You are on page 1of 4

THE BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1984) 7, 473-475

Printed in the United States of America

The operant behaviorism


of B. F. Skinner
A. Charles Catania
Department of Psychology, University of Maryland Baltimore County,
Catonsville, Md. 21228

Of all contemporary psychologists, B. F. Skinner is per- Biography


haps the most honored and the most maligned, the most
widely recognized and the most misrepresented, the Burrhus Frederic Skinner was born on March 20,1904, in
most cited and the most misunderstood. Some still say Susquehanna, Pennsylvania. After majoring in English at
that he is a stimulus-response psychologist (he is not); Hamilton College, he tried a career at writing but gave it
some still say that stimulus-response chains play a central up after finding he had nothing to say. Having a long-
role in his treatment of verbal behavior (they do not); standing interest in human and animal behavior and some
some still say that he disavows evolutionary determinants familiarity with the writings of Watson, Pavlov, and
of behavior (he does not). These and other misconcep- Bertrand Russell, he then entered the graduate program
tions are common and sometimes even appear in psychol- in psychology at Harvard University (Skinner 1976).
ogy texts (e.g. Todd & Morris 1983). How did they come There he began a series of experiments that led to more
about, and why do they continue? Although the present than two dozen journal articles and culminated in The
BBS treatments will probably not provide an answer, Behavior of Organisms (1938). In the manner of The
they may help to clarify some of the misunderstandings. Integrative Action of the Nervous System (Sherrington
The articles sampled here represent a range of Skin- 1906) and Behavior of the Lower Organisms (Jennings
ner's work (in the treatments, each article is referred to by 1906), the work presented a variety of novel research
its abbreviated title). The first but most recent, "Selec- findings and provided a context for them. The extensive
tion by Consequences " ("Consequences," Skinner 1981), data illustrated many properties of reinforcement and
relates operant theory to other disciplines, and in particu- extinction, discrimination and differentiation; the con-
lar to biology and anthropology. The second, "Methods cept of the three-term contingency was to become the
and Theories in the Experimental Analysis of Behavior" cornerstone for much else that would follow.
("Methods"), outlines some of the basic concepts of oper- In 1936, after three years as a Junior Fellow in the
ant theory in the context of a discussion of methodological Harvard Society of Fellows, Skinner moved to the Uni-
and theoretical issues; it is an amalgamation of revised versity of Minnesota. His basic research continued, but
versions of "The Flight from the Laboratory" (Skinner during World War II he also worked on animal applica-
1961) and "Are Theories of Learning Necessary?" (Skin- tions of behavior principles, including the training of
ner 1950) and a portion of the preface to Contingencies of pigeons to guide missiles (Skinner 1960; 1979). Although
Reinforcement (Skinner 1969). "The Operational Analy- the project never got beyond demonstrations, a major
sis of Psychological Terms" ("Terms," Skinner 1945) is fringe benefit was the discovery of shaping, the technique
the earliest work treated; its special concern is with the for creating novel forms of behavior through the differen-
language of private events, and many features of Skin- tial reinforcement of successive approximations to a
ner's analysis of verbal behavior are implicit in it. "An response.
Operant Analysis of Problem Solving" ("Problem Solv- Another product of those days was the Aircrib, which
ing," Skinner 1966a), continues the interpretation of Skinner built for his wife and his second daughter (Skin-
verbal behavior in distinguishing between rule-governed ner 1945). It was a windowed space with temperature and
and contingency-shaped behavior. "Behaviorism at humidity control that improved on the safety and comfort
Fifty" ("Behaviorism-50," Skinner 1963) addresses the of the ordinary crib while making the care of the child less
status of behaviorism as a philosophy of science, and burdensome. It was not used for conditioning the infant
points out some of the difficulties that must be overcome (contrary to rumor, neither of Skinner's daughters devel-
by any science of behavior. "The Phylogeny and On- oped emotional instability, psychiatric problems or sui-
togeny of Behavior" ("Phylogeny," Skinner 1966b), the cidal tendencies). Soon after came the Utopian novel,
last of the works sampled, considers how evolutionary Walden Two (1948). Some who later criticized the specif-
variables combine with those operating within an organ- ics of that planned society failed to observe that its
ism's lifetime to determine its behavior. experimental character was its most important feature:

