You are on page 1of 6

Finestra I

Jan-Arjen Mondria
Groningen Myths about vocabulary acquisition

Vrai ou faux? Voilà la question. In the past twenty-five years, there However, there is a problem… That
«Les listes de vocabulaire sont has been an increased interest in is, the ideas mentioned above are not
inutiles, parce qu’il faut toujours vocabulary acquisition and studies entirely correct, and some of them are
apprendre des mots en contexte, abound, exploring different aspects. even totally incorrect. In this article,
de préférence en déduisant le sens Thus, language teachers who keep I will try to make clear what is incor-
des mots à partir du contexte. abreast of the professional literature rect: the myths (or half-truths) about
De cette façon-là, les chances de will have come across the majority of vocabulary acquisition. Furthermore,
retenir les mots sont optimales. the following ideas. I will explain how it really works:
Heureusement, il n’est pas 1. Some words occur far more often the facts.
nécessaire d’apprendre beaucoup than other ones. Consequently,
de mots: quand on connaît les knowing a relatively small number
deux mille mots les plus fréquents of words takes you far.
d’une langue, on comprend déjà 2. Word lists are actually only of Myth 1: “Knowing a relatively small
80% des mots d’un texte moyen. limited value, as many words are number of words takes you far.”
Finalement, il va de soi qu’il ne forgotten in the course of time. Research has shown that languages are
faut pas tester séparément le 3. Words that belong to the same in certain respects quite economically
vocabulaire des apprenants, parce lexical set, for example colours, ani- structured. That is, in every language
que l’évaluation doit s’effectuer mals and clothes, are best learned there is a limited number of words
au niveau des compétences together, because this corresponds that occur very frequently, and a great
linguistiques (compréhension de to the way the words are stored in number of words that occur relatively
l’écrit et de l’oral, production our mental lexicon. infrequently. As a result, knowing the
écrite et orale), et non pas au 4. Context helps in retaining words. two thousand most frequent words of
niveau des mots.» Therefore, words should always be a language – the exact number may
Dans cet article j’essaie de revoir learned in context. vary slightly per language – enables
quelques mythes concernant 5. Words whose meanings have been you to understand already about 80%
l’acquisition du vocabulaire. inferred with the aid of the context of the words of an average text (Nation
are retained better. That is, during & Waring, 1997).
the inferencing process the learner The question is how well you un-
is actively processing the word and derstand a text of which you know
its meaning, more than when the 80% of the words. In order to get an
meaning is given to him. impression of this, Figure 1 shows
6. There is a clear distinction between an English text from which the 20%
understanding a word (receptive least frequent words have been omit-
knowledge) and being able to use ted, leading to a text coverage level of
a word (productive knowledge). 80%. (see Figure 1)
Productive learning is more dif-
ficult, but has the advantage of the At this level of text coverage, there is no
words being better retained. adequate text comprehension yet. Only
7. The aim of foreign-language teach- at a text coverage level of 95%, the
ing is not vocabulary knowledge, majority of the readers will understand
but language proficiency (listening, a text reasonably well (Laufer, 1989).
speaking, reading, and writing). For such a text coverage, you need a
Therefore, vocabulary knowledge knowledge of 3,000 to 5,000 word
should not be tested separately, families, a word family being defined
as the learner’s vocabulary is as a base word with its inflected forms
automatically assessed when his and transparent derivations (e.g. think,
language skills are tested. thinking, thinker) (Nation & Waring,
1997). For university studies, however,

