You are on page 1of 9

Original Article

Journal of Child Neurology


1-9
Early Infant Development and Intervention ª The Author(s) 2015
Reprints and permission:
sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav
for Autism Spectrum Disorder DOI: 10.1177/0883073815601500
jcn.sagepub.com

Lori-Ann R. Sacrey, PhD1,2, Jeffrey A. Bennett, MSc1,


and Lonnie Zwaigenbaum, MD, MSc1,2

Abstract
Objective: The objective is to overview recent findings on early detection/diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders, as well as
clinical trials of early interventions for toddlers at risk for/diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Findings: Prospective studies
of infants at high risk of autism spectrum disorder have yielded significant advances in understanding early development in autism
spectrum disorder. Findings from prospective studies indicate that abnormalities in social communication and repetitive behaviors
emerge during the second year, whereas additional ‘‘prodromal features’’ (motor and sensory abnormalities) emerge in the first
year. Subsequently, exciting progress has been made in establishing the efficacy of autism spectrum disorder–specific interventions
for toddlers as young as 15 months. Finally, efforts occur to characterize autism spectrum disorder–specific characteristics in
genetic syndromes with concurrent autism spectrum disorder symptomatology. Conclusion: Substantial progress in char-
acterizing early developmental trajectories as well as the identification of specific behavioral markers has aided early detection.
Work remains to ensure that research findings are translated into clinical practice for uptake in the health care system.

Keywords
early identification, infant siblings, early intervention, autism spectrum disorder, review

Received July 21, 2015. Accepted for publication July 25, 2015.

The heterogeneity of autism spectrum disorders, both clinically is exciting progress in establishing the efficacy of autism spec-
and etiologically, creates challenges in identifying a set of early trum disorder–specific interventions for toddlers as young as
risk markers that would be informative across the continuum of 18 months.4,12 Finally, efforts have been made to characterize
severity and complexity of affected children. However, despite autism spectrum disorder–specific characteristics in genetic
these challenges, there are many reasons to continue to strive syndromes with concurrent autism spectrum disorder sympto-
toward earlier detection and diagnosis of the disorder. First, matology.13-15 These efforts together bring us closer to the core
there is an unacceptable delay between parents’ first concerns mechanisms underlying autism spectrum disorder. The authors
and confirmation of a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder,1,2 summarize advances in each of these areas in this review.
which creates added stress for families and may adversely
impact on subsequent engagement with health care providers.3
Second, earlier diagnosis creates opportunities for children Prospective Design: Application to Studies of
with autism spectrum disorder to benefit more fully from ear- Early Development of Autism Spectrum
lier initiation of intervention, which benefits both the child and Disorder
the community.4,5 There is also broader scientific relevance to
Prospective studies of high-risk infant cohorts (generally,
delineating the earliest expression of the autism spectrum dis-
younger siblings of children diagnosed with autism spectrum
order phenotype in terms of understanding underlying mechan-
disorder) are well suited to understand early development in
isms, and in particular, to be able to study the neurological
features of autism spectrum disorder early in development.
Early detection efforts have benefitted from recent advances 1
Department of Pediatrics, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada
in research, including broad implementation of prospective 2
Autism Research Centre, Glenrose Rehabilitation Hospital, Edmonton,
study designs involving high-risk infants6 and technological Alberta, Canada
advances in assessing early brain structure and function.7-9
Corresponding Author:
Indeed substantial progress has been made in characterizing Lori-Ann R. Sacrey, PhD, Autism Research Centre—E209, Glenrose Rehabilitation
early behavioral and neurobiological markers of autism spec- Hospital, 10230 111 Ave, Edmonton, Alberta, Canada T5G 0B7.
trum disorder in high-risk infant cohorts.10,11 In addition, there Email: sacrey@ualberta.ca

