You are on page 1of 11

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 31, No.

1, 2001

Taking a Closer Look at Functional Play


in Children with Autism

Emma Williams,1,2 Vasudevi Reddy,1 and Alan Costall1

Research evidence indicates that children with autism may experience problems with func-
tional play, in addition to their well-documented deficits in symbolic play. However, as a re-
sult of the tendency of previous studies to group all functional play into a single category, the
precise nature and extent of this deficit remains unclear. The present study undertook a more
refined analysis of such play, subtyping the functional acts into various categories, in terms
of the developmental progression suggested by research with typical infants. The functional
play of children with autism was compared to that of developmentally matched children with
Down syndrome and typical infants. Although there were no group differences in overall mea-
sures of the proportion of total play time spent in functional play and in the number of func-
tional acts performed, a closer analysis of the composition of this play did reveal striking,
qualitative differences. The functional play of the autism group was less elaborated, less var-
ied, and less integrated than that of the controls. The implications of these findings are ex-
plored in relation to current theoretical models of autism and in relation to the role of other
people in mediating the appropriate use of objects.

KEY WORDS: Autism; functional play.

INTRODUCTION only as the result of a slow, hard, progressive process:


“The child does not do this all at once because it is ter-
Early semiotic theories viewed symbolic develop- ribly difficult for a child to sever the thought (the mean-
ment as a gradually unfolding process, rooted firmly in ing of a word) from the object” (p. 97). In light of these
early sensorimotor behavior (Piaget, 1962; Werner & theoretical approaches, research on children’s play has
Kaplan, 1963). Piaget (1962) proposed that symbols been concerned to identify the composition of various
emerge by means of a progressive differentiation be- play behaviors at different ages and to establish the se-
tween the “signifier” (the actual object or action used by quence of changes that occur with increasing maturity
the child) and the “signified” (the absent object or ac- (Belsky & Most, 1981; El’Konin, 1966; Fein & Apfel,
tion being represented) which is mediated by a number 1979; Fenson, Kagan, Kearlsey, & Zelazo, 1976;
of transitional behaviors. Similarly Vygotsky (1978) por- Marino, 1988; McCune-Nicolich & Fenson, 1984; Nicol-
trayed the development of play as one involving a grad- ich, 1977; Vondra & Belsky, 1989). Three major trends
ual separation of meaning from the object and of mean- in the development of pretense were identified: decon-
ing from action. Far from arising from a sudden moment textualization, which allowed pretend3 play to occur with
of insight, he suggests that the decoupling comes about decreasing environmental support; decentration, where
symbolic actions are freed from the child’s body, al-
1
lowing the use of dolls and other people for expressing
Department of Psychology, University of Portsmouth, Portsmouth,
PO1 2DY, United Kingdom.
2 3
Address all correspondence to E. Williams, Department of Psycho- Unless otherwise stated, where the term “pretend” or “pretense” is
logy, King Alfred’s College, Sparkford Road, Winchester, SO22 used in this paper this will be taken to include both functional and
4NR; e-mail: e.williams@wkac.ac.uk symbolic play.

67
0162-3257/01/0200-0067$19.50/0 © 2001 Plenum Publishing Corporation
68 Williams, Reddy, and Costall

pretend actions and allowing the adoption of others’ ac- between the autism and control groups (matched for
tions; and integration, leading to sequentially and later nonverbal mental age and verbal comprehension) with
hierarchically organised play (Fenson & Ramsay, 1980; regard to the numbers of children who produced sym-
McCune-Nicolich & Fenson, 1984). bolic acts, no such group differences were identified in
In drawing a stark, categorical distinction between relation to the production of functional acts.
functional and symbolic play, Leslie’s (1987) analysis However, recent reviews of the functional and sym-
of pretence represents a radical departure from previous bolic play abilities of children with autism suggest that
assumptions of a gradual evolution from simple to pro- the empirical findings may not be so clear-cut (Jarrold,
gressively more elaborate play behaviors (Piaget, 1962; 1997; Jarrold, Bouchen, & Smith, 1993; Williams,
Vygotsky, 1978; Werner & Kaplan, 1963). According to Costall, & Reddy, 1999). For example, while Libby,
Leslie (1987), symbolic play can take three forms: ob- Powell, Messer, and Jordan (1998) observed no reliable
ject substitution, the attribution of false properties, and differences in the amount of time children with autism
the attribution of presence to imaginary objects. Func- engaged in functional play, compared to controls
tional play is defined as “the appropriate use of an object matched for expressive and receptive language abilities,
or the conventional association of two or more objects, other studies have found that children with autism spent
such as a spoon to feed a doll, or placing a teacup on a significantly less time playing functionally than learn-
saucer” (Ungerer & Sigman, 1981, p. 320). Leslie (1987) ing disabled and typically developing controls (Jarrold,
proposed that symbolic play is underpinned by a more Boucher, & Smith, 1996; Lewis & Boucher, 1988;
complex representational system than that which under- Sigman & Ungerer, 1984). In addition, where functional
lies the production of functional play. “True” pretence acts are produced by children with autism, there is some
(i.e., symbolic play), he argued, requires not only a first- evidence to suggest they are less varied, integrated, and
order representation of the pen as a rocket, for example, other-directed than those produced by controls matched
but also a second-order representation about this repre- for general mental age (Atlas, 1990; Wing, Gould,
sentation (a metarepresentation), namely, that the repre- Yeates, & Brierley, 1977; Sigman & Ungerer, 1984).
sentation is not true, the object is not really a rocket: “In The identification of both quantitative and qualitative
both functional play and error acting as if,4 the as-if com- differences in the functional play of children with
ponent really only exists from the observer’s point of autism, along with corresponding findings that symbolic
view. From the actor’s point of view, the actions are se- play in structured situations is not entirely absent (Lewis
rious. But in pretense, the actor is acting as if from the & Boucher, 1988; Libby et al., 1998; Sigman & Ungerer,
actor’s point of view as well” (p. 414). 1984; Whyte & Owens, 1989) is incompatible with
In other words, functional play does not necessar- those theoretical accounts which argue for a specific
ily involve pretense (as defined by Leslie, 1987) since, metarepresentational deficit in autism and which also
as Baron-Cohen (1987) has noted, the child may regard insist that functional play is not reliant on the ability to
a toy cooker, for example, as a small, yet real, cooker. metarepresent (Baron-Cohen, 1995; Leslie, 1987).
Leslie (1987) cited evidence that children with autism In light of such inconsistencies alternative accounts
experience particular difficulties with symbolic play have been proposed which do not presume a qualita-
(Gould, 1986; Riguet, Taylor, Benaroya, & Klein, tive distinction between functional and symbolic play
1981; Sigman & Ungerer, 1984) in support of his ar- (Harris, 1993; Jarrold, 1997; Russell, Mauthner, Sharpe
gument that autism derives from an specific impair- & Tidswell, 1991; Williams et al., 1999). Instead they
ment in the decoupling mechanism necessary for the argue that the pattern of social-communicative deficits
emergence of metarepresentations, which in turn are characteristic of autism are a product of a general basic
required for the production of symbolic play and for underlying deficit either in executive control and plan-
the development of a theory of mind (Baron-Cohen, ning or in relating to other people. In common with
Leslie, & Frith, 1985). Baron-Cohen’s (1987) study lent Vygotsky (1978), executive dysfunction accounts view
further empirical support to this hypothesis and the pro- the development of play as involving a gradual move
posed dissociation between the representational sys- away from actions determined by externally imposed
tems underlying functional and symbolic play. He constraints towards the imposing of internal executive
found that, although there were significant differences control on actions by the children themselves, such that
they can act independently from what they see (Harris,
4
“error acting as if” is where a child uses an object as if it was some-
1993; Jarrold, 1997; Russell et al., 1991). They are
thing else in error, rather than pretending it is something else, while therefore better able to account for the existence of
at the same time being aware of what the object actually is. deficits in functional play in autism, in addition to the
Autism and Functional Play 69

