You are on page 1of 16

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/225359859

Sexual Self-Esteem in American and British College Women: Relations with


Self-Objectification and Eating Problems

Article in Sex Roles · February 2009


DOI: 10.1007/s11199-008-9517-0

CITATIONS READS

74 1,410

2 authors:

Rachel M Calogero Joel Kevin Thompson


Western University University of South Florida
103 PUBLICATIONS 6,132 CITATIONS 311 PUBLICATIONS 29,502 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

All content following this page was uploaded by Rachel M Calogero on 30 June 2015.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Sex Roles (2009) 60:160–173
DOI 10.1007/s11199-008-9517-0

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Sexual Self-Esteem in American and British College Women:


Relations with Self-Objectification and Eating Problems
Rachel M. Calogero & J. Kevin Thompson

Published online: 3 August 2008


# Springer Science + Business Media, LLC 2008

Abstract The present study extended Objectification Introduction


Theory (Fredrickson and Roberts, Psychol Women Q
21:173–206, 1997) to test the role of sexual self-esteem in According to Objectification Theory (Fredrickson and
models of disordered eating. Measures of self-objectification, Roberts 1997), women’s exposure to chronic sexual
sexual well-being, and disordered eating were completed by objectification can lead to chronic self-objectification,
American (N=104) and British (N=111) college women. In which, in turn, has been associated with a variety of
Study 1, higher self-objectification was associated with negative consequences for women’s physical and mental
lower sexual self-esteem, which, in turn, mediated the well-being. The present research tests an understudied
relationship between self-objectification and disordered proposition of Objectification Theory which states that
eating in American women. In Study 2, path analyses self-objectification may be associated with a variety of
indicated that self-objectification led to sexual self-esteem negative consequences for women’s sexual well-being. The
and body shame, which led to disordered eating in British central purpose of the present research was to provide
women. This pattern of results was replicated, albeit preliminary, cross-sectional investigations of the relation-
weaker, when sexual self-competence replaced sexual self- ship between self-objectification and women’s sexual well-
esteem in the model. Discussion considers the significance being, and, in turn, to examine if this relationship predicts
of self-objectification and sexual self-esteem for women’s disordered eating among women from two different highly
well-being. developed Westernized societies. This research aims to
build on the extant literature by presenting two studies that
Keywords Self-objectification . Sexual self-esteem . conducted a new test of Objectification Theory as it relates
Sexual self-competence . Disordered eating to women’s sexual well-being and disordered eating.
Being sexually objectified is a pervasive aspect of girls’
and women’s social lives in Westernized societies (Bartky
1990; Calogero et al. 2007; Huebner and Fredrickson 1999;
Swim et al. 2001; Thompson et al. 1999). Indeed, despite
the heterogeneity among women with regard to societal
Parts of this manuscript were presented at the International Conference background, ethnicity, class, sexuality, and age, “having a
of Eating Disorders in Barcelona, Spain, June 2006. reproductively mature body may create a shared social
experience, a vulnerability to sexual objectification, which
R. M. Calogero (*)
Department of Psychology, Keynes College, University of Kent, in turn may create a shared set of psychological experi-
CT2 7NP Canterbury, UK ences” (Fredrickson and Roberts 1997, p. 3). Experiences
e-mail: R.M.Calogero@kent.ac.uk of sexual objectification occur at a very young age, with a
disturbing 75% of American elementary school girls
J. K. Thompson
Department of Psychology, PCD 4118, University of South Florida, reporting experiences of sexual harassment (Murnen and
Tampa, FL 33620-8200, USA Smolak 2000). Particularly insidious is the exposure to
Sex Roles (2009) 60:160–173 161

sexualized media environments that objectify women, Australian samples of women to demonstrate that (a) self-
which has been linked to the development of adolescent objectification directly contributes to particular subjective
girls’ and boys’ notions of women as sex objects (American experiences among women, including body shame, appear-
Psychological Association, Task Force on the Sexualization ance anxiety, and lower interoceptive awareness (e.g.,
of Girls 2007; Grogan and Wainwright 1996; Harper and Calogero 2004; Noll and Fredrickson 1998; Tiggemann
Tiggemann 2008; Peter and Valkenberg 2007; Ward and and Slater 2001; Tylka and Hill 2004), and (b) self-
Friedman 2006). Moreover, these experiences of objectifi- objectification directly and indirectly (via these subjective
cation endure in the lives of women, with research from a experiences) contributes to women’s disordered eating (e.g.,
nationally representative cohort of middle-aged women in Calogero et al. 2005; Fredrickson et al. 1998; Slater and
Britain demonstrating that body-related comments received Tiggemann 2002; Tiggemann and Slater 2001), thereby
in childhood continue to be associated with low body supporting several tenets of Objectification Theory.
esteem across the life span (McLaren et al. 2004). While this evidence highlights self-objectification and
Over a decade ago, Fredrickson and Roberts (1997) several of its related consequences as the critical psycho-
offered Objectification Theory as a framework for system- logical links between experiences of sexual objectification
atically testing how exposure to this chronic sexual and eating disturbances (e.g., Fredrickson et al. 1998;
objectification negatively and disproportionately affects Kozee et al. 2007; Moradi et al. 2005; Tiggemann and
multiple dimensions of women’s lives. According to Slater 2001; Tylka and Hill 2004), there remains significant
Objectification Theory, encounters with sexual objectifica- variance unaccounted for in these models of disordered
tion socialize girls and women to internalize an objectifying eating. It is clear that other psychological mechanisms must
gaze such that they come to view their bodies from an be operating on the relationship between self-objectification
objectifying observers’ or third-person perspective instead of and disordered eating. Given that Objectification Theory
a first-person perspective, referred to as self-objectification. predicts that self-objectification triggers both poor sexual
Importantly, this self-perspective does not merely reflect functioning and disordered eating (Fredrickson and Roberts
social comparison with others, or the fact that women 1997), the present research offers a preliminary exploration
simply do not like the size or shape of their bodies, but of the sexual consequences of self-objectification as an
actually reflects a view of the body as belonging “less to additional psychological mechanism related to women’s
them and more to others” (Fredrickson and Roberts 1997, disordered eating.
p. 193) because women learn that it is normative for their North American and British researchers have documented
bodies to be looked at, commented on, evaluated, and that women’s sexuality and sexual lives are intertwined with
sexually harassed by others. This particular self-perspective their identities as women, suggesting that sociocultural
is considered to be the primary psychological consequence factors contribute to the development of women’s sexual
for girls and women living in a culture that sexually self-image (Althof et al. 2005; Andersen and Cyranowski
objectifies women’s bodies. Women’s habitual monitoring 1994; Lavie and Willig 2005; Oliver and Hyde 1993; Tiefer
of the body’s external appearance (i.e., body surveillance) 2001). Fredrickson and Roberts (1997) proposed that self-
is the primary manifestation of self-objectification and it is objectification may trigger a variety of negative consequen-
the proposed mechanism by which self-objectification ces for women’s sexual well-being, including negative
exerts its negative effects on women’s mental and physical feelings about the sexual aspects of the self, sexual
health (e.g., Fredrickson and Roberts 1997; Slater and dissatisfaction, and/or sexual dysfunction. Indeed, because
Tiggemann 2002). the sexual self almost invariably involves the body, self-
Trait and state levels of self-objectification have been objectification may be particularly relevant to women’s
associated with a variety of negative emotional and sexual well-being. This proposal raises the possibility that
cognitive consequences (i.e., body shame, appearance the degree to which women like themselves as sexual
anxiety, diminished internal bodily awareness, and de- beings, value their own sexuality, and accept their sexuality
creased opportunities for attaining peak motivational states) as part of their self-concept is another direct consequence of
and increased mental health risks (i.e., depression, sexual self-objectification. Recent empirical evidence from a
dysfunction, and disordered eating). In particular, the sample of Australian women suggests that objectification
pervasive sexual objectification of women, and resultant theory is a valid framework for the study of women’s sexual
self-objectification, is one explanation for the disproportion- functioning (Steer and Tiggemann 2008). Thus, it is possible
ate rate of eating disorders among women from Westernized that the more that women view themselves as sexual objects
societies (Calogero et al. 2005; Fredrickson and Roberts for men’s pleasure, the more likely they are to hold negative
1997; McKinley and Hyde 1996; Striegel-Moore and views of their sexual selves and sexual worth.
Smolak 2001; Thompson et al. 1999). Considerable empir- Moreover, considerable empirical evidence supports a
ical research has accumulated from North American and link between sexuality and eating disorder symptomatology
162 Sex Roles (2009) 60:160–173