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on September 3, 2015


2 Journal of Child Neurology

autism spectrum disorder. The prospective, high-risk design impairments, emerge during the first 12 months, and atypical
has been used to study other disorders, such as schizophrenia,16 brain development and function are apparent in the first 12
but the advantage of utilizing this method in autism spectrum months and may precede behavioral symptoms. Each set of
disorder is the early onset of the disorder. The prospective findings will be discussed in turn.
design offers additional advantages. First, infants can be fol-
lowed from a very early age, allowing examination of early
systems, such as motor development and visual attention.
Diagnostic Symptomatology
Second, behaviors can be studied longitudinally to age at diag- Abnormalities in social communication include deficits in
nosis, which may help address questions regarding develop- social reciprocity, nonverbal communication, developing rela-
mental trajectories of autism spectrum disorder. Third, tionships, and verbal communication. By 12 months of age,
clinical assessments can be correlated with neurobiological high-risk infants show reduced orienting to name,24,25 gazing
phenomena, such as underlying EEG measurement. Fourth, to faces, and directed vocalizations,26 and by 18 months of age,
early behavioral signs of autism spectrum disorder can be mea- showed reduced social smiling and social engagement.26,27 In
sured under standardized conditions, allowing greater compar- addition, deficits in responding to referential cues during joint
ability both within and between individuals over time. Finally, attention occur early in the second year of life for children who
interventions can be implemented at an earlier age in infants later are diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder.28,29 Other
and toddlers who show behavioral signs of autism spectrum nonverbal skills, such as the use of gestures, also show early
disorder. impairments. High-risk infants later diagnosed with autism
To what degree can younger siblings be considered ‘‘high spectrum disorder are less likely to show an object or use
risk’’; that is, what is the recurrence risk of autism spectrum directed pointing at 12 months of age,30 and have a smaller
disorder (ie, the likelihood of autism spectrum disorder for each inventory of gestures.29,31 High-risk infants who are later diag-
later-born sibling) compared to general population risk? One of nosed with autism spectrum disorder also show reduced atten-
the first epidemiological studies of recurrence risk in autism tiveness to their mother during naturalistic interactions, as well
spectrum disorder was completed in the 1980s at the University as reduced dyadic mutuality.32,33 Several studies have reported
of Utah.17 The recurrence risk was estimated at 8.6%. More delays in receptive language by 12 months of age in infants
recently, a multisite international network, the Baby Siblings who are later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. These
Research Consortium reported on 664 infant siblings of chil- siblings understood fewer phrases between 12 to 14 months, as
dren with autism spectrum disorder followed to age 3, when reported by parents on the Communicative Developmental
they were classified as having or not having autism spectrum Inventories,31 as well as understanding fewer phrases with or
disorder. Overall, the recurrence rate was 18.7%.18 However, without accompanying gestures on the Mullen Scales of Early
3 recent large population-based studies have reported lower Learning.25,34 In addition to receptive problems, language pro-
recurrence rate estimates. A Danish birth cohort study involv- duction is also impaired in children who are later diagnosed
ing 1.5 million children born between 1980 and 2004, esti- with autism spectrum disorder. Parents of these children report
mated recurrence rate for autism spectrum disorder at only fewer words on the Communicative Developmental Invento-
6.9%.19 Similarly, recurrence rate was 10.1% among younger ries at 18 months of age,25,31 and these children score lower
siblings of individuals with autism spectrum disorder in a on the expressive language component of the Mullen Scales
1990-2003 California birth cohort (n ¼ 6616),20 and 12.9% of Early Learning.34
in a population-based cohort of Swedish children born between Abnormalities in restricted interests or repetitive behaviors
1982 and 2006 (n ¼ 2 049 973).21 Despite this broad range of include repetitive motor movements, atypical use of objects,
recurrence risk estimates (roughly 8-18%), it is clear that the and atypical sensory interests. Reports of repetitive movements
rate of autism spectrum disorder among younger siblings is suggest that they lack early specificity in autism spectrum dis-
higher than that in the general population, most recently esti- order. Loh and colleagues35 analyzed repetitive movements
mated at 1.5% (or 1 in 68 children) by the US Centers for Dis- during a semistructured assessment and identified the fre-
ease Control and Prevention (CDC).22 quency of arm waving as the only item that distinguished 12-
and 18-month-old siblings who were later diagnosed with aut-
ism spectrum disorder from other high-risk and low-risk sam-
Prospective Studies of Infants at Risk of
ples. In contrast, for use of objects, 12-month-olds often used
Autism Spectrum Disorder: Key Findings play materials in stereotyped, self-stimulatory ways (eg, an
Converging findings from prospective studies of infants at risk infant might wave a string of beads in front of his/her eyes).36
of autism spectrum disorder (younger siblings) suggest that Similarly, Ozonoff and colleagues37 noted that 12-month-old
‘‘diagnostic’’ symptoms (that is, behavioral features that high-risk infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder
related to diagnostic criteria defined by current frameworks showed more atypical uses of objects, particularly unusual
such as the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Man- visual exploration of objects, rotating, and spinning, but did not
ual of Mental Disorders)23 are expressed by 12-18 months in differ on throwing or mouthing of objects. High-risk children
many children with autism spectrum disorder. However, ‘‘pro- later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder also show addi-
dromal’’ (ie, earlier) symptoms, such as sensory and motor tional reactivity and sensory issues. Parents of these infant

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on September 3, 2015


Sacrey et al 3

siblings later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder reported Zwaigenbaum and colleagues25 using a semistructured play
atypical auditory processing and lower sensory registration on assessment. Similar findings are seen in computer based visual
the Infant-Toddler Sensory Profile at 24 months of age.38 assessments.36,47 Elsabbagh et al8 reported that high-risk
infants who were diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder at
24-36 months showed increases in their latency to disengage
Prodromal Symptoms when looking at objects between 7 and 14 months, whereas dis-
engagement latencies in typically developing children and
A growing literature suggests that early sensory and motor dif-
high-risk infants without autism spectrum disorder decreased
ferences, and possible emotional regulation differences, form a
or remained the same. In addition, Elison et al9 found longer
prodrome of autism spectrum disorder that manifests in the lat-
latencies to disengage attention from objects in 7-month-old
ter half of the first year of life,39-42 before the appearance of
infants with high Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule
social-communication and restrictive behavioral differences
scores at 24 months. In addition to prolonged disengagement
more directly related to autism spectrum disorder diagnostic
from objects, autism spectrum disorder is associated with
criteria (reviewed in Zwaigenbaum et al11).
diminished attention to faces (no relative difference in eyes
Motor. Prospective studies of high-risk siblings who are later versus mouth) in videotaped social scenes at 6 months of age
diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder identify delays in in high-risk infants who are later diagnosed with autism spec-
gross and fine motor skills on the Mullen Scales of Early trum disorder.48 Similarly, a prospective longitudinal study of
Learning at 14 and 24 months of age, but not at 6 months.26,34,43 infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder show
Zwaigenbaum and colleagues25 also report lower fine and gross declines in percentage of time gazed at eyes versus mouth on
motor scores on the Mullen Scales of Early Learning compared a face shown on a video during early infancy. In fact, the sever-
with nondiagnosed high-risk infants and controls at 12 and ity of decline between 2 and 6 months of age was informative
18 months, but not at 6 months.27 for autism spectrum disorder risk.49
Rather than looking at scale scores, Libertus and col-
leagues42 assessed toy play during the Mullen Scales of Early Temperament. Prospective studies have shown evidence of
Learning and found that high-risk infants exhibited less mature reduced positive affect in high-risk infants who are later diag-
object manipulation and reduced grasping activity than infants nosed with autism spectrum disorder by the first birthday when
with no family history of autism spectrum disorder at 6 months, compared to nondiagnosed siblings and low-risk controls.29,50
with grasping activity increasing in high-risk infants between 6 Zwaigenbaum and colleagues25 reported fewer observed posi-
and 10 months. Additional evidence of atypical motor develop- tive affective responses and increased distress in high-risk
ment include reduced motor maturity at 3 and 6 months in high- infants with autism spectrum disorder at 12 months, as coded
risk infants versus low-risk infants (without outcome informa- from the Autism Observation Scale for Infants, compared to
tion) using the Alberta Infant Motor Scales,44 and pronounced those with typical development, consistent with concurrently
head lag during pull-to-sit at 6 months, delayed independent collected parental reports of temperament. In a subsequent
sitting, reduced postural stability, and reduced rhythmic arm analysis of high-risk infant siblings (n ¼ 138) and low-risk
movements in high-risk infant siblings.45,46 Further clarifica- infants (n ¼ 73), Garon et al51 examined the relationship
tion is needed on whether abnormalities in high-risk infants are between temperament at 24 months and 3-year outcomes.
specific to those who later are diagnosed with autism spectrum Using discriminant function analysis, they identified profiles
disorder or are shared by a broader group of high-risk infants that were informative for general differences between high-
who show later social-communication impairments.44 risk siblings and low-risk comparison infants, and that were
specific to siblings subsequently diagnosed with autism spec-
Sensory. Coding of home videos of children who are diagnosed trum disorder. A profile that included poor regulation of nega-
with autism spectrum disorder show differences in sensory tive emotions, and difficulty with attention control (increased
behaviors at 9 and 12 months of age, particularly visual explo- attention shifting) distinguished the high-risk sibling group
ration of their visual environment, differentiated infants with (as a whole) from comparison infants, whereas a second profile
autism spectrum disorder from those who had developmental that included low positive affect and increased duration of
disabilities or who were typically developing.39 Similar find- attention, was associated specifically with autism spectrum dis-
ings come from prospective studies of high-risk infant siblings order. Similarly, Clifford and colleagues52 noted that parents of
of children with autism spectrum disorder. Ozonoff and col- high-risk infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder
leagues37 reported that 12-month-old high infants who were reported reduced positive affect, but not increased negative
later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder were different affect, as characterizing beginning at 7 months. As well, Feld-
from children with developmental disability and typically man and colleagues53 noted that community diagnosed chil-
developing infants in atypical uses of objects, such as rotating, dren with autism spectrum disorder were rated by the parents
spinning and unusual visual exploration. Atypical sensory- as more difficult when waiting to have their needs met at 9,
oriented behaviors at 12 months (including intense visual 12, and 18 months of age. These results suggest that emotional
inspection) were also identified as an early risk marker of regulation differences may precede the core symptomatology
autism spectrum disorder in high-risk infants assessed by of autism spectrum disorder.

