You are on page 1of 4

Dispatch

R995

by the Press would make higher would not cheat. Thus the present 3. Milinski, M., Semmann, D., and Krambeck, H.-J.
(2002). Reputation helps solve the ‘‘tragedy of
donations to charity when observed study reinforces the belief that people the commons’’. Nature 415, 424–426.
than when not observed. In contrast, with ASD are transparently trustworthy. 4. Wedekind, C., and Braithwaite, V.A. (2002).
in this experiment, autistic people did This reminds us of the possibility that The Long-term benefits of human generosity
in indirect reciprocity. Curr. Biol. 12,
not show hypocrisy; they did not vary before autism was recognised some 1012–1015.
their donations in relation to whether affected individuals were probably 5. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., and Frith, U. (1986).
Mechanical, behavioural and intentional
an observer was present or not. This venerated as saints and as blessed understanding of picture stories in autistic
is as would be predicted from if they fools [13]. children. Brit. J. Dev. Psychol. 4, 113–125.
lacked mentalizing. What would autistic people do under 6. Tennie, C., Frith, U., and Frith, C. (2009).
Reputation management in the age of the
While mentalising was initially tested more explicit conditions? Might this world-wide web. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14,
solely in terms of explicit tests, such rob them of their sainthood? The 482–488.
7. Chiu, P.J., Kayali, M.A., Kishida, K.T.,
as the ‘Sally-Anne’ task [9], which updated version of the mindblindness Tomlin, D., Klinger, M.R., and Montague, P.R.
children can pass from about age five, account is that there is a deficit only (2008). Self responses along cingulate
it is now known that the spontaneous in spontaneous mentalizing. We would cortex reveal quantitative neural phenotype
for high-functioning autism. Neuron 57,
and automatic ability to mentalize is predict that people who have acquired 463–473.
present even in infants under one year an explicit Theory of Mind and can use 8. Frith, C.D., and Frith, U. (2008). The self and its
reputation in autism. Neuron 57, 331–332.
old [10]. Furthermore, in its automatic mental states to explain and justify 9. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A., and Frith, U. (1985).
form, as assessed by involuntary eye behaviour would be amenable to being Does the autistic child have a ‘‘theory of mind’’?
gaze, it is absent even in able autistic taught about reputation management. Cognition 21, 37–46.
10. Kovács, Á.M., Téglás, E., and Endress, A.D.
adults [11]. Interestingly, this absence They might be induced to donate more (2010). The social sense: Susceptibility to
does not preclude the acquisition of generously in the presence of an others’ beliefs in human infants and adults.
Science 330, 1830–1834.
an explicit ‘Theory of Mind’. Many able observer if told in advance about 11. Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., and Frith, U.
autistic adults can pass explicit tests, possible benefits in terms of applause (2009). Mindblind eyes: an absence of
but their ability to use mentalizing in and attention. This is not a very subtle spontaneous Theory of Mind in Asperger
Syndrome. Science 325, 883–885.
an implicit form has rarely been tested. strategy and the increased donation 12. Zajonc, R.B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science
Given this updated account of would be an instrumental act rather 149, 269–274.
13. Houston, R., and Frith, U. (2000). Autism
mindblindness in autism as a lack of than a clever form of reputation in History: The Case of Hugh Blair of Borgue
implicit mentalizing, we would like to management. However, if this sort of (Oxford: Blackwell).
suggest that the hypocrisy revealed by explicit teaching worked and was
1Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience,
Izuma et al. [1] in ordinary participants widely applied, then perhaps the novel
was implicit. However, it remains to be test provided by Izuma and colleagues University College London, 17 Queen Square,
seen whether this is the case. would no longer be able to differentiate London WC1N 3AR, UK. 2Interacting Minds
Project — MINDLab, Aarhus University,
In a second task, Izuma et al. [1] autistic and neurotypical groups. Nørrebrogade 44, 8000 Aarhus C, Denmark.
investigated whether autistic people 3Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging at