among women in Western cultures (e.g., Evans and Katz and Farrow 2000; O’Sullivan et al. 2006; Rosenthal et
Wertheim 1998; Rodriguez et al. 2007; Wiederman 1996; al. 1991; Snell and Papini 1989; Wiederman and Allgeier
Wiederman et al. 1996). In particular, there is evidence 1993; Zeanah and Schwarz 1996). For example, O’Sullivan
indicating that women who report higher body dissatisfac- et al. (2006) created and utilized a modified version of
tion and other eating disorder symptoms also report less Rosenthal et al.’s measure of sexual self-esteem, which was
comfort with their sexual selves. For example, several previously adapted from Offer (1969) and Marsh’s (1986)
researchers have demonstrated that greater dissatisfaction research on global self-esteem. O’Sullivan et al. indicated
with sexual performance, sexual encounters, and the body’s that they used seven of the original 18 items from
sexual parts is associated with more eating disorder Rosenthal et al.’s measure to assess girls’ esteem regarding
symptomatology in samples of American women (Ackard their ability to attract a sexual partner. However, some of
et al. 2000; Cash 2002; Raciti and Hendrick 1992; the sample items provided for this measure could be
Wiederman et al. 1996). Moreover, a positive relationship considered as representing a more general conception of
has been demonstrated between body satisfaction and the sexual self-esteem (“I feel comfortable with my sexuality”)
frequency of masturbation, at least among European or even a different construct such as body satisfaction (“I
American women, suggesting that more positive feelings am proud of my body”) instead of esteem related to the
about the body may be linked with more comfort with their ability to attract a sexual partner. The point here is not to
sexual selves (Ellison 2000; Shulman and Horne 2003). critique this particular measure per se, but rather to
More focused research examining both general and highlight a broader issue which is that the distinctions
sexual components of self-esteem raises the possibility that between different components of sexual self-esteem (and
sexual self-esteem is uniquely associated with body image sometimes between different constructs) are often blurred
and eating problems. The concept of sexual self-esteem, across studies. Despite the obvious agreement that sexual
elaborated largely from models of global self-esteem self-esteem is an important aspect of women’s well-being,
(Gaynor and Underwood 1995; Rosenthal et al. 1991; the research on sexual self-esteem is difficult to consolidate
Zeanah and Schwarz 1996), has been described as an because of the variability and inconsistency in the mea-
individual’s sense of self as a sexual being, and includes the surement of the concept.
value that individuals’ place on their sexual identity and The present research cannot circumvent these more
sexual acceptability (Hendrick and Hendrick 1983; Mayers systemic problems entirely. However, one clear intention
et al. 2003; Snell and Papini 1989; Zeanah and Schwarz of the present research was to assess self-evaluations about
1996). Beyond the fact that low general self-esteem is an one’s sexuality generally without an emphasis on either
established risk factor for eating disorders (Fairburn et al. sexual competence or in relation to a sexual partner. For
1999), researchers have shown that low general self-esteem example, many of the items in Snell and Papini’s (1989)
mediates the relationship between sexually-objectifying sexual self-esteem subscale refer to one’s self-evaluation as
experiences and eating disorder symptoms (Harned and a sexual partner. According to Wiederman and Allgeier
Fitzgerald 2002; Mayers et al. 2003; Pitts and Waller 1993). (1993), “It may be argued that the definition of sexual
More specifically, Raciti and Hendrick (1992) demonstrated esteem given by Snell and Papini (1989) is too narrow to
a significant negative relationship between sexual self- allow for wide applicability (e.g., individuals who have not
esteem and disordered eating among American female had a sexual partner)” (p. 99). Indeed, while these items
undergraduates, such that women with lower sexual self- may represent some aspects of the sexual self-concept,
esteem also reported more disordered eating attitudes and these items also assume some degree of interpersonal
behaviors. More than a decade later, Weaver and Byers sexual experience to provide a basis for people’s evalua-
(2006) demonstrated similar patterns in a large sample of tions of themselves on these dimensions. Moreover, a
Canadian college women, such that high body dissatisfac- woman’s sexual self-esteem is arguably more complex and
tion and high situational body image dysphoria was broader than her perception of her ability to sexually attract
associated with lower sexual self-esteem across a variety a partner. Thus, an important focus of the present effort was
of social and non-social situations. In sum, this research to measure sexual self-esteem as general feelings about the
raises the possibility that sexual self-esteem is uniquely sexual self independent of actual interpersonal sexual
related to eating problems. experiences or sexual performance. Yet, it could be argued
Like many psychological constructs, the construct of that feeling competent as a sexual partner may also be
sexual self-esteem has been measured in a variety of ways. important in the relationship between self-objectification
Several adapted versions of sexual self-esteem scales have and disordered eating. That is, self-objectification may also
been utilized in the literature, with many of them explicitly be linked to individuals’ beliefs about one’s competency
elaborated from models and measures of global self-esteem and skill as a sexual partner, which in turn, may trigger
(Fortenberry et al. 2005; Gaynor and Underwood 1995; disordered eating. Since this is a new area of inquiry, it
Sex Roles (2009) 60:160–173 163

would be important also to test these relations using a more Howard 2004; Grogan 2006; Ussher 1989), even in girls as
specific and well-validated measure of sexual competence young as 8 years old (Grogan and Wainwright 1996). One
in addition to the measure of sexual self-esteem. recent study has provided some preliminary evidence
In the present research, two studies were conducted to suggesting that British women do report comparable levels
test the relations between women’s self-objectification and of self-objectification to American and Australian women
sexual well-being as they relate to disordered eating. The and comparable relations between self-objectification and
goal of the first study was to extend existing research on disordered eating with body shame as a significant mediator
Objectification Theory by testing the relationship between of these relations (Calogero, manuscript submitted for
self-objectification and sexual self-esteem, and, in turn, the publication). Moreover, the British Medical Association
potential mediating role of sexual self-esteem in the has identified women’s exposure to objectified and ultra
relationship between self-objectification and disordered thin images of women in the British media as playing a
eating in a sample of American college women. That is, significant role in the promotion of eating disorders (British
similar to the patterns observed among self-objectification, Medical Association 2000). Despite these apparent similar-
body shame, and disordered eating in prior research with ities in exposure to sociocultural appearance pressures
American and Australian women (e.g., Noll and Fredrickson among women representing different highly developed
1998; Tiggemann and Slater 2001), it was expected that Westernized societies (Calogero et al. 2007; Orbach 1993;
sexual self-esteem would mediate the relationship between Thompson et al. 1999), the research findings from
self-objectification and disordered eating, thereby providing American and/or Australian samples of women, which
an indirect link between these phenomena. That is, some have dominated the objectification literature, cannot neces-
individuals may come to feel that the sexual aspects of the sarily be generalized to women in other Westernized
self are undesirable and/or worthless in response to self- societies for which these objectification processes may
objectification (i.e., lower sexual self-esteem), and thus apply. Such tests of the proposed pathways in women
come to feel that they want to change their size and shape representing two different Westernized societies are impor-
more directly to improve their sexual self-image and/or be tant for increasing our theoretical understanding of objec-
viewed as more sexually desirable. This idea that feelings of tification processes and consequences. Therefore, the tests
low sexual self-esteem and high sexual dissatisfaction may of the proposed relations in the second study were
evoke control behaviors in the form of eating disorder conducted with a sample of British college women.
symptoms to alleviate the negative feelings is not new (e.g.,
Polivy and Herman 2002; Troop 1998); however, to date,
Study 1
the role of sexual self-esteem in the context of Objectifica-
tion Theory has not been examined.
The first study attempted to combine research in women’s
The goal of the second study was to extend Objectifi-
self-objectification, sexual well-being, and disordered eat-
cation Theory by testing a causal path model of self-
ing to provide a novel test of the relationship between self-
objectification, sexual self-esteem, and disordered eating (a)
objectification and sexual well-being. There were two aims
in a sample of women from a different Westernized society
of this study. The first aim was to test the zero-order
(Britain), (b) when the robust relations between self-
correlations between sexual self-esteem, self-objectifica-
objectification and body shame with disordered eating have
tion, and disordered eating in a sample of American college
been accounted for in the model (e.g., Noll and Fredrickson
women. The second aim was to test a mediational model of
1998; Tiggemann and Kuring 2004; Tiggemann and Lynch
self-objectification, sexual self-esteem, and disordered
2001; Tiggemann and Slater 2001; Tylka and Hill 2004),
eating in a sample of American college women in
and (c) when a different measure of women’s sexual well-
accordance with the steps outlined by Baron and Kenny
being is included in the model instead of sexual self-
(1986) for testing mediational models using a series of
esteem, which we refer to as sexual self-competence (Snell
multiple regression analyses (see Fig. 1). Five hypotheses
and Papini 1989; Wiederman and Allgeier 1993).
were tested in the present study:
It is important to highlight that while numerous studies
have supported many of the propositions of Objectification Hypothesis 1: Sexual self-esteem will be negatively
Theory in samples of American and Australian women, correlated with self-objectification and dis-
there is a dearth of research available examining specific ordered eating whereas self-objectification
propositions of Objectification Theory in British women, will be positively correlated with disor-
despite the high levels of body dissatisfaction and disor- dered eating.
dered eating in response to sociocultural pressures that have Hypothesis 2: Self-objectification (the initial variable)
been documented consistently in samples of British women will predict disordered eating (the out-
(e.g., Charles and Kerr 1986; Dittmar 2005; Dittmar and come variable).
164 Sex Roles (2009) 60:160–173