widely accepted problems with symbolic acts. More re- METHOD


cently it has been proposed that deficits in the appro-
priate, functional use of objects are a logical corollary Participants
of more basic difficulties in relating to other people
(Williams et al., 1999). People play an important role Forty-five children, between the ages of 11 months
in introducing the child to the proper use of things by and 5 years 5 months, participated in the study. These
making certain aspects salient in the context of joint included 15 typically developing infants, 15 children
attention and imitation. Thus, Williams et al. argued with autism, and 15 children with Down syndrome who
that, to the extent that children with autism fail to en- were part of an ongoing research project on person-
gage other people in their use of objects, or use them directed play in which the authors were already partic-
to guide their own dealings with objects (Landry & ipating. The method of selection for the typical infants
Loveland, 1988; Mundy, Sigman, Ungerer, & Sherman, was through the records of family doctors. A letter was
1986, 1987; Mundy, Sigman, & Kasari, 1990), they are sent out from the doctor’s clinics to parents of suitably
excluded from a great deal of information about how aged infants requesting volunteers for the study. The
to use them in functionally appropriate ways. children with autism were selected by approaching pro-
In view of the equivocal nature of the evidence fessional diagnostic clinics in the South of England.
with regard to the functional play abilities of children Each child had received a primary diagnosis of child-
with autism, further investigations are required in order hood autism or autistic disorder according to the crite-
to assess the competing claims of the metarepresenta- ria stated in either the International Classification of
tional-deficit theories versus accounts which argue for Diseases, 10th edition (ICD-10; World Health Organi-
more basic cognitive or social deficits. Furthermore zation [WHO], 1990) or the Diagnostic and Statistical
there is a need to extend the precision of measurement Manual IV (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association
and definition of functional play behaviors in order to [APA], 1994), respectively. Those with Down syndrome
exclude the possibility that the failure of some studies were recruited with the help of the Sarah Duffen Cen-
to find functional play deficits in children with autism tre (Portsmouth Down Syndrome Trust) based at the
was simply the result of their use of limited measure- University of Portsmouth. The children with Down syn-
ment and coding criteria. For example, Baron-Cohen’s drome were selected to act as a suitable control group
(1987) study compared the numbers of children in each for the children with autism as, although they experi-
group who demonstrated any examples of such play but ence learning difficulties, they do not exhibit the spe-
no measure was taken of differences in either the du- cific problems in communication, socialization, and
ration or the quality of the play between the groups. In imagination associated with autism. The children were
addition, the criteria for coding an act as a functional individually matched for general developmental age
one included the appropriate naming of an object. It using the second edition of the Bayley Scales of Infant
could be argued that this may have led to an overesti- Development (BSID II; Bayley, 1993) which includes a
mation of the ability of the children with autism to pro- measure of both cognitive and language ability. Each
duce functional acts as it is questionable how far nam- child with autism was matched to one child with Down
ing can be said to involve an appropriate use of an syndrome and one typically developing child. We de-
object (Jarrold, 1997). Another problem lies in the ten- cided against using a nonverbal cognitive measure for
dency of previous studies to group all forms of func- matching the groups on the grounds that this may have
tional play under one heading. As Libby et al. (1998) selectively disadvantaged the children with autism,
themselves noted, their failure to find functional play given that their verbal skills are usually less advanced
deficits in the children with autism may be a result of than their cognitive ones. Equating the participants using
the lack of precision in their coding scheme. The cur- a nonverbal cognitive measure therefore runs the risk of
rent study was therefore designed to take a more fine- making a Type 1 error, as it increases the likelihood
grained look at functional play in children with autism, of finding qualitative impairments in the functional
through the development of a categorization scheme play of the children with autism. The groups were not
that identifies the different kinds of functional play that matched for chronological age (CA) or gender (Table I).
can occur. Functional play acts were subtyped in order In addition the groups were assessed for language
to evaluate them in terms of the developmental pro- production and general communicative competence
gression suggested by research with typical infants and (combined receptive and expressive ability). The as-
measures were taken of their variety, frequency, dura- sessment of language production was made using the
tion, and integration. MacArthur Communicative Inventory (Fenson et al.,
70 Williams, Reddy, and Costall

