate over taxes (4th ed.). Cambridge, MA: The MIT
normative conduct: When norms do and do not affect behavior. Press.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(8), 1002–1012. Torgler, B. (2001). Is tax evasion never justifiable? Journal of Public
Kaplan, S., Newberry, K., & Reckers, P. M. (1997). The effect of Finance and Public Choice, XIX, 143–168.
moral reasoning and educational communications on tax evasion Torgler, B. (2002). Speaking to theorists and searching for facts: Tax
intentions. Journal of the American Taxation Association, 19(2), morale and tax compliance in experiments. Journal of Economic
38–54. Surveys, 16(5), 657–683.
Korobow, A., Johnson, C., & Axtell, R. (2007). An agent-based Torgler, B. (2004). Moral suasion: An alternative tax policy strategy?
model of tax compliance with social networks. National Tax Evidence from a controlled field experiment in Switzerland.
Journal, 60(3), 589–604. Economics of Governance, 5, 235–253.
Mason, J. D., & Levy, L. G. (2001). The use of the latent construct Torgler, B. (2007). Tax compliance and tax morale. Northampton,
method in behavioral accounting research: The measurement of MA: Edward Elgar Publishing.
client advocacy. Advances in Taxation, 13, 123–139. US Department of Treasury. (2009). Update on reducing the tax gap and
Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (1998). Internal and external motivation improving voluntary compliance (July 8). http://www.irs.gov/
to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social pub/newsroom/tax_gap_report_-final_version.pdf. Accessed 27
Psychology, 75(3), 811–832. July 2011.
Pommerehne, W., Hart, A., & Frey, B. (1994). Tax morale, tax Webley, P., Cole, M., & Eidjar, O. (2001). The prediction of self-reported
evasion, and the choice of tax policy instruments in different and hypothetical tax-evasion: Evidence from England France, and
political systems. Public Finance, 49(Supplement), 52–69. Norway. Journal of Economic Psychology, 22, 141–155.
Porcano, T., & Price, C. (1993). The effects of social stigmatization Weisbach, D. A., & Plesko, G. A. (2007). A legal perspective on
on tax evasion. Advances in Taxation, 5, 197–217. unanswered questions in tax research. The Journal of the
Randall, D. M., & Fernandes, M. (1991). The social desirability American Taxation Association, 29(2), 107–113.
response bias in ethics research. Journal of Business Ethics, Wenzel, M. (2004). An analysis of norm processes in tax compliance.
10(11), 805–817. Journal of Economic Psychology, 25, 213–228.
Reckers, P. M. J., Sanders, D. L., & Roark, S. J. (1994). The influence Wenzel, M. (2005). Motivation or rationalization? Causal relations
of ethical attitudes on taxpayer compliance. National Tax between ethics, norms, and tax compliance. Journal of Economic
Journal, 47(4), 825–836. Psychology, 26, 491–508.

123

View publication stats

You might also like