63 Babylonia 2/07 www.babylonia.ch


Figure 1: Text comprehension at a text coverage level of 80% (Nation, 1990: of a word to be learned in a series does
242-243) no longer wrongly offer any help.
To those who would like to go a step
In 1978/1979 New Zealand produced 9.15 million _____ meters of _____ logs ( further, I suggest the so-called ‘hand
_____ _____ _____ ) of which 59 percent was _____ (as newsprint, _____, sawn computer’ (Leitner, 1972; Mondria &
_____, logs, and so on). Productive _____ is expected to remain at about this level Mondria-de Vries, 1994): a deck of
throughout most of this _____. But based on the _____ of wood which will become cards with a sophisticated repetition
_____ from existing forests and planned new plantings, production will progres- system on the basis of ever bigger inter-
sively increase to 20 million _____ meters a year by the turn of the century. vals, guaranteeing optimal long-term
If _____ planting rates are _____ with planting _____ satisfied in each
retention (see Figure 2). And those who
_____ and the forests milled at the earliest opportunity, the _____ wood supplies
could further increase to about 36 million ______ meters _____ in the period 2001-
prefer a computer program based on
2015. The additional _____ supply should greatly _____ _____ _____, even if the same principles, can find all kinds
much is used for _____ production. of information at: http://de.wikipedia.
Even if used in an _____ form, the increasing wood supplies will _____ org/wiki/Lernkartei-Software. (see
a larger _____ force, an improved roading network, and _____ _____ and _____ Figure 2)
_____. If the trees are to be _____ then certain _____ must be made. They will
include _____ in:
• logging machinery and _____;
• logging trucks, and other _____ _____ for the _____ of _____ products; Myth 3: “Presenting words in
• _____ and _____ roads (or rail or coastal shipping _____ where _____ ); semantic sets facilitates learning.”
• _____ _____.
In several coursebooks and vocabulary
books, new words are presented in
a knowledge of 10,000 word families have been ordered on the basis of their semantic sets: groups of words that
would be necessary (Hazenberg & frequency in actual language use (e.g. are semantically related and share a
Hulstijn, 1996). West, 1953). This basic vocabulary common superordinate, such as col-
Therefore, the conclusion should be should absolutely be known by the ours, animals, and clothes. It is often
that the idea that the knowledge of a learners because any gap in this basic thought that this way of presenting
couple of thousand words takes you vocabulary can cause problems. There- related words together facilitates
far is a myth. The fact is that this fore, word lists and frequency lists are learning. And indeed, when we think
knowledge takes you a long way, but excellent checklists for determining of the mental lexicon, the dictionary
not far enough. which important words learners do in our head, this seems logical. Call-
not know yet. ing into mind one of the members of
Does this imply that learners should a semantic set, for example an article
also learn those words with the aid of of clothing, will call into mind other
Myth 2: “Word lists are of limited such a list? No, that is not sensible. It is members of the set as well. Thus, the
value.” much better to put those words – only members of a semantic set are closely
Word lists (lists of words to be learned) the words that the learners do not know linked in our mind, so it seems logical
have all kinds of disadvantages, as we yet – on cards. The simplest way is to to learn them together.
know from experience. First, they are put the foreign-language word on one Research, however, has shown that
not particularly interesting to learn side (e.g. the French word une canne) learning words in semantic sets is
(although this argument does not hold and the translation (a walking stick) on not the best option. On the contrary,
for certain types of learners). Second, the other. The advantages of learning related words are more easily confused
words that have been learned from a list words with the help of these word cards (interference) and learning them takes
are easily mixed-up, because they do are the following: (1) learners do not considerably more time than learning
not have a context – I will return to that at once see the translation of the word unrelated words (Tinkham, 1993,
later on. Third, words that are ‘known’ to be learned, which stimulates them 1997; Waring, 1997) (see Table 1).
within the list may not be known out- to first think before checking whether
side the list, in actual usage. they know the word (‘learning by test- Does this imply that language teachers
Are these reasons to advise against ing’); (2) the words learners know are should never make use of semantic
using word lists? Certainly not. The put aside, so that they can concentrate sets? Certainly not. Working with sets
fact is that word lists in principle on those words they do not know yet; of semantically related words can be
contain the most frequent words from (3) the order of a series of words to be quite useful, but only at a later stage.
frequency lists, lists in which the words learned is flexible, so that the position It is only when learners already know