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on September 3, 2015


4 Journal of Child Neurology

Neurobiological Markers directed or averted eye gaze in 7-month-olds and compared it


to infants’ overt behavior during a caregiver-child dyadic free
Atypical brain development and function are evident in the first
play session.57 The results suggested that infants with more
12 months in high-risk infants who are later diagnosed with
positive affect exhibited stronger differentiation to gaze stimuli
autism spectrum disorder using magnetic resonance imaging
in respect to P400 responses to directed gaze versus averted
(MRI) and electroencephalogram (EEG).
gaze. These results suggest the integrity of the neural response
is associated with overt, observed behavioral phenomenon.
MRI. Hazlett and colleagues54 compared brain volumes at 2 and
4 years of age in children at high risk of autism spectrum dis- Other. Increased head circumference in children with autism
order and without familial risk of autism spectrum disorder. spectrum disorder is one of the most consistent findings in aut-
They reported generalized cortical enlargement in children ism spectrum disorder. Yet, this association is now in question
with autism spectrum disorder at both 2 and 4 years of age, and due to problems in interpretation related to using CDC norms
after controlling for total brain volume, children with autism for comparison. A systematic review by Raznahan et al58 iden-
spectrum disorder had disproportionate enlargement of tem- tified 5 independent longitudinal cohorts of typically developing
poral lobe white matter. The authors note that there was no sig- children that demonstrate trajectories in head circumference
nificant difference from controls in the rate of brain growth at z scores that deviate from CDC norms.59 To understand whether
this age interval, suggesting that the brain enlargement took head growth in the first 3 years differed between high-risk
place prior to 2 years of age.54 Investigation of white matter infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder versus
tract organization from 6 to 24 months of age in children at high-risk infants who were not diagnosed with autism spectrum
high risk of autism spectrum disorder showed higher fractional disorder and community controls, Zwaigenbaum and col-
anisotropy values at 6 months, followed by slower changes leagues60 obtained prospective and growth-records of head cir-
over time in comparison to infants without autism spectrum cumference from 492 high-risk infants (77 of whom were later
disorder. In a related investigation, measuring visual orienting diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder) and 333 low-risk
latencies in 7-month-old infants at high risk for autism spec- community controls. Overall, there were no significant differ-
trum disorder and associating outcomes with the microstructure ences comparing head growth between high-risk infants (regard-
of the corpus callosum, Elison and colleagues9 found that high- less of outcome) and low-risk controls in the first 3 years of life.
risk infants who were later diagnosed with autism spectrum Furthermore, there were no differences in head growth related to
disorder showed longer disengagement latencies than both clinical outcome within the high-risk group (when subtyping
high-risk negative infants and control infants. Furthermore, the based on autism spectrum disorder, developmental disability,
disengagement latencies were associated with the microstruc- and typical outcomes). The authors concluded that head growth
ture organization of the splenium of the corpus callosum in the was largely uninformative as a risk-marker for autism spectrum
control infants, but this association was not apparent in high- disorder, at least within the reported high-risk cohort.
risk infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder,
suggesting early atypical development in autism spectrum dis-
order. Finally, Lewis and colleagues55 measured differences in Early Intervention for Autism Spectrum Disorder
brain connectivity in autism spectrum disorder at 24 months of As detailed above, prospective studies of high-risk infants who
age, an age at which the diagnostic features of autism spectrum are later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder indicate
disorder become clear. Network efficiency was analyzed by impairments appear early in development for sensory and
measuring the length and strength of connections between ana- motor ability, visual attention, social-emotional regulation, and
tomical regions, resulting in significantly decreased local and communication. The atypical development of these domains
global efficiency over the temporal, parietal, and occipital may lead to disruptions in early interactions with the environ-
lobes in high-risk infants classified as autism spectrum disor- ment, resulting in cascading effects during the first 3 years of
der. The authors suggest that delays or deficits in network effi- life. Moving forward, it becomes necessary to develop earlier
ciency are associated with the impairments in audition, visual interventions to coincide with earlier identification of autism
processing, language, and nonlinguistic social stimuli. spectrum disorder and improve lifelong outcomes for these
children. For a recent review of potential treatment targets for
EEG. A prospective longitudinal study of infants at high risk of interventions, see Brian et al,12 and for a recent review article
autism spectrum disorder examined whether neural responses that summarizes key feature shared in common across recently
to eye gaze at 6-10 months of age was associated with autism reported interventions for infants and toddlers with autism
spectrum disorder symptomatology.7,56 Event-related poten- spectrum disorder or at risk of autism spectrum disorder, see
tials were recorded in response to infants viewing faces with Schreibman et al.5
the eye gaze either directed toward or away from the infant and
noted that changes to the characteristic of the event-related
potential response, specifically the P400 response, was associ-
Clinical Trials
ated with an outcome of autism spectrum disorder. In a subse- Dawson and colleagues61 provided the first randomized con-
quent study, the authors measured the P400 response to trolled trial to demonstrate efficacy for a behavioral