Table I. Participant Characteristics each visit. These visits were made 2 weeks apart. The
procedure was designed to be naturalistic and flexible,
Groupa Typical Autistic Down
in light of the known difficulties of working with young
Male/Female 6/9 13/2 8/7 children with autism. A set of toys were laid out, in
Chronological age (months) random order, on the floor in front of the child. The
M 16.6b 48.8b,c 40.3c same toys were presented to every child who took part
SD 0.5 8.3 11.9 in the study. Before videotaping began the parent was
Range 11–24 36–62 18–65
Bayley developmental
asked if they would try and ensure that their child was
age (months) turned towards the camera. They were instructed to
M 17 16.8 17.7 respond naturally if their child showed distress or ap-
SD 4.8 4.4 5.4 proached them during the videotaped session, but to
Range 11–24 11–26 11–25 refrain from giving directions or demonstrating partic-
Vineland, communication
domain (months)
ular play behaviors. The child was then videotaped
M 16.5 15.5 18.2 playing with the toys for a total of 15 minutes. The ses-
SD 5.3 8.0 6.9 sions took place in a room of the parent’s choice, typ-
Range 9–30 7–32 7–30 ically a front room or a playroom. Videotaping was
MacArthur, language continuous, unless the child wandered out of the view
production (months)
M 15.8 14.7 17.8
of the camera, or became disengaged with the objects
SD 4.0 6.2 5.2 for longer than 30 seconds. When this happened the
Range 8–24 8–27 8–26 experimenter asked the parent to try and attract their
child’s attention to the objects. Videotaping was re-
a
n = 15 in each group. sumed when the child once again became engaged with
b
Autistic mean significantly higher than Typical at p < .0001.
the toys. Throughout this process the experimenter tried
c
Autistic mean significantly higher than Down at p < .05.
to remain as unobtrusive as possible. Following the
observation of the object play the same experimenter
administered the baseline psychometric tests.
1993) which generates scores for vocabulary produc-
tion based on parental report. A combined measure of
receptive and expressive ability was obtained using Scoring
the communication domain of the Vineland Adaptive
Play Behaviors
Behavior Scales (Sparrow, Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984)
where information is obtained from parents by means The behavior categories expanded on previous
of a semistructured interview. Psychometric tests based schemes (Baron-Cohen, 1987; Nicolich, 1977; Ungerer
on parental report were used to obtain the language & Sigman, 1981) and were further developed from an
measures in order to reduce the assessment pressure on inductive analysis of the video recordings. The ratio-
the very young children with autism. One-way analy- nale for the development of the scheme was to conduct
ses of variance revealed no significant differences be- a more detailed analysis of the functional play acts than
tween the groups of children on the scores obtained that undertaken in previous studies. The functional play
from any of the measures. behaviors were grouped into two main categories: sim-
ple functional and elaborated functional, within which
there were a number of subcategories. The terms sim-
Materials
ple and elaborated as used here are not meant to imply
The materials consisted of a set of traditional toys a qualitative distinction in the sense that Leslie (1987)
(Appendix). A Sony Camcorder (CCD F335) was used used the categories “functional” and “symbolic,” but to
for recording the play of the children as it was light, reflect the developmental progression found in typical
portable, and suitable for videotaping in small areas in development. Full details of the coding scheme are
the home, where children are likely to be mobile. given in Table II.
Ten minutes of play time for each child were ana-
lyzed from the video recordings for instances of func-
Design and Procedure
tional play, using the Observer (version 3.0) software.
Each child was visited at home either twice or A combination of video and timecode hardware (Pana-
three times, depending on how much was achieved at sonic AG 7355B VCR and RS 232 Interface) with the
Autism and Functional Play 71

Table II. Description of the Coding Scheme

Behavior Definition and examples

Simple functional play

Functional association The child combines two objects that are functionally related to
each other (e.g., putting a cup on a saucer, placing a lid on a
teapot, putting a peg in a hole).
Functional use of single object The child acts on an object in a manner that reflects it’s
“proper” conventional use (e.g., bringing a baby bottle or
toy cup to the mouth, brushing own hair with a toy brush,
placing a toy telephone to the ear).
Elaborated functional play
Functional use of multiple objects The child uses two or more objects appropriately together
accompanied by a clear supporting gesture (e.g., stirring a
spoon in a pot, tipping a jug over a cup, as if pouring some-
thing into it. To be included in this category a child must
stir the spoon around, rather than simply place it in the pot,
thus distinguishing the behavior from simple functional
association.
Functional act supported by The child acts on an object in a manner that reflects its “proper”
appropriate vocalization/gesture conventional use and accompanies this with an appropriate
vocalization or exaggerated gesture (e.g., placing a toy
telephone to the ear and vocalizing, making slurping noises
while drinking from a baby bottle, drinking from a cup and
throwing head back in an exaggerated drinking gesture).
Acts involving the appropriate use of multiple objects which
also include a relevant vocalization or exaggerated gesture
were coded in this category.
Doll-directed functional acts The child carries out an act involving the use of a doll
(e.g., brushing a doll’s hair with a toy brush, putting a doll
in the bath). Acts involving a doll accompanied by a relevant
vocalization were coded in this category.

