You are on page 1of 13

Piaget and play: translating theory into

practice

S. Gray Garwood, PhD


Associate Professor
Department of Psychology
T HE DEVELOPMENT and functions of
play are becoming increasingly impor-
tant areas of investigation, especially the sig-
Newcomb College nificance of play for cognitive (e.g., Elder &
Tulane University Pederson, 1978; Fein, 1981; Ghiaci & Rich-
New Orleans, Louisiana ardson, 1980; Ungerer, Zelazo, Kearsley, &
O'Leary, 1981) and social development (e.g.,
Bruner, Jolly, & Sylva, 1976; Garvey, 1977;
Guralnick, 1981; Vandenberg, 1981). In addi-
tion, play behavior is being used to understand
atypical development (e.g., Ungerer & Sig-
man, 1981; Whittaker, 1980; Wing, Gould,
Yeates, & Brierly, 1977), and play is coming to
be seen as both an assessment (e.g., Belsky &
Most, 1981) and an intervention strategy (e.g.,
Guralnick, 1981; Pelligrini, 1980).
Play is a difficult concept to define, how-
ever. As Vandenberg (1978) pointed out, part
of this difficulty is due to the multiplicity of
behaviors subsumed under the concept: "The
serious assembly of airplane models by human
adolescents is lumped together with the frolic
of dogs and the repetitive motor play of
infants" (p. 725). Despite such difficulty,
Garvey (1977) isolated five components to
play behavior. Play is

TECSE, 1982, 2(3), 01-13


0271-1214/82/0023-0001$2.00
© 1982 Aspen Systems Corporation

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 13, 2016


TOPICS IN EARLY CHILDHOOD SPECIAL EDUCATION / OCTOBER 1982

• pleasurable and positively valued for its they grow older and that the earlier forms are
own sake by the person playing; developmentally less sophisticated than later
• intrinsically, not extrinsically, motivated; forms. Consequently, one would expect that
• spontaneous and voluntary; parallel and solitary play, which in Parten's
• characterized by some active involvement scheme implies more egocentric and self-
by the player; and centered behavior, would occur more fre-
• related in certain systematic ways to what quently among younger children and that
is not play. associative and cooperative play, which
Although there is confusion regarding what requires more social interaction ability, would
actually constitutes play, most scientists agree occur more frequently among older preschool-
on its function and agree that it includes ers. Although this is generally so, Parten's
developmental sequences. hierarchy has recently been challenged in
terms of the developmental placement and
VIEWS ON PLAY BEHAVIOR function of solitary play. For example, Moore,
Evertson, and Brophy (1974) suggested that
Parten's typology solitary play is not necessarily an immature
Based on systematic observations of 2- to form of play behavior because it involves
5-year-old nursery school children in play goal-directed activity, large muscle play, and
situations Parten (1932) devised a system for educational play.
classifying children's developing sociability in In a longitudinal study of social participa-
terms of play behavior they were likely to tion, using time-sampling observation meth-
engage in at different ages. Parten's system ods, Smith (1978) observed 48 preschool chil-
includes the following five developmental dren over a period of 9 months, beginning
play levels: when they were, on the average, 33 months
• solitary play, characterized by children old. His data indicated that play behavior
playing alone with their own toys or activ- develops through three different sequences
ities and making no attempts to interact rather than in one developmental trend, as
with other nearby children; suggested by Parten. Smith found that some
• onlooker behavior, characterized by inter- preschoolers start with group play and remain
est in the activities of other children but at that level, though some solitary play
no active participation in their play occurred among children in this group. Other
behavior; children started with solitary play behavior
• parallel play, characterized by one child's and then directly moved to group play behav-
independent play activities occurring in ior, although some children used parallel play
tandem with another child's play, thus behavior as an intermediate step. Finally,
promoting close contact but little if any Smith found that some children either
mutual play; remained at solitary play activities over the 9
• associative play, characterized by the months or varied between solitary and parallel
sharing, borrowing, and lending of play play behavior. Also, whereas children in the
materials among children; and first-mentioned group were significantly older
• cooperative play, characterized by goal- than the children in the remaining two groups,
oriented group activity that is typically no age differences distinguished the remain-
directed by one or two group members. ing two groups. Smith concluded that solitary
Parten proposed that children pass through play is not necessarily a less mature form of
these successively more social stages of play as social interaction among children. Instead, it

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 13, 2016


PIAGET AND PLAY

may reflect a coping mechanism used by observed nursery school children during six
children to rehearse particular skills before exposures to a novel object (a complicated
trying them out in group situations; thus soli- gadget). The behavior comprising exploration
tary play may be a more mature form than was more tightly organized than was play
parallel play. behavior.

