You are on page 1of 3

Dispatch

R63

generated by a target on one part of the more about how dragonflies live their 2. Summerfield, C., and Egner, T. (2009).
Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition.
eye could mask any response fascinating lives, but will also advance Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 403–409.
generated by a ‘competitor’ on our general conceptual understanding 3. Sareen, P., Wolf, R., and Heisenberg, M. (2011).
another part of the eye. In this case, for how selective attention is achieved Attracting the attention of a fly. Proc. Natl.
Acad. Sci. USA 108, 7230–7235.
one might predict that the distribution in any system. Selective attention is 4. Wiederman, S.D., and O’Carroll, D.C. (2013).
of inhibition would be spatially tuned, a complex cognitive phenomenon, Selective attention in an insect visual neuron.
Curr. Biol. 23, 156–161.
and roughly map the inverse of this and this paper shows us that the 5. Nordström, K., and O’Carroll, D.C. (2009).
cell’s receptive field. Finally, it is worth hallmark characteristics observed Feature detection and the hypercomplex
noting that this cell’s architecture is at the organismal level are also property in insects. Trends Neurosci 32,
383–391.
such that it sends information back demonstrated at the single cell level
out toward the sensory periphery within an experimentally tractable
in a top-down manner. As such, the insect model system. That’s super Howard Hughes Medical Institute,
responses of this cell may well be cool. Department of Integrative Biology and
Physiology, University of California Los
used as feedback to shape incoming
Angeles, Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA.
sensory information, effectively filtering E-mail: frye@ucla.edu
its own selectivity. References
1. Cherry, E.C. (1953). Some experiments on the
The outcome of such lines of recognition of speech, with one and with two
investigation will not only tell us ears. J. Acous. Soc. Am. 25, 975–979. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.12.004

Brain Size Evolution: How Fish Pay under controlled experimental


for Being Smart conditions. They confirm a trade-off
between brain size and reproductive
output, and revive the ‘expensive
tissue hypothesis’ [4], which proposed
An artificial selection experiment demonstrates that large-brained guppies
a trade-off between gut and
learn better, but produce less offspring and have smaller guts. A close link
brain-size was allowing for brain
between brain size and fertility suggests that energetic trade-offs play an
expansion in human evolutionary
important role in brain size evolution.
history.
Similar selection experiments for
Karin Isler shorter time. Such experiments keep brain size were conducted more
the often unknown interdependencies than 30 years ago in mice, but the
Marveling at our own enormous among the traits of an individual effect on learning performance was
brains, we humans are fascinated by intact and help to narrow down the weak at best, perhaps due to small
the existing variation in brain size and numerous effects compatible with sample size [5]. Within humans,
cognitive abilities across the animal the results of broad comparative we cannot exclude the possibility
kingdom (Figure 1). Why did some analyses. Most artificial selection that the reported correlation between
species evolve to be more intelligent experiments have been done on brain size and IQ [6] may be
than others? Answering this question insects [2], but only a vertebrate model thoroughly confounded by underlying
unfortunately entails some awkward organism with reasonably short factors that affect both, such as
methodological complications. First, generation time could get us closer to child-rearing conditions or
cognitive abilities are very difficult to understanding cognitive evolution in socio-economic status. Thus,
compare between species that differ our own lineage. Now, in a new study the most convincing demonstrations
in motivation and sensorimotor in this issue of Current Biology, of a link between brain size and
adaptations. A simple morphological Kotrschal and colleagues [3] cognitive abilities so far have
proxy of ‘intelligence’, such as brain demonstrate the consequences of come from comparisons between
size or brain size relative to body brain size evolution with selection species. Between primate species,
size, would facilitate comparisons, experiments in guppies (Poecilia for example, brain size is
but first its validity would need to be reticulata). a reasonably good indicator of
established. Second, although The authors [3] found that after performance in cognitive tasks [7].
there has been much progress in just two generations, guppies Now, the guppy results [3] present
comparative methods that take selected for large brains differed from so far the strongest evidence for
phylogenetic relatedness into small-brained ones in several respects: a direct effect of brain size on
account, between-species large-brained female guppies, but not cognitive abilities within a species.
comparisons are inherently prone to males, performed better in a visual However, the absence of an effect
spurious findings due to the learning task, and they produced less in male guppies remains to be
unrecognized influence of hidden offspring at first birth (guppies are explained. Would the males do better
variables [1]. The only alternative is to live-bearing). Moreover, large-brained in a task based on olfactory rather
conduct a selection experiment under guppies had smaller guts, especially than visual cues? Additional tests
controlled conditions that mimics the males. These results demonstrate would be necessary to clarify
evolutionary change over a much direct effects of a change in brain size this finding.
Current Biology Vol 23 No 2
R64