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on September 3, 2015


Sacrey et al 5

intervention for toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Chil- Parents completed 12 weeks of in-home training and chil-
dren diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder between 18 and dren were followed up 24 weeks later to examine communi-
30 months (n ¼ 48) were enrolled in a randomized, controlled cation ability and shared positive affect. Overall, children
trial to evaluate the efficacy of the early start Denver model, a showed improved language performance and functional ver-
comprehensive developmental behavioral intervention for balizations, with increases in responsivity, initiations, social
improving outcomes of toddlers diagnosed with autism spec- orienting, and shared smiling.
trum disorder. Following random assignment in 1 of 2 groups
(Early Start Denver model for 2 years versus ‘treatment as
usual’ [typical community intervention]), those children who
Other Intervention Studies
received Early Start Denver model showed significant
improvements in IQ, adaptive behavior, and autism spectrum Pivotal response treatment is an evidence-based, manualized
disorder diagnosis (showing fewer autism spectrum disorder intervention for individuals with autism spectrum disorder with
symptoms) compared to the treatment as usual group. Carter empirical support for use with preschool children with autism
et al62 reported a randomized controlled trial comparing spectrum disorder.65 Steiner and colleagues66 developed an
Hanen’s ‘More Than Words’, a parent-implemented interven- adaptation of pivotal response treatment and piloted, via a brief
tion for 3.5 months, to a ‘treatment as usual’ group in 20- parent-training model, the intervention with 12-month-old
month-old children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder infants at risk for autism (n ¼ 3). The results provided prelim-
(n ¼ 62). Overall, there were no main effects of the More Than inary support for the feasibility and utility of pivotal response
Words intervention on parental responsivity or children’s com- treatment for very young children at risk for autism spectrum
munication ability compared to the treatment as usual group disorder. That is, utilizing a multiple baseline design, the
receiving community intervention, but the More Than Words introduction of pivotal response treatment resulted in
intervention showed differential effects on child increases in the infants’ frequency of functional communica-
communication depending on their baseline performance. tion and parents’ fidelity of implementation of pivotal
Wetherby et al63 completed a randomized control trial of the response treatment procedures. Landa et al67 reported on a
‘Early Social Interaction’ Project for 16- to 20-month-old tod- sample of 2- and 3-year-old children with autism spectrum
dlers diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Children were disorder (n ¼ 48) who participated in a research-based, 6-
matched on nonverbal developmental level and were randomly month comprehensive intervention at 10 hours per week
assigned to an individualized Early Social Interaction treatment within a nursery classroom. Parents received weekly on-site
(1-on-1 parent coaching 2-3 times per week) or group Early education and monthly home-based coaching on teaching
Social Interaction treatment (group parent coaching offered strategies. From pre- to postintervention, significant gains
once per week) conditions for 9 months. The children enrolled were made in IQ and communication scores on the Vineland,
in the individualized Early Social Interaction treatment showed as well as a reduction in autism spectrum disorder severity
improvements on observational measures of social communi- scores. Measurements completed at 6-month and long-term
cation and receptive language skills, parent-reported measures follow up after intervention found that IQ and communication
of communication, daily living, social skills, and stability over- stabilized, but autism spectrum disorder severity scores
time. In contrast, children enrolled in the group Early Social increased. Pre- to post-follow-up trajectories showed that
Interaction treatment showed worsening or no changes to these robust gains were observed for both IQ and communication
skills, highlighting the importance of parent coaching in an scores, with no changes in autism spectrum disorder symp-
individualized environment. Similarly, Kasari et al64 per- tom severity. Recently, Rogers et al4 developed and piloted
formed a randomized control trial on a parent-mediated inter- the feasibility of a manualized, parent-delivered intervention
vention for 15- to 21-month-old toddlers at high risk of for infants aged 6-15 months who were highly symptomatic
autism spectrum disorder. Parents were enrolled in 12 ses- for autism spectrum disorder (n ¼ 7 high risk and n ¼ 7 low
sions of individualized parent education intervention or to risk for autism spectrum disorder). The intervention aimed
a control group involving 4 sessions of behavioral support at 6 target symptoms (visual fixation, repetitive behaviors,
for 3 months. Parental responsiveness improved signifi- communicative acts, turn taking, phonemic development,
cantly in the individualized treatment group compared to the and gaze) and consisted of 12 weekly, 1-hour sessions.
control group over the 3-month period, however it had Follow-up at 36 months showed that the treatment group
declined in the 12-month follow-up. Although children in had lower rates of autism spectrum disorder symptomatol-
both groups made gains in cognitive and language ability ogy and fewer developmental quotients under 70 on the
over the 12 months, there were no treatment effects on joint Mullen Scales of Early Learning than high-risk infants with
attention for the individualized treatment group. Finally, autism spectrum disorder who were not enrolled in the treat-
Brian and colleagues12 completed a clinical trial on a ment intervention. Overall, the results of the intervention
parent-mediated intervention, the ‘Social ABCs’, based on studies reviewed above underscore the importance of early
adapted pivotal response treatment principles and strategies. detection of autism spectrum disorder, noting promising
The intervention relied on in-home, parent-mediated strate- outcomes of short and long-term intervention in toddlers
gies with in-vivo coaching by a trained interventionist. with autism spectrum disorder.