Ethology Cognitive-developmental perspective

In ethological theory, play is viewed as an Piaget (1962) wrote extensively about the
important survival mechanism. According to contribution of play to intellectual develop-
Vandenberg (1978), ment; his views provide the theoretical ration-
ale for the use of play as an intervention
Those that must adapt to a more social world strategy.
require more play than those who do not. Similarly, Piaget (1962) regarded play as critical to
with ontogenetic development, the structure of play cognitive development, especially in the
must also change ... if play is to serve the evolving child's early years. He distinguished three
and changing adaptive needs of the organism.
developmental levels of play: sensorimotor,
These changes in the form of play are intimately
meshed with the organism's intentional abilities and symbolic, and games with rules. Sensorimotor
its cognitive and representational abilities (p. 736). play usually consists of repeating various pre-
viously learned behaviors, apparently to pro-
What this means is that as organisms become vide pleasure and not to attain a particular
more complex and less subject to strictly bio- goal. This type of play activity predominates
logical or reflexive responding, they must during the child's first 2 years of life, before
symbolic and communicative abilities devel-
op.
Play becomes more critical with Symbolic play, which parallels preopera-
increasing phylogenetic complexity and tional thought, increases as the child acquires
increasing environmental demands. ability to use mental symbols to represent
experience. During this period, pretend or
make-believe play begins (see Fein, 1981, for a
develop their own coping mechanisms. In chil- recent review). Symbolic play requires the
dren, these coping skills follow a sequence: child not only to begin to take the role of
exploration (tentative interactions with envi- another (either animate or inanimate) but also
ronmental objects or events to determine their to recall past experience for present use.
nature and safety), play (non-goal-oriented Obviously, both of these are cognitively
experimental activity), and application (goal- based.
oriented behavior). Play becomes more critical In addition, as Sigel and Cocking (1977)
with increasing phylogenetic complexity and pointed out, "Ordering the play themes and
increasing environmental demands. This view the sequence of events is not fixed in the
is compatible with Smith's reinterpretation of physical world but rather fixed in the heads of
the function of solitary play. the participants. Thus to engage in play, espe-
It follows that play and exploratory activity cially when it involves others, requires the
differ, both in function (exploration is an child to engage in an array of interactions
information-seeking activity) and in organiza- which can produce conflict, logical necessity,
tion (to obtain useful information behavior and consequently the need for resolution of
must be organized). Hughes (1978, 1979) these difficulties" (p. 58).

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 13, 2016


TOPICS IN EARLY CHILDHOOD SPECIAL EDUCATION / OCTOBER 1982

Games with rules, the third level of play, tation, and play are concerned, "imitation is a
parallels concrete operational abilities and continuation of accommodation, play a contin-
requires higher levels of sociability; rule-based uation of assimilation, and intelligence a har-
cooperative and competitive activities domi- monious combination of the two'' (p. 104).
nate the school years. (This last stage of play
will not be considered here because it is PLAY AND IMITATION DURING THE
beyond the scope of this journal.) SENSORIMOTOR YEARS
According to Piaget, the development of
intelligence is aided by development of both During the first few years, cognitive devel-
imitation and play behavior. Imitation is abil- opment is based primarily in physical action.
ity to reproduce observable behavior or unob- Although apparently simple, sensorimotor
servable behavior or both. Imitation is linked development is complex enough to entail six
to mental ability because it not only requires descriptive substages, which are discussed in
making observations of one's own and others' the following sections.
complex behavior but also requires repre-
Stage One: reflexive activity (birth to about
senting these observations by either internal
symbolic processes or overt mediational 1 month)
behavior or both. Piaget believed imitative Throughout their first month, infants'
skills increase tremendously from birth to reflexive behavior becomes organized into
about age 2. Imitative skills aid mental growth schemes through exercise. Infants assimilate
by contributing to the differentiation, coordi- experience without making the accommoda-
nation, and generalizing of existing behaviors tions necessary for adaptation. They neither
and by enabling the child to add new behav- imitate nor play. The repetitious motor prac-
iors. "Imitation through action, an accommo- tice at this stage is instead probably the exer-
dation to models that are present, gradually cise of essential instincts (Piaget, 1962).
extends to deferred imitation and finally to
interiorized imitation. In this last form it con- Stage Two: primary circular reactions
stitutes the origin of mental imagery" (Piaget, (about 1-4 months)
1976, p. 173). During this stage, infants begin to attempt
As imitation is accommodatory, play is to adapt to experience. Among the more sig-
assimilatory. That is, in play "the characteris- nificant adaptations is the coordination and
tics of the object are not taken into account i n t e g r a t i o n of p r e v i o u s l y i n d e p e n d e n t
except insofar as they are consistent with the schemes, which enable the infant to control
subject's momentary interests. . . . The most actions in order to look at what is heard, to
common form of this . . . in the play of the suck what is grasped, and eventually to reach
child is the 'symbolic games' . . . in which for what is seen. Sporadic imitation occurs
objects at his command are used only to repre- during this state. Once reflexive schemes
sent what is imagined'' (Piaget, 1976, p. 173). develop into differentiated responses, new
Through play young children develop particu- gestures become possible. Finger sucking, dif-
lar skills and gain knowledge of how responses ferentiated crying, vocalizing and visual
can be coordinated for better problem-solving accommodation all involve imitation as well as
ability. Play is thus an important factor in the adaptation.
development of intelligence. As Piaget (1962) Play behavior develops from these adaptive
noted, so far as sensorimotor intelligence, imi- reactions. That is, the child may continue to