64 Babylonia 2/07 www.babylonia.ch


Figure 2: Hand computer (Mondria & Mondria-de Vries, 1994) several words of a set that it makes
sense to put these words together and
The hand computer is a deck of cards with a sophisticated repetition system. This to examine what the learners know
deck of cards – in its simplest form a shoebox – is divided into five compartments of them exactly, and subsequently
of increasing size (with a width of 1, 2, 5, 8 and 14 cm respectively). Another pos- to add new words or new shades of
sibility is taking a series of five audio-cassette boxes, but in that case there is no in- meaning.
creased size of the compartments. The advantage, however, of this shape is that the
material can easily be carried around, and can be practiced at any spare moment.

Myth 4: “Words should always be


learned in context.”
There is little doubt about the helpful-
ness of contexts such as a sentence or
a text in the process of learning words.
A context not only shows the word and
its use, but it can also help in retaining a
word and its meaning (Mondria, 1996).
For example, someone can learn the
Trajectory of a known word French word canne with the help of
Trajectory of a word not known or no longer known the sentence Le vieil homme marche
à l’aide d’une canne. When, later on,
How does one work with the hand computer? Every word to be learned is put on a the learner does not remember what
card: the foreign-language word on one side, the translation on the other (preferably the word canne means, he or she may
in a different colour, so that it is immediately clear which is the front and which is remember that the word occurred in
the back). The cards are placed in compartment 1 (30 40 cards at the most), after a sentence with vieil homme and/or
which they are practiced. The words the learner knows are moved on to compart- marche, which reminds him or her
ment 2; the ones the learner does not know are put in the back of compartment 1 of the meaning of canne. Thus, con-
again. When there are only a few words left in compartment 1, the compartment is
text can help in retaining words, and
replenished with 30 40 new words, and words can be learned and moved on again.
After this has been repeated a few times, compartment 2 becomes filled up, and the
therefore it seems logical to argue
time is ripe to repeat the words in compartment 2: the ones the learner still knows that words should always be learned
go into compartment 3; the ones that are immediately recalled go back into the back in context.
of compartment 1. In this way about the breadth of a finger is cleared out of com- However, there are two caveats to this
partment 2. This is also the principle of the hand computer in a nutshell: whatever ‘rule of thumb’. First, many (concrete)
is ‘known’ goes into the next compartment; anything ‘not known/no longer known’ words can be learned efficiently with-
should go back into compartment 1. out context. Presenting such words
After some time compartments 3 and 4 will become filled up. In that case the same without a context – for example when a
procedure is adopted as in compart¬ment 2. When eventually compartment 5 also learner asks for them – can be a practi-
becomes filled up and room is to be made there too, the learner may in principle cal method that prevents the teacher
throw the cards away if he or she still knows them: for by then the words have been
from having to invent an interesting
seen so often, and so much distributed in time at that, that we may speak of real
knowledge. An alternative possibility is to work with a compartment 5 that is so or useful context, which subsequently
big that it need not be cleared out. In this case the cards may be kept and occasion- has to be written down by the learner
ally the learner can draw a sample of ten cards from it in order to see if the words and so on, each time.
are still known. In this way, compartment 5 contains the learner’s complete word Second, and this is actually the main
knowledge. point, learning a word in a particular
context may result in a learner know-
ing the word only in that context, or
worse: not even recognizing the word
Table 1: Learning load of related and unrelated words (Waring, 1997)
outside that context. Every teacher
Words to be learned # Learning trials required will be familiar with the learner who
Related 9.6 says, “In the original sentence, I
Unrelated 6.5 know what the word means, but now
that you have put the word in another
sentence, I do not know the meaning