Observer software enabled us to code the functional play exclude cases where the same action pattern is per-
behaviors (with their associated time codes) directly formed on a series of objects indiscriminately, for ex-
from the videotapes to an IBM-compatible computer. ample, shaking a rattle, then shaking a doll, then a baby
The analysis consisted of the first 10 minutes of con- bottle. Multischeme combinations were defined as the
tinuous footage, excluding times where the child was juxtaposition of two or more different functional acts
not engaged with the toys or where it was difficult to directed towards the same recipient. These acts could
see what they were doing. Four separate measures were either follow each other in a logically ordered sequence,
obtained: duration: the proportion of total playtime that such as tipping a teapot over a cup, then putting the cup
each child spent in a particular play category; frequency: to the mouth and making slurping noises, or reflect no
the total number of acts performed in each play cate- essential order, for example putting a doll in the bath,
gory; diversity: the total number of new versus re- then feeding the doll. The component functional acts
peated acts in each category; integration: the extent to and integrated/combined functional acts were coded as
which the functional acts produced were appropriately separate analyses, in order to avoid the loss of impor-
combined or integrated. These included single-scheme tant information about a child’s play performance, with
and multischeme combinations (following Fenson & regard to both quality and diversity. This means that in
Ramsay, 1980). Single-scheme combinations were de- the analysis of component acts the actions of stirring a
fined as two consecutive functional acts in which the spoon around in one cup and then in another would be
same action was directed towards two different recipi- coded as two elaborated functional acts, one new (if it
ents, for example, feeding one doll, then another doll, had been performed by the child for the first time) and
or drinking from one cup and then another. This would one repeated. In the analysis of the level of integration
72 Williams, Reddy, and Costall

of the functional acts the same actions would be scored 2000). A Friedman two-way ANOVA by ranks revealed
as one integrated single scheme combination. that there were no significant differences between the
The reliability of the coding scheme was assessed three groups of children in the total amount of play time
by having a trained graduate student code four tapes, spent in functional play acts, χ2 (2) = 0.93, ns, or in the
randomly selected, from each of the groups of children. total number of functional acts produced, χ2 (2) = 2.48,
This amounted to approximately 25% of the corpus. Al- ns. However, finer analysis did reveal reliable
though the graduate was aware of the first author’s area differences in both the variety and quality of the func-
of research, she was blind to the specific purpose of the tional play between the different groups of children.
current study. The student was previously trained in the
coding procedure and operation of the Observer sys-
tem, using six tapes, two from each of the groups. Re- Diversity of Functional Play
liability coding was begun once a satisfactory overall
New versus repeated functional acts. The func-
percentage agreement of 70% or more was reached dur-
tional play was first broken down into new and repeated
ing training, between the graduate coder and the first
acts to examine its diversity (Table III). A Friedman
author. The resulting Cohen’s kappas were all within
two-way ANOVA revealed a significant difference be-
acceptable limits. Those for the five functional play cat-
tween the three groups with respect to both the num-
egories listed in Table II ranged from .79–.91 with a
ber of new acts produced, χ2 (2) = 8.87, p = .012 and
mean of .84. Kappas for new versus repeated and inte-
the mean percentage of total time coded spent per-
grated versus nonintegrated functional acts were .81
forming new functional acts with the toys, χ2 (2) = 8.13,
and .75 respectively (Cohen, 1960).
p = .017. Pairwise comparisons using the Wilcoxon
signed rank test indicated that the children with autism
RESULTS spent significantly less of their play time in perform-
ing new functional acts relative to both the typical in-
Exploratory data analysis revealed that the as- fants (z = 2.27, p = .023) and the children with Down
sumptions both of normality and homogeneity of vari- syndrome (z = 3.81, p = .001). (SPSS version 7.51 now
ance between groups were not satisfied. As a result the automatically converts the T to a z value, so that it can
decision was made to use nonparametric statistics (Field, calculate the exact probability of the test statistic by

Table III. Mean Duration (as a Percentage of Total Play Time) and Mean Frequency of Different
Types of Functional Play Behaviors Produced by Each Group of Children

Typical Autistic Down

Type of functional play M SD M SD M SD

Simple
% Duration 7.18 8.00 13.64 11.22 11.68 9.69
Frequency 7.60 6.39 12.60 11.21 12.40 9.37
Elaborated
% Duration 10.44 11.68 0.10a 0.37 10.07 7.85
Frequency 8.27 8.82 0.13a 0.52 7.93 7.59
New
% Duration 8.21 7.48 2.83b 1.83 8.82 4.86
Frequency 7.33 4.86 3.40b 1.96 7.80 4.16
Repeated
% Duration 9.47 9.05 10.95 10.53 12.97 11.92
Frequency 8.67 6.72 9.47 10.60 12.53 12.15
Integrated
% Duration 3.29 5.88 0.10c 0.37 2.84 4.17
Frequency 1.33 1.95 0.07c 0.26 1.07 1.28

a
Autistic mean significantly lower than both Down and Typical at p < .01.
b
Autistic mean significantly lower than Down at p < .01 and from Typical at p < .05.
c
Autistic mean significantly lower than both Down and Typical at p < .05.
Autism and Functional Play 73