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on September 3, 2015


6 Journal of Child Neurology

Genetic Disorders and Autism Spectrum Down Syndrome


Disorder Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder in Down syndrome was
Specific genetic syndromes associated with autism spectrum reported as 18% (24% of males and 9% of females), based on clin-
disorder represent other potential high-risk groups that can be ical diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder in a cohort of 123 indi-
studied prospectively beginning in infancy. However, the ques- viduals with Down syndrome,81 also coinciding with the review
tion of co-occurring autism spectrum disorder in genetic syn- by Moss and Howlin,14 reporting 5-39% of individuals with
dromes can be complex, as reported prevalence rates vary Down syndrome had co-occurring autism spectrum disorder.
widely,14,15 and may be driven by particular behavioral symp- Interestingly, it appears as though autism spectrum disorder is
toms. One possible reason for variability in reported prevalence most frequently found in children with Down syndrome who also
could be the use of different assessment tools, including autism have intellectual disability. Indeed, the Moss and Howlin14
spectrum disorder screening questionnaires68-70 and gold stan- review found that individuals with Down syndrome who met cri-
dard autism spectrum disorder diagnostic tools,71-73 as well as teria for autism spectrum disorder also fell within the ‘‘moderate
parent report surveys.74,75 Although a complete review of to severe’’ degree of intellectual disability. Molloy et al82 found
genetic syndromes associated with autism spectrum disorder that the deficits in social communication and restricted and repe-
is beyond the scope of this article, for illustration, the following titive behaviors are not entirely explained by the intellectual dis-
section reports on 3 syndromes with elevated incidence of aut- ability, suggesting that the autism spectrum disorder in Down
ism spectrum disorder: fragile-X syndrome, Down syndrome, syndrome is not directly caused by cognitive impairment. Rather,
and Prader-Willi syndrome. children with Down syndrome and autism spectrum disorder
begin to display atypical behaviors during infancy or toddlerhood,
including social disinterest, lack of sustained joint attention, few
Fragile-X Syndrome gestures, repetitive motor behavior, self-injurious behaviors, sen-
sory seeking, and unusual play with objects.83 As with fragile-X
On a national parent survey in the United States, more than
syndrome, diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder in Down syn-
1000 parents of children with fragile-X syndrome were asked
drome can also reliably be performed in the preschool years.81
about co-occurring conditions. In total, parents reported that
40% (46% of males and 16% of females) had a diagnosis of aut-
ism spectrum disorder,74 consistent with a recent review citing Prader-Willi Syndrome
a range of 21-50% of autism spectrum disorder in genetic dis- The prevalence of autism spectrum disorder in Prader-Willi syn-
orders.14 The results from the national parent survey also found drome was reported as 25% in a recent systematic review,84 con-
that fragile-X syndrome and autism spectrum disorder were firming previous findings.85 One interesting recurring finding is a
rarely reported in isolation; rather, they frequently coincided higher prevalence of autism spectrum disorder symptoms in the
with attention problems, anxiety, and hyperactivity.74 Asses- uniparental disomy genetic subtype of Prader-Willi syndrome
sing the clinical utility of using the CHAT to identify autism when compared to the deletion subtype,86,87 presumably due to
spectrum disorder in young children with fragile-X the correlation between chromosome 15q11-13 and autism spec-
syndrome, Scambler et al76 noted that using the Denver trum disorder.88,89 Specifically, adolescents and adults with the
Criteria,77 the CHAT showed high levels of sensitivity (75%) uniparental disomy subtype have scored similar in autism spec-
and specificity (92%) in identifying those children with trum disorder symptomatology to comparison groups with autism
fragile-X syndrome who also had concurrent autism spectrum spectrum disorder, and higher than those with the deletion sub-
disorder prior to age 3. Similarly, Rogers et al78 compared chil- type.86 Of note, individuals with Prader-Willi syndrome and aut-
dren with autism spectrum disorder, developmental delays, and ism spectrum disorder display repetitive behaviors, a need for
fragile-X syndrome on two gold-standard autism spectrum dis- sameness, verbal perseverations, poor peer relations, and anxiety,
order instruments and noted that the subgroup of children with symptoms also present in autism spectrum disorder.90 However,
fragile-X syndrome but without autism spectrum disorder per- there is paucity in the literature concerning autism spectrum dis-
formed similarly to the children with developmental delays, order in younger children with Prader-Willi syndrome. To date,
whereas the subgroup of children with fragile-X syndrome and no study investigating autism spectrum disorder in Prader-Willi
autism spectrum disorder were virtually identical to the chil- syndrome has had a mean age below 8 years. Recent research has
dren with autism spectrum disorder. Overall, children with suggested that autism spectrum disorder symptoms might not
fragile-X syndrome and autism spectrum disorder have a sim- emerge in Prader-Willi syndrome until children are older,91,92 but
ilar profile of scores on the Autism Diagnostic Observation further research is required to confirm these findings.
Schedule79 as children with autism spectrum disorder; how-
ever, the individuals with fragile-X syndrome and autism spec-
trum disorder also perform significantly lower on performance
and verbal IQ.13 These studies and a longitudinal study of boys
Summary
with fragile-X syndrome find that autism spectrum disorder can Prospective studies of autism spectrum disorder with high-risk
be reliably diagnosed in fragile-X syndrome during the pre- infants have been informative for identifying early behaviors
school years.80 that are predictive of autism spectrum disorder. Interestingly,