65 Babylonia 2/07 www.babylonia.ch


anymore…” In order to prevent this ring is an effective learning strategy, I esting when we take into account the
kind of situation, it should be made carried out a learning experiment with amount of time spent by the pupils on
clear to the learner that in the end the Dutch pupils in secondary education the different learning methods. Then
words have to be known without the (Mondria 1996, 2003). They had to it turns out that the meaning-inferred
original context. Therefore, in the learn French words (French-Dutch) method takes considerably more time
final stage of the learning process, the with the aid of four different learning (in the experiment about 25% more)
context should not be (immediately) methods: (1) inferring the meaning than the meaning-given method. Con-
visible anymore. Thus, the word should of the target word with the help of sequently, the efficiency of the mean-
be ‘decontextualized’ as it were: the a sentence context; (2) inferring fol- ing-inferred method is lower than that
physical (textual) association between lowed by verifying the correctness of of the meaning-given method.
the word and its context should even- the inferred meaning with the help of Does this imply that learners should
tually become a mental association in an alphabetical word list; (3) inferring not infer word meanings from context
the learner’s mind. and verifying followed by memorizing any more? Of course not, as inferring
Incidentally, this process of ‘contex- the inferred and verified meaning of is a useful compensation strategy when
tualizing-decontextualizing’ can be the target word (‘meaning-inferred our vocabulary knowledge is limited.
excellently realized with the help of method’); (4) memorizing the given However, inferring is not the most
word cards, namely by putting on the meaning of the target word, presented efficient learning strategy.
front the word to be learned (e.g. une in a sentence context (‘meaning-
canne), and on the back on top the given method’). After two weeks, the
context sentence (e.g. Le vieil homme learning results were as follows (see
marche à l’aide d’une canne.) and at Table 2). Myth 6: “Words learned produc-
the bottom the translation (a walking tively are retained better.”
stick). In this way, the learner has the The learning effect of inferring per It is often believed that words are
context at hand as a cognitive foothold se is rather limited: after two weeks, better retained when they are learned
while learning, and at the same time, only 6% of the inferred word mean- productively, that is, in order to be
he works up to knowing the meaning ings were remembered. The addition used. For that reason, many teachers
of the word without any context. Thus, of a verifying stage led to an extra assume that it is useful to have learn-
context helps in learning, and at the retention of 9%. However, it is only ers learn words both receptively and
same time, it becomes clear to the when the word meanings are inten- productively, even when learners need
learner that words should not always tionally memorized that the learning to know the words only receptively
be learned in context. effect becomes substantial, as shown (i.e. understand the words). This as-
by the retention figures of the mean- sumption sounds plausible because
ing-inferred method (47%) and the productive learning is more difficult
meaning-given method (50%). What and consequently more intensive, as
Myth 5: “Words whose meanings is most striking is that the meaning- a result of which the words will be
have been inferred from context inferred method (inferring + verifying retained better. Besides, if you know
are retained better.” + memorizing) does not lead to bet- a word productively, you know it re-
When the meaning of a word is inferred ter retention than the meaning-given ceptively as well, don’t you?
with the help of the context, the word method (memorizing the given mean- However, in this case too, the facts are
and its meaning will be retained better ings): the level of retention is similar. slightly different. In an experiment,
than when the meaning is ‘given’, for Thus, in this experiment no evidence I asked pupils in Dutch secondary
example in the form of a translation. can be found for the idea that inferred education to learn French words in
The explanation for the retention effect word meanings are retained better. three different ways: (1) receptively
of inferring is that inferring creates all The results become even more inter- (French-Dutch); (2) productively
kinds of links (elaborations) between
the word, its meaning, the context, and
Table 2: Learning effect of inferring (Mondria, 1996, 2003)
the knowledge already present in the
learner. These links provide additional
Learning method % Receptive retention
retrieval routes, which increase the
chance that the word and its mean- Inferring 6
ing will be remembered (Anderson, Inferring + verifying 15
Inferring + verifying + memorizing (‘Meaning-inferred method’) 47
1990).
Memorizing (’Meaning-given method’) 50
In order to investigate whether infer-