Sexual Inventory-2 (Garner 1991) were used, in combination, to


Self-Esteem tap a general construct that focuses on attitudes that are
associated with a greater risk for eating disorders. The
Self- Disordered Drive for Thinness subscale contains seven items that
Objectification Eating assess fear of weight gain and weight preoccupation (e.g.,
“I am terrified of gaining weight”). The Bulimia subscale
contains seven items that assess the tendency to engage in
Fig. 1 Proposed pathways for the mediational model with self- and/or think about engaging in uncontrollable episodes of
objectification, general sexual self-esteem, and disordered eating in overeating (e.g., “I stuff myself with food”). The Body
Study 1. Solid lines indicate direct links between variables whereas Dissatisfaction subscale contains nine items that assess
dashed lines indicate indirect links between variables.
dissatisfaction with specific body parts and overall weight
(e.g., “I think that my hips are too big”). Participants were
Hypothesis 3: Self-objectification will predict sexual self- asked to rate each item from 1 (never) to 6 (always). All 23
esteem (the mediator). items were summed to create a composite measure of
Hypothesis 4: Self-objectification will predict disordered disordered eating, with higher scores indicating more
eating when sexual self-esteem is controlled. disordered eating. In this study, the entire range of possible
Hypothesis 5: Sexual self-esteem will partially mediate the scores was used as recommended for non-clinical samples
relationship between self-objectification and (Schoemaker et al. 1994). This composite measure of eating
disordered eating. disorder symptomatology is a well-known method for
assessing disordered eating in non-clinical samples, dem-
onstrating good psychometric properties (Adkins and Keel
Method 2005; Tiggemann and Lynch 2001; Welch et al. 1988).
High internal reliability was demonstrated in the present
Participants study (α=.95).

One hundred four undergraduate women from a north Self-objectification The Body Surveillance subscale of the
eastern American university were recruited from under- Objectified Body Consciousness Scale (McKinley and
graduate psychology courses and given course credit for Hyde 1996) was used to measure the degree to which
their participation. The power to detect a large effect individuals view their bodies as an outside observer, thus
(R2 =.40) at an alpha level of .01 with a sample size of focusing more on how their bodies look than on how their
104 is approximately 1.00 (Cohen 1988, 1992). Therefore, bodies feel. Participants were asked to rate 8 items from 1
this sample size was considered to have sufficient power to (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree), such as “I rarely
detect actual effects of the variables tested in this research. worry about how I look to other people” (reverse scored), “I
Mean age was 18.63 years (SD=1.14), ranging from 17 to am more concerned with what my body can do than how it
27. Mean self-reported weight was 59.51 kg (SD=9.44) or looks” (reverse scored), “During the day, I think about how
131.20 lbs (SD=20.81), ranging from 45.36 to 90.72 kg I look many times”. Higher scores indicate frequent
(100 to 200 lbs). Mean self-reported height was 1.66 m monitoring of one’s appearance and thoughts about how
(SD=.07) or 65 in. (SD=2.76), ranging from 1.50 to the body looks. High internal reliability (α=.89) and good
1.88 m (59 to 74 in.). Mean body mass index (BMI) was construct validity have been demonstrated (McKinley and
21.71 (SD=3.53). The ethnic composition of the sample Hyde 1996). High internal reliability was demonstrated in
included 87% European American (n=91), 10% African the present study (α=.88). Based on findings that incorpo-
American (n=10), and 3% Asian American (n=3) women. rated two different measures of self-objectification as
predictors of disordered eating, Tiggemann and Slater
Measure (2001) have recommended using McKinley and Hyde’s
specific measure of body surveillance as the manifestation
Background information Participants provided information of self-objectification opposed to the general measure of
about age, ethnicity, year in school, weight, and height. self-objectification (see Noll and Fredrickson 1998) when
Body mass index (BMI) was calculated for each participant testing for unique relations with eating disorder constructs.
with the formula kilogram/square meters (Garrow and
Webster 1985). Sexual self-esteem A modified version of Rosenberg’s
Global Self-Esteem Scale (Rosenberg 1986) was used to
Disordered eating The Drive for Thinness, Bulimia, and measure the degree to which individuals feel positively or
Body Dissatisfaction subscales of the Eating Disorder negatively about the sexual aspects of their self-concept.
Sex Roles (2009) 60:160–173 165

Table 1 Zero-order correlations among Study 1 variables. To test the mediational model, a series of multiple
BMI SSE SO DE regression analyses were conducted to estimate the paths
among self-objectification, sexual self-esteem, and disor-
BMI – −.20* .17 .36** dered eating. Following the steps outlined by Baron and
SSE – – −.56** −.50** Kenny (1986), it was tested whether (1) self-objectification
SO – – – .67**
(the initial variable) predicts disordered eating (the outcome
M (SD) 21.71 (3.53) 2.09 (.46) 3.95 (1.02) 3.03 (.87)
variable), (2) self-objectification predicts sexual self-esteem
Higher scores indicate higher levels of each construct. (the mediator), (3) self-objectification predicts disordered
SSE sexual self-esteem (1–4); SO self-objectification (1–7); DE eating when sexual self-esteem is controlled, and (4) sexual
disordered eating (1–6); BMI body mass index self-esteem partially mediates the relation between self-
*p<.05; **p<.01
objectification and disordered eating. According to Baron
and Kenny, to establish significant mediation, the effect of
Sexual self-esteem was defined as people’s self-evaluations self-objectification on disordered eating must be less in the
of their sexual qualities and value as a sexual person in third equation compared to the first equation. Sobel’s
general. Ten items were adapted to focus on general sexual formula provided a conservative and more formal media-
aspects of the self-concept: (a) “My sexual self makes me tion test to determine whether the indirect effect was
feel like a person of worth, at least on an equal plane with significantly different from zero (Baron and Kenny 1986;
others,” (b) “I feel that I have a number of good sexual MacKinnon et al. 2002; Preacher and Hayes 2004; Preacher
qualities,” (c) “All in all, I am inclined to feel that I am a and Leonardelli 2007).
failure sexually,” (d) “I am able to be as sexual as most As shown in Fig. 2, all of the steps in the mediational
other people,” (e) “I am not proud of my sexual self,” (f) “I analysis indicated nonzero beta coefficients. In the first step,
take a positive attitude toward my self as a sexual being,” self-objectification predicted disordered eating, F(1,102)=
(g) “On the whole, I am satisfied with the sexual aspects of 84.07, p<.001 (Hypothesis 2), accounting for 45% of the
myself,” (h) “I wish I could have more respect for myself as variance. In the second step, self-objectification predicted
a sexual person,” (i) “I certainly feel useless at times sexual self-esteem, F(1,102)=47.51, p<.001 (Hypothesis 3),
sexually,” (j) “At times I think my sexual self is no good at accounting for 32% of the variance. In the third and fourth
all.” Participants were asked to rate these 10 items from 1 step, the relationship between self-objectification and
(strongly agree) to 4 (strongly disagree), with higher scores disordered eating was partially mediated by sexual self-
indicating more positive sexual self-esteem. An exploratory esteem (indirect effect=.10, z=2.04, p<.05) (Hypothesis 3),
factor analysis on this adapted sexual self-esteem scale and self-objectification remained a significant predictor
revealed that one factor was the best solution, with no of disordered eating after controlling sexual self-esteem
individual factor loading lower than .43. High internal (sr2 =.11) (Hypothesis 4). The overall mediational model
reliability was demonstrated in the present study (α=.85). accounting for 47% of the variance in disordered eating,
R2 =.47, F(2,101)=45.57, p<.001, power=1.00.
Consistent with Objectification Theory and the predictions
Procedure for the present study, higher self-objectification was associ-
ated with lower sexual self-esteem. In addition, lower sexual
Study sessions were facilitated by a female experimenter in self-esteem was also associated with more eating disorder
groups of five to ten women. Upon arrival participants read symptomatology, which is consistent with prior research
a brief description of the study and then provided informed linking low sexual self-esteem to body image and eating
consent to participate. Participants completed the self-report
measures described above in counterbalanced order, pro-
viding background information (i.e., age, ethnicity, weight,
Sexual
and height) at the end of the study. Upon completion of the ß = -.56** Self-Esteem ß = -.18*
measures, participants were fully debriefed.
Self- Disordered
Results and Discussion Objectification Eating
ß = .67**