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 13, 2016


PIAGET AND PLAY

reproduce efforts to accommodate purely for Stage Four: coordination of secondary


practice or pleasure, without any expectation schemes (about 8-12 months)
that the previously learned adaptive outcome This stage marks the emergence of inten-
will occur. For example, Piaget noted that his tional goal-directed behavior. Infants begin to
son, at about 3 months of age, learned to throw anticipate external events that occur indepen-
his head back to look at familiar things from a dently of their own behavior and also adapt
different perspective but once he had learned more effectively to new and unfamiliar expe-
this response and its consequences, he repeat- riences. Infants are increasingly able to recom-
edly threw his head back, laughing loudly bine schemes developed during Stage Three
each time. Thus, Stage Two play varies slightly into schemes that are more useful for attaining
from adaptive assimilation and is mostly motor goals, solving problems, and adapting to new
practice. experiences; these schemes no longer serve
only to reproduce interesting or familiar
Stage Three: secondary circular reactions events.
(about 4-8 months) The infant begins to use objects as tools to
The accomplishment of eye-hand coordina- attain goals and is better able to ignore
tion during Stage Two contributes to a major acquired associations of objects with responses;
cognitive breakthrough: Infants begin to shift the infant is more interested in exploring novel
their attention to effects their responses have objects. During Stage Three novelty captured
on the external environment. Stage Three is the infant's curiosity only briefly, and intellec-
thus characterized by attempts to consolidate tual interest was centered on the object pri-
and coordinate responses to produce interest- marily because of its function. During Stage
ing environmental effects. Schemes so pro- Four, "the unfamiliar obviously represents to
duced represent acquired patterns of respond- the child an external reality, to which he must
ing; they are not yet used to intentionally adapt himself.... [T]he child .. . gives more
adapt to new experiences. the impression of making an experiment than
During this stage, an infant can only imitate of generalizing his behavior patterns: He tries
familiar and observable gestures. Differences to Understand' " (Piaget, 1962, p. 259).
in the quality of play are tied now to the Imitation of own body movements that are
child's growing reliance on the use of objects. not easily observed by the child (i.e., invisible
Once the infant has accommodated to an movements) and of new models begins to
object and exploratory behavior ceases, the occur with coordination of schemes, for this
child's behavior with that object becomes allows coordination of the visual and tactile-
gamelike. For example, at 3 months of age, kinesthetic schemes needed for such imitation.
Piaget's daughter discovered she could make However, such imitation is imprecise. Coordi-
objects hanging from the top of her crib swing. nation of schemes also makes possible a change
"Subsequently . . . [between 4 and 9 months] in the nature of play. First, the behavior
. . . she never indulged in this activity . . . learned through the process of combining
without a show of great joy and power. Assimi- known schemes to solve a new problem can be
lation was no longer accompanied by accom- exercised as play practice. Second, and per-
modation and therefore was no longer an haps of more interest, the child is now capable
effort at comprehension" (1962, p. 82). Thus, of combining schemes for playful (ludic) pur-
play continues to be motor practice but begins poses, that is, for a purpose other than problem
to involve external objects. solving.