© 1984 Cambridge University Press 0140-525XI84/040473-03I$06.00 473


Catania: Skinner's behaviorism
Any practice that did not work was to be modified until a consequences of stepping on the brake pedal or the gas
more effective version was found. pedal, for example, depend on whether the traffic light is
In 1945, Skinner assumed the chairmanship of the red or green). When a stimulus sets the occasion on which
Department of Psychology at Indiana University. Then, responding will have a particular consequence, the stim-
after delivering the 1947 William James Lectures at ulus is said to be discriminative. If responses then come to
Harvard University on the topic of verbal behavior, he depend on, or come under the control of, this stimulus,
returned permanently to the Harvard Department of the response class is called a discriminated operant. Both
Psychology (Skinner 1983). There he completed his book respondents and discriminated operants involve an ante-
Verbal Behavior (1957) and, in collaboration with Charles cedent stimulus, but the distinction between them is
B. Ferster, developed the subject matter of schedules of crucial and depends on whether consequences of re-
reinforcement (Ferster & Skinner 1957). Much else has sponding play a role. A response that depends only on the
been omitted here (e.g. Science and Human Behavior presentation of a stimulus, as in a reflex relation, is a
[1953] and teaching machines); the articles and books member of a respondent class. One that depends on the
Skinner has since written are too numerous to list. All but relations among the three terms - stimulus, response,
one of the articles treated ("Terms") are drawn from those consequence — is a member of a discriminated operant
later pieces; they constitute a sample of his most seminal class. Thus, discriminated operants are said to be defined
works. Many others are cited in the course of the treat- by a three-term contingency. The three-term contingen-
ments. cy is often neglected by those who think of behavior
change only in terms of the instrumental and classical
procedures of earlier conditioning theories.
Operant behaviorism Much of the research that helped to establish this
vocabulary was conducted in the experimental chamber
Operant behaviorism (or radical behaviorism) is the vari- that for a while was known as the Skinner box (that term
ety of behaviorism particularly identified with Skinner's was more often used by those outside than by those
work. It provides the systematic context for the research within the experimental analysis of behavior). Simple
in psychology sometimes referred to as the experimental stimuli (lights, sounds), simple responses (lever presses,
analysis of behavior. Behavior itself is its fundamental key pecks), and simple reinforcers (food, water) were
subject matter; behavior is not an indirect means of arranged for studying the behavior of rats or pigeons.
studying something else, such as cognition or mind or Many responses automatically have particular conse-
brain. quences (to see something below eye level, for example,
A primary task of an experimental analysis is to identify we look down rather than up). But natural environments
classes of behavior on the basis of their origins. Some do not ordinarily include levers on which presses produce
classes of responses, respondents, originate with the food pellets only when lights are on. Operant chambers
stimuli that elicit them (as illustrated by the stimulus- were designed to create arbitrary contingencies; they
response relations called reflexes). Others, called oper- were arbitrary, but only in this sense. As for responses
ants, are engendered by their effects on the environment; such as the pigeon's key peck:
because they do not require eliciting stimuli, they are Such responses are not wholly arbitrary. They are
said to be emitted rather than elicited. Admitting the chosen because they can be easily executed, and be-
possibility that behavior could occur without eliciting cause they can be repeated quickly and over long
stimuli was a critical first step in operant theory. Earlier periods of time without fatigue. In such a bird as the
treatments had assumed that for every response there pigeon, pecking has a certain genetic unity; it is a
must be a corresponding eliciting stimulus. The rejection characteristic bit of behavior which appears with a well-
of this assumption did not imply that emitted responses defined topography. (Ferster & Skinner 1957, p. 7)
were uncaused; rather, the point was that there are other Given this recognition of genetic determinants in the
causes of behavior besides eliciting stimuli. Adding oper- specification of operant classes, it is ironic that some
ants to respondents as behavior classes did not exhaust species-specific characteristics of lever presses and key
the possibilities, but it was critical to recognize that the pecks later became the basis for criticisms of operant
past consequences of responding are significant determi- theory. Perhaps these responses were not arbitrary
nants of behavior. enough. But given that the concern was to study the
The consequences of a response may either raise or effects of the consequences of responding, it would hardly
lower subsequent responding. Consequences that do so have been appropriate to have sought out response class-
are respectively called reinforcers and punishers (punish- es so highly determined in other species-specific ways
that they would have been insensitive to their conse-
ment has sometimes been confused with negative rein-
quences.
forcement, but positive and negative reinforcement both
involve increases in responding; they differ in whether There are "natural lines of fracture along which behav-
the consequence of responding is the addition to or ior and environment actually break" (Skinner 1935, p.
removal of something from the environment, as in the 40). "We divide behavior into hard and fast classes and
difference between appetitive procedures and those in- are then surprised to find that the organism disregards
volving escape or avoidance). The particular reh; tions that the boundaries we have set" (Skinner 1953, p. 94). Oper-
can be established between responses and their conse- ant theory is not compromised by demonstrations that
quences are called contingencies of reinforcement or some response classes are more easily established than
punishment. others, or that some discriminations can be more easily
But the consequences of responding are also typically established with some reinforcers than with others. Con-
correlated with other features of the environment (some sequences are important, but they do not operate to the