66 Babylonia 2/07 www.babylonia.ch


(Dutch-French); (3) receptively + pro- Table 3: Learning effects of receptive and productive learning (Mondria &
ductively (French-Dutch and Dutch- Wiersma, 2004)
French) (Mondria & Wiersma, 2004).
Two weeks later, their knowledge of Learning method % Receptive retention
the words was tested. The receptive Receptive 48
knowledge of the words turned out to Productive 42
be as follows (see Table 3). Receptive + productive 49

Contrary to expectation, pupils who many of the problems that learners Second, vocabulary tests can show
had learned the words both receptively experience, for example in reading the progress of learners. This can be
and productively did not perform better comprehension. This diagnosis can an important stimulus for them to
(49%) than the pupils who had learned act as a stimulus to invest more time continue learning. Here it should be
the words just receptively (48%): the in vocabulary learning. taken into account that an increase in
retention was similar. This implies that
if the learning aim is receptive retention Table 4: Myths, facts, and didactic suggestions for vocabulary acquisition
– and in many cases this will be suf-
ficient as it is by no means necessary Myth Fact Didactic suggestion
to know all words productively, and
certainly not immediately – it does 1. Number of words
not make sense to have learners learn Knowing a relatively Knowing a relatively small Learn a great number
the words productively as well, as this small number of words number of words takes you a of words.
would take extra time without leading takes you far. long way, but not far enough.
to better retention. 2. Word lists
Word lists are of li- Word lists are essential, but Use words lists as a ba-
mited value. they have to be used in the right sis, but put the words
manner. to be learned on cards.
Myth 7: “Vocabulary knowledge
should not be tested separately.” 3. Semantic sets
Words learned in se- Words learned in semantic Do not learn words in
The main aim of foreign-language
mantic sets are retai- sets are more easily confused. semantic sets.
teaching is of course the acquisition ned better.
of language skills: listening, speak-
ing, reading, and writing. When these 4. Context
skills are tested, for example in order Words should always Words should not always be Learn words in con-
to assess how far someone’s learning be learned in context. learned in context. text, but not in the final
process has progressed, vocabulary stage of the learning
knowledge is automatically taken into process.
account. Therefore, there seems to be 5. Inferring
no reason to test vocabulary knowledge Words whose mea- Words whose meanings have Do not use inferencing
separately, that is, not integrated into nings have been in- been inferred from context as a preferred vocabu-
a skills test. ferred from context are not retained better, and lary learning strategy.
Nevertheless, there is at least one are retained better. the meaning-inferred method
good reason for testing vocabulary takes more time and is there-
knowledge separately, and that is that fore less efficient.
vocabulary tests can stimulate learners 6. Productive learning
to learn, namely in the following ways Words learned pro- Words learned productively Do not learn words
(Mondria, 2004). ductively are retained are not retained better, and productively if it is not
First, vocabulary tests can make better. productive learning takes necessary.
learners aware of the size of their more time.
vocabulary. If their vocabulary turns
7. Testing
out to be small, or if there are serious
Vocabulary know- Testing vocabulary know- Test vocabulary know-
gaps, or if the knowledge of learners ledge should not be ledge separately can stimulate ledge separately on a
lags behind that of their fellow learn- tested separately. vocabulary learning. regular basis.
ers, this may be an explanation for