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 13, 2016


TOPICS IN EARLY CHILDHOOD SPECIAL EDUCATION / OCTOBER 1982

Furthermore, these combinations begin to


take on the appearance of a ritual. Piaget This increased ritualization of schemes
noted that his daughter, while holding a implies that the child is making
fringed pillow, noticed the fringe and began to additional progress toward symbolic
suck it. "This action, which reminded her of play: Because these frivolous rituals
what she did everyday before going to sleep, contain borrowed elements, they reflect
caused her to lie down on her side, in the abstractions of schemes from their
position for sleep, holding a corner of the original context
fringe and sucking her thumb" (1962, p. 93).
This behavior, which lasted only a few sec-
onds, was interpreted by Piaget to be a precur- to side. But as she had looked under the skin
sor to symbolic play. "All that is needed for the just before putting it in motion, she did it again
ludic ritual to become a symbol is that the . . . at least twenty times; she took the peel,
child, instead of merely following the cycle of turned it over, put it down again, made it sway
his habitual movements, should be aware of and then this began all over again" (1962, p.
the make-believe, i.e., that he should 'pretend' 95). This increased ritualization of schemes
to sleep" (1962, pp. 93-94). implies that the child is making additional
progress toward symbolic play: Because these
Stage Five: tertiary circular reactions (about frivolous rituals contain borrowed elements,
12-18 months) they reflect abstractions of schemes from their
Tertiary circular reactions occur when original context.
familiar responses applied to new objects pro-
duce unexpected effects. The resulting con- Stage Six: invention of new means through
flict stimulates the toddler to explore this new mental combinations (about 18-24 months)
object. The child repeatedly touches the In Stage Six a child's perspective changes
object, varying the form of response each time. dramatically. Children now begin to represent
Children discover new means by active explo- experiences symbolically. Consider the follow-
ration. Although some of the properties may ing interaction between Piaget and his daugh-
be difficult for the child to assimilate to known ter, Lucienne. Piaget hid a watch chain in an
schemes, by systematically manipulating re- empty matchbox. Because the matchbox open-
sponses, he or she begins to discover new ing was only 3 mm wide, Lucienne was unable
means for adapting to novel objects. These to use a familiar response scheme, for example,
new means can then be applied to other new putting her finger inside. Her response,
experiences. instead, was quite sophisticated. Following an
Imitation of invisible movements and of initial attempt to poke her finger into the
new models becomes more systematic, ena- opening,
bling the child to refine imitative activity
through organized and controlled trial and a pause follows during which Lucienne manifests a
very curious reaction. . . . She looks at the slit with
error. Play takes on a frivolous and more
great attention; then several times in succession, she
diverse quality when, through experimenta- opens and shuts her mouth, at first slightly, then
tion, the child creates interesting combinations wider and wider!. . . Apparently Lucienne under-
that then almost instantly become ritualized stands the existence of a cavity subjacent to the slit
play. For example, when Piaget's daughter and wishes to enlarge that cavity. . . . [D]ue to
was about 14 months old "she amused herself inability to think out the situation in words or clear
by making an orange skin . . . sway from side visual images she uses a simple motor indicator as

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 13, 2016


PIAGET AND PLAY

"signifier" or symbol. . . . [T]he motor reaction his daughter's playful symbolic representation
which presents itself for filling this role is none of a drinking utensil "had been prepared for
other than imitation. . . . This scheme of imitation, during the preceding month or two by a
with which she is familiar, constitutes for her the progressive ritualization, the principal stages
means of thinking out the situation. . . . Soon after
of which consisted in playing at drinking out
this phase of plastic reflection, Lucienne unhesitat-
of empty glasses and then repeating the action
ingly puts her fingers in the slit, and, instead, of
making noises with lips and throat" (1962, p.
trying as before to reach the chain, she pulls so as to
enlarge the opening. She succeeds and grasps the 97).
chain. (Piaget, 1962, pp. 337-338) Thus, for the first time the child pretends
during play. The child uses familiar schemes
Although Lucienne might have been able to but without the objects to which they are
solve this problem earlier by trial and error typically applied. The schemes are applied to
(Stage Five), her reaction in this situation new objects, not for any adaptive purpose but
clearly indicated an advance to Stage Six; she only to mimic the original relationships to
reduced overt manipulation before she discov- produce pleasure. (See Fein, 1981, for a thor-
ered an appropriate response. Instead of physi- ough review of research dealing with pretend
cally manipulating the box, she manipulated play.)
her representation of it and identified an
appropriate response before using that PLAY DURING EARLY CHILDHOOD:
response. SYMBOLIC PLAY
Just as problem solving is characterized in
Stage Six by internalized experimentation and The symbolic schemes which make their
coordination before external motor activity, so appearance in Stage Six of the sensorimotor
too is imitation. That is, the accommodatory period are transitional; they are self-oriented,
aspect of adaptation is internalized, enabling repeating only the child's own actions. How-
the child to imitate new models immediately. ever, beginning at about age 2, these symbolic
More importantly, perhaps, the child is now schemes give way to symbolic games, as the
also capable of deferring imitation to a time character of play becomes less self-oriented.
when the model is not present. "The child This new type of symbolic play helps the child
becomes capable of imitating internally a assimilate reality to the ego while freeing the
series of models in the form of images or ego from the need to accommodate and adapt
suggestions of actions. Imitation thus begins to too early to a reality not yet well understood.
reach the level of representation" (Piaget,
1962, p. 62). Piaget *s categories
In Stages One-Five, play moves from Piaget divided symbolic games into several
strictly motor practice to ludic ritualization. levels, ranging over the preoperational period
This transformation is a necessary precursor to of cognitive development. Stage I and Stage II
symbolic play. In Stage Six, this process is symbolic games (1) imply representation of an
completed as ludic symbols become disasso- absent object because the child is comparing a
ciated from ludic ritualizations to form the given and an imagined object; (2) imply make-
symbolic schemes that are necessary for sym- believe or pretense because the comparison is
bolic play. When Piaget's daughter was 19 distorting assimilation (e.g., a box is a glass); (3)
months old, she first pretended to drink from a only subordinate the sensorimotor practice ele-
box. Following this she held the box up to the ment, which in reality may continue as a part
mouths of others present. According to Piaget, of play throughout life; and (4) deviate more