approximation to the normal distribution, Field, 2000.) produced virtually no elaborated functional play, analy-
They also produced significantly fewer new functional ses of group differences between the children with
play acts than either the typically developing infants autism and the two control groups in these subcategories
(z = 2.59, p = .01) or the children with Down syndrome were not undertaken. Wilcoxon matched-pairs tests
(z = 2.84, p = .005). There were no significant differ- showed that the only significant difference with regard
ences between the other two groups in either frequency to the subcategories of simple functional play was the
or duration of new functional play acts produced. A greater amount of time spent by the children with
Friedman two-way ANOVA indicated no significant autism in acts involving the functional use of single
differences between the groups with respect to the pro- objects (z = 1.93, p = .053) relative to the typically de-
portion of the total playtime spent producing acts that veloping infants. There were no significant differences
were repetitions of previous ones and the total number between the Down and typical groups in any of the five
of repeated acts performed. subcategories.
Correlations of Developmental age with elabo-
rated functional play. Within-group correlations were
Quality of Functional Play
performed for the typical and Down groups between
Simple and elaborated functional play. The func- the children’s Bayley scores and the proportion of time
tional play was further broken down into simple func- spent in elaborated functional play, in order to assess
tional play acts and more elaborated functional play acts whether the more developmentally advanced children
(see Table III). Friedman tests were performed to com- produced the most advanced play. Kendall’s Tau was
pare the groups on both the number of simple and elab- used, rather than the Spearman’s coefficient, due to the
orated acts produced and the percentage of total time number of tied ranks (Field, 2000). A highly positive
spent in performing each kind of play act. Although relationship was found between developmental age and
there were no significant differences between the groups the proportion of elaborated functional play in the typ-
on either of these measures with respect to simple func- ical group (Kendall’s T = 0.65, p = .001). In the case
tional play acts, there were highly reliable differences of the Down group the correlation coefficient was much
between the different children in both the duration, χ2 more modest (Kendall’s T = 0.32, p = .05) though still
(2) = 14.58, p = .001, and frequency, χ2 (2) = 14.58, significant. It was not possible to perform a correlation
p = .001, of elaborated functional play behaviors. Pair- in the case of the children with autism, given that they
wise comparisons revealed that the children with autism produced virtually no elaborated functional play.
spent less of their total play time performing elaborated
functional acts than both the children with Down syn-
Integrated Functional Play
drome (z = 3.18, p = .001) and the typical infants (z =
2.85, p = .004) and, in addition, produced fewer of these Although little integrated functional play occurred
type of acts than the Down (z = 3.19, p < .001) and typ- in any of the groups of children, Friedman tests revealed
ical (z = 2.85, p = .004) groups (see Table III). There significant between-group differences in duration, χ2
were no differences between these latter groups with re- (2) = 5.89, p = .05, and frequency, χ2 (2) = 5.89, p =
spect to either of the measures of elaborated functional .05. Both the typical and Down syndrome groups spent
play. The children with autism produced significantly more time in integrated sequences of functional acts
fewer elaborated functional acts relative to simple func- (typical: z = 2.20, p = .028, Down: z = 2.31, p = .021)
tional acts (z = 3.30, p = .001) and spent less of their and produced a higher frequency of such actions (typi-
total play time in performing elaborated acts (z = 3.30, cal: z = 2.12, p < .034, Down: z = 2.39, p = .016).
p = .001) compared to simple functional acts. In fact
only 1 of the 15 children with autism produced any act
DISCUSSION
in this category, compared to 11 of the typical children
and 13 of the Down group. In contrast there were no
Summary of Findings
differences in these two categories of play in the other
two groups of children. Although the children with autism spent less time
Subcategories of simple and elaborated functional engaged in functional play and produced fewer func-
play. In addition to analyses of the main categories of tional acts than either of the control groups, these dif-
functional play (simple and elaborated), between-group ferences did not reach significance. However, a closer
differences in the subcategories of simple functional analysis of the composition of the functional play shown
play were also investigated. As the children with autism by each of the groups did reveal distinct differences in
74 Williams, Reddy, and Costall