Means, standard deviations, and zero-order correlations are


presented in Table 1. Consistent with predictions, sexual self- ß = .57**
esteem was negatively correlated with self-objectification Fig. 2 Beta coefficients for the pathways among self-objectification,
and disordered eating whereas self-objectification was general sexual self-esteem, and disordered eating in Study 1. *p<.05;
positively correlated with disordered eating (Hypothesis 1). **p<.001.
166 Sex Roles (2009) 60:160–173

problems (Raciti and Hendrick 1992; Weaver and Byers In this study, path analyses were planned to test two
2006). The mediational analysis further delineated these causal models of disordered eating, with a weak causal
relationships, such that sexual self-esteem partially mediated ordering of variables that was consistent with the assump-
the relationship between self-objectification and disordered tions of Objectification Theory (Fredrickson and Roberts
eating; thereby demonstrating that self-objectification may 1997) and findings from previous research with other
exert its effects on eating problems directly or indirectly via samples (Raciti and Hendrick 1992; Sanchez and Kiefer
lower sexual self-esteem. Thus, it appears that for some 2007; Tiggemann and Kuring 2004; Tiggemann and Slater
women who self-objectify, the sexual components of the self 2001). Although some of these relationships are likely
may be emphasized, and for these women it is their feelings reciprocal (e.g., self-objectification affects sexual self-esteem
about the sexual self that become associated with negative as much as sexual self-esteem affects self-objectification),
evaluations of one’s body and eating problems. These reciprocal causality cannot be statistically tested in correla-
findings offer the first evidence for a relationship between tional designs. Thus, the present study tested and hypothe-
self-objectification and women’s sexual self-esteem, and for sized the direction of causality that was most consistently
their joint effects on disordered eating. discussed in the literature.
The first path diagram was constructed such that self-
objectification was theorized to lead to body shame (e.g.,
Study 2
Tiggemann and Slater 2001) and sexual self-esteem, body
shame was theorized to lead to sexual self-esteem (e.g.,
The second study attempted to provide further evidence of
Sanchez and Kiefer 2007), and in turn, both body shame
the relations between self-objectification and women’s
and sexual self-esteem were theorized to lead to eating
sexual well-being in a different Westernized sample of
problems (e.g., Raciti and Hendrick 1992; Tiggemann and
women, that of British college women. In this study, two
Kuring 2004; Weaver and Byers 2006) (see Fig. 3a). In this
path diagrams were constructed as a descriptive and
path model, sexual self-esteem was conceptualized as an
analytical tool to test the proposed causal models. Path
affective and experiential consequence of self-objectification
analysis is a technique which uses a least-squares approach
that may trigger disordered eating, which is consistent with
to estimate path coefficients from regression equations.
the Objectification Theory framework whereby subjective
Standardized partial regression coefficients for these path-
experiences such as body shame and interoceptive aware-
ways estimated the direct effects of these variables from
ness are considered to be experiential consequences of self-
multiple regression analyses where each variable is
objectification which trigger disordered eating (Fredrickson
regressed on all variables assumed to be causally prior,
and Roberts 1997; Slater and Tiggemann 2002). Based
thereby controlling for all prior variables (Bryman and
on this theoretical model, five specific hypotheses were
Cramer 1990; Pedhazur 1997). A full saturated model,
tested:
described by multiple R2, was estimated with all possible
direct and indirect pathways to test the mediating effects. Hypothesis 6: Self-objectification will predict body shame.

Fig. 3 a Proposed path diagram a


of women’s disordered eating Sexual Self-
with sexual self-esteem. Esteem
b Proposed path diagram of
women’s disordered eating with
sexual self-competence. Body
Self- Disordered
Objectification Shame Eating

b
Sexual Self-
Competence

Self- Body Disordered


Objectification Shame Eating
Sex Roles (2009) 60:160–173 167

Hypothesis 7: Self-objectification will predict sexual Method


self-esteem.
Hypothesis 8: Body shame will predict sexual self-esteem. Participants and Procedure
Hypothesis 9: Body shame will predict disordered eating.
Hypothesis 10: Sexual self-esteem will predict disordered One hundred eleven undergraduate women from a south
eating. eastern British university were recruited through advertise-
ments around campus and announcements via the psychol-
ogy department’s website. A total of 68 women completed
The second path diagram was constructed such that self- the measures in the Autumn term and 43 women completed
objectification was theorized to lead to body shame (e.g., the measures in the Spring term. A series of independent
Tiggemann and Slater 2001) and sexual self-competence, sample t-tests revealed no significant differences between
body shame was theorized to lead to sexual self-competence the two samples for any of the study variables: self-
(e.g., Sanchez and Kiefer 2007), and in turn, both body objectification, t(109)=−1.04, p=.30, body shame, t(109)=
shame and sexual self-competence were theorized to lead −.38, p=.70, sexual self-esteem, t(109)=.04, p=.97, sexual
to eating problems (e.g., Raciti and Hendrick 1992; self-competence, t(109)=.40, p=.67, disordered eating, t
Tiggemann and Kuring 2004; Weaver and Byers 2006) (109)=−1.54, p=.18, and BMI, t(109)=.34, p=.74. All
(see Fig. 3b). In this path model, sexual self-competence participants were given course credit or received five
was conceptualized as beliefs about one’s competency and pounds for their participation. The power to detect a large
skill as a sexual partner (Seal et al. 1997; Snell and Papini effect (R2 =.40) at an alpha level of .01 with a sample size
1989), which does not contain the same affective compo- of 111 is approximately 1.00 (Cohen 1988, 1992).
nent as sexual self-esteem. It was expected that sexual self- Therefore, this sample size was considered to have
esteem would be more strongly related to disordered eating sufficient power to detect actual effects of the variables
than sexual self-competence because research has indicated tested in this research.
that eating disorder symptoms may be adpoted in order to Mean age was 22.00 years (SD=3.81), ranging from 18
alleviate the negative feelings associated with low sexual to 39. Mean self-reported weight was 69.04 kg (SD=
self-esteem. Women may be attempting to manage how 12.21), ranging from 42 to 90 kg. Mean self-reported height
they feel about themselves as sexual beings (or as sexual was 1.72 m (SD=.13), ranging from 1.50 to 2.00 m. Mean
objects if high self-objectifiers) by engaging in more drastic BMI was 21.89 (SD=1.11). The ethnic composition of the
means to manipulate the body (e.g., starvation and/or binge- sample was 64.9% White British (n=72), 4.5% Black
purge practices) in order to attain the thin beauty ideal British (n=5), 14.4% Asian (n=16), 12.6% Southern
(Fredrickson and Roberts 1997; McCarthy 1990). Based on European (n=14), and 3.6% American (n=4).
this theoretical model, five specific hypotheses were tested: The same procedure from Study 1 was followed in Study
2. Two exceptions were the inclusion of a measure of body
Hypothesis 11: Self-objectification will predict body shame.
shame and a measure of sexual self-competence in the
Hypothesis 12: Self-objectification will predict sexual self-
packet of measures. The Body Shame subscale of the
competence.
Objectified Body Consciousness Scale (McKinley and
Hypothesis 13: Body shame will predict disordered eating.
Hyde 1996) was used to measure the degree to which
Hypothesis 14: Body shame will not predict sexual self-
individuals’ feel shame about their bodies when they
competence.
perceive themselves as not meeting cultural appearance
Hypothesis 15: Sexual self-competence will predict disor-
standards. Participants were asked to rate eight items from 1
dered eating.
(strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree), with higher scores
To summarize, the hypotheses for both path analyses indicating higher body shame (e.g., “When I’m not the size
were identical, except that (a) somewhat weaker relations I think I should be, I feel ashamed,” “I feel like I must be a
were expected between sexual self-competence and disor- bad person when I don’t look as good as I could,” and “I
dered eating compared to sexual self-esteem and disordered would be ashamed for people to know what I really
eating and (b) body shame was not expected to lead to weigh”). High internal reliability (α = .89) and good
sexual self-competence. BMI was also included as a construct validity have been demonstrated previously
covariate in these models to control the effects of variations (McKinley and Hyde 1996). High internal reliability was
in body size on the study variables. In addition, it was demonstrated in the present study (α=.82). A validated
hypothesized that the measures of sexual self-esteem and short form of the Sexual Self-Esteem subscale of Snell and
sexual self-competence would be positively correlated with Papini’s (1989) Sexuality Scale was used to measure the
each other and negatively correlated with self-objectification, degree to which individuals’ felt competent and skilled as a
body shame, and disordered eating (Hypothesis 16). sexual partner, referred to as sexual self-competence
168 Sex Roles (2009) 60:160–173