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 13, 2016


TOPICS IN EARLY CHILDHOOD SPECIAL EDUCATION / OCTOBER 1982

Table 1. Outline of Piaget s levels of stage I preoperational symbolic play behavior

Level 1

A. Projecting symbolic schemes onto new objects: B. Projecting imitation schemes onto new objects:
The child, using imitation and now-familiar The child borrows schemes from models, us-
self-oriented schemes, begins to apply these to ing imitation, and applies them to others. Such
other objects or people. Not only is object play schemes do not form part of the child's own
increasing in importance, but the symbol has activity in that he or she is drawing from al-
now become completely disassociated from ready learned self-oriented symbolic schemes.
sensorimotor practice and is used as an inde-
pendent representation.

Level 2

A. Simple identification of one object with anoth- B. Games of imitation:


er: T h e child pretends to be another and carries
The child, seeing the mother's hair, may say out the other's activity, e.g., pretending to be
fur (i.e., cat) or, seeing a shell, may say cup. mother, the doctor, the cleaning person, etc.

Level 3

A. Symbolic combinations: B. Compensatory combinations:


Now children become better able to construct The child is still using make-believe to
whole scenes and not just isolated fragments assimilate reality, but the focus is more on
and this enables them to begin to extend their correcting reality, doing something through
ability to reproduce reality. make-believe that one cannot do in reality
or neutralizing something fearful that
exists in reality (play as catharsis).
C. Liquidating combinations:
In 3B, a child w ho is faced with a difficult sit-
uation can either compensate for it or accept
it. In 3C, the child tries to relive and change
the outcome symbolically to disassociate it
from its unpleasant context

Note: The letters A, B, and C represent different aspects of symbolic play behavior at each of the three levels of play.

and more from mere practice as they acquired member, too, that thought is becoming more
additional functions (e.g. compensation, ful- logical and orderly.) In Stage II, symbolic
fillment of wishes, and liquidation of con- games are marked by (1) the increasing order-
flicts). liness of ludic constructions; (2) the increasing
During Stage I, covering ages 2 to 3 or 4, desire for accuracy and exactness in the child's
symbolic games may be divided into several imitation of reality; and (3) the appearance of
levels, which are briefly outlined in Table 1. collective symbolism, which involves the dif-
During Stage II, covering ages 3 or 4 to ferentiation and adjustment of roles. Hence
about 7, symbolic games begin to lose some of social interactions become more important, a
their importance. They do not necessarily finding borne out by Parten's (1932) descrip-
become less numerous or less intense; rather, tion of social play among preschool children.
the symbol more closely approximates reality As Piaget put it, "the ludic symbol is evolving
and thus loses some of its ludic aspects. (Re- towards a straightforward copy of reality, only