terms of its diversity, elaboration, and integration. In those which occur at the earliest stage of this decon-
contrast to the children with Down syndrome and the textualization process, such as bringing a bottle, cup, or
typically developing infants, who spent equal amounts spoon to the mouth, may indicate a difficulty in elabo-
of time in simple and elaborated functional play, the rating on their initial ability to associate a particular
functional play produced by the autistic group consisted object function with a particular miniaturised toy.
almost entirely of simple acts involving single objects, Likewise, the data from this study provide some ev-
such as bringing a cup to the mouth, or pushing a car idence indicating that the children with autism may have
along the ground. In fact, only one child with autism pro- difficulty in making the transition from self-directed to
duced any behavior that could be classed in the elabo- other-directed functional play. Initially, pretend actions
rated functional play category. Further analysis of the are directed towards the self, for example, the child
diversity of functional acts revealed that the children brushes their own hair, before being directed towards
with autism produced fewer different acts than the typ- another, such as their parent or a doll, and then to these
ical or Down syndrome children and spent less of their as an agent. No child with autism showed any functional
play time in functional play that was new compared to act with a doll, whereas doll-directed acts were the most
both control groups. The children with autism also spent common elaborated functional acts produced by both
less time in integrated functional play and produced control groups. Although no category of self-directed
fewer functional sequences than the controls. Caution functional play was included in the coding scheme, a
needs to be exercised in relation to this latter finding, review of the tapes revealed that all groups of children
however, as only half of the children in each of the con- produced, on average, two different self-directed func-
trol groups produced integrated functional combinations. tional acts, suggesting that those with autism did not
In order to examine this particular aspect of play a more have a particular difficulty with this kind of functional
developmentally advanced group would be required. play. However, to fully support the proposal that chil-
dren with autism experience difficulties in decentra-
tion, a dimension of self- versus other-directed functional
Problems in Decontextualization, Decentration,
acts would need to be included in the coding scheme.
and Integration
Although a high level of sequencing of functional acts
The finding that the functional play of children was not demonstrated by any of the groups, there is ad-
with autism is less elaborated, less diverse, and less in- ditional evidence that supports the interpretation that
tegrated than that shown by the control groups is not the children with autism may be at an earlier point than
consistent with the theory-of-mind hypothesis that this the controls in the move towards the integration of pre-
type of play should be intact, whereas “symbolic” play tend play. It could be argued that the subcategories,
(the only play supposedly involving metarepresentation) functional use of multiple objects and functional acts
is impaired (Baron-Cohen et al., 1985). Rather, it sug- supported by relevant vocalization/exaggerated gestures,
gests that the children with autism experience problems also involve a degree of integration. Only one child
at a much earlier point than that where “true symbolic with autism produced an act that could be classed in
play” (according to the definition of Leslie, 1987) is either of these categories.
said to emerge. Early forms of pretend play occur when The finding that the children with autism produced
the infant takes functional acts, such as putting a spoon virtually no elaborated functional play, along with the
to the mouth or talking on the telephone, and reproduces significant positive correlations5 indicating that in both
them in play. These are decontextualized behaviors, de- control groups the more developmentally advanced chil-
tached from the situational context in which they orig- dren tended to produce the most advanced functional
inally occurred and goals they usually accomplish (e.g., play, suggests that the children with autism were at a
eating or communicating). Research investigating the lower level of development in play than the other chil-
play behavior of typical infants suggests that this dren. However, the findings of this study do not, nec-
process begins with initial approximations, such as essarily, point simply to a developmental delay in the
bringing a toy spoon to the lips, which are then elabo- case of the children with autism. The reduced diversity
rated (e.g., bringing a spoon to the lips and smacking and elaboration of their functional play may be better
the lips) before becoming gradually less dependent on
the conventional object being present, so that a substi- 5
The more modest correlation coefficient found in the Down group
tute object, such as a rod, can replace the spoon (Vondra may be explained by the lower levels of variation among the chil-
& Belsky, 1991). The finding that almost all of the func- dren with Down syndrome in the amount of elaborated functional
tional acts produced by the children with autism are play produced, relative to the typically developing children.
Autism and Functional Play 75

explained, not as a result of any specific problems with sity and elaboration of functional play exhibited by the
functional play per se, but as stemming from more gen- children with autism might arise as a consequence of
eral difficulties, whether in executive functioning, in their difficulties in relating to other people, rather than
relating to other people, or a combination of both. deficits in executive functioning. Parents and caregivers
play a crucial role in introducing their child to the
shared meanings of things. They highlight the salient
Executive Dysfunction Hypothesis
features of objects, explicitly demonstrate their func-
In two particular respects the data from this study tional use, physically structure their child’s actions with
are more consonant with the predictions of those theo- an object, verbally suggest (and prompt) functional
rists who propose that autism is an executive disorder, acts, and provide a model (not necessarily intentional)
rather than one involving a metarepresentational deficit. for their child to imitate. It is likely that such processes
First, the finding that the functional play of the chil- of social mediation would be seriously disrupted in
dren with autism was less diverse than that of the con- the case of young children with autism who show spe-
trols and involved proportionately more repetition than cific deficits in language, imitation, and joint attention
novelty is consistent with the predictions made by pro- (Abrahamson & Mitchell, 1990; Dawson & Adams,
ponents of an executive dysfunction in autism. The 1984; Landry & Loveland, 1988; Loveland & Landry,
production of fewer different functional acts can be 1986; Mundy et al., 1986; Smith & Bryson, 1994) and
explained as a result of the difficulty that individuals have particular problems in making use of other peo-
with autism have in generating new schemes. Accord- ple’s actions and affective responses to objects to guide
ing to this approach the predominance of repetitive over their own use of the same objects (Sigman, Kasari,
novel functional behavior might arise as a secondary Kwon, & Yirmiya, 1992).
consequence of the primary deficit in generating alter- Problems in using other people as a source of guid-
native behavior (Jarrold, 1997). The converse is also ance for how to use particular objects may account for
possible, that a tendency to engage in perseverative be- the reduced diversity of the functional play produced by
havior interferes with the initiation of new acts (Lewis the children with autism. Without inspiration from other
& Boucher, 1988; Russell et al., 1991). The production people these children are left with their own, more lim-
of fewer new acts, whether a product of generative or ited, ideas of interesting actions on objects (Lord, 1985).
perseverative executive deficits, may serve to disrupt The finding that the children with autism had particular
the differentiation process, delaying the emergence of difficulty with elaborated, but not simple, functional play
more elaborated functional and symbolic play acts. Sec- could be explained as a result of the former being much
ond, the finding of qualitative differences in the func- more dependent than the latter on mediation by other
tional play produced by the autistic group is not in- people. Whereas simple functional acts such as pushing
consistent with the executive dysfunction hypothesis a car along the ground, or bringing a baby bottle to the
which, unlike the metarepresentational account, does mouth, can be acquired through instruction by other peo-
not make an a priori distinction between functional and ple, they could equally well be arrived at through soli-
symbolic play (Jarrold, 1997). According to this ap- tary exploration of the object itself (see Rocissano,
proach pretense develops gradually, with the develop- 1982). In other words, the physical structure of the ob-
mental trends of decontextualization and decentration ject itself may help to shape the functional action with-
observed in typical development reflecting a shift away out a need to fully appreciate its significance in social-
from externally driven and habitual schemes towards behavioral terms. This raises the question of whether
more flexible, internally generated, and planned action such acts indeed necessarily represent a “functional” use
(Harris, 1993; Jarrold, 1997). of a toy from the child’s point of view, or only from our
own. In contrast, the nature of the object itself is insuf-
ficient to support or constrain the production of elabo-
Social Deficit Hypothesis
rated functional acts, which are less open to acquisition
Another plausible hypothesis is that the problems via solitary exploration. Such conventional actions may
lie not with the production of functional schemes but require a greater degree of participation with other peo-
with their acquisition. Conventional actions on objects ple in a shared environment “which predisposes people
are not predetermined but are acquired from other mem- to use objects, interpret events, and so on, in particular
bers of the social group (Leontiev, 1981; Loveland, ways” (Loveland, 1991, p. 110). To produce acts such
1991; Rocissano, 1982; Valsiner, 1987; Williams et al., as vocalizing on a toy phone or putting a doll to bed, the
1999). According to this proposition the lower diver- child needs to go beyond the physical properties of an
76 Williams, Reddy, and Costall