474 THE BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1984) 7:4


References /Catania: Skinner's behaviorism
exclusion of other sources of behavior, including phy- editing, logical verbal behavior, and so on. The nestings
logenic ones. Phenomena such as autoshaping (producing and orderings and coordinations of these processes are
a pigeon's pecks on a key by repeatedly lighting the key intricate, but they can nevertheless be accommodated by
and then operating the feeder) were discovered in the the discriminative stimuli and the responses and the
course of operant research, and present no more of a consequences of the three-term contingency. This sort of
problem to operant accounts than do the respondent analysis is illustrated in "Terms"; although that article
conditioning phenomena studied by Pavlov. predated Skinner's development of the vocabulary of
The discovery that behavior could be maintained easily Verbal Behavior, these relations are implicit in it, and
even when only an occasional response was reinforced led more is involved in it than simply the tacting of private
to the investigation of schedules of reinforcement. Sched- events.
ules arrange reinforcers on the basis of the number of These and other aspects of operant behaviorism are
responses, the time at which responses occur, the rate of discussed in the treatments that follow. For the commen-
responding, or various combinations of these and other tators, the articles are the stimuli, their commentaries are
variables. In more complicated cases, different schedules the responses, and Skinner's replies are the conse-
operate either successively or simultaneously in the pres- quences. For Skinner, the commentaries are the stimuli
ence of different stimuli or for different responses. Rein- and his replies are the responses; some of the conse-
forcement schedules have proven useful in such areas as quences will be evident only in the effects of the treat-
psychopharmacology and behavioral toxicology. The per- ments on their readers. Other potential responses and
formances generated by complex schedules are also consequences produced by these treatments are even
sometimes analogous to performances that in humans are more remote and also remain to be seen. To the extent
discussed in terms of preference, self-control, and so on that they may correct some misreadings of operant theo-
(e.g. see "Methods"). ry, they are steps in the right direction. Given that we
In its extension to verbal behavior, a primary task of an have already taken more than a single step, our journey
operant analysis is again that of identifying the various has already begun. This is as it should be, because there is
sources of behavior. Its concern is with the functions of much to explore and the journey will not be short.
language rather than with its structure. In the tact rela-
tion, for example, an object or event is a discriminative ACKNOWLEDGMENT
stimulus that sets the occasion for a particular utterance, Preparation of the introductory and concluding remarks was
supported in part by NSF grant BNS82-03385 to the University
as when one says "apple" upon seeing an apple (tacting is of Maryland Baltimore County. Some passages from the intro-
not equivalent to naming or referring to; the relation duction were excerpted from Catania (1980), with permission of
called reference involves another class of behavior, called the publisher.