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 13, 2016


PIAGET AND PLAY

the general themes remaining symbolic, which


the detail of the scenes and of the constructions ln play, especially dramatic play,
tends toward exact accommodation and fre- children can act out conflictual
quently towards properly intelligent adapta- situations so that they come out the
tion" (1962, p. 137). winners and so that sense of mastery or
competence is not totally diminished or
Smilansky's categories altered by conflictual experiences.
Smilansky (1968) elaborated on Piaget's
classification scheme, suggesting four levels of
play that are developmentally related to
increases in prerequisite cognitive skills: ished or altered by conflictual experiences.
• functional play, characterized by simple, Viewing play as a way of working out emo-
repetitive muscle movements, with or tional problems has generated a popular form
without objects; of child psychotherapy called play therapy
• constructive play, characterized by ma- (Axline, 1947).
nipulation of objects in making play
things, for example, using blocks to build a ASSESSMENT APPROACHES
tower;
• dramatic play, which requires the devel- Several researchers have developed scales to
opment of imaginary situations to satisfy assess the developmental progression of play.
the child's wishes or needs; and Nicolich (1977), for example, devised a five-
• games with rules, characterized by the level ordinal scale covering Piaget's sensorimo-
acceptance of prearranged rules and the tor and symbolic games play stages. These five
adjustment of behavior to these rules. levels describe the transition from the child's
Smilansky's functional play is similar to fusion of meaning and action to the point at
Piaget's sensorimotor stage; constructive and which action is internally directed. The stages
dramatic are typically preoperational play are (1) presymbolic games (meaningful use of
forms, and play categorized as games with actions apart from their habitual objectives),
rules is associated with concrete abilities. (2) autosymbolic games (playful use of famil-
Obviously, more advanced cognitij^ play iar schemes), (3) decentered games (reliance
requires more social interaction. on more abstract schemes), (4) combinatorial
Rubin and Maioni (1975) provide support symbolic games (combination of schemes into
for Smilansky's developmental scheme. They sequences), and (5) internally directed sym-
found that preschoolers engaged in signifi- bolic games (mental generation of games
cantly more functional and constructive than before action occurs).
dramatic or games play. They also reported McCune-Nicolich (1981) provides support
that the use of dramatic play was positively for this Piagetian sequence of play develop-
correlated with spatial and classification abili- ment, drawing from numerous studies with
ties but that functional play and these same over 500 subjects. Hill and McCune-Nicolich
abilities were negatively correlated, evidence (1981) demonstrated that this scale also ade-
of the relationship between play and cognitive quately assesses the development of play in
growth. In play, especially dramatic play, chil- Down's syndrome children. Data from this
dren can act out conflictual situations so that study indicated four clearly different play
they come out the winner and so that sense of behavior levels among 30 Down's syndrome
mastery or competence is not totally dimin- infants, aged 20 to 53 months. The second and

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 13, 2016


TOPICS IN EARLY CHILDHOOD SPECIAL EDUCATION / OCTOBER 1982

third levels were collapsed into one, but the and Fein (1981) recently validated the tempo-
remaining levels maintained their distinctive- ral and cross-situational characteristics of this
ness, indicating that this scale can be used with scale and found it a "useful tool for examining
atypically as well as normally developing chil- various aspects of children's spontaneous social
dren. play behavior in specific preschool settings"
Lowe (1975) devised a scale based on (p. 761).
Piaget's views regarding the transition of play The work of Rubin and his colleagues with
from self-oriented action to other-directed the Parten-Smilansky scale also suggests that
action. Whittaker (1980) used this scale with some of the differences in types of play behav-
hospitalized profoundly retarded children, all ior observed among similarly aged children
with a social age below 3*/2 years and with may be traced to social class differences. These
either no speech or the ability to use no more researchers, in attempting to compare Parten's
than 15 single words; no child in this group and Smilansky's play categories in a group of
could produce two-word utterances. Whit- 40 white middle- and lower-class children,
taker found the same developmental pro- with an average age of 3.8 years, found that
gression in both the handicapped children and middle-class children used more constructive
the normal young children, suggesting that (Smilansky) and more associative and coopera-
play behavior follows a developmental tive (Parten) play, and lower-class children
sequence that is relatively independent of used more functional (Smilansky) and more
experience. parallel (Parten) play. The play of the middle-
Belsky and Most (1981) developed a Pia- class children was more cognitively and
getian-based behavioral observation strategy socially advanced (according to these two clas-
that includes a developmental sequence of 12 sification systems) than was the play of the
exploration/play behaviors: mouthing, simple lower-class children. Since play and sociability
manipulation, functional, relational, function- are integrally related and play is centered
al-relational, enactive naming, pretend self, around toys and a variety of meaningful peer
pretend external, substitution, sequence pre- experiences, it is likely that such social class
tend, sequence pretend substitution, and dou- differences can be traced to experiential dif-
ble substitution. Results from two investiga- ferences (Rubin et al., 1976).
tions using this 12-step developmental
sequence suggest that the sequence accurately SOMEftONSIDERATIONS FOR
assesses play development in infants and INTERVENTION
young children. Furthermore, because the Knowledge of the behavioral characteristics
behavior observed is nonverbal and self-gener- of play and of its developmental progression
ated, this strategy may lend itself to the assess- can help us understand and enhance delayed
ment of children who have difficulty learning or deviant behaviors in young handicapped
language (Beizer, Hrncir, Naifeh, Most, & children. Not only would such knowledge pro-
Belsky, 1981). mote a better understanding of behavior in
Rubin created a play scale by combining general, but it would allow more systematic
Parten's social play categories with Smilan- and accurate observation of, and attempts to
sky's cognitive play categories. Each of Part- intervene in, the developmental progress of
en's categories is subdivided into Smilansky's these children.
three sequential categories (Rubin, Maioni, & Knowledge about play behavior can be
Hornung, 1976). This scale appears to be use- incorporated into an intervention program in
ful in assessing play in young children. Enslein order to help the child grow cognitively,

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 13, 2016


PIAGET AND PLAY

Area: Cognition (Play)

Goal: Child will perform two related actions in sequence using objects in play.