would be subject to the audience effect References University College London, 12 Queen Square,
1. Izuma, K., Matsumoto, K., Camerer, C.F., and London WC1N 3BG, UK. 4All Souls College,
when this did not involve mentalizing. Adolphs, R. (2011). Insensitivity to social
The audience effect is associated with reputation in autism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA High Street, Oxford OX1 4AL, UK.
facilitation of performance on a 108, 17302–17307. E-mail: u.frith@ucl.ac.uk
2. Nowak, M.A., and Sigmund, K. (1998). Evolution
moderately easy task by the mere of indirect reciprocity by image scoring. Nature
presence of others, via an increase 393, 573–577. DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.001
in arousal [12]. The authors chose the
Continuous Performance Task: this
task is attention demanding, but easy
to perform. Here, there was an
audience effect, an increase in
performance when an observer was
present, not only in the control group,
Plant Evolution: Pulses of Extinction
but also in the autistic group. This is
an important finding, as it rules out that
and Speciation in Gymnosperm
the autistic participants were simply
not affected by the presence of others.
Diversity
This is consistent with anecdotal
observations that people with autism Living gymnosperms represent the survivors of ancient seed plant lineages
spectrum disorder (ASD) enjoy the whose fossil record reaches back 270 million years. Two recent studies find
attention they get from others when that recent pulses of extinction and speciation have shaped today’s
displaying their special talents. gymnosperm diversity, contradicting the widespread assumption that
Our desire for a good reputation is gymnosperms have remained largely unchanged for tens of millions of years.
not only seen in admirable and
apparently altruistic behaviour when
observed, but also in selfish behaviour Charles C. Davis* cycads, ginkgos, and the lesser-known
when not observed. It would therefore and Hanno Schaefer gnetophytes (Figure 1). These plants
be predicted that in situations where are of huge economic importance,
a benefit can be obtained by cheating Gymnosperms are a group of woody especially for their timber and
when unobserved, autistic people seed plants that includes conifers, horticultural value. Their name means
Current Biology Vol 21 No 24
R996

A Conifers, ~500 sp. B Cycads, ~145–300 sp. gymnosperm and angiosperm species
diversity can be explained primarily by
extinction in gymnosperms or whether
lower speciation rates are also
implicated in this pattern. Nagalingum
et al. [2], in contrast, avoided
estimating extinction rates, arguing
that the more recent cycad fossil
record is too imperfectly known.
Both studies demonstrate that the
living gymnosperms are not relicts,
so-called ‘living fossils’, but are instead
recent. In their global analysis, Crisp
C Ginkgo, 1 sp. D Gnetophytes, ~120 sp. and Cook [1] found that most Cenozoic
crown group gymnosperms are
significantly younger than their
angiosperm counterparts (median age
w32 Ma (Oligocene) versus w50 Ma
(Eocene)). The more taxonomically
focused study by Nagalingum et al. [2]
corroborates these findings. They
found that crown group diversifications
in all five major cycad clades occurred
between around 5 and 10 Ma (Figure 2).
Their cycad phylogeny is characterized
Current Biology by very long internal branches
juxtaposed against very short tips.
Figure 1. Living representatives of the major gymnosperm lineages with extant species. Thus, the phylogeny resembles
(A) Conifers (bristlecone pine, Pinus longaeva), (B) cycads (Armstrong’s cycad, Cycas a coppiced sycamore — a tree with
armstrongii), (C) ginkgo (Ginkgo biloba), and (D) gnetophytes (Welwitschia mirabilis). Images long stems and shallow, bushy crowns.
copyright J. Gordon, J. Tann, M. LaBar, and P. Kosina, respectively.
These findings, combined with
numerous recent molecular divergence
‘naked seed’, and refers to one of their extinction and surprisingly recent time studies for gymnosperms,
defining features — the seed is not bursts of speciation of today’s living including Agathis [6], Cedrus [7],
borne in a carpel, the protective descendants. Ephedra [8], Gnetum [9], Juniperus [10],
structure that is a hallmark of their The two studies used very different, Phyllocladus [11], Pinus [12],
close relatives, the flowering plants but complementary, approaches to Podocarpaceae [13], and Pseudotsuga
(angiosperms). The evolutionary shed light on the evolutionary history of [14], paint a new picture of living
history of gymnosperms traces back the gymnosperms. Crisp and Cook [1] gymnosperms that is characterized
tens of millions of years, and these sampled species broadly to include by their origin on a more recent
plants reached their dominance representatives of all living landscape. What distinguishes the
when non-avian dinosaurs roamed gymnosperm lineages. In contrast, study by Crisp and Cook [1] is their
the Earth. Today’s gymnosperms, Nagalingum et al. [2] sampled global analysis of extinction and its
however, are but a shadow of their maximally within a single lineage, the influence on gymnosperm diversity.
former glory — only 850 to 1,000 cycads (Figure 1), which include What distinguishes the study by
species presently inhabit our planet. 145–300 species [3,4] distributed in Nagalingum et al. [2] is not that they
Scientists have long assumed that tropical and subtropical regions around found recent origins of crown group
much of the living gymnosperm the world. Both studies assembled cycads — similar divergence time
diversity is relictual, representing the DNA sequence data and estimated estimates for cycads had been
last remnants of their formidable past. divergence times using reported previously [15] — but rather
This assumption is due in part to the fossil-calibrated phylogenies. Crisp that all major cycad subclades are
fact that the overall morphology of and Cook [1] additionally assessed found to exhibit nearly simultaneous
numerous living species, including the speciation and extinction patterns diversification on separate continents.
charismatic ginkgo, dawn redwood, during the Cenozoic (65 million years These studies point toward a pattern
and Wollemia pine, has remained ago (Ma) to the present). Estimating of major extinction and speciation
similar for millions of years. Two recent extinction rates from living diversity pulses in gymnosperms beginning
phylogenetic studies [1,2], published alone is controversial [5]. To alleviate around the end of the Eocene
in New Phytologist and Science, these concerns, Crisp and Cook [1] (w35 Ma). One major factor that may
challenge this view, however, and integrated the fossil record to assess have triggered these phenomena is
instead paint a very different picture the relative roles of extinction and global climate change. Gymnosperms
of the tempo and mode of gymnosperm speciation for gymnosperms versus probably occupied warmer, wetter,
diversification. What emerges is angiosperms. This integration then aseasonal environments for much of
a history of gymnosperms allowed them to determine whether the their early evolution [16]. The end of the
characterized by pulses of recent great disparity between living Eocene, however, marked a major shift
Dispatch
R997