object and appreciate its social significance (Lord, 1985). Baby bottle
A third position is also possible, that executive dys- Bath
functions and impoverished social interactions both im- Cloth (30 × 20 cms.)
pact adversely on play in autism. Children with autism Cooker (Hob)
may be thrown back on their own ideas of interesting A selection of plastic food
actions as a result of a difficulty in learning about ob- Two saucepans
jects from other people, which is further compounded Hand puppet
by problems in generating knowledge-of-object-use Finger puppet
schemes. Problems in elaborating on simple functional Train
acts may therefore reflect a lesser degree of participa- Police car
tion with other people in a shared environment combined Sports car
with a difficulty in moving away from actions deter- Small ball
mined by externally imposed constraints. Rattle
Telephone
Hair brush
Future Studies Dustpan and brush
The ways in which specific executive and social Music box
deficits might impact adversely on the play of children Plastic hinged box
with autism need to be explored further. The issue of Watering-can
the social mediation of object use has been particularly
neglected. To assess how difficulties in social interac- ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
tion might affect the development of functional play in
children with autism object-centered play with parents This research was supported by Grant R00023
needs to be examined. However it is not only the de- 4949 from the Economic and Social Research Council.
velopment of functional play in autism that requires We are extremely grateful to all the parents and chil-
further research. What is needed are longitudinal stud- dren who participated in this study for their time, in-
ies following the development of play over time, both terest and helpfulness. We also thank Sarah Libby,
solitary and dyadic, using a detailed coding scheme to Mike Van Duuren, and three anonymous reviewers for
cover play behaviors from the earliest manipulations to their invaluable comments on earlier versions of the
the most elaborate forms of pretense. At present we manuscript.
know virtually nothing about the precursors to func-
tional play in young children with autism and such stud-
ies would enable us to examine how early behaviors REFERENCES
might impact on later ones. Gender and CA also need Abrahamsen, E. P., & Mitchell, J. R. (1990). Communication and
to be taken into account in future studies, as both could sensorimotor functioning in children with autism. Journal of
affect the child’s experience with the toys. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 75–85.
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical
In conclusion, although the data from this study manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington DC.: Author.
do not address the question of the relationship between Atlas, J. A. (1990) Play in assessment and intervention in child-
functional and symbolic play (as defined by Leslie, hood psychoses. Child Psychiatry in Human Development, 21,
119–133.
1987), it has clearly revealed qualitative differences in Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and
the functional play of the children with autism that theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
would not be predicted by the metarepresentational Baron-Cohen, S. (1987). Autism and symbolic play. British Journal
of Developmental Psychology, 5, 139–148.
theory of symbolic play. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M. & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic
child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition, 21, 37–46.
Bayley, N. (1993). Bayley Scales of Infant Development (2nd ed.).
San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace.
APPENDIX Beeghly, M. (1998). Emergence of symbolic play: perspectives from
typical and atypical development. In J. A. Burack, R. M. Ho-
List of Toys Used in Study dapp, & E. Zigler (Eds.), Handbook of mental retardation and
development (pp. 240–289) . Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Uni-
Tea-set (including cups, saucers, jug, plates, bowls versity Press.
Belsky, J., & Most, R. K. (1981). From exploration to play: A cross-
and cutlery) sectional study of infant free play behaviour. Developmental
Two dolls Psychology, 17, 630–639.
Autism and Functional Play 77

Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20, 37–46. of joint attention and language development in autistic children.
Dawson, G., & Adams, A. (1984). Imitation and social responsive- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 115–128.
ness in autistic children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1986). Defin-
12, 209–225. ing the social deficits of autism: The contribution of non-verbal
El’Konin. (1966). Symbolics and its function in the play of children. communication measures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy-
Soviet Education, 8, 139–148. chiatry, 27, 657–669.
Fein, G. G., & Apfel, N. (1979). The development of play: Style Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ungerer, J., & Sherman, T. (1987). Nonver-
structure and situations. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 99, bal communication and play correlates of language development
231–250. in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis-
Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D., Bates, E., Hartung, orders, 17, 349–364.
J. P., Pethick, S., & Reilly, J. S. (1993). MacArthur Commu- Nicolich, L. (1977). Beyond sensorimotor intelligence. Merrill-
nicative Development Inventories. San Diego, CA: Singular. Palmer Quarterly, 23, 89–99.
Fenson, L., Kagan, L., Kearsley, R. B., & Zelazo, P. R. (1976). The Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. London:
developmental progression of manipulative play in the first Routledge & Kegan-Paul.
2 years. Child Development, 47, 232–236. Riguet, C. B., Taylor, N. D., Benaroya, S., & Klein, L. S. (1981).
Fenson, L., & Ramsay, D. S. (1980). Decentration and integration of Symbolic play in autistic, Down, and normal children of equiv-
the child’s play in the second year. Child Development, 51, alent mental age. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis-
171–178. orders, 11, 439–448.
Field, A. (2000). Discovering statistics using SPSS for Windows. Rocissano, L. (1982). The emergence of social conventional behav-
London: Sage. iour: evidence from early object play. Social Cognition, 1, 50–69.
Gould, J. (1986) The Lowe and Costello symbolic play test in so- Russell, J., Mauthner, N., Sharpe, S., & Tidswell, T. (1991). The
cially impaired children. Journal of Autism and Developmental “windows task” as a measure of strategic deception and pre-
Disorders, 16, 199–213. schoolers and autistic subjects, British Journal of Developmen-
Harris, P. L. (1993). Pretending and planning. In S. Baron-Cohen, tal Psychology, 9, 331–349.
H. Tager-Flusberg, D. Cohen, & F. Volkmar (Eds.), Under- Sigman, M., Kasari, C., Kwon, J., & Yirmiya, N. (1992) Responses
standing other minds: Perspectives from autism. Oxford: Oxford to the negative emotions of others by autistic, mentally retarded,
University Press. and normal children. Child Development, 63, 796–807.
Jarrold, C. (1997). Pretend play in autism: executive explanations. Sigman, M., & Ungerer, J. A. (1984). Cognitive and language skills
In J. Russell (Ed.), Autism as an executive disorder. Oxford: in autistic, mentally retarded and normal children. Develop-
Oxford University Press. mental Psychology, 20, 293–302.
Jarrold, C., Boucher, J., & Smith, P. (1993). Symbolic play in chil- Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (1994). Imitation and action in autism.
dren with autism: A review. Journal of Autism and Develop- Psychological Bulletin, 116, 259–273.
mental Disorders, 23, 281–309. Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984). Vineland
Jarrold, C., Boucher, J., & Smith, P. (1994). Executive function deficits Adaptive Behavior Scales, Interview edition. Circle Pines, MN:
and the pretend play of children with autism. A research note. American Guidance Service.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1473–1482. Ungerer, J. A., & Sigman, M. (1981). Symbolic play and language
Jarrold, C., Boucher, J., & Smith, P. (1996). Generativity deficits in comprehension in autistic children. Journal of the American
pretend play in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psy- Academy of Child Psychiatry, 20, 318–337.
chology, 14, 275–300. Valsiner, J. (1987). Culture and the development of children’s action:
Landry, S. H., & Loveland, K. A. (1988). Communication behav- a cultural historical theory. Chichester, UK: Wiley.
iours in autism and developmental language delay. Journal of Volkmar, F. R., & Cohen, D. J. (1985). Infantile autism: A first-
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 29, 621–634. person account by Tony W. Journal of Autism and Develop-
Leontiev, A. N. (1981). Problems of the development of mind. mental Disorders, 15, 47–54.
Moscow: Progress Publishers. Vondra, J., & Belsky, J. (1989). Infant play at one year: Character-
Leslie, A. (1987) Pretence and representation in infancy: Origins of istics and early antecedents. In J. J. Lockman & N. L. Hazen
“theory of mind.” Psychological Review, 94, 84–106. (Eds.), Action in social context (pp. 173–206). New York:
Lewis, V., & Boucher, J. (1988). Spontaneous, instructed and Plenum Press.
elicited play in relatively able autistic children. British Journal Vondra, J., & Belsky, J. (1991). Infant play as a window on compe-
of Developmental Psychology, 6, 325–338. tence and motivation. In C. E. Schaefer, K. Gitlin, & A. Sandgrund
Libby, S., Powell, S., Messer, D., & Jordan, R. (1998). Spontaneous (Eds.), Play. Diagnosis & assessment. Toronto, Canada: Wiley.
pretend play in children with autism: A reappraisal. Journal of Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. The development of higher
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, 487–497. psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Lord, C. (1985). Autism and the comprehension of language. In Press.
E. Shopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Communication problems Werner, H., & Kaplan, K. (1963). Symbol formation. An organismic-
in autism (pp. 257–281). New York: Plenum Press. developmental approach to language and the expression of
Loveland, K. A. (1991). Social affordances and interaction II: Autism thought. New York: Wiley.
and the affordances of the human environment. Ecological Psy- Whyte, J., & Owens, A. (1989). Language and symbolic play: some
chology, 3, 99–119. findings from a study of autistic children, Irish Journal of Psycho-
Loveland, K. A., & Landry, S. H. (1986). Joint attention and lan- logy, 10, 317–332.
guage in autism and developmental language delay. Journal of Williams, E., Costall, A., & Reddy, V. (1999). Children with autism
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16, 335–349. experience problems with both objects and people. Journal of
Marino, B. L. (1988). Assessment of infant play: Applications to Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 367–378.
research and practice. Issues in Comprehensive Paediatric Wing, L., Gould, J., Yeates S. R., & Brierley, L. M. (1977). Sym-
Nursing, 11, 227–240. bolic play in severely mentally retarded and in autistic children.
McCune-Nicolich, L., & Fenson, L. (1984). Methodological issues Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 18, 167–178.
in studying early pretend play. In T. D. Yawkey & A. D. Pelle- World Health Organization. (1990). International Classification of Dis-
grini (Eds.), Child’s play: Developmental and applied issues. eases (10th ed). Diagnostic criteria for research (draft). Geneva:
Hillsdale, N. J. LEA. Author.

You might also like