Rationale: In play, child now begins to integrate separate acts into simple sequences. Child must
mentally relate two different actions that, in many instances, may reflect a degree of pre-
planning. Imitation of behaviors seen in the environment (such as the mother cooking and
serving food) and memory skills are being strengthened.

Materials: Toy cooking utensils, eating and Cautions:


drinking implements, dolls, doll
furniture, trucks, blocks.

Position: Any position allowing child to play freely.

TEACHING PROCEDURES
Step 1: Observe child in free play. The two actions that will be combined into an ordered sequence must
be present as single acts in child's free-play repertoire.
Step 2: Parallel play (play alongside, not with child) for several minutes performing two familiar related
actions in sequence with two or more groups of objects. Verbalize what you are doing, e.g., "I'm
going to cook some food" (stirring in pot) "and put it on the plate" (spooning pretend food from
pot onto plate). Allow child to imitate if child chooses.
Step 3: Move out of play area and observe child for 3 minutes. Score + if child performs any two related
actions in sequence. Score 0 if child does not perform two related actions in sequence. Stop
scoring at end of 3-minute period or when child performs three sequences of two related
actions.
Step 4: If, after several sessions, child has not performed two related actions in sequence with parallel
play, caregiver may engage child directly in the play sequences, helping child follow both steps.
Score (0) for these sessions. Return to parallel play and observe.

TEACHING NOTES
Other two-action examples: "pouring" from toy pitcher to cup and "drinking" from cup; loading blocks into
dump truck and pushing truck around; placing doll in bed and covering with blanket; feeding doll and wiping
doll's face.

EVALUATION CRITERION
Child will perform three sequences of two related actions within a short play period for 4 consecutive days.

Figure 1. Teaching play. The author thanks Patsy Poche and Marcia Lobman of LSU Medical School, Allied
Health Services, for permission to use this play curriculum item.

socially, and perhaps linguistically. A careful activity in young handicapped children. This
review of the types and sequences of play particular item is derived from Piaget's theory
behavior outlined earlier and in Table 1 should and reflects play behavior likely to be
suggest a variety of approaches. Figure 1 observed either during Stage Six of sensorimo-
contains but one of many possible theoretically tor development or during the first level of
based curriculum items that can augment play preoperational symbolic games. This item also

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 13, 2016


TOPICS IN EARLY CHILDHOOD SPECIAL EDUCATION / OCTOBER 1982

would characterize play behavior typical of Piagetian theory also provides a means of
Nicolich's (1977) fourth level, combinatorial assessing behavioral status. Piaget suggests that
symbolic games. Behavior described in this once young children have extracted the adap-
scale, in the Parten-Smilansky scale, in Belsky tive significance of an object through explora-
and Most's observation strategy and, of course, tion, more and varied motor practice is likely
in Piaget's theory, provide numerous examples to occur. Infants, handicapped or not, who
of play behavior that can be used in interven- have already acquired a particular intellectual
tion curriculum items. behavioral scheme before being assessed on
Knowledge of play characteristics can also some standardized development scale, like the
affect the way behavioral objectives are Bayley Scales of Infant Development (1969),
selected and adjusted for handicapped chil- may not then display the expected normative
dren. For example, intervention programs for behavior; instead, the infants may manifest
young children frequently provide goals for apparently meaningless stereotypy or disinter-
the child. Once he or she has mastered an est. To an infant, however, this behavior could
objective, it is checked off and replaced by a be ritualized play or varied motor practice
new objective. Behavioral gains would be rather than adaptive responding. Alterna-
enhanced if, instead of ceasing to work on the tively, if play enables infants and toddlers to
now mastered behavior, the child was allowed consolidate more complex behaviors, then an
time and space to practice the newly mastered infant who has not mastered a behavior during
motor or symbolic schemes in different con- an intervention program might benefit from
texts. This would facilitate generalization and directed experiences and opportunities for
consolidation. Both Dunst (1981) and Case varied play, focusing on the particular aspect
(1978) have suggested strategies for interve- of the behavior that is the obstacle to progress.
nors to use to vary environmental contexts and These and many other aspects of Piaget's
intervention strategies and to allow such gen- theory can suggest still other approaches for
eralization and consolidation to occur. enhancing child development.