in the terrestrial environment that left 1.4 Cycas


the world much cooler, drier, and more Zamia
seasonal [17]. Crisp and Cook [1] Chigua 0.7
Microcycas
identified that living gymnosperm Ceratozamia

Per myr rates of diversification


diversity can be best explained by the Almargemia 0
exceptionally high rates of extinction, Macrozamia 1.0 Encephalartos
perhaps due to this cooling, and Lepidozamia
Encephalartos 0.5
not by lower speciation rates. Bororoa 0
Angiosperms, it appears, were more Menucoa 1.4
Fascisvarioxlyon Macrozamia
resistant to extinction during this
Charmogia
time [18]. Crisp and Cook [1] did Dioon 0.7
not assess earlier pulses of extinction, Lyssoxylon
but a comprehensive assessment Dioonopsis 0
Kurtziana 1.6 Zamia
of the Southern Hemisphere Ticoa
gymnosperm fossil record points NilAnBe.
toward climate change during Bowenia 0.8
Stangeria
the recent Neogene (23–2.5 Ma), Mesodescolea
especially drying and cooling at mid Sueria 0
latitudes, as the driving factor leading Ctenis 1.0 Ceratozamia
Bjuvia
to the more restricted present-day Cycas 0.5
distribution and diversity of cycads Crossozamia
Antarcticycas 0
[16]. All of these data suggest that 12 6 0
Michelilloa
the declines in cycad diversity may Millions of years
have also been strongest during the
Cenozoic, and not during earlier Figure 2. Phylogeny of cycads.
time periods. Living (black) and extinct (red) lineages are shown. Diversification rates through time plots of
At the same time, these climatic main cycad lineages are redrawn from [2] and color-coded. Pictures representing all major
changes may have triggered pulses genera shown. Images copyright from top to bottom, G. Shepherd, T. Rulkens, D. Fletcher,
of diversification among the lucky M. Lundy and W. Atkinson. Phylogeny adapted from [20].
survivors. The available divergence
time estimates across multiple clades, placement of the fossils used to transitions, and diversification
however, are not as conclusive about constrain their molecular divergence patterns in this charismatic and
the contemporaneous nature of their time estimates. Nagalingum et al. [2] important group of plants.
diversification. Nagalingum et al. [2] placed the oldest known cycad fossil,
suggest that the diversification of major Crossozamia (w270 million years old),
References
crown group cycads occurred between at the stem node of the cycad 1. Crisp, M.D., and Cook, L.G. (2011).
around 5 and 10 Ma. These age lineage, whereas Crisp and Cook [1] Cenozoic extinctions account for the
low diversity of extant gymnosperms
estimates have wide confidence followed earlier recommendations compared with angiosperms. New Phytol. 192,
intervals (around 10 million years), by Hermsen et al. [20] and placed 997–1009.
and furthermore, their diversification it at the crown node. Similarly, 2. Nagalingum, N.S., Marshall, C.R., Quental, T.B.,
Rai, H.S., Little, D.P., and Mathews, S. (2011).
rate plots suggest multiple pulses Nagalingum et al. [2] assigned two Recent synchronous radiation of a living fossil.
of diversification within this window Eocene-aged cycad fossils to the Science 334, 796–799.
3. Pryer, K.M., Schneider, H., Zimmer, E.A., and
(Figure 2). For example, the large genus respective stem group nodes of the Banks, A.J. (2002). Deciding among green
Encephalartos peaks in diversification genera Lepidozamia and Bowenia, plants for whole genome studies. Trends
around 10 Ma whereas Ceratozamia while Crisp and Cook [1] placed Plant Sci. 7, 550–554.
4. Hill, K.D. (2004). World List of Cycads.
peaks at around 5 Ma. Given the nearly these fossils in the crown nodes. http://plantnet.rbgsyd.nsw.gov.au/PlantNet/
300 million year history of cycads, The approach of Nagalingum et al. [2] cycad/wlist.html
5. Rabosky, D.L. (2010). Extinction rates should
these radiations might still be biases the estimated cycad crown not be estimated from molecular phylogenies.
considered contemporaneous, but ages to be younger, while the Evolution 64, 1816–1824.
on closer inspection they may be the approach of Crisp and Cook [1] 6. Biffin, E., Hill, R.S., and Lowe, A.J. (2010). Did
Kauri (Agathis: Araucariaceae) really survive the
result of different triggers, perhaps biases the estimated ages to be Oligocene drowning of New Zealand? Syst.
continent-specific climate change or older. The truth is likely to be Biol. 59, 594–601.
7. Qiao, C.Y., Ran, J.H., Li, Y., and Wang, X.Q.
coevolution with weevil or thrips somewhere in between, and these (2007). Phylogeny and biogeography of Cedrus
pollinators [19]. Additionally, the crown differences demonstrate the need (Pinaceae) inferred from sequences of seven
group age estimates for cycads in the for a better integration of the fossil paternal chloroplast and maternal
mitochondrial DNA regions. Ann. Bot. 100,
Crisp and Cook study [1] are much record to improve our understanding 573–580.
older and range from 10 to 50 million of the tempo and mode of 8. Ickert-Bond, S.M., Rydin, C., and Renner, S.S.
(2009). A fossil-calibrated relaxed clock for
years. Although their cycad taxon diversification in the gymnosperm tree Ephedra indicates an Oligocene age for the
sampling is not nearly as rich, of life. Such analyses will also depend divergence of Asian and New World clades and
increasing this sampling is bound to on a profound understanding of the Miocene dispersal into South America. J. Syst.
Evo. 47, 444–456.
push the crown group ages even morphology of both extant and fossil 9. Won, H., and Renner, S.S. (2006). Dating
further into the distant past. The gymnosperms. Only then will we be dispersal and radiation in the gymnosperm
Gnetum (Gnetales): clock calibration when
major difference between these two able to assess phylogenetic and outgroup relationships are uncertain. Syst. Biol.
analyses is in the interpretation and biogeographical affinities, evolutionary 55, 610–622.
Current Biology Vol 21 No 24
R998