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on September 3, 2015


Sacrey et al 7

such studies have proposed that the diagnostic characteristics 8. Elsabbagh M, Fernandes J, Webb SJ, et al. Disengagement of
of the disorder, social-communication and restricted interests visual attention in infancy is associated with emerging autism in
and repetitive behaviors, may be preceded by prodromal symp- toddlerhood. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74(3):189-194.
toms, specifically, sensory and motor impairments. Indeed, 9. Elison JT, Paterson SJ, Wolff JJ, et al. White matter microstruc-
early differences have been identified in the brains of children ture and atypical visual orienting in 7-month-olds at risk for autism.
who will later be diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder as Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170(8):899-908.
early as 6 to 9 months of age, appearing prior to or concurrently 10. Jones EJ, Gliga T, Bedford R, Charman T, Johnson MH. Devel-
with symptom onset. There has been progress in establishing opmental pathways to autism: a review of prospective studies of
developmentally appropriate and evidence-based interventions, infants at risk. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;39:1-33.
yet scholars still have a long way to go toward the goal of ear- 11. Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Garon N. Early identification of aut-
lier diagnosis and intervention for all children with autism ism spectrum disorders. Behav Brain Res. 2013;15:133-146.
spectrum disorder. As such, research findings need to be trans- 12. Brian JA, Bryson SE, Zwaigenbaum L. Autism spectrum disor-
lated into clinical practice must address health systems issues, ders in infancy: developmental considerations in treatment tar-
including access to specialized care. gets. Curr Opin Neurol. 2015;28:117-123.
13. Dissanayake C, Bui Q, Bulhak-Paterson D, Huggins R, Loesch D.
Author Contributions Behavioural and cognitive phenotypes in idiopathic autism versus
LRS and LZ developed the objectives for this review article. LRS, autism associated with fragile X syndrome. J Child Adolesc Psychol
JAB and LZ drafted initial sections of the manuscript and LRS inte- Psychiatry. 2009;50:290-299.
grated the sections into an overall first draft. LRS, JAB and LZ criti- 14. Moss J, Howlin P. Autism spectrum disorders in genetic syn-
cally revised the manuscript and approved of its final version. dromes: implications for diagnosis, intervention and understand-
ing the wider autism spectrum disorder population. J Intellect
Declaration of Conflicting Interests Disability Res. 2009;53(10):852-873.
The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to 15. Zafeiriou DI, Ververi A, Vargiami E. Childhood autism and asso-
the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. ciated comorbidities. Brain Dev. 2007;29(5):257-272.
16. Buka SL, Seidman LJ, Tsuang MT, Goldstein JM. The New England
Funding and Family Study High-risk Project: neurological impairments
The authors received no financial support for the research, authorship, among offspring of parents with schizophrenia and other psychoses.
and/or publication of this article. Am J Genet B Neuropsychiatry Genet. 2013;162B(7):653-660.
17. Ritvo ER, Jorde LB, Mason-Brothers A, et al. The UCLA-
References University of Utah epidemiologic survey of autism: recurrence
1. Rowberry J, Macari S, Chen GS, et al. Screening for autism spec- risk estimates and genetic counseling. Am J Psychiatry. 1989;
trum disorders in 12-month-old high-risk siblings by parental 146(8):1032-1036.
report. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45(1):221-229. 18. Ozonoff S, Young GS, Carter A, et al. Recurrence risk for autism
2. Sacrey LR, Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, et al. Can parents’ con- spectrum disorders: a Baby Siblings Research Consortium study.
cerns predict autism spectrum disorder? A prospective study of Pediatrics. 2011;128(3):e488-e495.
high-risk siblings from 6-to-36 months of age. J Am Acad Child 19. Grønborg TK, Schendel DE, Parner ET. Recurrence of autism
Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015;54(6):470-478. spectrum disorders in full- and half-siblings and trends over time:
3. Ryan S, Salisbury H. ‘‘You know what boys are like’’: pre- a population-based cohort study. JAMA Pediatr. 2013;167(10):
diagnosis experiences of parents of children with autism spectrum 947-953.
conditions. Br J Gen Pract. 2012;62:e378-e383. 20. Risch N, Hoffmann TJ, Anderson M, Croen LA, Grether JK,
4. Rogers SJ, Vismara L, Wagner AL, McCormick C, Young G, Windham GC. Familial recurrence of autism spectrum disorder:
Ozonoff S. Autism treatment in the first year of life: a pilot study evaluating genetic and environmental contributions. Am J Psy-
of infant start, a parent-implemented intervention for symoto- chiatry. 2014;171(11):1206-1213.
matic infants. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(12):2981-2995. 21. Sandin S, Lichtenstein P, Kuja-Halkola R, Larsson H, Hultman
5. Schreibman L, Dawson G, Stahmer AC, et al. Naturalistic devel- CM, Reichenberg A. The familial risk of autism. JAMA. 2014;
opmental behavioral interventions: empirically validated treat- 311(17):1770-1777.
ments for autism spectrum disorder [published online ahead of 22. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Prevalence of autism
print 2015]. J Autism Dev Disorders. doi:10.1007/s10803-015- spectrum disorder among children ages 8 years—autism and
2407-8. developmental disabilities network. 11 sites, United States.
6. Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Lord C, et al. Clinical assessment and 2010. MMWR Surveillance Summaries. 2014;63(2):1-21.
management of toddlers with suspected autism spectrum disorder: 23. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical
insights from studies of high-risk infants. Pediatrics. 2009;123: Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Washington, DC: American
1383-1391. Psychiatric Association; 2013.
7. Elsabbagh M, Mercure E, Hudry K, et al. Infant neural sensitivity 24. Nadig AS, Ozonoff S, Young GS, Rozga A, Sigman M, Rogers
to dynamic eye gaze is associated with later emerging autism. SJ. A prospective study of response to name in infants at risk for
Curr Biol. 2012;22(4):338-342. autism. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161(4):378-383.