REFERENCES

Axline, J. Play therapy. Boston: Houghton-Mifflin, 1947. Elder, J.L., & Pederson, DR. Preschool children's use of
Bayley, N. Manual for the Bayley Scales of Infant objects in symbolic play. Child Development, 1978, 49,
Development. New York: Psychological Corporation, 500-504.
1969. Enslein, J.P., & Fein, G.G. Temporal and cross-situational
Beizer, L., Hrncir, E., Naifeh, M, Most, R., & Belsky, J. stability of children's social and play behavior. Develop-
From exploration to play in infancy. Paper presented mental Psychology, 1981,17, 760-761.
at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Fein, G.G. Pretend play in childhood: An integrative
Development, Boston, April 1981. review. Child Development, 1981, 52, 1095-1118.
Belsky, J., & Most, R.K. From exploration to play: A Garvey, C. Play. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University
cross-sectional study of infant free play behavior. Press, 1977.
Developmental Psychology, 1981, 17, 630-639. Ghiaci, G., & Richardson, J.T. The effects of dramatic
Bruner, J.S., Jolly, A., & Sylva, K. (Eds.). Play: Its role in play upon cognitive development and structure. Jour-
development and evolution. New York. Basic Books, nal of Genetic Psychology, 1980, 136, 77-83.
1976. Guralnick, M.J. The social behavior of preschool children
Case, R. A developmentally based theory and technology at different developmental levels: Effects of group
of instruction. Review of Educational Research, 1978, composition. Journal of Experimental Child Psycholo-
48, 439-463. gy, 1981, 31, 115-130.
Dunst, C. Infant learning: A cognitive-linguistic inter- Hill, P.M., & McCune-Nicolich, L.M. Pretend play and
vention strategy. Hingham, Mass.: Teaching Resources, patterns of cognition in Down's syndrome children.
1981. Child Development, 1981, 52, 611-617.

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 13, 2016


PIAGET AND PLAY

Hughes, M Sequential analysis of exploration and play. 1975,22,171-179.


International Journal of Behavioral Development, Rubin, K.H., Maioni, T.L., & Hornung, M. Free play
1978, i, 83-97. behaviors in middle- and lower-class preschoolers:
Hughes, M. Exploration and play revisited: A hierarchical Parten and Piaget revisited. Child Development, 1976,
analysis. International Journal of Behavioral Develop- 47, 414-419.
ment, 1979, 2, 215-224. Sigel, I.E., & Cooking, R.R. Cognitive development from
Lowe, M. Trends in the development of representational childhood to adolescence. New York: Holt, Rinehart &
play in infants from one to three years: An observational Winston, 1977.
study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Smilansky, S. The effects of sociodramatic play on disad-
1975,16, 33-47. vantaged preschool children. New York: Wiley, 1968.
McCune-Nicolich, L.M. Toward symbolic functioning: Smith, P.K. A longitudinal study of social participation in
Structure of early pretend games and potential parallels preschool children: Solitary and parallel play reexam-
with language. Child Development, 1981,52, 785-797. ined. Developmental Psychology, 1978,14, 517-523.
Moore, W.V., Evertson, CM., & Brophy, J.E. Solitary Ungerer, J.A., & Sigman, M. Symbolic play and language
play: Some functional considerations. Developmental comprehension in autistic children. Journal of Child
Psychiatry, 1981, 20, 318-337.
Psychology, 1974, 10, 830-834.
Ungerer, J.A., Zelazo, P.R., Kearsley, R.B., & O'Leary, K.
Nicolich, L.M. Beyond sensorimotor intelligence: Assess-
Developmental changes in the representation of objects
ment of symbolic maturity through analysis of pretend
in symbolic play from 18 to 34 months of age. Child
play. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1977, 23(2), 89-99.
Development, 1981, 52,186-195.
Parten, M.B. Social participation among preschool chil-
Vandenberg, B. Play and development from an ethologi-
dren. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, cal perspective. American Psychologist, 1978, 33, 724-
1932,27, 243-269. 738.
Pelligrini, A.D. The relationship between kindergartners' Vandenberg, B. Environmental and cognitive factors in
play and achievement in prereading, language, and social play. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
writing. Psychology in the Schools, 1980,17,530-535. 1981,31,169-175.
Piaget, J. Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New Whittaker, C.A. A note on developmental trends in the
York: Norton, 1962. symbolic play of hospitalized profoundly retarded chil-
Piaget, J. Piaget's theory. In P. Neubauer (Ed.), The dren. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
process of child development. New York: New Ameri- 1980,21, 253-261.
can Library, 1976. Wing, L., Gould, J., Yeates, S.R., & Brierly, L.M. Symbolic
Rubin, K.H., & Maioni, T.L. Play preference and its play in severely mentally retarded and autistic children.
relationships to egocentrism, popularity and classifica- Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1977, 18,
tion skills in preschoolers. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 167-178.

Downloaded from tec.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on September 13, 2016

You might also like