67 Babylonia 2/07 www.babylonia.ch


retention]. Doctoral dissertation, University
of Groningen, the Netherlands.
Mondria, J.-A. (2003). The effects of inferring,
verifying, and memorizing on the retention of
L2 word meanings: An experimental compari-
son of the ‘meaning-inferred method’ and the
‘meaning-given method’. Studies in Second
Language Acquisition 25, 473-499.
Mondria, J.-A. (2004). Vocabulaireverwerving
stimuleren door toetsen? Tien minder bekende
toetsvormen. [Stimulating vocabulary acqui-
sition by testing? Ten less known test formats].
Levende Talen Tijdschrift 5/4, 3-14.
Mondria, J.-A. / Mondria-De Vries, S. (1994).
Efficiently memorizing words with the help
of word cards and ‘hand computer’: Theory
and applications. System 22, 47-57.
Mondria, J.-A. / Wiersma, B. (2004). Receptive,
productive, and receptive + productive L2
vocabulary learning: What difference does it
make? In Bogaards, P. & Laufer, B. (Eds.),
Vocabulary in a second language: Selec-
tion, acquisition, and testing (pp. 79-100).
UNIVAC III (Otto Press Photo) (1961)
Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins.
Nation, I.S.P. (1990). Teaching and learning
vocabulary. New York: Newbury House.
vocabulary knowledge can already be seven myths and facts, each case sup- Nation, P. & Waring, R. (1997). Vocabulary size,
observed after a relatively short period, plemented with a didactic suggestion text coverage and word lists In Schmitt, N. &
while it takes longer before an increase (see Table 4). Mccarthy, M. (Eds.), Vocabulary: Descrip-
in other skills can be observed. tion, acquisition and pedagogy (pp. 6-19).
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Third, tests can focus learners’ atten- Tinkham, T. (1993). The effect of semantic
tion on vocabulary in an attractive way, clustering on the learning of second language
thus stimulating vocabulary acquisi- Note vocabulary. System 21, 371-380.
This article is the English version of my Dutch Tinkham, T. (1997). The effects of semantic and
tion. I think especially of alternative, thematic clustering on the learning of second
article ‘Mythen over vocabulaireverwerving’,
informal and less well-known test published in 2006 in Levende Talen Tijdschrift language vocabulary. Second Language
formats (suggestions can be found in 7/4, p. 3-11, and in Bossers, B. (Ed.). Vakwerk Research 13, 128-163.
Mondria, 2004, and on Tom Cobb’s 3: Achtergronden van de NT2-lespraktijk. Waring, R. (1997). The negative effects of learn-
Lezingen NT2-conferentie Hoeven-Bovendonk ing words in semantic sets: A replication.
web site: http://www.lextutor.ca). System 25, 261-274.
2006. Amsterdam, BV NT2, p. 27-37.
By the way, the very fact that a teacher West, M. (1953). A general service list of English
(regularly) tests vocabulary knowledge words. London: Longman.
is important, as it is a signal to the
learners that vocabulary acquisition
References
is essential. Conversely, if a teacher Anderson, J.R. (1990). Cognitive psychology
considers sound vocabulary knowl- and its implications (3rd ed.). New York: Jan-Arjen Mondria
Freeman. obtained his doctorate in applied linguistics
edge essential without testing it, the with a dissertation on vocabulary acquisition
Hazenberg, S. & Hulstijn, J.H. (1996). Defining
wrong signal is given. In sum, there a minimal receptive second-language vocabu- in foreign-language teaching. He is currently
is every reason for testing vocabulary lary for non-native university students: An subject librarian for linguistics and communi-
knowledge separately as well. empirical investigation. Applied Linguistics cation studies at the University Library of the
7, 145-163. University of Groningen, the Netherlands.
Laufer, B. (1989). What percentage of text-lexis
is essential for comprehension? In Laurén,
C. & Nordman, M. (Eds.), Special language:
Summary: myths, facts, and di- From humans thinking to thinking machines
(pp. 316-323). Clevedon: Multilingual
dactic suggestions Matters.
In this article, I have tried to show that Leitner, S. (1972). So lernt man lernen. Freiburg:
the facts about vocabulary acquisition Herder.
are sometimes just a bit different or Mondria, J.-A. (1996). Vocabulaireverwerving
in het vreemde-talenonderwijs: De effecten
more complex than some myths would van context en raden op de retentie. [Vocabu-
have us believe. Finally, by way of lary acquisition in foreign-language teach-
summary, I have set side by side the ing: The effects of context and guessing on

68 Babylonia 2/07 www.babylonia.ch

You might also like