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on September 3, 2015


8 Journal of Child Neurology

25. Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Rogers T, Roberts W, Brian J, Szat- 42. Libertus K, Sheperd KA, Ross SW, Landa RJ. Limited fine motor
mari P. Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of and grasping skills in 6-month-old infants at high risk for autism.
life. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2005;23:143-152. Child Dev. 2014;85(6):2218-2231.
26. Ozonoff S, Iosif AM, Baguio F, et al. A prospective study of the 43. Leonard HC, Bedford R, Charman T, et al. Motor development in
emergence of early behavioral signs of autism. J Am Acad Child children at risk of autism: a follow-up study of infant siblings.
Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49(3),256-266. Autism. 2014;18(3):281-291.
27. Brian J, Bryson SE, Garon N, et al. Clinical assessment of autism 44. Bhat AN, Galloway JC, Landa RJ. Relation between early motor
in high-risk 18-month-olds. Autism. 2008;12:433-456. delay and later communication delay in infants at risk for autism.
28. Sullivan M, Finelli J, Marvin A, Garrett-Mayer E, Bauman M, Infant Behav Dev. 2012;35(4):838-846.
Landa R. Response to joint attention in toddlers at risk for autism 45. Flanagan JE, Landa R, Bhat A, Bauman M. Head lag in infants at
spectrum disorder: a prospective study. J Autism Dev Disord. risk for autism: a preliminary study. Am J Occup Ther. 2012;
2007;37(1):37-48. 66(5):577-585.
29. Landa RJ, Holman KC, Garrett-Mayer E. Social and communica- 46. Iverson JM, Wozniak RH. Variation in vocal-motor development
tion development in toddlers with early and later diagnosis of aut- in infant siblings of children with autism. J Autism Dev Disorders.
ism spectrum disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:853-864. 2007;37(1):158-170.
30. Rozga A, Hutman T, Young GS, et al. Behavioral profiles of 47. Sacrey LR, Bryson SE, Zwaigenbaum L. Prospective examination
affected and unaffected siblings of children with autism: contribu- of visual attention during play in infants at high-risk for autism
tion of measures of mother-infant interaction and nonverbal com- spectrum disorder: a longitudinal study from 6 to 36 months of
munication. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011;41:287-301. age. Behav Brain Res. 2013;256:441-450.
31. Mitchell S, Brian J, Zwaigenbaum L, et al. Early language and 48. Charwarska K, Macari S, Shic F. Decreased spontaneous attention
communication development of infants later diagnosed with aut- to social scenes in 6-month-olds later diagnosed with autism spec-
ism spectrum disorder. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2006;27(2 suppl): trum disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74(3):195-203.
S69-S78. 49. Jones W, Klin A. Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2-6-
32. Wan MW, Green J, Elsabbagh M, et al. Quality of interaction month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature. 2013;
between at-risk infants and caregiver at 12-15 months is associ- 507(7480):427-431.
ated with 3-year autism outcome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 50. Yoder P, Stone WL, Walden T, Malesa E. Predicting social
2012;54(7):763-771. impairment and ASD diagnosis in younger siblings of children
33. Wan MW, Green J, Elsabbagh M, et al. Parent-infant interaction with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39:
in infant siblings at risk of autism. Res Dev Disabilities. 2012; 1381-1391.
33(3):924-932. 51. Garon N, Bryson SE, Zwaigenbaum L, et al. Temperament and its
34. Landa R, Garret-Mayer E. Development in infants with autism relationship to autistic symptoms in a high-risk infant sib cohort. J
spectrum disorders: a prospective study. J Child Psychol Psychia- Abnorm Child Psychol. 2009;37(1):59-78.
try. 2006;47(6):629-638. 52. Clifford SM, Hudry K, Elsabbagh M, Charman T, Johnson MH,
35. Loh A, Soman T, Brian J, et al. Stereotyped motor behaviors asso- the BASIS Team. Temperament in the first 2 years of life in
ciated with autism in high-risk infants: a pilot videotape analysis infants at high-risk for autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev
of a sibling sample. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37:25-36. Disord. 2013;43(3):673-686.
36. Bryson SE, Zwaigenbaum L, Brian J, et al. A prospective case 53. Feldman MA, Ward RA, Savona D, et al. Development and initial
series of high-risk infants who developed autism. J Autism Dev validity of a parent report measure of the behavioral development
Disord. 2007;37:12-24. of infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Dis-
37. Ozonoff S, Heung K, Byrd R, et al. The onset of autism: patterns order. 2012;42:13-22.
of symptom emergence in the first years of life. Autism Res. 2008; 54. Hazlett HC, Poe MD, Gerig G, et al. Early brain overgrowth in
1:320-328. autism associated with an increase in cortical surface area before
38. Germani T, Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, et al. Brief report: assess- age 2 years. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(5):467-476.
ment of early sensory processing in infants at high-risk of 55. Lewis JD, Evans AC, Pruett JR, et al. Network inefficiencies in
autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(12): autism spectrum disorder at 24 months. Translational Psychiatry.
3264-3270. 2014;6(4):e388.
39. Baranek GT. Autism during infancy: a retrospective video analy- 56. Elsabbagh M, Volein A, Csibra G, et al. Neural correlates of eye
sis of sensory-motor and social behaviours at 9-12 months of age. gaze processing in the infant broader autism phenotype. Biol Psy-
J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;29:213-224. chiatry. 2009;65(1):31-38.
40. Yirmiya N, Charman T. The prodrome of autism: early behavioral 57. Elsabbagh M, Bruno R, Wan MW, et al. Infant neural sensitive to
and biological signs, regression, peri- and post-natal development dynamic eye gaze relates to quality of parent-infant interaction at
and genetics. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;51(4):432-458. 7-months in infants at-risk for autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;
41. Trevarthen C, Delafield-Butt JT. Autism as a developmental dis- 45(2):283-291.
order in intentional movement and affective engagement. Fron- 58. Raznahan A, Wallace GL, Antezana L, et al. Compared to what?
tiers Integrative Neurosci. 2013;17:49. doi:10.3389/fnint.2013. Early brain overgrowth in autism and the perils of population
00049. norms. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74(8):563-575.