10. Mao, K.S., Hao, G., Liu, J.Q., Adams, R.P., and 14. Wei, X.X., Yang, Z.Y., Li, Y., and Wang, X.Q. 19. Brenner, E.D., Stevenson, D.W., and
Milne, R.I. (2010). Diversification and (2010). Molecular phylogeny and biogeography Twigg, R.W. (2003). Cycads: evolutionary
biogeography of Juniperus (Cupressaceae): of Pseudotsuga (Pinaceae): Insights into innovations and the role of plant-derived
variable diversification rates and multiple the floristic relationship between Taiwan neurotoxins. Trends Plant Sci. 8, 446–452.
intercontinental dispersals. New Phytol. 188, and its adjacent areas. Mol. Phyl. Evol. 55, 20. Hermsen, E.J., Taylor, T.N., Taylor, E.L., and
254–272. 776–785. Stevenson, D.W. (2006). Cataphylls of the
11. Wagstaff, S.J. (2004). Evolution and 15. Treutlein, J., and Wink, M. (2002). Molecular Middle Triassic cycad Antarcticycas schopfii
biogeography of the austral genus phylogeny of cycads inferred from rbcL and new insights into cycad evolution. Am.
Phyllocladus (Podocarpaceae). J. Biogeogr. 31, sequences. Naturwissenschaften 89, 221–225. J. Bot. 93, 724–738.
1569–1577. 16. Hill, R.S., and Brodribb, T.J. (1999). Turner
12. Gernandt, D.S., Magallon, S., Lopez, G.G., Review No. 2-Southern conifers in time and
Flores, O.Z., Willyard, A., and Liston, A. (2008). space. Austral. J. Bot. 47, 639–696. Department of Organismic and Evolutionary
Use of simultaneous analyses to guide 17. Zachos, J., Pagani, M., Sloan, L., Thomas, E., Biology, Harvard University, Herbaria,
fossil-based calibrations of Pinaceae and Billups, K. (2001). Trends, rhythms, and 22 Divinity Avenue, Cambridge,
phylogeny. Int. J. Pl. Sci. 169, 1086–1099. aberrations in global climate 65 Ma to present. MA 02138, USA.
13. Biffin, E., Brodribb, T.J., Hill, R.S., Thomas, P., Science 292, 686–693.
and Lowe, A.J. (2011). Leaf evolution in 18. Crepet, W.L., and Niklas, K.J. (2009). Darwin’s
*E-mail: cdavis@oeb.harvard.edu
Southern Hemisphere conifers tracks the second ‘‘abominable mystery’’: Why are there
angiosperm ecological radiation. Pro. R. Soc. so many angiosperm species? Am. J. Bot. 96,
Biol. Sci. 10.1098/rspb.2011.0559. 366–381. DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.020