Table 2 Zero-order correlations among Study 2 variables.

BMI SSC SSE SO BS DE

BMI – .07 −.05 −.14 −.07 −.07


SSC – – .57** −.36** −.12 −.33**
SSE – – – −.38** −.35** −.45**
SO – – – – .44** .50**
BS – – – – – .59**
M(SD) 21.87 (1.11) .38 (.66) 2.28 (.55) 4.17 (1.18) 3.23 (1.10) 2.82 (1.02)

Higher scores indicate higher levels of each construct.


SSC sexual self-competence (−2–2); SSE sexual self-esteem (1–4); SO self-objectification (1–7); BS body shame (1–7), DE disordered eating
(1–6); BMI body mass index
*p<.05; **p<.01

(Wiederman and Allgeier 1993). Participants rated five ing that the relationship between self-objectification and
items about how they feel and think about themselves as a disordered eating was only partially mediated by body
sexual partner from −2 (disagree) to 2 (agree), with higher shame and sexual self-esteem. The full model described
scores indicating higher sexual self-competence: (a) “I 56% of the variance in women’s disordered eating, R2 =.56,
think of myself as a good sexual partner,” (b) “I would F(3,110)=40.98, p<.001, power=1.00.
rate my sexual skill quite highly,” (c) “I think of myself as Figure 4b depicts the significant pathways for the model
a very good sexual partner,” (d) “I would rate myself low as with sexual self-competence. It can be seen from the path
a sexual partner,” and (e) “I am confident about myself as a diagram that similar non-zero pathways emerged. Specifi-
sexual partner.” High internal reliability (α=.94) and good cally, self-objectification led directly to body shame
construct validity have been demonstrated previously (Hypothesis 11), which in turn, was directly linked to
(Wiederman and Allgeier 1993). High internal reliability disordered eating (Hypothesis 13) but not to sexual self-
was demonstrated in the present study (α=.75). competence (Hypothesis 14). It can also be seen that self-
objectification led directly to sexual self-esteem (Hypothesis
Results and Discussion 12), which in turn, was directly linked to disordered eating
(Hypothesis 15). Again, self-objectification was directly
Means, standard deviations, and zero-order correlations are linked to disordered eating, although this was not predicted,
presented in Table 2. It can be seen that the mean levels of indicating that the relationship between self-objectification
sexual self-esteem, self-objectification, and disordered eat-
ing in this sample of British women were comparable to the
mean levels of these constructs in the sample of American
Table 3 Standardized partial regression coefficient for pathways in
women tested in Study 1, although the mean levels in the causal model of disordered eating.
British sample were slightly elevated relative to the
American sample. As predicted, sexual self-esteem and Pathway Beta values
sexual self-competence were positively correlated with each SSE
other and negatively correlated with self-objectification, SO-BS .42**
body shame (not significant for sexual self-competence), SO-SSE −.36**
and disordered eating (Hypothesis 16). SO-DE .23*
Table 3 displays the standardized partial regression BS-SSE .21*
coefficients for all pathways in the two models for BS-DE .52**
SSE-DE −.37**
disordered eating. Figure 4a depicts the significant pathways
SSC
for the model with sexual self-esteem. It can be seen from
SO-BS .42**
the path diagram that self-objectification led directly to body SO-SSC −.35**
shame (Hypothesis 6), which in turn, was directly linked to SO-DE .27*
sexual self-esteem (Hypothesis 8) and disordered eating BS-SSC .05
(Hypothesis 9). It can also be seen that self-objectification BS-DE .50**
led directly to sexual self-esteem (Hypothesis 7), which in SSC-DE −.23*
turn, was directly linked to disordered eating (Hypothesis
SSE general sexual self-esteem, SSC sexual self-competence, SO self-
10). Importantly, self-objectification was directly linked to objectification, BS body shame, DE disordered eating
disordered eating, although this was not predicted, indicat- *p<.05; **p<.001
Sex Roles (2009) 60:160–173 169

Fig. 4 a Beta coefficients a


(standardized partial regression Sexual Self-
coefficients) in path diagram of Esteem
-.37**
women’s disordered eating with -.36** .21*
sexual self-esteem. *p<.05;
**p<.001. b Beta coefficients .42** Body .52**
(standardized partial regression Self- Disordered
Objectification Shame Eating
coefficients) in path diagram of
women’s disordered eating with .23*
sexual self-competence. *p<.05;
**p<.001. b
Sexual Self-
Competence
-.23*
-.35** .05

.42** Body .50**


Self- Disordered
Objectification Shame Eating
.27*

and disordered eating was only partially mediated by body role as a mediator between self-objectification and disor-
shame and sexual self-esteem. The full model described dered eating. Importantly, in this sample of women who
46% of the variance in women’s disordered eating, R2 =.46, represent a different Westernized society from those typical-
F(3,110)=30.60, p<.001, power=1.00. ly represented in the objectification literature (i.e., North
American, Australian), but for whom high levels of
sociocultural appearance pressures, body dissatisfaction,
General Discussion and disordered eating have been reported (Charles and Kerr
1986; Dittmar 2005; Dittmar and Howard 2004; Grogan
The purpose of the present research was to provide further 2006; Grogan and Wainwright 1996), the path model
investigations of Objectification Theory by testing the demonstrated a good fit to the theory, whereby self-
proposition that self-objectification would be negatively objectification led to body shame and sexual self-esteem,
related to women’s sexual well-being as well as to examine and both, in turn, led to more disordered eating. In addition,
how these variables may be jointly related to disordered body shame also directly led to sexual self-esteem, provid-
eating. Consistent with Objectification Theory and prior ing more support for the notion that body image concerns
research on women’s sexual well-being (Fredrickson and are associated with women’s feelings about the sexual self.
Roberts 1997; Raciti and Hendrick 1992; Wiederman 2002), When sexual self-competence replaced sexual self-esteem
the present research demonstrated that American and British in the second path model, a similar pattern of relations
college women with higher self-objectification reported emerged, whereby self-objectification led to body shame and
lower sexual self-esteem and more disordered eating. sexual self-competence, and both, in turn, led to more
Extending Objectification Theory, the first study demon- disordered eating. In the context of Objectification Theory, it
strated that sexual self-esteem partially mediated the would be argued that socializing women to take a
relationship between women’s self-objectification and dis- perspective on the self whereby the body is felt to belong
ordered eating, thereby indicating that self-objectification less to the self and more to others (i.e., high self-
may directly and indirectly (via sexual self-esteem) predict objectification) leads to more negative feelings about the
eating disorder symptomatology in American college sexual aspects of the self as well as more negative
women. This pattern of relations with sexual self-esteem evaluations of one’s sexual competence, and, indeed, this
positioned as a mediator between self-objectification and was demonstrated in the second study. However, in this
disordered eating is similar to the pattern of relations among second path model body shame was unrelated to sexual self-
self-objectification, experiences of body shame, and disor- competence, and the relationship between self-objectification
dered eating reported in prior research with North American and sexual self-competence was weaker than the relation-
and Australian women (Calogero et al. 2005; Noll and ship between self-objectification and sexual self-esteem,
Fredrickson 1998; Tiggemann and Slater 2001; Tylka and indicating that women’s body image concerns are not related
Hill 2004). to all aspects of women’s sexual well-being. A plausible
In a more rigorous test of the role of sexual self-esteem in explanation for these findings is that women’s sexuality is
British college women, the second study demonstrated that often defined as being for the purpose of attracting a sexual
sexual self-esteem is independent from body shame in its partner, and thus is heavily appearance focused vs.
170 Sex Roles (2009) 60:160–173