performed worse in learning tasks


[14]. In sum, together with the
guppy results [3], there is strong
evidence for a direct energetic trade-off
between brain power and
reproduction.
Complicating the picture, the
possible effects of sexual selection
need also to be considered. A recent
study in great tits [15], for instance,
suggests a trade-off between
competitive strength and cognitive
abilities. The interplay between
sexual selection and physiological
trade-offs has not been investigated
yet, but the guppy seems to be an
optimal model system here as well.
Interestingly, the reduction of gut
mass was more pronounced in
male large-brained guppies than
in females [3]. It seems possible
that females put their enhanced
Figure 1. Variation in animal brain size. cognitive abilities into service of
Across the animal kingdom, brains differ in size relative to body mass. The vertical distance finding more food. Perhaps they
between a species’ data point and the regression line of its class denotes whether the species just ate a larger amount of the
is relatively large-brained or not. common share than their male
roommates. In consequence, they
would rather pay for the larger brain
Information processing comes at the trade-off between brain size and by reducing fertility than by
a cost, as neuronal computation is reproduction [11]. shrinking the gut. But it remains
energetically demanding [8]. To pay for Experimental evidence is needed to unclear to what use the males put their
a larger brain, an organism can either evaluate the direct effects that cause brains.
increase its overall energy turnover, or these patterns of correlated evolution. In all these studies, it is difficult to
allocate the available energy differently But in vertebrates, practical limitations distinguish between phenotypic
[9]. The latter option would result in hamper a full experimental exploration plasticity of the traits, which may be
a trade-off between brain size and of the quantitative genetics aspects triggered in each generation de novo,
other expensive functions such as of brain size evolution. It is simply and fixed traits that evolved as
offspring production or digestion. not feasible to measure all traits adaptive responses to differential
On a macroevolutionary scale, the simultaneously in a sufficiently large selective pressures. In Drosophila,
negative correlation between brain number of individuals to achieve the a single change of providing either less
size and fertility between species hints statistical power that would be needed or more food than before has been
at such as trade-off. Large-brained for determining the variance within shown to increase later learning ability,
species produce fewer offspring and across traits (the g-matrix). In irrespective of whether the change was
per year, which is not completely consequence, artificial selection from less to more food or vice versa
compensated for by prolonging their studies have mainly been done in [16]. Butterfly mushroom bodies grow
reproductive lifespan [10]. As a result, insects [2]. with time and experience [17],
populations of relatively large-brained In support of an energetic view of indicating that global costs of cognition
species have slower maximum growth the link between brain size and life may be reduced by a flexible
rates, and face a higher risk of going history, selection experiments on adjustment of brain power according to
extinct after catastrophic population Drosophila have demonstrated the whether conditions necessitate
crashes. Demographic viability thus global and inductive fitness costs of learning. In addition, a trade-off
puts an upper boundary to the relative enhanced learning and memory: in between different cognitive functions
brain size of a species in any given lines selected for high learning may exist, as has been shown for two
lineage. This ‘grey ceiling’ can only be abilities, larvae were less resistant to kinds of memory in Drosophila [18].
overcome if a change in lifestyle opens adverse conditions [12]. This global Plasticity in brain structures according
up new energy resources for the brain. cost was incurred regardless of to practice has also been found in
As females usually carry the bulk whether the animals had to do some humans [19], but it is unknown whether
of offspring production costs, one learning tasks or not. In addition, this affects the overall size or energy
possible change is the evolution of learning trials reduced subsequent consumption of the brain. This growing
a breeding system where helpers fertility [13], suggesting an inductive body of evidence for brain plasticity
provide energy subsidies for mothers cost that depends on the utilization of demonstrates that there are probably
and offspring. We have recently shown the larger brains. On the other hand, many ways to evolve brain power
that across mammals, help by fathers fruit flies selected on their ability to according to constraints and potential
and other group members alleviates survive environmental stress benefits.
Dispatch
R65