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on September 3, 2015


Sacrey et al 9

59. Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Guo SS, et al. 2000 CDC growth 77. Baird G, Charman T, Baron-Cohen S, et al. A screening instru-
charts for the United States: methods and development. Vital ment for autism at 18 months of age: a 6-year follow-up study.
Health Stat Series. 2000;11(246):1-190. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39:694-702.
60. Zwaigenbaum L, Young GS, Stone WL, et al. Early head growth in 78. Rogers SJ, Wehner DE, Hagerman R. The behavioral phenotype
infants at risk of autism: a Baby Siblings Research Consortium study. in fragile X: symptoms of autism in very young children with fra-
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53(10):1053-1062. gile X syndrome, idiopathic autism, and other developmental dis-
61. Dawson G, Rogers S, Munson J, et al. Randomized, controlled orders. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2001;22(6):409-417.
trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: the Early Start 79. Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore PC, Risi S. The Autism Diagnostic
Denver Model. Pediatrics. 2010;125(1):e17-23. Observation Schedule (ADOS). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psy-
62. Carter AS, Messinger DS, Stone WL, Celimli S, Nahmias AS, chological Services; 2000.
Yoder P. A randomized controlled trial of Hanen’s ‘‘More Than 80. Hernandez RN, Feinberg RL, Vaurio R, Passanante NM, Thomp-
Words’’ in toddlers with early autism symptoms. J Child Psychol son RE, Kaufmann WE. Autism spectrum disorder in fragile X
Psychiatry. 2011;52(7):741-752. syndrome: a longitudinal evaluation. Am J Med Genet A. 2009;
63. Wetherby AM, Guthrie W, Woods J, et al. Parent-implemented 149A(6):1125-1137.
social intervention for toddlers with autism: an RCT. Pediatrics. 81. DiGuiseppi C, Hepburn S, Davis JM, et al. Screening for autism
2014;134(6):1084-1093. spectrum disorders in children with Down syndrome: population
64. Kasari C, Siller M, Huynh LN, et al. Randomized controlled trial prevalence and screening test characteristics. J Devel Behav
of parental responsiveness intervention for toddlers at high risk Pediatr. 2010;31(3):181-191.
for autism. Infant Behav Dev. 2014;37(4):711-721. 82. Molloy CA, Murray DS, Kinsman A, et al. Differences in the clin-
65. Koegel RI, Frea WD. Treatment of social behavior in autism ical presentation of trisomy 21 with and without autism. J Intellect
through the modification of pivotal social skills. J Appl Behav Disability Res. 2009;53(2):143-151.
Anal. 1993;26(3):369-377. 83. Capone G. Down syndrome and autistic spectrum disorders. In:
66. Steiner AM, Gengoux GW, Klin A, Chawarska K. Pivotal Cohen W, Madnick M, eds. Down Syndrome: Visions of the
response treatment for infants at-risk for autism spectrum disor- 21st Century. New York, NY: John Wiley; 2002: 327-338.
der: a pilot study. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43(1):91-102. 84. Bennett JA, Germani T, Haqq AM, Zwaigenbaum L. Autism
67. Landa RJ, Kalb LG. Long-term outcomes of toddlers with autism spectrum disorder in Prader-Willi syndrome: a systematic review.
spectrum disorders are exposed to short-term intervention. Pedia- Unpublished manuscript; 2015.
trics. 2012;130(suppl 2):S186-S190. 85. Veltman MW, Craig EE, Bolton PF. Autism spectrum disorders in
68. Hatton DD, Sideris J, Skinner M, et al. Autistic behavior in chil- Prader-Willi and Angelman syndromes: a systematic review. Psy-
dren with fragile X syndrome: prevalence, stability, and the chiatry Genet. 2005;15(4):243-254.
impact of FMRP. Am J Med Genet A. 2006;140A(17):1804-1813. 86. Dimitropoulos A, Ho A, Feldman B. Social responsiveness and
69. Moss J, Oliver C, Arron K, Burbidge C, Berg K. The prevalence competence in Prader-Willi syndrome: direct comparison to
and phenomenology of repetitive behavior in genetic syndromes. autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;
J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39(4):572-588. 43(1),103-113.
70. Warner G, Moss J, Smith P, Howlin P. Autism characteristics and 87. Milner KM, Craig EE, Thompson RJ, et al. Prader-Willi syn-
behavioural disturbances in *500 children with Down’s syn- drome: intellectual abilities and behavioural features by genetic
drome in England and Wales. Autism Res. 2014;7(4):433-441. subtype. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Disciplines. 2005;
71. Bruining H, Eijkemans MJ, Kas MJ, Curran SR, Vorstman JA, 46(10):1089-1096.
Bolton PF. Behavioral signatures related to genetic disorders in 88. Vorstman JA, Staal WG, van Daalen E, van Engeland H, Hoch-
autism. Mol Autism. 2014;5(1):11. stenbach PF, Franke L. Identification of novel autism candidate
72. Falkmer T, Anderson K, Falkmer M, Horlin C. Diagnostic proce- regions through analysis of reported cytogenetic abnormalities
dures in autism spectrum disorders: a systematic literature review. associated with autism. Mol Psychiatry. 2006;11(1):18-28.
Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;22(6):329-340. 89. Vorstman JAS, Ophoff RA. Genetic causes of developmental dis-
73. Hall SS, Lightbody AA, Hirt M, Rezvani A, Reiss AL. Autism in orders. Curr Opin Neurol. 2013;26(2):128-136.
fragile X syndrome: a category mistake? J Am Acad Child Ado- 90. Dykens EM, Lee E, Roof E. Prader-Willi Syndrome and autism
lesc Psychiatry. 2010;49(9):921-933. spectrum disorders: an evolving story. J Neurodev Disorders.
74. Bailey DB, Raspa M, Olmsted M, Holiday DB. Co-occurring condi- 2011;3(3):225-237.
tions associated with FMR1 gene variations: findings from a national 91. Lo ST, Siemensma E, Collin P, Hokken-Koelega A. Impaired the-
parent survey. Am J Med Genet A. 2008;146(16):2060-2069. ory of mind and symptoms of autism spectrum disorder in chil-
75. Reilly C, Senior J, Murtagh L. ASD, ADHD, mental health con- dren with Prader-Willi syndrome. Res Dev Disabilities. 2013;
ditions and psychopharmacology in neurogenetic syndromes: par- 34(9):2764-2773.
ent survey. J Intellect Disability Res. 2015;59(4):307-318. 92. Ogata H, Ihara H, Murakami N, Gito M, Kido Y, Nagai T. Autism
76. Scambler DJ, Hepburn SL, Hagerman RJ, Rogers SJ. A prelimi- spectrum disorders and hyperactive/impulsive behaviors in Japa-
nary study of screening for risk of autism in children with fragile nese patients with Prader-Willi syndrome: a comparison between
X syndrome: testing two risk cut-offs for the Checklist for Autism maternal uniparental disomy and deletion cases. Am J Med Genet
in Toddlers. J Intellect Disabilities Res. 2007;51(4):269-276. A. 2014;164A(9):2180-2186.

Downloaded from jcn.sagepub.com at UNIV CALIFORNIA SAN DIEGO on September 3, 2015

You might also like