competence or performance focused (i.e., women as sexual sexual risk-taking (Seal et al. 1997). In addition, it might be
objects vs. women as sexual subjects) (e.g., Busby and expected that both types of sexual self-esteem would be
Leichty 1993; Krassas et al. 2003; Linder 2004; Reichert related to sexual functioning (e.g., arousal, inorgasmia).
and Carpernter 2004; Ward et al. 2006; Ussher 1989). Thus, Related research has indicated that American women
the fact that women’s sexual self-esteem was predicted by who engage in higher body-monitoring and body self-
body shame, but not women’s sexual self-competence, consciousness during sex report lower sexual esteem, less
suggests that women’s global self-evaluations about the sexual assertiveness, less confidence in their sexual func-
value of their sexual selves may be more closely linked with tioning, and more emotional disengagement from sexual
their sense of their sexual attractiveness rather than their experiences (Dove and Wiederman 2000; Wiederman 2000;
sexual competence or performance. Future research should Yamamiya et al. 2006). In addition, recent research has
continue to examine the relations among women’s self- demonstrated that women’s body shame is significantly
objectification and sexual well-being, and their consequen- associated with more sexual problems, and this relationship
ces for women’s body image and eating problems. was mediated by sexual self-consciousness during physical
The major contribution of the present study is that it is intimacy with a partner (Sanchez and Kiefer 2007).
the first empirical demonstration of potential causal links Interestingly, these findings demonstrated that self-
between self-objectification, sexual well-being (in the form objectification was negatively related to sexual self-esteem
of general sexual self-esteem and sexual self-competence), and sexual self-competence, which suggests that a woman’s
and disordered eating in American and British college perception of herself as a sexual object, as a body that exists
women. In particular, these findings add to the literature on for the use and pleasure of others, has consequences for the
Objectification Theory by indicating that women’s sexual value and worth she places on her own sexuality and sexual
self-esteem may stem from self-objectification and be qualities. Consonant with these findings is other research
causally linked to body shame and disordered eating, indicating that women with high self-objectification hold
thereby providing an additional potential psychological more negative attitudes toward women’s bodily functions,
mechanism by which self-objectification may be linked to such as menstruation (Roberts 2004) and breast feeding
disordered eating. It is also important to note that self- (Johnston-Robledo et al. 2007). Thus, body image concerns
objectification exerted direct and indirect effects on appear to be associated with both women’s sexual and
disordered eating, which converges with prior research eating problems. Future research should continue to build
demonstrating that women who self-objectify may bypass upon this preliminary work by exploring other sexual and
negative feelings about the body by engaging in disordered eating consequences of women’s self-objectification and
eating directly to control their appearance and maintain sexual self-esteem across intrapersonal and interpersonal
satisfaction (e.g., Noll and Fredrickson 1998; Tiggemann contexts.
and Slater 2001): One pathway by which self-objectification As described earlier, the problems in the measurement of
can influence disordered eating is by lowering women’s sexual self-esteem make it difficult to consolidate the
sexual self-esteem; however, the experience of low sexual literature on sexual self-esteem; however, sexual self-
self-esteem can be bypassed if women who self-objectify esteem does appear to be a multi-dimensional construct
directly engage in disordered eating behaviours to manipu- (e.g., Gaynor and Underwood 1995; Zeanah and Schwarz
late the body in an attempt to “conform to social norms 1996), and therefore future research that incorporates these
regarding physical attractiveness and what’s considered multiple dimensions may begin to disentangle the relations
‘sexy’” (Dove and Wiederman 2000, p. 74). between evaluations of the sexual self and eating disorder
The weaker findings for sexual self-competence in the symptomatology. One consideration for research on the
full model of disordered eating provides some evidence to sexual self-concept is to include a phenomenological
indicate that general esteem and competence represent measure of sexual self-esteem that does not directly ask
different components of women’s sexual self-concept, and about feelings related to the sexual self, but instead assesses
may be differentially related to women’s eating disorder the motivational and behavioural components of sexual
symptomatology, especially affective components such as self-esteem. A phenomenological measure of body shame
body shame. However, while these findings indicated that has been used in previous research to circumvent the
sexual self-esteem is more strongly associated with disor- difficulty of assessing shame directly (Calogero 2004;
dered eating than sexual self-competence, it is plausible that Fredrickson et al. 1998; Tangney et al. 1996), and includes
sexual self-competence might be more strongly related to items such as “I wish I were invisible” or “I wish I could
sexual behaviours (e.g., number of partners, use of disappear.” The same rationale for its use with body shame
condoms). For example, research has shown that sexual may apply in the case of sexual self-esteem.
self-esteem is negatively associated with sexual risk-taking Several limitations of the present research should be
whereas sexual self-efficacy is positively associated with acknowledged. First, although samples of college women
Sex Roles (2009) 60:160–173 171