autoclitic). Through the tact relation, verbal behavior
makes contact with events in the world. Other relations
include (but are not limited to) the intraverbal, in which
verbal behavior serves as a discriminative stimulus for References
ather verbal behavior (as in learning addition or multi-
plication tables), the textual, in which written text pro- Catania, A. C. (1980) Operant theory: Skinner. In: Theories of learning, ed.
vides the discriminative stimuli (as in reading aloud), and G. M. Gazda & R. Corsini. F. E. Peacock.
Ferster, C. B. & Skinner, B. F. (1957) Schedules of reinforcement. Appleton-
:he mand, in which the verbal response specifies a conse- Gentury-Crofts.
quence (as in making a request or asking a question). Any Jennings, H. S. (1906) Behavior of the lower organisms. Macmillan.
utterance, however, is likely to involve these and other Sherrington, C. (1906) The integrative action of the nervous system.
relations in combination; verbal behavior is a product of Scribner's.
Skinner, B. F. (1935) The generic nature of the concepts of stimulus and
multiple causation. Novel utterances may be dealt with
response. Journal ofCeneral Psychology 12:40-65.
by showing how their various components (words, (1938) The behavior of organisms. Appleton-Ceiitury-Crofts.
phrases, grammatical forms) have each been occasioned (1945) The operational analysis of psychological terms. Psychological Review
by particular aspects of a current situation; novelty, in 42:270-77; 291-94.
other words, comes about through novel combinations of (1948) Walden two. Macmillan.
existing verbal classes. (1950) Are theories of learning necessary? Psychological Review 57:193-216.
(1953) Science and human behavior. Macmillan.
More important, these elementary relations are only (1957) Verbal behavior. Appleton-Century-Crofts.
the raw materials from which verbal behavior is con- (1960) Pigeons in a pelican. American Psychologist 15:28-37.
structed. A sentence cannot exist solely as a combination (1961) The flight from the laboratory. In: Current trends in psychological
of these elementary units. Speakers report on the condi- theory, ed. Wayne Dennis et al. University of Pittsburgh Press.
tions under which they are behaving verbally (as when (1963) Behaviorism at fifty. Science 140:951-58.
(1966a) An operant analysis of problem solving. In: Problem solving:
someone says, "I am happy to report that . . ."), they Research, methods, and theory, ed. B. Kteinmuntz. John Wiley & Sons.
cancel the effects of their own verbal behavior (as when (1966b) Phylogeny and ontogeny of behavior. Science 153:1205-13.
they include "not" in a sentence), they indicate its (1969) Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. Prentice-Hall.
strength (as when they speak of being sure or uncertain), (1976) Particulars of my life. Knopf.
and so on. In each of these cases, some parts of the (1979) The shaping of a behaciorist. Knopf.
(1981) Selection by consequences. Science 213:501-4.
speaker's verbal behavior are under the discriminative (1983) A matter of consequences. Knopf.
control of the various other verbal relations. These pro- Todd, J. T. & Morris, E. K. (1983) Misconception and miseducation:
cesses, called autoclitic, are the basis for larger verbal Presentations of radical behaviorism in psychology textbooks. Behavior
units (e.g. sentences) and for the complexities of self- Analyst 6:153-60.