Chemoreception: Identifying Friends genes in the VNO of female mice


and Foes that had been exposed to bedding
used by male mice. One gene, Egr1,
was found to be induced particularly
strongly. Egr1 induction was confirmed
The vomeronasal organ detects chemical cues that trigger sexual, aggressive
to reflect neuronal activation, as
and defensive behaviors. An in situ hybridization analysis has identified the
determined by calcium imaging
specificities of nearly a hundred VNO receptors and elucidated the logic by
and patch-clamp electrophysiology.
which they encode these cues.
Thus, by performing double labeling
with Egr1 and VNO receptor probes,
Tong-Wey Koh and John R. Carlson behaviors [4]. The VNO acts in the it was possible to detect the
sensing of individual differences, activation of neurons expressing
In his seminal essay ‘The Hedgehog sex and the physiological status of particular receptors by particular
and the Fox’, Sir Isaiah Berlin divided conspecifics, in addition to the chemical cues.
thinkers into two categories. Plato, detection of kairomones from Mice were exposed to a panel of w30
Pascal, and Dostoevsky are like predators [5–7]. chemical cues, including conspecific
hedgehogs, which ‘know one big The VNO expresses more than 250 scents derived from the same or
thing’, whereas Aristotle, Montaigne, putative chemoreceptors. Most of opposite sex, and heterospecific
and Goethe are like foxes, which these receptors belong to two families scents representing predators, prey,
‘know many things’. While Berlin of heterotrimeric G-protein-coupled or neutral species. The heterospecific
later revealed that he had intended this receptors, the V1Rs and the V2Rs cues were derived from: mammalian
celebrated distinction as a kind of [8–12]. V1Rs and V2Rs are thought to predators, including foxes, ferrets,
game, the ability to distinguish be expressed in a one receptor–one and bobcats; avian predators,
hedgehogs and foxes is not a game neuron pattern, with the exception of including hawks and owls; reptilian
for mice. It can mean the difference the broadly -expressed V2R2 clade predators, including snakes and
between life and death. A remarkable [13,14]. Expression of V1R and V2R alligators; potential prey, represented
new study by Catherine Dulac and members are spatially segregated, with by insect larvae; related rodents
colleagues [1] has provided new V1Rs expressed in the apical layer of that are sympatric with the wild
insight into the molecular the VNO neuroepithelium and V2Rs ancestors of laboratory mice; and
mechanisms by which mice make in the basal layer. presumably neutral species such
such distinctions. For only a very few of these receptors as woodchucks. Using probes
How do mice identify the presence have ligands been previously identified. that target 139 VNO receptors,
of animals that pose a threat, and of Efforts to map ligands to VNO Isogai et al. [1] were able to identify
those that present opportunities for receptors have focused on a single 88 receptors (56 V1Rs and 32 V2Rs)
food or reproduction? Animals can be receptor or a single ligand [15,16]. that responded to subsets of this
identified on the basis of pheromones Isogai et al. [1], in a tour de force, panel of animal scents.
(conspecific cues) or kairomones systematically characterized the Isogai et al. [1] found that 28
(heterospecific cues that benefit the functional specificities of nearly receptors responded to mouse cues,
recipient) [2]. The vomeronasal organ a hundred VNO receptors [1]. To and 26 of these responded to
(VNO) is exquisitely sensitive to accomplish this, they first improved sex-specific cues. Some receptors
pheromones, containing neurons that an existing method of detecting VNO were activated by female-specific
are narrowly tuned to specific ligands responses. Immediate early genes cues in both males and females, while
[3]. Genetic or surgical disruption of are induced in vomeronasal neurons other receptors were activated by
VNO function in mice leads to profound by chemical cues. Isogai et al. [1] female-specific cues only in males.
but specific alterations of social screened a panel of immediate early Some receptors responded to

You might also like