from two different highly developed Westernized societies and intervention strategies aimed at reducing the risk for
were represented, the samples were restricted to predomi- eating disorders among girls’ and women in Westernized
nantly White, undergraduate women. While young adult societies.
women on college campuses represent an important
population to study with regard to experiences of sexual
objectification and eating disorder pathology, these results References
cannot be generalized to other samples of women who may
differ in age, ethnicity, nationality, or social and economic Ackard, D. M., Kearney-Cooke, A., & Peterson, C. B. (2000). Effect
status. Moreover, the current hypotheses were tested in of body image and self-image on women’s sexual behaviours.
American and British samples, and therefore should be The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 28, 422–429.
Adkins, E. C., & Keel, P. K. (2005). Does “excessive” or “compulsive”
tested among women representing other societies and best describe exercise as a symptom of bulmia nervosa. The
cultural backgrounds to provide a more comprehensive International Journal of Eating Disorders, 38, 24–29.
picture of objectification processes and consequences in American Psychological Association, Task Force on the Sexualization
women’s lives. Second, the path analyses tested theoretical of Girls, (2007). Report of the APA Task Force on the
sexualization of girls. Washington, DC: American Psychological
assumptions for a causal model of objectification processes Association. Retrieved June 10, 2008, from http://www.apa.org/
and disordered eating in women. While the present findings pi/wpo/sexualization_report_summary.pdf
are generally consistent with the causal links reported in Andersen, B. L., & Cyranowski, J. M. (1994). Women’s sexual self-
other studies on objectification processes in North Ameri- schema. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67,
1079–1100.
can and Australian women (e.g., Tiggemann and Kuring Althof, S. E., Rosen, R. C., Derogatis, L., Corty, E., Quirk, F., &
2004; Tylka and Hill 2004), support for the causal Symonds, T. (2005). Outcome measurement in female sexual
assumptions does not prove that the causal assumptions dysfunction clinical trials: review and recommendations. Journal
are correct. In addition, the data were collected at a single of Sex & Marital Therapy, 31, 153–166.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator
point in time, with further prevents drawing definitive variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual,
conclusions about the order of variables within the path strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality
analyses. Prospective and experimental designs are needed and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.
to provide convergent evidence for the pathways demon- Bartky, S. (1990). Femininity and domination. New York: Routledge.
British Medical Association, (2000). Eating disorders, body image, and
strated here. Third, as indicated above, different components the media. Retrieved June 10, 2008, from http://www.bma.org.uk/
or qualities of the sexual self-concept as well as other sexual ap.nsf/Content/Eatingdisorders?OpenDocument&Highlight = 2,
consequences that may stem from self-objectification should body,image
be assessed. Bryman, A., & Cramer, D. (1990). Quantitative data analysis for
social scientists. London: Routledge.
In conclusion, the present research supports the argu- Busby, L., & Leichty, G. (1993). Feminism and advertising in
ments of objectification theorists which claim that cultural traditional and nontraditional women’s magazines, 1950s–
practices of sexually objectifying the female body can have 1980s. The Journalism Quarterly, 70, 247–264.
profound negative effects on women’s perceptions and Calogero, R. M. (2004). A test of objectification theory: effect of the
male gaze on appearance concerns in college women. Psychology
evaluations of themselves, and place them at increased risk of Women Quarterly, 28, 16–21.
for eating disorders (Fredrickson and Roberts 1997; Kashak Calogero, R. M., Boroughs, M., & Thompson, J. K. (2007). The
1992; McKinley and Hyde 1996). Because women are impact of Western beauty ideals on the lives of women and men:
socialized to experience their efforts to monitor and A sociocultural perspective. In V. Swami, & A. Furnham (Eds.),
Body beautiful: Evolutionary and sociocultural perspectives (pp.
improve their physical appearance as self-chosen or 259–298). New York: Palgrave Macmillan.
“natural” (Costanzo 1992; Fredrickson and Roberts 1997), Calogero, R. M., Davis, W. N., & Thompson, J. K. (2005). The role of
many women who are at risk for eating disorders or self-objectification in the experience of women with eating
undergoing eating disorders treatment are unlikely to disorders. Sex Roles, 52, 43–50.
Cash, T. F. (2002). Women’s body images. In G. M. Wingood, &
identify self-objectification as a problem because they are R. J. DiClemente (Eds.), Handbook of women’s sexual and
unaware that it is a potentially harmful, socially constructed reproductive health: Issues in women health (pp. 175–194).
form of self-perception that contributes to feelings of New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
powerlessness over one’s own body (Peterson et al. 2008) Charles, N., & Kerr, M. (1986). Food for feminist thought. The
Sociological Review, 34, 537–572 ah, NJ.
and interferes with positive psychological functioning Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral
(Mercurio and Landry 2008). This occurs in spite of sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Earlbaum.
evidence demonstrating markedly high levels of self- Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–
objectification among women in eating disorders treatment 159.
Costanzo, P. R. (1992). External socialization and the development
programs (Calogero et al. 2005). Thus, this research provides of adaptive individuation and social connection. In D. N. Ruble,
more direct evidence for including self-objectification, body P. R. Costanzo, & M. E. Oliveri (Eds.), The social psychology of
shame, and sexual self-esteem as targets in prevention mental health (pp. 55–80). New York: Guilford.
172 Sex Roles (2009) 60:160–173

Dittmar, H. (2005). Vulnerability factors and processes linking interpersonal sexual objectification scale. Psychology of Women
sociocultural pressures and body dissatisfaction: introduction to Quarterly, 31, 176–189.
special issue: body image. Journal of Social and Clinical Krassas, N. R., Blauwkamp, J. M., & Wesselink, P. (2003). “Master
Psychology, 24, 1003–1009. your Johnson”: sexual rhetoric in Maxim and stuff magazines.
Dittmar, H., & Howard, S. (2004). Professional hazards?: the impact Sexuality & Culture, 7, 98–119.
of models’ body size on advertising effectiveness and women’s Lavie, M., & Willig, C. (2005). “I don’t feel like melting butter”: an
body-focused anxiety in professions that do and do not interpretative phenomenological analysis of the experience if
emphasize the cultural ideal of thinness. The British Journal of ‘inorgasmia’. Psychology & Health, 20, 115–128.
Social Psychology, 43, 477–497. Linder, K. (2004). Images of women in general interest and fashion
Dove, N. L., & Wiederman, M. W. (2000). Cognitive distraction and advertisements from 1955 to 2002. Sex Roles, 51, 409–421.
women’s sexual functioning. Sex Roles, 52, 463–475. MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., &
Ellison, C. R. (2000). Women’s sexualities: Generations of women Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test the
share intimate secrets of sexual self-acceptance. Oakland, CA: significance of the mediated effect. Psychological Methods, 7,
New Harbinger. 83–104.
Evans, L., & Wertheim, E. H. (1998). Intimacy patterns and Marsh, H. W. (1986). Global self-esteem: its relation to specific facets
relationship satisfaction of women with eating problems and the of self-concept and their importance. Journal of Personality and
mediating effects of depression, trait anxiety, and social anxiety. Social Psychology, 51, 1224–1236.
Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 44, 355–365. Mayers, K. S., Heller, D. K., & Heller, J. A. (2003). Damaged sexual
Fairburn, C. G., Cooper, Z., Doll, H. A., & Welch, S. L. (1999). Risk self-esteem: a kind of disability. Sexuality and Disability, 21,
factors for anorexia nervosa: three integrated case-control 269–282.
comparisons. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 468–476. McCarthy, M. (1990). The thin ideal, depression and eating disorders
Fortenberry, J. D., Sayegh, M. A., & Orr, D. P. (2005). The association in women. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 205–215.
of sexual self-concept with oral–genital sex among adolescent McKinley, N. M., & Hyde, J. S. (1996). The objectified body
women. The Journal of Adolescent Health, 36, 110. consciousness scale. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20, 181–
Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. A. (1997). Objectification theory: 215.
towards understanding women’s lived experience and mental McLaren, L., Kuh, D., Hardy, R., & Gauvin, L. (2004). Positive and
health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 173–206. negative body-related comments and their relationship with body
Fredrickson, B. L., Roberts, T. A., Noll, S. M., Quinn, D. M., & dissatisfaction in middle-aged women. Psychology & Health, 19,
Twenge, J. M. (1998). That swimsuit becomes you: sex differences 261–272.
in self-objectification, restrained eating, and math performance. Mercurio, A. E., & Landry, L. J. (2008). Self-objectification and well-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 269–284. being: the impact of self-objectification on women’s overall sense
Garner, D. M. (1991). EDI-2: Eating disorder inventory professional of self-worth and life satisfaction. Sex Roles, 58, 458–466.
manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Moradi, B., Dirks, D., & Matteson, A. V. (2005). Roles of sexual
Garrow, J. S., & Webster, J. (1985). Quetelet’s index (w/h2) as a objectification experiences and internalization of standards of
measure of fatness. International Journal of Obesity, 9, 147–153. beauty in eating disorder symptomatology: a test and extension
Gaynor, P. A., & Underwood, J. K. (1995). Conceptualizing and of objectification theory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 51,
measuring sexual self-esteem. In S. T. Fiske, & P. E. Shrout 50–56.
(Eds.), Personality research, methods, and theory (pp. 333–347). Murnen, S. K., & Smolak, L. (2000). The experience of sexual
Hillsdale, NJ: LEA. harassment among grade-school students: early socialization of
Grogan, S. (2006). Body image and health: contemporary perspec- female subordination? Sex Roles, 43, 1–17.
tives. Journal of Health Psychology, 11, 523–530. Noll, S. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). A mediational model linking
Grogan, S., & Wainwright, N. (1996). Growing up in the culture of self-objectification, body shame, and disordered eating. Psychology
slenderness: girls’ experiences of body dissatisfaction. Women’s of Women Quarterly, 22, 623–636.
Studies International Forum, 19, 665–673. Offer, D. (1969). The psychological world of the teenager: A study of
Harned, M. S., & Fitzgerald, L. F. (2002). Understanding a link between normal adolescent boys. New York: Basic Books.
sexual harassment and eating disorder symptoms: a mediational Oliver, M. B., & Hyde, J. S. (1993). Gender differences in sexuality: a
analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70, meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 29–51.
1170–1181. Orbach, S. (1993). Hunger strike: The anorectics struggle as a
Harper, B., & Tiggemann, M. (2008). The effect of thin ideal images metaphor for our age. London: Penguin.
on women’s self-objectification, mood, and body image. Sex O’Sullivan, L. F., Meyer-Bahlburg, H. F. L., & McKeague, I. W.
Roles, 58, 649–657. (2006). The development of the sexual self-concept inventory for
Hendrick, C., & Hendrick, S. (1983). Liking, loving, and relating. early adolescent girls. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 30, 139–
Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole. 149.
Huebner, D. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1999). Gender differences in Pedhazur, E. J. (1997). Multiple regression in behavioral research
memory perspectives: evidence for self-objectification in women. (3rd ed.). Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace.
Sex Roles, 41, 459–467. Peter, J., & Valkenberg, P. M. (2007). Adolescents’ exposure to a
Johnston-Robledo, I., Wares, S., Fricker, J., & Pasek, L. (2007). sexualized media environment and their notions of women as sex
Indecent exposure: self-objectification and young women’s objects. Sex Roles, 56, 381–395.
attitudes toward breastfeeding. Sex Roles, 56, 429–437. Peterson, R. D., Grippo, K. P., & Tantleff-Dunn, S. (2008).
Kashak, E. (1992). Engendered lives: A new psychology of women’s Empowerment and powerlessness: a closer look at the relation-
experience. New York: Basic Books. ship between feminism, body image, and eating disturbance. Sex
Katz, J., & Farrow, S. (2000). Discrepant self-views and young Roles, 58, 639–648.
women’s sexual and emotional adjustment. Sex Roles, 42, 781– Pitts, C., & Waller, G. (1993). Self-denigratory beliefs following
805. sexual abuse: association with the symptomatology of bulimic
Kozee, H. B., Tylka, T. L., Augustus-Horvath, C. L., & Denchik, A. disorders. The International Journal of Eating Disorders, 13,
(2007). Development and psychometric evaluation of the 407–410.
Sex Roles (2009) 60:160–173 173

Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (2002). Causes of eating disorders. Annual Thompson, J. K., Heinberg, L. J., Altabe, M. N., & Tantleff-Dunn, S.
Review of Psychology, 53, 187–213. (1999). Exacting beauty: Theory, assessment and treatment of
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for body image disturbance. Washington, DC: American Psycholog-
estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior ical Association.
Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 717–731. Tiefer, L. (2001). A new view of women’s sexual problems: why new?
Preacher, K. J., & Leonardelli, G. J. (2007). Calculation for the Sobel Why now? Journal of Sex Research, 38, 89–96.
test: An interactive calculation tool for mediation tests. Retrieved Tiggemann, M., & Kuring, J. K. (2004). The role of body
October 24, 2007, from http://www.people.ku.edu/∼preacher/ objectification in disordered eating and depressed mood. The
sobel/sobel.htm British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43, 299–311.
Raciti, M., & Hendrick, S. S. (1992). Relationships between eating Tiggemann, M., & Lynch, J. E. (2001). Body image across the life
disorder characteristics and love and sex attitudes. Sex Roles, 27, span in adult women: the role of self-objectification. Develop-
553–564. mental Psychology, 37, 243–253.
Reichert, T., & Carpernter, C. (2004). An update on sex in magazine Tiggemann, M., & Slater, A. (2001). A test of objectification theory in
advertising: 1983 to 2003. Journalism & Mass Communication former dancers and non-dancers. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
Quarterly, 81, 823–837. 25, 57–64.
Roberts, T. A. (2004). Female trouble: the menstrual self-evaluation Troop, N. A. (1998). Eating disorders as coping strategies: a critique.
scale and women’s self-objectification. Psychology of Women European Eating Disorders Review, 6, 229–237.
Quarterly, 28, 22–26. Tylka, T. L., & Hill, M. S. (2004). Objectification theory as it relates to
Rodriguez, S., Mata, J. L., Lameiras, M., Fernandez, M. C., & Vila, J. disordered eating among college women. Sex Roles, 51, 719–730.
(2007). Dyscontrol evoked by erotic and food images in women with Ussher, J. (1989). The psychology of the female body. Routledge:
bulimia nervosa. European Eating Disorders Review, 15, 231–239. London.
Rosenberg, M. (1986). Conceiving the self. Malabar, FL: Krieger. Ward, L. M., & Friedman, K. (2006). Using TV as a guide:
Rosenthal, D., Moore, S., & Flynn, I. (1991). Adolescent self-efficacy, associations between television viewing and adolescents’ sexual
self-esteem and sexual risk-taking. Journal of Community & attitudes and behavior. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 16,
Applied Social Psychology, 1, 77–88. 133–156.
Sanchez, D. T., & Kiefer, A. K. (2007). When the relationship Ward, L. M., Merriwether, A., & Caruthers, A. (2006). Breasts are for
becomes her: revisiting women’s body consciousness from a men: media, masculinity ideologies, and men’s beliefs about
relationship contingency perspective. Psychology of Women women’s bodies. Sex Roles, 55, 703–714.
Quarterly, 31, 401–414. Weaver, A. D., & Byers, S. (2006). The relationships among body
Schoemaker, C., van Strien, T., & van der Staak, C. (1994). Validation image, body mass index, exercise, and sexual functioning in
of the eating disorder inventory in a nonclinical population using heterosexual women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 30, 333–
transformed and untransformed scores. The International Journal 339.
of Eating Disorders, 15, 387–393. Welch, G., Hall, A., & Walkey, F. (1988). The factor structure of the
Seal, A., Minichiello, V., & Omodei, M. (1997). Young women’s eating disorders inventory. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44,
sexual risk taking behaviour: re-visiting the influences of sexual 51–56.
self-efficacy and sexual self-esteem. International Journal of Wiederman, M. W. (1996). Women, sex, and food: a review of
STD & AIDS, 8, 159–165. research on eating disorders and sexuality. Journal of Sex
Shulman, J. L., & Horne, S. G. (2003). The use of self-pleasure: Research, 33, 301–311.
masturbation and body image among African American and European Wiederman, M. W. (2000). Women’s body image self-consciousness
American women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 27, 262–269. during physical intimacy with a partner. Journal of Sex Research,
Slater, A., & Tiggemann, M. (2002). A test of objectification theory in 37, 60–68.
adolescent girls. Sex Roles, 26, 343–349. Wiederman, M. W. (2002). Body image and sexual functioning. In
Snell, W. E., & Papini, D. R. (1989). The sexuality scale: an T. F. Cash, & T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body image: A handbook of
instrument to measure sexual self-esteem, sexual depression, theory, research, and clinical practice (pp. 287–294). New York:
and sexual preoccupation. Journal of Sex Research, 26, 256–263. Guilford.
Steer, A., & Tiggemann, M. (2008). The role of self-objectification in Wiederman, M. W., & Allgeier, E. R. (1993). The measurement of
women’s sexual functioning. Journal of Social and Clinical sexual esteem: investigation of Snell and Papini’s (1989)
Psychology, 27, 205–225. sexuality scale. Journal of Research in Personality, 27, 88–102.
Striegel-Moore, R. H., & Smolak, L. (2001). Eating disorders: Wiederman, M. W., Pryor, T., & Morgan, C. D. (1996). The sexual
Innovative directions in research and practice. Washington, experience of women diagnosed with anorexia nervosa or
DC: American Psychological Association. bulimia nervosa. The International Journal of Eating Disorders,
Swim, J. K., Hyers, L. L., Cohen, L. L., & Ferguson, M. J. (2001). 19, 109–118.
Everyday sexism: evidence for its incidence, nature, and Yamamiya, Y., Cash, T. F., & Thompson, J. K. (2006). Sexual
psychological impact from three daily diary studies. The Journal experiences among college women: the differential effects of
of Social Issues, 57, 31–53. general versus contextual body images on sexuality. Sex Roles,
Tangney, J. P., Miller, R. S., Flicker, L., & Barlow, D. H. (1996). Are 55, 421–427.
shame, guilt, and embarrassment distinct emotions? Journal of Zeanah, P. D., & Schwarz, J. C. (1996). Reliability and validity of the
Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1256–1269. sexual self-esteem inventory for women. Assessment, 3, 1–15.
COPYRIGHT INFORMATION

TITLE: Sexual Self-Esteem in American and British College


Women: Relations with Self-Objectification and Eating
Problems
SOURCE: Sex Roles 60 no3/4 F 2009

The magazine publisher is the copyright holder of this article and it


is reproduced with permission. Further reproduction of this article in
violation of the copyright is prohibited. To contact the publisher:
http://springerlink.metapress.com/content/1573-2762/

View publication stats

You might also like