THE BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1984) 7:4 475


Call for Papers

Investigators in " . . . superbly presented . . . the result is


practically a vade mecum or Who's Who in

Psychology, each subject. [Articles are] followed by pithy


and often (believe it or not) witty comments
questioning, illuminating, endorsing or just
plain arguing . . . I urge anyone with an inter-

Neuroscience, est in psychology, neuroscience, and behav-


ioural biology to get access to this jour-
nal."—New Scientist

Behavioral Biology, and "Care is taken to ensure that the commentaries


represent a sampling of opinion from scientists
throughout the world. Through open peer com-
mentary, the knowledge imparted by the target

Cognitive Science article becomes more fully integrated into the


entire field of the behavioral and brain sciences.
This contrasts with the provincialism of special-
ized journals . . ."—Eugene Garfield Current
Contents
Do you want to:
"The field covered by BBS has often suf-
• draw wide attention to a particularly fered in the past from the drawing of battle
lines between prematurely hardened posi-
important or controversial piece of work? tions: nature v. nurture, cognitive v. behav-
iourist, biological v. cultural causation. . . .
• solicit reactions, criticism, and feedback [BBS] has often produced important articles
from a large sample of your peers? and, of course, fascinating interchanges....
the points of dispute are highlighted if not
• place your ideas in an interdisciplinary, always resolved, the styles and positions of
the participants are exposed, hobbyhorses
international context? are sometimes ridden with great vigour, and
mutual incomprehension is occasionally
made very conspicuous . . . . commentaries
are often incisive, integrative or bring highly
relevant new information to bear on the sub-

The Behavioral ject."— Nature


" . . . a high standard of contributions and dis-
cussion. It should serve as one of the major

and Brain Sciences B stimulants of growth in the cognitive sciences


over the next decade." — Howard Gardner
(Education) Harvard
an extraordinary journal now in its seventh year, provides a
" . . . keep on like this and you will be not
special service called Open Peer Commentary to re- merely good, but essential..."—D.O. Hebb
searchers in any area of psychology, neuroscience, (Psychology) Dalhousie
behavioral biology or cognitive science. " . . . a unique format from which to gain some
Papers judged appropriate for Commentary are circulated appreciation for current topics in the brain sci-
ences . . . [and] by which original hypotheses
to a large number of specialists who provide substantive may be argued openly and constructively."—
criticism, interpretation, elaboration, and pertinent com- Allen R. Wyler (Neurological Surgery)
Washington
plementary and supplementary material from a full cross-
disciplinary perspective. " . . . one of the most distinguished and use-
ful of scientific journals. It is, indeed, that
Article and commentaries then appear simultaneously with rarity among scientific periodicals: a crea-
tive forum . . ."—Ashley Montagu (Anthro-
the authors formal response. This BBS "treatment" pology) Princeton
provides in print the exciting give and take of an interna-
"I think the idea is excellent."—Noam Chomsky
tional seminar. (Linguistics) M.I.T.
The editor of BBS is calling for papers that offer a clear " . . . open peer commentary . . . allows the
reader to assess the 'state of the art' quickly
rationale for Commentary, and also meet high standards of in a particular field. The commentaries pro-
conceptual rigor, empirical grounding, and clarity of style. vide a 'who's who' as well as the content of
Contributions may be (1) reports and discussions of empiri- recent research."—Journal of Social and Bi-
ological Structures
cal research of broader scope and implications than might
" . . . presents an imaginative approach to learn-
be reported in a specialty journal; (2) unusually significant ing which might be adopted by other jour-
theoretical articles that formally model or systematize a nals."—Library Journal
body of research; and (3) novel interpretations, syntheses or "Neurobiologists are acutely aware that
critiques of existing theoretical work. their subject is In an explosive phase of de-
velopment . . . we frequently wish for a fo-
Although the BBS Commentary service is primarily devoted rum for the exchange of ideas and interpre-
to original unpublished manuscripts, at times it will be ex- tations . . . plenty of journals gladly carry the
facts, very few are willing to even consider
tended to precis of recent books or previously published promoting ideas. Perhaps even more impor-
articles. tant is the need for opportunities publicly to
criticize traditional and developing concepts
Published quarterly by Cambridge University Press. Edi- and interpretations. [BBS] is helping to fill
torial correspondence to: Stevan Harnad, Editor, BBS, these needs."—Graham Hoyle (Biology)
Oregon
Suite 240, 20 Nassau Street, Princeton, NJ 08542.

You might also like