Therefore, experiments can yield without compromising fertility. 8. Niven, J.E. (2008). Energy limitation as
a selective pressure on the evolution of sensory
a proof-of-principle of energetic Based on comparative evidence, systems. J. Exp. Biol. 211, 1792–1804.
trade-offs, but they cannot tell us which bipedalism, a more stable diet of 9. Isler, K., and van Schaik, C.P. (2009). The
particular trade-off would be favored higher energy content and Expensive brain: a framework for explaining
evolutionary changes in brain size. J. Hum.
by selection under specific natural cooperation in rearing children could Evol. 57, 392–400.
conditions. For example, the all have played a role [20]. Now, the 10. Isler, K., and van Schaik, C.P. (2009). Why are
there so few smart mammals (but so many
large-brained guppy lines were found guppy results bring a gut–brain smart birds)? Biol. Lett. 5, 125–129.
to exhibit smaller guts [3]. But would trade-off back into the picture. 11. Isler, K., and van Schaik, C.P. (2012).
this trait combination be favored in More studies on artificially selected Allomaternal care, life history and brain size
evolution in mammals. J. Hum. Evol. 63, 52–63.
a natural setting, where food resources fish (or rodents) are needed to test 12. Mery, F., and Kawecki, T.J. (2003). A fitness
may be limited and competition is high? under which environmental cost of learning ability in Drosophila
melanogaster. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci.
The fact that there are no other poeciliid conditions these potential effects 270, 2465–2469.
fish species with much larger brains are found, how they interact with 13. Mery, F., and Kawecki, T.J. (2005). A cost of
and much smaller guts than guppies each other, and whether they are long-term memory in Drosophila. Science 308,
2005–2005.
indicates that adverse consequences flexible or fixed. But we also must 14. Kolss, M., and Kawecki, T.J. (2008). Reduced
for fitness most likely prevent such keep in mind that the adaptive learning ability as a consequence of
evolutionary adaptation to nutritional stress in
a trade-off in nature. Across mammals, responses of large, long-lived, and Drosophila melanogaster. Ecol. Entomol. 33,
neither the mass of guts nor of another socially bonded animals may not be 583–588.
expensive organ correlates negatively exactly the same. 15. Cole, E.F., and Quinn, J.L. (2012). Personality
and problem-solving performance explain
with brain size [20], calling into competitive ability in the wild. Proc. R. Soc.
question the validity of the expensive References
Lond. B Biol. Sci. 279, 1168–1175.
16. Kotrschal, A., and Taborsky, B. (2010).
tissue hypothesis [4] as a general 1. Healy, S.D., and Rowe, C. (2007). A critique of Environmental change enhances cognitive
principle. comparative studies of brain size. Proc. R. Soc. abilities in fish. PLoS Biol. 8,
Lond. B Biol. Sci. 274, 453–464. e1000351–e1000351.
Similarly, selection would only favor 2. Burns, J.G., Foucaud, J., and Mery, F. (2011). 17. Snell-Rood, E.C., Papaj, D.R., and
a combination of larger brains and Costs of memory: lessons from ‘mini’ brains. Gronenberg, W. (2009). Brain size: A global or
Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 278, 923–929.
reduced fertility if enhanced cognition 3. Kotrschal, A., Rogell, B., Bundsen, A.,
induced cost of learning? Brain Behav. Evol. 73,
111–128.
is indeed able to promote survival. Svensson, B., Zajitschek, S., Brännström, I., 18. Lagasse, F., Moreno, C., Preat, T., and Mery, F.
If unavoidable mortality is high, one Immler, S., Maklakov, A.A., and Kolm, N. (2013). (2012). Functional and evolutionary trade-offs
Artificial selection on relative brain size co-occur between two consolidated memory
would not expect larger-brained in the guppy reveals costs and benefits phases in Drosophila melanogaster. Proc. R.
species to evolve. Moreover, the of evolving a larger brain. Curr. Biol. 23, Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 279, 4015–4023.
168–171.
characteristics of each taxon are likely 4. Aiello, L.C., and Wheeler, P. (1995). The
19. May, A. (2011). Experience-dependent
structural plasticity in the adult human brain.
to have an impact on which of the expensive-tissue hypothesis - the brain and the Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 475–482.
potential trade-offs are chosen by digestive-system in human and primate 20. Navarrete, A.F., van Schaik, C.P., and Isler, K.
evolution. Curr. Anthropol. 36, 199–221. (2011). Energetics and the evolution of human
natural selection. For instance, in 5. Jensen, C., and Fuller, J. (1978). Learning brain size. Nature 480, 91–93.
precocial mammals that produce performance varies with brain weight in
heterogeneous mouse lines. J. Comp. Physiol.
only one offspring at a time, we Psychol. 92, 830–836. Anthropological Institute and Museum,
found that relatively large-brained 6. McDaniel, M.A. (2005). Big-brained people are University of Zürich-Irchel,
species prolong the time span smarter: A meta-analysis of the relationship Winterthurerstrasse 190, CH-8057 Zürich,
between in vivo brain volume and intelligence.
between subsequent births, whereas Intelligence 33, 337–346. Switzerland.
in altricials they bring forth less 7. Reader, S.M., Hager, Y., and Laland, K.N. E-mail: kisler@aim.uzh.ch
(2011). The evolution of primate general and
offspring per litter [9]. Accordingly, cultural intelligence. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol.
only large-brained precocial Sci. 366, 1017–1027. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.11.042
young take longer to mature than
small-brained ones, but large-brained
altricial young don’t. Thus, we would
need to study both an altricial and
a precocial mammal species, and
ideally also other animals from other Development: New Wrinkles on
lineages such as birds or reptiles, to
fully explore all the potential effects. Genetic Control of the MBT
But, unfortunately, artificial selection
experiments such as those performed
Three recent studies revise the prevailing view of regulation of the mid-blastula
by Kotrschal et al. [3] are simply
transition in Drosophila, indicating particular requirements for the Cdc25
not feasible in large, slow-growing
phosphatase Twine and for zygotic transcription of a specific set of genes.
animals. In addition to experiments,
we thus do need to take a comparative
approach investigating evolutionary Paul Lasko in different organisms, but most
trajectories in various groups of usually during the blastula stage of
related species. Animal embryos depend for their initial development [1]. This is when the
In the human lineage, brain development on maternally expressed fertilized egg has progressed through
expansion was most likely achieved mRNAs that are deposited into the numerous rounds of mitosis to form
by a combination of several changes egg during oogenesis. Activation of a single layer of cells, but before
in lifestyle that allowed for a larger zygotic genes (the maternal–zygotic gastrulation and specification of
supply of energy for the brain transition) occurs at different times the ectoderm, mesoderm, and

You might also like